Next Article in Journal
Viewing Garden Scenes: Interaction Between Gaze Behavior and Physiological Responses
Previous Article in Journal
A Two-step Approach for Interest Estimation from Gaze Behavior in Digital Catalog Browsing
 
 
Journal of Eye Movement Research is published by MDPI from Volume 18 Issue 1 (2025). Previous articles were published by another publisher in Open Access under a CC-BY (or CC-BY-NC-ND) licence, and they are hosted by MDPI on mdpi.com as a courtesy and upon agreement with Bern Open Publishing (BOP).
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Pursuit Eye Movements in Dyslexic Children: Evidence for an Immaturity of Brain Oculomotor Structures?

by
Simona Caldani
1,2,
Christophe-Loïc Gerard
3,
Hugo Peyre
1,3,4 and
Maria Pia Bucci
1,2
1
CNRS-Université Paris Nanterre, France
2
EFEE-Centre d'exploration fonctionnelle de l’équilibre chez l’enfant, Robert Debré Hospital, Paris, France
3
Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Department, Robert Debré Hospital, Paris, France
4
Paris Diderot University, France
J. Eye Mov. Res. 2020, 13(1), 1-10; https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.13.1.5
Submission received: 22 October 2019 / Published: 25 May 2020

Abstract

:
Background: Dyslexia is a disorder found in 5–10% of school-aged children. Several studies reported visual deficits and oculomotor abnormalities in dyslexic children. The objective of our study was to examine horizontal pursuit performance in dyslexic children, despite its poor involvement in reading. Methods: Eye movements were recorded by video-oculography in 92 children (46 dyslexic children, mean age: 9.77 ± 0.26 and 46 non dyslexic, IQand age-matched children). Both the number of catch-up saccades occurring during pursuit task and the gain of pursuit were measured. Results: Catch-up saccades were significantly more frequent in the dyslexic group than in the non-dyslexic group of children. Pursuit performance (in terms of the number of catchup saccades and gain) significantly improved with increasing age in the non-dyslexic children group only. Conclusions: The atypical pursuit patterns observed in dyslexic children suggest a deficiency in the visual attentional processing and an immaturity of brain structures responsible for pursuit triggering. This finding needs to be validated by neuroimaging studies on dyslexia population.

Introduction

Dyslexia is a disorder found in 5–10% of school-aged children (Peterson & Pennington, 2012). The origin of dyslexia is still not well known; it is a complex reading disorder involving genetic and environmental factors (Bishop, 2015). The hypothesis of a phonological deficit in dyslexia has been shared by several authors (Brady, Shankweiler, & Mann, 1983; Bruck, 1992; Snowling, 1995) but other researchers suggested that the phonological theory is partially correct and that it cannot explain all deficiencies reported in dyslexia and other theories have been proposed such as auditory, visual perception, working memory, and attentional abnormalities (Brosnan et al., 2002; Facoetti, Lorusso, Paganoni, Cattaneo, Galli & Mascetti, 2003; Nicolson & Fawcett, 1990; Stein, Riddell & Fowler, 1988; Tallal, 1980). For instance, several studies reported visual deficits and oculomotor abnormalities in dyslexic children, supporting the hypothesis of deficiencies in the magnocellular system (Stein, Riddell & Fowler, 1988; Eden, Stein, Wood & Wood, 1994) and of a cerebellar impairment (Nicolson, Fawcett & Dean, 2001; Freedman, Molholm, Gray, Belyusar & Fox, 2017). However Kronbichler, Hutzler & Wimmer (2002) and Hutzler, Kronbichler, Jacobs & Wimmer (2006) did not support the hypothesis of deficiencies of the magnocellular system. Our group further explored oculomotor capabilities in dyslexic children and suggested a deficit in visual attentional processing in relationship with an immaturity of cortical structures responsible for saccade triggering (Bucci, Nassibi, Gerard, Bui-Quoc & Seassau, 2012; Seassau, Gerard, Bui-Quoc & Bucci, 2014; Tiadi, Gérard, Peyre, Bui-Quoc & Bucci, 2016). Note however that some of the cited authors have conducted more recent studies in which they reported the difficulties to know if visual deficits are causal or consequential of dyslexia (Carroll, Solity & Shapiro, 2016). These authors reported deficits in visual search as well as in auditory processing in children that were poor readers, suggesting that causes of dyslexia could be multiple, interacting and probabilistic, rather than deterministic. Finally, the hypothesis of an impairment of the visual system in dyslexia is still under debate given that some recent researchers think that this deficiency could be a consequence and not the cause of dyslexia (see Stein, 2018, 2019; Blythe, Kirkby & Liversedge, 2018). Studies done in monkeys by recording middle temporal visual area (MT) and medial superior temporal area (MST) (Maunsell & Newsome, 1987; Newsome & Pare, 1988) reported that the magnocellular system provides the major input to cortical motion-processing during pursuit. Recall that pursuit eye movements are used to pursue a slowly moving small object. Subjects match the velocity of the eyes to the velocity of the object in order to keep its images on the foveae and to ensure online processing of visual signals during object movement (Leigh & Zee, 2015). Frequently, when the eyes are not able to keep up with the motion of the object, subjects make saccadic movements (catchup saccades) in order to reduce or eliminate the positional error. Moreover, in the literature, it has been shown that an essential prerequisite for a correct pursuit is maintaining attention, since diverting attention impairs pursuit performance, particularly the gain (Kerzel, Souto & Ziegler, 2008). In a more recent study, Chen, Valsecchi & Gegenfurtner (2017), in order to study the relationship between attention and pursuit performances, recorded visual potentials and showed that the attentional engagement started with the beginning of the pursuit movement. However, the mechanisms controlling both catch-up saccades and pursuits are still poorly understood, although common structures seem to be involved in their triggering. For instance, motor or position error signals in the superior colliculus could be shared by the saccadic and pursuit systems (Krauzlis, 2004) and cerebellar lesions, particularly of the oculomotor vermis affect performance of both saccades and pursuits (Takagi, Zee & Tamargo, 2000). At the cortical level, there is anatomical evidence for connections between structures responsible for saccades and pursuits (Agarwal et al., 2016). Pursuit movements in dyslexia have received relatively little attention, most likely because of their poor involvement in reading; however, exploring pursuit performance in dyslexic children could give some insight into the cortical/subcortical impairment and visual attention capabilities in the dyslexic population. Interestingly, Callu et al. (2005) suggested that phonological awareness and pursuits could share common cognitive components, probably important for later reading. Pursuit can be stimulated and recorded in different ways and this is, most likely, the consequence of different results reported in the dyslexic population. Pavlidis (1981) was the first to report, in a small group of dyslexic children (12), more erratic eye movements than control children when following sequentially illuminated light sources. Black, Collins, De Roach & Zubrick, (1984) reported that pursuit performance was similar between dyslexic (26) and non-dyslexic (34) children, but that about 25% of dyslexics showed more intrusive saccades during pursuit movements. In addition, Eden, Stein, Wood & Wood (1994) found poor pursuit in dyslexic children (twenty-six), particularly when pursuing a target moving from left to right and Fukushima, Tanaka, Jeremy, Williams & Fukushima (2005), reporting poor gain of pursuits in children with dyslexia (18), suggested deficits in the frontal cortex, particularly frontal eye fields and supplementary eye fields, in line with electrophysiological studies done on monkeys (Gottlieb, MacAvoy & Bruce, 1994; Fukushima, Sato, Fukushima, Shinmei & Kaneko, 2000; Fukushima, Akao, Takeichi, Kaneko & Fukushima, 2004). Other studies did not find such differences concerning between dyslexic and non-dyslexic children, most likely due to different methods used to stimulate and record pursuit eye movements (Brown et al., 1983; Stanley, Smith & Howell, 1983; Olson, Kliegl & Davidson, 1983; Ygge, Lennerstrand, Rydberg, Wijecoon & Pettersson, 1993). Indeed Brown et al. (1983) analysed only the presence of predictive eye movements during pursuit and its performance were just only subjectively judged. More precisely in this study eye movements were recorded by using a limbal sensing device (with low sampling rate of 50 Hz) and the quality of pursuit was qualitatively evaluated by a "blind" observer into seven categories, ranging from good to poor. Stanley, Smith & Howell (1983) used five light-emitting diodes (LEDs) flashed sequentially one at a time with onset of the next light following immediately upon offset of the preceding light, at speed of 500/1000 ms and used an eye tracker with one/two degrees of absolute positional accuracy. Olson, Kliegl & Davidson, (1983) recorded pupil and corneal reflection positions by using a television image. Ygge, Lennerstrand, Rydberg, Wijecoon & Pettersson (1993) also evaluated smooth pursuit eye movement’s performance only subjectively and subjects were invited to follow the investigator’s index finger. Based on all these findings, the goal of the present study was to record pursuits with an eye tracker with a precision of about 0.25° in a large groupof Frenchdyslexic children, and to compare these results with those obtained from an IQ-, age-matched non-dyslexic group of children. We want to explore further pursuits performance in this population by measuring the gain and the number of catch up saccades. Our hypothesis was that by comparing dyslexic children with age-matched non-dyslexic children we could report deficits in visual attentional capabilities in dyslexia population maybe due immaturity of central structures responsible of pursuit triggering. Note, however, that this study cannot resolve the open question where visual deficits are the cause or the consequence of dyslexia.

Methods

Forty-six dyslexic children from 7.5 to 13 years old (mean age: 9.77 ± 0.26 years) participated in the study. Dyslexic children were recruited from Robert Debré pediatric hospital, to which they had been referred for a complete evaluation of their dyslexia including an extensive examination of their neurological/psychological and phonological capabilities. For each child, we measured the time they required to read a text passage, assessed their general text comprehension, and evaluated their ability to read words and pseudo-words using the L2MA battery (Chevrie-Muller, Simon & Fournier, 1997). This is the standard test in France. It was developed by the “Centre de Psychologie appliquée de Paris” and is used to detect dyslexic populations. Inclusion criteria were scored on the L2MA, and were: more than two standard deviations from the mean, a normal mean intelligence quotient (IQ, evaluated using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for ChildrenFourth Edition, WISC-IV), namely between 85 and 115, and normal visual acuity for both distance vision and near vision (both eyes ≥10/10). Note that, as reported by Capano, Minden, Chen, Schachar & Ickowicz (2008) the association between dyslexia and ADHD is high given that about 26% of ADHD had a comorbid reading disability. More recently, McGrath and Stoodley (2019) in a metaanalysis reported an overlap of gray matter between dyslexia and ADHD in the right caudate that is related to share cognitive activities for executive functions and/or procedural learning. Consequently in the present study any hyperactivity deficit was excluded using the ADHD Rating Scale-parental report (ADHD-RS) (Du Paul, Power, Anastopoulos & Reid, 1998).
Note also that the selection of children with dyslexia follows the criteria of DMS V even if recently Miciak & Fletcher (2020) suggested to use a Multi-Tiered Systems of Support model (MTSS) for diagnosis of dyslexia. However, this model needs to be tested in further studies.
Forty-six chronological agematched non-dyslexic children (mean age: 9.81 ± 0.33 years old) were also examined. The inclusion criteria were as follows: no known neurological or psychiatric abnormalities, no history of reading difficulty, no visual impairment, or difficulty with near vision. The recruitment of TD children was based on voluntary participation; they were sons and daughters of hospital employees. Also measure of IQ was estimated in the control group using two subtests, one assessing their verbal capability (similarities test) and one assessing their logic capability reasoning (matrix reasoning test). The normal range for both tests is 10 ± 3 (WISC-IV, Sattler & Dumont, 2004). All the healthy children we tested had normal verbal (11.45 ± 0.8) and logic (10.94 ± 0.5) capabilities. Clinical characteristics of the two groups of children are shown in Table 1.
The reading age of all children was assessed using the ELFE test (Evaluation of Fluency Reading) (www.cognisciences.com, Grenoble), in which the child have to read aloud a text for duration of one minute. Children came from different social and ethnic backgrounds, thus representing the French population.
The investigation adhered to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Institutional Human Experimentation Committee of CPP Ile de France I (Hotel-Dieu Hospital).

Pursuit task 

The pursuit task requires participants to follow a slowly moving visual target. Stimuli were displayed on a 22’ computer monitor. A red circular target waveform, approximately 0.5° of visual angle, was used twice with the same velocity (15°/s). The target was initially placed in the central position (0°) and then moved horizontally to one side until it reached the ±20° location, where it reversed abruptly and moved to the opposite side. A total of nine trials were run and included in the analysis. Children were instructed to keep their eyes on the target, wherever it moved. After a short break, children had to perform the same test again (Lions, Bui-Quoc, Wiener-Vacher, Seassau & Bucci, 2013).

Eye movement recording 

Eye movements were recorded by a non-invasive system using infrared camera to record horizontal and vertical eye position independently and simultaneously for each eye: the Mobile EyeBrain Tracker (SuriCog). The PC monitor was placed 60 cm with a screen of 22 inch, the resolution was 1920×1080 and the refresh rate was 60 Hz. This eye tracker is a medical device EC-marked for medical purposes. Recording frequency was set to 300 Hz. The mobile EBT is linear and the precision of the system is 0.25 deg. Calibration was done at the beginning of each eye movement recording. Calibration was done under binocular viewing. During the calibration procedure, children were asked to fixate a grid of 13 points (diameter 0.5 deg) mapping the screen. Point positions in degree in the horizontal/vertical plans were: 20.9°/12.2°;0°/12.2°; 20.9°/12.2°; -10.8°/6.2°; 10.8°/6.2°;-20.9°/0°; 0°/0°; 20.9°/0°; 10.8°/-6.2°; 10.8°/-6.2°; -20.9°/-12.2°; 0°/-12.2° and 20.9°/-12.2°. Each calibration point required a fixation of 250 ms to be validated. A polynomial function with five parameters was used to fit the calibration data and to determine the visual angles. There is no obstruction of the visual field with the recording system and the calibrated zone covers a visual angle of ±22 deg.

Procedure 

Children were seated on a chair in a dark room, in front of a flat screen displaying the target stimulating pursuit eye movements. The head of the child was held straight with a headrest; viewing was binocular.

Data analysis 

Calibration factors for each eye were determined from the eye positions during the calibration procedure (Lions, Bui-Quoc, Wiener-Vacher, Seassau & Bucci, 2013). The software MeyeAnalysis (provided with the eyetracker) was used to extract pursuit eye movements from the data. Detection of saccades during pursuit was based on criteria of minimum amplitude (2°) and velocity (30°/s). Catch-up saccades were defined as saccades in the target direction that served to reduce position error and to bring the eye closer to the target. The number of catchup saccades was counted for each trial. Amplitude was measured for each catch-up saccade. Pursuit gain corresponds to the relationship between the speed of the eye and the speed of the target and it was obtained by dividing eye velocity by target velocity for each trial. If its value is around 1 it means that the correspondence between the target and the eye is perfect. Scores were then averaged across trials for each test (Lions, Bui-Quoc, Wiener-Vacher, Seassau & Bucci, 2013). Statistical analysis was performed with the Statistica software. In order to compare group differences, we used a univariate one-way ANOVA in which the dependent variables were the number of saccades and the gain, and the independent variable was the group. In the case of significant effects post-hoc Fischer’s test (LSD) was performed. The effect of a factor was considered significant when the p-value was below 0.05. Correlations between gain and catchup were examined in the two groups of participants with Pearson correlation coefficients (significant p-value below 0.05). A linear regression model was used for eye movements’ parameters (catch-up saccades and gain value) with the predictor variable being the chronological age in order to investigate the influence of chronological age on eye movements’ performances (significant p-value below 0.05).

Results

Figure 1 shows the mean number of saccades (A) and the gain (B) for both groups of children examined; ANOVA reported a significant group effect between the dyslexic and the non dyslexic children.
The number of catch-up saccades occurring during pursuit movements was significantly higher in dyslexic children (F(1,90) = 58.26, p< 0.0001) and the gain of pursuits was significantly lower in dyslexic with respect to nondyslexic children (F(1,90) = 35.2, p< 0.0001). Only for the non-dyslexic children a significant correlation between the number of saccades and gain was also found (R2= 0.09, p < 0.03) (see Figure 2).
Figure 3 shows the number of catch-up saccades measured during pursuits for dyslexic and non-dyslexic children examined as function of the age (in years) of each child tested. We found an association between number of saccades and age among children in the non-dyslexic group (R²=0.22 β = -2.27, p-value < 0.001), with a decrease of two saccades per year (in this group of children). However, we did not find any significant association for the dyslexic children (R2 = -0.000024, p = 0.9). The interaction of age x group was not significant for the number of saccades (F(2)=1.91, p = 0.1546).
Furthermore, the mean gain (ratio of the eye velocity over the target velocity, see Figure 4) increased significantly with the increasing age of the non-dyslexic children (R2=0.16, β =0.03, p<0.004).
In other words, non-dyslexic children showed an improvement in the value of the gain of 3% every year. Concerning dyslexic children, however, the gain did not change with increasing the age (R2 = 0.008, p = 0.5).
We found also an interaction age x group for the value of the gain (F(2)=3.91, p < 0.0237), suggesting that the gain in non-dyslexic children improved with age compared to dyslexic children.

Discussion

The main results reported in the present study were: i) dyslexic children showed a higher number of catch-up saccades and poor precision (that is lower gain) values during pursuit with respect to non-dyslexic age-matched children; moreover a significant correlation was found only in the group of non-dyslexic children; ii) Contrary to non-dyslexic age-matched children, dyslexic children did not show any improvement in pursuit performance with age. These findings are discussed below.

Poor pursuit performance in dyslexic children 

Dyslexic children, when compared to age-matched nondyslexic children, showed a larger number of catch-up saccades and reduced gain values. This finding confirms and enlarges some previous studies done on a small number of children (Adler-Grinberg & Stark, 1978; Black, Collins, DeRoach & Zubrick, 1984; Eden, Stein, Wood & Wood, 1994; Fukushima et al., 2005). Nevertheless, some studies, probably because of the different methodologies used, found no differences between dyslexic and non-dyslexic children (Brown et al., 1983; Stanley Smith & Howell, 1983; Olson, Kliegl & Davidson, 1983, Ygge, Lennerstrand, Rydberg, Wijecoon & Pettersson, 1993). Research has suggested that an elevated important number of catchup saccades is observed when subjects have to compensate for pursuit deficiencies because the eye velocity is reduced, or increased, compared to target velocity (Damilou, Apostolakis, Thrapsanioti, Theleritis & Smyrnis, 2016). In the literature it has been shown that parietal areas are related to the suppression of saccades during pursuit as well as cerebellum (Nagel et al., 2006). Moreover, microstimulation studies have shown that the oculomotor vermis could trigger saccades during pursuit (Krauzlis & Miles, 1998; Takagi, Zee & Tamargo, 1998). Based on these findings, we could hypothesize that dyslexic children present dysfunctions in these cerebral areas, even if neuroimaging studies will be necessary to confirm our hypothesis. In line with that, the lack of correlation between the number of saccades and gain for the dyslexic children suggested the presence of an inhibitory control defect for this group of children. This finding could be explained by the presence of a deficit of prefrontal and fronto-striatal circuits that seem to be related to the ability to inhibit saccades during the pursuit task (Leigh & Zee, 2015). Furthermore, reduced pursuit gain could be related to abnormalities in processing visual motion information in the extrastriate cortex, or in its visuo-motor transformation in the parietal or frontal cortex (Lencer et al., 2015). In line with these studies, we could assume that dyslexic children present cerebral dysfunction also in these regions. Finally, as already mentioned in the Introduction section, attentional deficiencies could play a pivotal role in performing pursuit movements correctly. In the literature it has been reported that dyslexic children have a specific disability in orienting as well as in sustained focusing of visuo-spatial attention (Facoetti, Lorusso, Paganoni, Cattaneo, Galli & Mascetti, 2003). In view of the fact that attention and eye movements share the same cerebral circuits as well as parietal and frontal areas (Sheliga, Craighero, Riggio & Rizzolatti, 1997) we could suggest an alteration of these cortical structures in dyslexic children. Moreover Biscaldi, Fischer & Hartnegg (2000) showed that a specific visual attentional training in dyslexic children improved saccadic performance, particularly concerning voluntary saccade that are elicited by frontal lobe.

Absence of improvement with age in pursuit performance in dyslexic children 

In the literature it has been shown that the pursuit system is immature in the first years of life and that it improves until adolescence (Luna, Velanova & Geier, 2008). These authors reported that the development of the cortical network correlated with eye movement triggering is in relationship with the processes of myelination, synaptic pruning and maturation of gray matter, which reaches adult levels in adolescence. Given the several cortical and sub-cortical structures implicated for pursuit triggering (Fukushima, Akao, Kurkin, Kaneko & Fukushima, 2006), we suggest that the absence of improvement of pursuit performances with age in dyslexic children could be due to immaturity of these areas responsible for pursuit realization. Moreover, as was mentioned in the introduction, we also have to point out that the attention system represents a cognitive factor that is active during pursuit, and in the literature it has been demonstrated that attentional functions are not mature yet in childhood. Indeed, Konrad et al. (2005), using fMRI, explored neural mechanisms of attention and showed that children, when compared to adults, reported significantly reduced neural activity in defined regions-of-interest for attentional mechanisms (particularly in right-sided frontal-midbrain regions during alerting, in the right-sided temporo-parietal junction during reorienting of attention, and in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex during executive control of attention). It is also well known that dyslexic subjects show visuo-attentional difficulties (Facoetti, Lorusso, Paganoni, Cattaneo, Galli & Mascetti, 2003), and impairment in attentional engagement has been observed under different tasks (Konrad et al., 2005; Valdois, Bosse & Tainturier, 2004; Facoetti et al., 2006; Bosse, Tainturier & Valdois, 2007). According to these studies, we could hypothesise that poor pursuit performance could be due to immaturity of cerebral areas controlling attention capabilities.

Conclusion

Our data suggest that dyslexic children show atypical pursuit patterns (a lack of improvement in pursuit performance with age, a higher number of catch-up saccades and poor gain values) with respect to non-dyslexic agematched children, probably due to an immaturity or dysfunction of brain structures responsible for pursuit triggering, as well as to a deficiency in the visual attentional processing.

Limitations and further research 

In term of limitations we suggest that, in order to study and, eventually confirm, the presence of an interaction between age and group of children (dyslexic children as well as non-dyslexic age-matched children) in pursuit performance, it is necessary to replicate the same results on a larger sample. Moreover, in the future, in order to explore more deeply the presence of cerebral dysfunction and/or immaturity in dyslexic children we could use other oculomotor paradigms (as well as reflexive or voluntary saccade or fixation) that solicited others brain areas. Finally, it could be interesting to study the effects of a visual reading training on the cerebral area implicated in the triggering of pursuit; indeed, as shown by Olulade, Napoliello & Eden (2013), there is a relationship between reading ability and the activity of area V5/MT during visual motion processing. Moreover in the future, it could be interesting to further explore the relationship between attentional capabilities and pursuit performances, given the high percentage of attentional deficits comorbidity in subjects with dyslexia (Goldfarb and Shaul, 2013).

Ethics and Conflict of Interest

The author(s) declare(s) that the contents of the article are in agreement with the ethics described in http://biblio.unibe.ch/portale/elibrary/BOP/jemr/ethics.html and that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

References

  1. Adler-Grinberg, D., and L. Stark. 1978. Eye movements, scan paths, and dyslexia. American journal of optometry and physiological optics 55, 8: 557–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Agarwal, M., J.L. Ulmer, T. Chandra, A.P. Klein, L.P. Mark, and S. Mohan. 2016. Imaging correlates of neural control of ocular movements. European radiology 26, 7: 2193–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Biscaldi, M., B. Fischer, and K. Hartnegg. 2000. Voluntary saccadic control in dyslexia. Perception 29, 5: 50921. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Bishop, D.V. 2015. The interface between genetics and psychology: lessons from developmental dyslexia. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 282, 1806: 20143139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Black, J.L., W.K. Collins, J.N. DeRoach, and S.R. Zubrick. 1984. Smooth pursuit eye movements in normal and dyslexic children. Perceptual and Motor Skills 59: 91–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Brady, S., D. Shankweiler, and V. Mann. 1983. Speech perception and memory coding in relation to reading ability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 35, 2: 345–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Brosnan, M., J. Demetre, S. Hamill, K. Robson, H. Shepherd, and G. Cody. 2002. Executive functioning in adults and children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 40, 12: 2144–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Bruck, M. 1992. Persistence of dyslexics' phonological deficits. Developmental Psychology 28, 5: 874–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Blythe, H.I., J.A. Kirkby, and S.P. Liversedge. 2018. Comments on: "What Is Developmental Dyslexia? "Brain Sci. 2018, 8, 26. The Relationship between Eye Movements and Reading Difficulties. Brain Sciences 8, 6: 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Bosse, M.L., M.J. Tainturier, and S. Valdois. 2007. Developmental dyslexia: the visual attention span deficit hypothesis. Cognition 04, 2: 198–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Brown, B., G. Haegerstrom-Portnoy, C.D. Yingling, J. Herron, D. Galin, and M. Marcus. 1983. Tracking eye movements are normal in dyslexic children. American Journal of Optometry and Physiological Optics 60: 376–383. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Bucci, M.P., N. Nassibi, C.L. Gerard, E. Bui-Quoc, and M. Seassau. 2012. Immaturity of the oculomotor saccade and vergence interaction in dyslexic children: evidence from a reading and visual search study. PLoS One 7, 3: e33458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Callu, D., I. Giannopulu, S. Escolano, F. Cusin, M. Jacquier-Roux, and G. Dellatolas. 2005. Smooth pursuit eye movements are associated with phonological awareness in preschool children. Brain Cognition 58, 2: 217–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Capano, L., D. Minden, S. Chen, R. J. Schachar, and A. Ickowicz. 2008. Mathematical learning disorder in school-age children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. La Revue canadienne de psychiatrie 53, 6: 392–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Carroll, J.M., J. Solity, and L.R. Shapiro. 2016. Predicting dyslexia using prereading skills: the role of sensorimotor and cognitive abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 57, 6: 750–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Chen, J., M. Valsecchi, and K.R. Gegenfurtner. 2017. Attention is allocated closely ahead of the target during smooth pursuit eye movements: Evidence from EEG frequency tagging. Neuropsychologia 28, 102: 206–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Chevrie-Muller, C., A.M. Simon, and S. Fournier. 1997. Batterie Langage oral ecrit. Mémoire. Attention (L2MA) Paris, Editions du Centre de Psychologie appliquée. [Google Scholar]
  18. Damilou, A., S. Apostolakis, E. Thrapsanioti, C. Theleritis, and N. Smyrnis. 2016. Psychophysiology Shared and distinct oculomotor function deficits in schizophrenia and obsessive compulsive disorder. Psychophysiology 53: 796–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Du Paul, G.J., T.J. Power, A.D. Anastopoulos, and R. Reid. 1998. ADHD Rating Scale-IV: checklists, norms and clinical interpretation. Guilford: New York, NY. [Google Scholar]
  20. Eden, G.F., J.F. Stein, H.M. Wood, and F.B. Wood. 1994. Differences in eye movements and reading problems in dyslexic and normal children. Vision Research 34, 10: 1345–1358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Facoetti, A., M.L. Lorusso, P. Paganoni, C. Cattaneo, R. Galli, and G.G. Mascetti. 2003. The time course of attentional focusing in dyslexic and normally reading children. Brain Cognition 53, 2: 1814. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Facoetti, A., M. Zorzi, L. Cestnick, M.L. Lorusso, M. Molteni, P. Paganoni, C. Umilta, and G.G. Mascetti. 2006. The relationship between visuo-spatial attention and nonword reading in developmental dyslexia. Cognitive Neuropsychology 23, 6: 841–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Freedman, E.G., S. Molholm, M.J. Gray, D. Belyusar, and J.J. Foxe. 2017. Saccade adaptation deficits in developmental dyslexia suggest disruption of cerebellardependent learning. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 9, 1: 36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Fukushima, K., T. Sato, J. Fukushima, Y. Shinmei, and C.R.S. Kaneko. 2000. Activity of smooth pursuit-related neurons in the monkey periarcuate cortex during pursuit and passive whole body rotation. Journal of Neurophysiology 83: 563–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Fukushima, J., T. Akao, N. Takeichi, C.R.S. Kaneko, and K. Fukushima. 2004. Pursuit-related neurons in the supplementary eye fields: discharge during pursuit and passive whole body rotation. Journal of Neurophysiology 91: 2809–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Fukushima, J., S. Tanaka, D. Jeremy, J.D. Williams, and K. Fukushima. 2005. Voluntary control of saccadic and smooth-pursuit eye movements in children with learning disorders. Brain & Development 27: 579–588. [Google Scholar]
  27. Fukushima, J., T. Akao, S. Kurkin, C.R. Kaneko, and K. Fukushima. 2006. The vestibular-related frontal cortex and its role in smooth-pursuit eye movements and vestibular-pursuit interactions. Journal of Vestibular Research 16, 1-2: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Goldfarb, L., and S. Shaul. 2013. Abnormal attentional internetwork link in dyslexic readers. Neuropsychology 27, 6: 725–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Gottlieb, J.P., M.G. MacAvoy, and C.J. Bruce. 1994. Neural responses related to smooth pursuit eye movements and their correspondence with electrically elicited slow eye movements in the primate frontal eye field. Journal of Neurophysiology 72: 1634–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Hutzler, F., M. Kronbichler, A.M. Jacobs, and H. Wimmer. 2006. Perhaps correlational but not causal: no effect of dyslexic readers' magnocellular system on their eye movements during reading. Neuropsychologia 44, 4: 637–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kerzel, D., D. Souto, and N.E. Ziegler. 2008. Effects of attention shifts to stationary objects during steadystate smooth pursuit eye movements. Vision Research 48, 7: 958–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Konrad, K., S. Neufang, C.M. Thiel, K. Specht, C. Hanisch, J. Fan, B. Herpertz-Dahlmann, and G.R. Fink. 2005. Development of attentional networks: an fMRI study with children and adults. Neuroimage 28, 2: 429–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Krauzlis, R.J., and F.A. Miles. 1998. Role of the oculomotor vermis in generating pursuit and saccades: effects of microstimulation. Journal of Neurophysiology 80, 4: 2046–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Krauzlis, R. J. Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system (2004). Journal of Neurophysiology 91, 2: 591–603. [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Kronbichler, M., F. Hutzler, and H. Wimmer. 2002. Dyslexia: verbal impairments in the absence of magnocellular impairments. Neuroreport 13, 5: 617–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Leigh, R.G., and D.S. Zee. 2015. The Neurology of Eye Movements, 5th ed. University Press: Oxford. [Google Scholar]
  37. Lencer, R., A. Sprenger, J.L. Reilly, J.E. McDowell, L.H. Rubin, J.A. Badner, M.S. Keshavan, G.D. Pearlson, C.A. Tamminga, E.S. Gershon, B.A. Clementz, and J.A. Sweeney. 2015. Pursuit eye movements as an intermediate phenotype across psychotic disorders: Evidence from the B-SNIP study. Schizophrenia Research 169, 1-3: 326–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Lions, C., E. Bui-Quoc, S. Wiener-Vacher, M. Seassau, and M.P. Bucci. 2013. Smooth pursuit eye movements in children with strabismus and in children with vergence deficits. PLoS One 8, 12: e83972. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Luna, B., K. Velanova, and C.F. Geier. 2008. Development of eye-movement control. Brain Cognition 68, 3: 293–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Maunsell, J.H.R., and W.T. Newsome. 1987. Visual processing in monkey extrastriate cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience 10: 363–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. McGrath, L.M., and C.J. Stoodley. 2019. Are there shared neural correlates between dyslexia and ADHD? A meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders 11, 1: 31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Miciak, J., and J. M. Fletcher. 2020. The Critical Role of Instructional Response for Identifying Dyslexia and Other Learning Disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Nagel, M., A. Sprenger, S. Zapf, C. Erdmann, D. Kömpf, W. Heide, F. Binkofski, and R. Lencer. 2006. Parametric modulation of cortical activation during smooth pursuit with and without target blanking. an fMRI study. Neuroimage 29, 4: 1319–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Newsome, W.T., and E.B. Pare. 1988. A selective impairment of motion perception following lesions of the middle temporal area (MT). Journal of Neuroscience 8: 2201–2211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Nicolson, R.I., and A.J. Fawcett. 1990. Automaticity: a new framework for dyslexia research? Cognition 35, 2: 159–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Nicolson, R.I., A.J. Fawcett, and P. Dean. 2001. Developmental dyslexia: the cerebellar deficit hypothesis. Trends in Neurosciences 24, 9: 508–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Olson, R.K., R. Kliegl, and B.J. Davidson. 1983. Dyslexic and normal readers’ eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 9: 816–825. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Olulade, O.A., E.M. Napoliello, and G.F. Eden. 2013. Abnormal visual motion processing is not a cause of dyslexia. Neuron 79, 1: 180–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Pavlidis, G.T. 1981. Do eye movements hold the key to dyslexia? Neuropsychologia 19, 1: 57–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Peterson, R.L., and B.F. Pennington. 2012. Developmental dyslexia. Lancet 379, 9830: 1997–2007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Sattler, J.M., and R. Dumont. 2004. Assessment of children: WISC-IV and WPPSI-III supplement. coll. J.M. Sattler Publisher. [Google Scholar]
  52. Sheliga, B.M., L. Craighero, L. Riggio, and G. Rizzolatti. 1997. Effects of spatial attention on directional manual and ocular responses. Experimental Brain Research 114, 2: 339–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Seassau, M., C.L. Gerard, E. Bui-Quoc, and M.P. Bucci. 2014. Binocular Saccade Coordination in Reading and Visual Search: A Developmental Study in Typical Reader and Dyslexic Children. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 8: 85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Snowling, M.J. 1995. Phonological processing and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading 18, 2: 132–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Stanley, G., G.A. Smith, and E.A. Howell. 1983. Eyemovements and sequential tracking in dyslexic and control children. British Journal of Psychology 74: 181–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Stein, J.F., P.M. Riddell, and S. Fowler. 1988. Disordered vergence control in dyslexic children. British Journal of Ophthalmology 72: 162–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Stein, J. 2018. What is Developmental Dyslexia? Brain Science 8, 2: 26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Stein, J. 2019. The current status of the magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 130: 66–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Takagi, M., D.S. Zee, and R.J. Tamargo. 1998. Effects of lesions of the oculomotor vermis on eye movements in primate: saccades. Journal of Neurophysiology 80, 4: 1911–1931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Takagi, M., D.S. Zee, and R.J. Tamargo. 2000. Effects of lesions of the oculomotor cerebellar vermis on eye movements in primate: smooth pursuit. Journal of Neurophysiology 83, 4: 2047–2062. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Tallal, P. 1980. Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children. Brain and Language 9, 2: 182–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Tiadi, A., C.L. Gérard, H. Peyre, E. Bui-Quoc, and M.P. Bucci. 2016. Immaturity of visual fixations in dyslexic children. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10: 58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Valdois, S., M.L. Bosse, and M.J. Tainturier. 2004. The cognitive deficits responsible for developmental dyslexia: review of evidence for a selective visual attentional disorder. Dyslexia 10, 4: 339–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Ygge, J., G. Lennerstrand, A. Rydberg, S. Wijecoon, and B.M. Pettersson. 1993. Oculomotor functions in a Swedish population of dyslexic and normally reading children. Acta Ophthalmologica 71: 10–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Mean of the number of catch-up saccades (A) and of the gain (B) measured during pursuits for dyslexic and nondyslexic children examined Vertical bars indicate the standard error. * p < 0.05.
Figure 1. Mean of the number of catch-up saccades (A) and of the gain (B) measured during pursuits for dyslexic and nondyslexic children examined Vertical bars indicate the standard error. * p < 0.05.
Jemr 13 00005 g001
Figure 2. Gain value and the number of catch-up saccades measured for dyslexic and non-dyslexic children.
Figure 2. Gain value and the number of catch-up saccades measured for dyslexic and non-dyslexic children.
Jemr 13 00005 g002
Figure 3. Number of catch-up saccades measured during pursuits for dyslexic and non-dyslexic children examined.
Figure 3. Number of catch-up saccades measured during pursuits for dyslexic and non-dyslexic children examined.
Jemr 13 00005 g003
Figure 4. Gain (ratio of the eye velocity over the target velocity) for dyslexic and non-dyslexic children examined.
Figure 4. Gain (ratio of the eye velocity over the target velocity) for dyslexic and non-dyslexic children examined.
Jemr 13 00005 g004
Table 1. Clinical characteristics of children.
Table 1. Clinical characteristics of children.
Dyslexic children
N = 46
Non Dyslexic children
N = 46
Chronological age (yrs)9.8 ± 0.39.8 ± 0.3
Reading age (yrs)7.8 ± 0.27.8 ± 0.2
Verbal IQ99.7± 1.1
Verbal Sc 11.45 ± 0.8
Logic IQ98.9 ± 0.9
Logic Sc 10.94 ± 0.5

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Caldani, S.; Gerard, C.-L.; Peyre, H.; Bucci, M.P. Pursuit Eye Movements in Dyslexic Children: Evidence for an Immaturity of Brain Oculomotor Structures? J. Eye Mov. Res. 2020, 13, 1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.13.1.5

AMA Style

Caldani S, Gerard C-L, Peyre H, Bucci MP. Pursuit Eye Movements in Dyslexic Children: Evidence for an Immaturity of Brain Oculomotor Structures? Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2020; 13(1):1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.13.1.5

Chicago/Turabian Style

Caldani, Simona, Christophe-Loïc Gerard, Hugo Peyre, and Maria Pia Bucci. 2020. "Pursuit Eye Movements in Dyslexic Children: Evidence for an Immaturity of Brain Oculomotor Structures?" Journal of Eye Movement Research 13, no. 1: 1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.13.1.5

APA Style

Caldani, S., Gerard, C.-L., Peyre, H., & Bucci, M. P. (2020). Pursuit Eye Movements in Dyslexic Children: Evidence for an Immaturity of Brain Oculomotor Structures? Journal of Eye Movement Research, 13(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.13.1.5

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop