Next Article in Journal
Effect of a Visual Tracking Intervention on Attention and Behavior of Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
Previous Article in Journal
Dog Eye Movements Are Slower than Human Eye Movements
 
 
Journal of Eye Movement Research is published by MDPI from Volume 18 Issue 1 (2025). Previous articles were published by another publisher in Open Access under a CC-BY (or CC-BY-NC-ND) licence, and they are hosted by MDPI on mdpi.com as a courtesy and upon agreement with Bern Open Publishing (BOP).
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Shorter Fixation Durations for Up-Directed Saccades During Saccadic Exploration: A Meta-Analysis

by
Harold H. Greene
1,
James M. Brown
2 and
Gregory P. Strauss
2
1
University of Detroit Mercy, Detroit, MI, USA
2
University of Georgia, Athens, GA, USA
J. Eye Mov. Res. 2019, 12(8), 1-12; https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.8.5
Submission received: 29 April 2019 / Published: 1 March 2020

Abstract

:
Utilizing 23 datasets, we report a meta-analysis of an asymmetry in presaccadic fixation durations for saccades directed above and below eye fixation during saccadic exploration. For inclusion in the meta-analysis, saccadic exploration of complex visual displays had to have been made without gaze-contingent manipulations. Effect sizes for the asymmetry were quantified as Hedge’s g. Pooled effect sizes indicated significant asymmetries such that during saccadic exploration in a variety of tasks, presaccadic fixation durations for saccades directed into the upper visual field were reliably shorter than presaccadic fixation durations for saccades into the lower visual field. It is contended that the asymmetry is robust and important for efforts aimed at modelling when a saccade is initiated as a function of ensuing saccade direction.

Introduction

…the model and good data go hand in hand in advancing the field.
(p. 7, Rayner, 2009)
The visual field is the area of visibility, with respect to the current eye fixation. An object positioned above or below the current eye fixation is respectively in the current upper or lower visual field. It is well established that temporal processing along the vertical meridian of the visual field is asymmetric (Previc, 1990; Skrandies, 1987; Woodworth, 1938; Y. Zhou, Yu, Xuefei, Wu, & Zhang, 2017). For example, keypress/manual reactions (MRTS) are usually faster for targets that appear in the lower, not upper visual field. These behavioral findings are supported by physiological findings in the forms of shorter latency, and larger amplitude evoked responses in retinal and occipital areas (Hagler, 2014; Kremlácek, Kuba, Chlubnová, & Kubová, 2004; Portin, Vanni, Virsu, & Hari, 1999; see also Skrandies, 1987 for a review). The accepted explanation is that the lower visual field advantage for manual reactions is a result of an asymmetrical representation of the lower visual field in the (upper) retina and (superior) visual cortex areas (see Holmes, 1945; Silva et al., 2018; Skrandies, 1987).
Given the structural limitations of the visual system, only a small central area of the visual field is accessible to detailed scrutiny. As such, it is necessary to make fast eye movements (i.e., saccades) to bring peripheral areas into central processing range. Saccades continually change the position of the visual field. In contrast to the lower visual field advantage for manual responses, the latency to initiate a saccade towards a target is usually shorter by 20ms-50ms, if the target appears in the upper visual field (e.g., Abegg, Pianezzi, & Barton, 2015; see also Greene, Brown, & Dauphin, 2014 for an overview). This upper visual field advantage for saccade reaction times (SRTs) is well established in the literature (Abegg et al., 2015; Bell, Everling, & Munoz, 2000; Goldring & Fischer, 1997; Hackman, 1940; Heywood & Churcher, 1980; Honda & Findlay, 1992; Miles, 1936; Pitzalis & Di Russo, 2001; Schlykowa, Hoffmann, Bremmer, & Thiele, 1996; Tzelepi, Laskaris, Amditis, & Kapoula, 2010; Tzelepi, Yang, & Kapoula, 2005; W. Zhou & King, 2002). As with MRTs, asymmetry in SRTs are also supported by physiological findings of sharper, faster and stronger upper visual field representation in subcortical and cortical oculomotor areas (Drager & Hubel, 1976; Hafed & Chen, 2016; Tzelepi et al., 2010). The characteristics of SRTs are important in clinical vision sciences. For example, they have a diagnostic value in some movement disorders (e.g., Huntington’s disease, and recessive cerebellar ataxia; see Lasker & Zee, 1997; Patel, Jankovic, Hood, Jeter, & Sereno, 2012; Termsarasab, Thammongkolchai, Rucker, & Frucht, 2015). Hence, typical SRT asymmetries may provide insight on the functioning of a healthy brain.
While knowledge of SRT characteristics is useful (e.g., in clinical vision sciences), there is a limitation with respect to temporal characteristics of saccadic exploration of targets in a visual field. In SRT tasks, the goal is to prepare and initiate a saccade as quickly as possible to one of a number of preset alternative locations. This is not the case during typical exploration of a visual scene, where the viewer decides autonomously, where to direct saccades. A comprehensive understanding of the visual brain (leading to an ability to predict, model, and clinically manage its operations) requires research on saccadic exploration characteristics. Characteristics may be classified as when a saccade is initiated, and where the saccade goes (i.e., directional and distance control). Many computational models of saccade exploration have focused on where saccades go in the visual field (e.g., Itti & Koch, 2000; Najemnik & Geisler, 2009; Parkhurst, Law, & Niebur, 2002; Rao, Zelinsky, Hayhoe, & Ballard, 2002; Zelinsky, 2008). Towards real-time modelling of saccadic exploration (e.g., Laubrock, Cajar, & Engbert, 2013; Nuthmann, 2017; Nuthmann & Henderson, 2010; Trukenbrod & Engbert, 2014), it is necessary to determine when a saccade is initiated in association with where the saccade goes in the visual field (e.g., Brown & Greene, 2018; Greene & Brown, 2017; Greene et al., 2014; Tatler & Vincent, 2009). In the context of saccadic exploration, a saccade is initiated after the duration of an eye fixation. Following three recent studies (Brown & Greene, 2018; Greene & Brown, 2017; Greene et al., 2014), we refer to this duration as the presaccadic fixation duration (PSFD). Unlike SRTs, PSFDs are confounded by the time taken to exploit information in the area fixated and the time required to program a saccade for exploration (Rayner, 1998; Sheth & Young, 2016). Un-confounding the two variables is beyond the goal of the present meta-analysis.
It has been reported that a vertical visual field asymmetry for PSFDs exists, such that fixation durations are shorter before the eyes are directed above eye fixation (Brown & Greene, 2018; Greene & Brown, 2017; Greene et al., 2014). A need exists to determine the replicability and generality of this asymmetry if it is to be considered in real-time modelling of saccadic exploration (see Maxwell, Lau, & Howard, 2015; Shrout & Rodgers, 2018 for discussions of the replication crisis in science). In the present study we report a meta-analysis of published and unpublished results of the vertical asymmetry in PSFDs. The goal was to summarize results of a diverse set of saccadic exploration studies, and to quantify the extent of visual field asymmetry for PSFD in these studies. If the purported asymmetry in PSFDs is reliable (Brown & Greene, 2018; Greene & Brown, 2017; Greene et al., 2014), a significant asymmetry was expected in overall effect size across different datasets. In addition to determining the overall effect size, four potential moderators of the effect were considered. First we reasoned that the differences in demands between searching for a target and viewing a scene justifies the inclusion of Task (Visual Search vs Scene Viewing) as a moderator variable.
Second, towards safeguarding against publication bias, unpublished data were included in the meta-analysis. For this reason, the variable Source (Published vs Unpublished) was included as a moderator. Some of the datasets included in the meta-analysis were created specifically to test for the existence of the purported asymmetry in PSFDs. To assess the potential influence of verification bias, Pre-planned Test (Yes vs No) was included as a moderator. Finally, some studies artificially constrained the observer’s head in a chinrest. Towards ecological validity, it was useful to examine the constraint of a chinrest as moderator: Chinrest (Yes vs No).

Method

Data Selection Criteria

Eye movement data were included from research conducted at three laboratories. As our concern was saccadic behavior, the specific aims of the different studies considered were not important. Published and new datasets were considered, to safeguard against publication bias. We were particularly interested in situations that reasonably simulated realistic looking demands. For inclusion in the meta-analysis, saccadic explorations had to have been made without gaze-contingent (GC) visual field restrictions on complex visual displays. Our inclusion criteria allowed real-world scenes, fractals, roadmaps, picture webpages, random dot displays, and ambiguous inkblot images. Studies that utilized structured alpha-numeric characters were excluded. For studies that utilized some GC manipulations, only the conditions without GC manipulations were included.

Description of Selected Datasets

Six of the datasets were from 3 published studies that reported the vertical asymmetry in PSFDs during saccadic exploration (Brown & Greene, 2018; Greene & Brown, 2017; Greene et al., 2014). Eight of the datasets were from 3 published studies of saccadic exploration that had not previously checked for the vertical asymmetry in PSFDs (Foulsham & Kingstone, 2010; Greene, Brown, & Paradis, 2013; Greene, Pollatsek, Masserang, Lee, & Rayner, 2010; Strauss, Ossenfort, & Whearty, 2016). Nine new eye-movement datasets were from the laboratories of the authors, and were included in the analysis, to minimize publication bias. Table 1 presents brief descriptions of the methods utilized for the datasets included in the meta-analysis. Figure 1 shows one urban scene used in previously unpublished scene viewing tasks, and a sample trial display with a target circle among ovals, used in visual search tasks. In one sceneviewing task, the stimuli were randomly chosen webpages, containing pictures and words. All other stimulus sets are well described in the published datasets (see Greene, Brown, & Paradis, 2013; Greene, Pollatsek, Masserang, Lee, & Rayner, 2010; Strauss, Ossenfort, & Whearty, 2016), and briefly described in Table 1.
In sum, 23 datasets were included in the metaanalysis (14 visual search, and 9 scene-viewing datasets). Eye movements were tracked using Eyelink systems at 250Hz, or higher, as participants explored presentations on a computer screen. All data collection was approved by the relevant university Institutional Review Boards.
They were conducted in accordance with the Belmont Report, and the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki). Participants were healthy adults with normal, or corrected-to-normal visual acuity.

Data Analysis

In-house codes were used to extract PSFDs associated with ensuing saccade directions within the quadrants of the visual field (e.g., Brown & Greene, 2018; Greene & Brown, 2017). A priori paired samples t tests were conducted to test for significant differences between PSFDs for saccades directed within the 90 deg radius above, and below each current eye fixation. The metaanalysis was conducted using Comprehensive MetaAnalysis V3 software. The data entered were the mean PSFDs for saccades directed within the 90 deg wedge above and below current eye fixation, the sample size, and the paired samples t value. Effect sizes for the difference in PSFDs when saccades were directed into the upper visual field (UpVF) and lower visual field (LoVF) were quantified as Hedge’s g (Hedges & Olkin, 1985).
Heterogeneity among datasets was assessed as the percentage of variance in effect (i.e., I² % = 100(Q − df)/Q; Higgins & Thompson, 2002, where in the present context, Q is the result of Cochran’s χ2 test with df degrees of freedom).

Results

In Table 1, we have presented PSFDs for paired samples comparisons of mean PSFDs associated with UpVF-directed and LoVF-directed saccades. A priori paired samples t tests revealed significant vertical visual field asymmetries in 19 of the 23 datasets (p < .05). The table shows that overall, there was an asymmetry in PSFDs such that PSFDs were shorter by 25ms for up-directed saccades. Standardized extent of asymmetry in PSFDs was quantified by Hedge’s g for each dataset.
There was statistically significant heterogeneity among the 23 datasets (Q(22) = 68.16, p < .01; percentage of variance in effect sizes due to heterogeneity: I² % = 67.72%). Given the heterogeneity of variances, a random effects model was preferred over a fixed effect model to quantify the pooled effect size. The random effects model produces a wider confidence interval, to compensate for the heterogeneity. The pooled effect size (shown as a diamond at the bottom of Figure 2) indicated significant asymmetry such that during saccadic exploration in a variety of tasks, PSFDs for saccades into the upper visual field were typically shorter than PSFDs for saccades into the lower visual field (pooled g overall = 0.97, z= 9.09, p < .01).

Moderator: Task (Visual Search vs. Scene Viewing)

Analysis of data subsets showed statistically significant heterogeneity among Scene Viewing tasks (Q(8) = 19.07, p < .01; I² % = 58.05%), and Visual Search tasks (Q(13) = 47.18, p < .01; I² % = 72.44%). With a random effects model, there was a significant asymmetry such that during saccadic exploration in Visual Search and in Scene Viewing tasks, PSFDs for saccades into the upper visual field were typically shorter than PSFDs for saccades into the lower visual field (pooled g for Visual Search = 1.05, z = 6.34; pooled g for scene viewing = .86, z = 6.56, all ps < .01).
The variable Task(Visual Search vs Scene Viewing), was not significant as a moderator Q(1) = .76, p > .05 (see also, overlapping 95% confidence interval diamonds for pooled Visual Search and pooled Scene Viewing effects in Figure 2). In sum, the effect size was not different between Visual Search and Scene Viewing tasks examined in the meta-analysis.

Moderator: Source (Published vs. Unpublished)

Analysis of data subsets showed statistically significant heterogeneity among Published studies (Q(13) = 42.92, p < .01; I² % = 69.71%), and Unpublished studies (Q(8) = 21.71, p < .01; I² % = 63.15%). With a random effects model, there was a significant asymmetry such that during saccadic exploration in Published and Unpublished datasets, PSFDs for saccades into the upper visual field were typically shorter than PSFDs for saccades into the lower visual field (pooled g for Published studies = .85, z = 6.37; pooled g for Unpublished studies = 1.19, z = 6.65, all ps < .01).
The variable Source (Published vs Unpublished), was not significant as a moderator Q(1) = 2.23, p > .05. In sum, the effect size was not different between Published and Unpublished studies examined in the meta-analysis.

Moderator: Pre-Planned Test (Yes vs. No)

Only visual search studies were in the pool of studies designed specifically to test for the purported asymmetry in PSFDs. There was no significant heterogeneity among these studies (i.e., Yes studies; Q(6) = 2.15, p > .05; I² % = 0%). In contrast, there was significant heterogeneity among (visual search and scene-viewing) studies that were not designed specifically to test for the purported asymmetry (i.e., No studies; Q(15) = 40.58, p < .01; I² % = 63.03%).
With a random effects model, there was a significant asymmetry such that during saccadic exploration in Yes, and No studies, PSFDs for saccades into the upper visual field were typically shorter than PSFDs for saccades into the lower visual field (pooled g for Yes studies = 1.46, z = 11.45; pooled g for No studies = .77, z = 6.79, all ps < .01).
The variable Pre-planned Test (Yes vs No), was a significant moderator Q(1) = 15.97, p < .01. In sum, the effect size was larger for studies (limited to visual search studies in our meta-analysis) specifically designed to test for the purported asymmetry in PSFDs.

Moderator: Chinrest (Yes vs. No)

Analysis of data subsets showed statistically significant heterogeneity among studies that used a chinrest (Q(9) = 31.55, p < .01; I² % = 71.47%), and studies that did not (Q(12) = 31.44, p < .01; I² % = 61.84%). With a random effects model, there was a significant asymmetry such that during saccadic exploration in Chinrest and No Chinrest datasets, PSFDs for saccades into the upper visual field were typically shorter than PSFDs for saccades into the lower visual field (pooled g for chinrest studies = .79, z = 4.87; pooled g for no chinrest studies = 1.13, z = 8.09, all ps < .01).
The variable Chinrest (Yes vs No), was not significant as a moderator Q(1) = 2.43, p > .05. In sum, the effect size was not different between Chinrest and No Chinrest studies examined in the meta-analysis.

Discussion

An asymmetry has been reported for PSFDs associated with saccades directed within the vertical visual field (Greene & Brown, 2017; Greene et al., 2014). An open question is the robustness of this reported asymmetry for future research endeavors. The present metaanalytic study, the first of its kind, had two objectives: to report PSFDs for vertically-directed saccades in a diverse set of saccadic exploration studies, and to quantify the extent of vertical asymmetry in PSFDs. Overall, there was a statistically significant asymmetry in PSFDs such that during saccadic exploration, fixation durations were briefer if saccades were directed into the upper visual field. The results of the meta-analysis support the published reports of PSFD asymmetry. The effect was not moderated by saccade exploration task (i.e., Visual Search vs Scene Viewing), publication bias, or the constraint of a chinrest. Whether or not a study was designed specifically to test for the PSFD effect did matter, such that studies designed to test for the effect showed a larger effect size. However, all the studies designed specifically to test for the effect were visual search studies. In the future, it would be useful to conduct a planned comparison of the effect for scene-viewing and visual search of the same stimulus set.

Speculative Explanations

Movement of sensors relative to objects in the environment is important for learning regularities of our environment (e.g. Hawkins, Ahmad, and Cui, 2017). As the visual brain reaches out to engage with the world, information above or below the current eye fixation is respectively positioned in the current upper or lower field, and saccades continually change what constitutes the upper and lower visual field. To date, it has not been established why PSFDs tend to be shorter when the ensuing saccade is directed upwards. An account is summarized here, to stimulate future research efforts on the question.
If one considers global optic flow while walking and looking straight ahead, the speed/velocity across the retina is least at the fixation point, and increasing in the periphery. In an outdoor setting with sky above, a greater amount of such stimulation would be in the lower visual field. A “check and detect” behavior pattern stemming from ecological constraints (e.g., Previc, 1990) may underlie the vertical visual field asymmetry for PSFDs. Given the position of the eyes atop the human body, information in far/extra-personal space tends to be in the UpVF, and information near the body tends to be in the LoVF. Hence, as one moves within one’s environment, gaze is typically directed some distance ahead to search for, and detect new information in the slower flowing upper optic array (i.e. UpVF). As one is covertly aware of near-body/peripersonal and ground-level space in the LoVF, one rarely looks down to check already covertly attended things in one’s faster flowing ground level. Occasionally, one makes saccades downwards to check the information in the current LoVF. Support for a “check and detect” behavior pattern comes from laboratory findings that saccades are more likely to be directed upwards to discern information in the current UpVF (see Greene et al., 2014; see also Foulsham & Kingstone, 2010 for possible influences of perceived scene orientation on the probability of saccade direction). The vertical asymmetry in PSFDs elicited by a “check and detect” mechanism may be grounded in neuroscience.
From a visibility perspective, greater representation of the lower visual field in the (upper) retina and (superior) visual cortex areas (Silva et al., 2018; Skrandies, 1987) results in higher contrast sensitivity below eye fixation. Hence, there may be a greater urgency to terminate a fixation, and make a saccade upwards, because for a given radius, objects above fixation are seen less clearly during a fixation. The visibility perspective predicts shorter fixation durations for ensuing saccades directed upwards because there is a greater need to disambiguate by looking, objects above fixation.
Beyond a visibility perspective, magnetoencephalography results (Tzelepi et al., 2010) suggest that frontal cortical areas involved in saccade preparation respond earlier when saccades are directed upwards, than when saccades are directed downwards. Additionally, when the intention is to make a saccade upwards, frontal cortex activity is lower than when a downward saccade is intended (Tzelepi et al., 2010). The findings suggest that saccade programming may require less cortical effort for saccades directed upwards in the visual field. In turn, less cortical effort may encourage shorter fixation durations for ensuing saccades directed upwards.
Beyond cortical effort, saccades are controlled by the cortex through the sub-cortical superior colliculus (see Goldberg and Walker, 2013, for an introduction). Recent findings suggest that the representation of the vertical visual field is not uniform in the superior colliculus (Hafed & Chen, 2016). Specifically, the UpVF representation in the superior colliculus is faster than the LoVF representation (Hafed & Chen, 2016). Indeed, the asymmetrical representation of the vertical visual field by the superior colliculus may also contribute to the asymmetry in vertical PSFDs. Together, a combination of adaptive mechanisms (e.g. check and detect behavior), and retinal, cortical and subcortical mechanisms may reasonably underlie the observed shorter fixation durations before saccades are directed upwards in the visual field. Perhaps, future animal neuroscience experiments could manipulate aspects of the saccadic circuitry to test the speculations proposed.

A Word on SRTs and PSFDs

Whereas effect sizes in our meta-analysis have demonstrated the robustness of the vertical field asymmetry in PSFDs (irrespective of statistical significance in null hypothesis tests), it is informative to ask whether SRTs are also similarly robust. Greene, Brown and Dauphin (2014) have argued that while the high level of experimental control in SRT tasks is useful for determining the operations of the saccadic circuitry, a limitation is that the task demand is different from saccadic exploration. Reasonably, SRTs index the time required to program a saccade, based on exogenously oriented attention. In contrast, during saccadic exploration (e.g., visual search and scene viewing), observers typically direct their attention endogenously, such that PSFDs heavily reflect both the time required to program a saccade and the amount of time taken to process the currently-fixated element in the visual field (see also, Nuthmann et al.,2010; Trukenbrod & Engbert, 2014). The systematic asymmetry in PSFDs probably reflects this added saccade programming time. To date, no meta-analysis has been conducted to ascertain this. Despite the difference in focus between SRTs and PSFDs, in their overview of the literature, Greene, Brown and Dauphin (2014) have suggested that the SRT asymmetry is noteworthy for convergent validity and computational modelling of saccadic behavior.
In the present study, the advantage of a metaanalysis is that it quantifies an effect in a manner that does not rely on a single dataset. Although selected datasets were not all created using the same methodology, the underlying mechanism of interest was the same--that is to say, saccadic explorations were made without GC manipulations on complex visual displays. The present study indicates that when there are no GC spatial constraints, there is a robust PSFD asymmetry for up- and down-directed saccades. Despite the difficulty of separating exploitation time from saccade preparation time in saccadic exploration tasks, PSFDs, like SRTs, are systematically shorter when the ensuing saccade is directed into the UpVF. Given the similarity in characteristics between SRTs and PSFDs, we contend that low-level saccade programming mechanisms affect PSFDs in predictable ways, which make PSFDs relevant in clinical vision sciences where they may have a diagnostic value in some movement disorders (e.g., Huntington’s disease, and recessive cerebellar ataxia; see Lasker & Zee, 1997; Patel et al., 2012; Termsarasab et al., 2015). It has also recently come to our attention that there are stable individual differences in saccadic indices (Castelhano & Henderson, 2008; Foulsham, Frost, & Sage, 2018; Henderson & Luke, 2014). This makes the reported asymmetry a worthwhile avenue for research on psychometric dynamics. Finally, we contend that the asymmetry in PSFDs is important for efforts aimed at modelling when a saccade is initiated as a function of ensuing saccade direction in the visual field (e.g., Laubrock et al., 2013; Nuthmann, 2017; Nuthmann, Smith, Engbert, & Henderson, 2010; Trukenbrod & Engbert, 2014).

Limitations

A potential limitation of the presented metaanalysis is the small number of datasets considered. Three moderators were not statistically significant. However, without regard to statistical significance, moderator analyses indicated a slightly larger asymmetry for visual search than scene viewing tasks (Δg = 1.05 − .86 = .19), unpublished than published studies (Δg = 1.19 − .85 = .34), and No Chinrest than Chinrest studies (Δg = 1.13 − .79 = .34). Perhaps most interesting from a theoretical point of view is the potential larger effect when a chin rest is not utilized. An ecological account of the asymmetry in vertical PSFDs predicts that the effect should be hindered by artificial head constraints. The availability of a larger set of studies in the future may contribute to assessing statistical significance of artificial (chinrest) constraints in the size of the effect.

Conclusion

We have shown that vertical visual field asymmetry for PSFDs is robust. Despite the small number of studies considered, it may reasonably be argued that the benefits of the present meta-analysis far outweigh its limitation. Of course, meta-analyses of the present sort may be facilitated by the deposition of data in publicly accessible databases (e.g. Dryad data repository, https://datadryad.org/). Ideally, for each fixation, deposited data should have at least, the components presented in italics, below.
<sampling rate>
<eye recorded>
<fixation_start_time_stamp> (optional)
<fixation_end_time_stamp> (optional)
<fixation_duration_in ms>
<average_horizontal_eye_position>
<average_vertical_eye_position>
To conclude, we predict that the vertical visual field asymmetry is present in datasets from other laboratories and encourage groups of researchers to conduct similar meta-analyses, towards establishing trustworthiness in the kinds of data used to guide computational modelling of real-time saccadic exploration. Indeed, “the model and good data go hand in hand in advancing the field.” (p. 7, Rayner, 2009). Given the theory of an ecology-driven functional specialization of the visual field above and below eye fixation (Previc, 1990), it may reasonably be predicted that the asymmetry in PSFDs is most apparent under typical environmental demands.

Ethics and Conflict of Interest

The author(s) declare(s) that the contents of the article are in agreement with the ethics described in http://biblio.unibe.ch/portale/elibrary/BOP/jemr/ethics.html and that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

Acknowledgments

We appreciate the recommendations of Jessica Matchynski-Franks and Latoya Taylor in the planning of the meta-analysis. We acknowledge Claudia Bernasconi and Alan Hoback for providing urban scenes. The comments of all formal, and informal reviewers of this work are greatly appreciated. Correspondence should be addressed to H. Greene, Department of Psychology, University of Detroit Mercy, Detroit, MI, 48221 (email: greenehh@udmercy.edu).

References

  1. Abegg, M.; Pianezzi, D.; Barton, J. A vertical asymmetry in saccades. Journal Of Eye Movement Research 2015, 8(5), 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Bell, A. H.; Everling, S.; Munoz, D. P. Influence of Stimulus Eccentricity and Direction on Characteristics of Proand Antisaccades in Non-Human Primates. Journal of Neurophysiology 2000, 84(5), 2595–2604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Brown, J. M.; Greene, H. H. Brown, J. M., Ed.; We’re going to study the mind. In Pioneer Visual Neuroscience: A Festschrift for Naomi Weisstein; New York: Routledge, 2018; pp. 6–32. [Google Scholar]
  4. Castelhano, M. S.; Henderson, J. M. Stable individual differences across images in human saccadic eye movements. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology 2008, 62(1), 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Drager, U. C.; Hubel, D. H. Topography of visual and somatosensory projections to mouse superior colliculus. Journal of Neurophysiology 1976, 39(1), 91–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Foulsham, T.; Frost, E.; Sage, L. Stable individual differences predict eye movements to the left, but not handedness or line bisection. Vision Research 2018, 144, 38–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Foulsham, T.; Kingstone, A. Asymmetries in the direction of saccades during perception of scenes and fractals: Effects of image type and image features. Vision Research 2010, 50(8), 779–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Goldring, J.; Fischer, B. Reaction times of vertical prosaccades and antisaccades in gap and overlap tasks. Experimental Brain Research 1997, 113(1), 88–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Goldberg, M. E.; Walker, M. F. Hudspeth, A. J., Schwartz, J. H., Jessell, T. M., Siegelbaum, S. A., Kandel, E. R., Eds.; The control of gaze. In Principles of Neural Science; McGraw-Hill; New York, NY, USA, 2013; pp. 894–916. [Google Scholar]
  10. Greene, H. H.; Brown, J. M. Where did I come from? Where am I going? Functional differences in visual search fixation duration. Journal Of Eye Movement Research 2017, 10(1), 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Greene, H. H.; Brown, J. M.; Dauphin, B. When do you look where you look? A visual field asymmetry. Vision Research 2014, 102, 33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Greene, H. H.; Brown, J. M.; Paradis, B. A. Luminance contrast and the visual span during visual target localization. Displays 2013, 34(1), 27–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Greene, H. H.; Pollatsek, A.; Masserang, K.; Lee, Y. J.; Rayner, K. Directional processing within the perceptual span during visual target localization. Vision Research 2010, 50(13), 1274–1282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Hackman, R. B. An experimental study of variability in ocular latency. Journal of Experimental Psychology 1940, 27(5), 546–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Hafed, Ziad M.; Chen, C.-Y. Sharper, stronger, faster upper visual field representation in primate superior colliculus. Current Biology 2016, 26(13), 1647–1658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Hagler, D. J., Jr. Visual field asymmetries in visual evoked responses. Journal of Vision 2014, 14(14), 13–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Hawkins, J.; Ahmad, S.; Cui, Y. A theory of how columns in the neocortex enable learning the structure of the world. Frontiers in Neural Circuits 2017, 11(81), 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Hedges, L.; Olkin, I. Statistical Methods for Meta-Analysis; San Diego: Academic Press, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  19. Henderson, J. M.; Luke, S. G. Stable individual differences in saccadic eye movements during reading, pseudoreading, scene viewing, and scene search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 2014, 40(4), 1390–1400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Heywood, S.; Churcher, J. Structure of the visual array and saccadic latency: implications for oculomotor control. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 1980, 32(2), 335–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Higgins, J. P. T.; Thompson, S. G. Quantifying heterogeneity in a meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine 2002, 21(11), 1539–1558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Holmes, G. M. Ferrier Lecture The organization of the visual cortex in man. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series B Biological Sciences 1945, 132(869), 348–361. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Honda, H.; Findlay, J. M. Saccades to targets in three-dimensional space: Dependence of saccadic latency on target location. Perception & Psychophysics 1992, 52(2), 167–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Itti, L.; Koch, C. A saliency-based search mechanism for overt and covert shifts of visual attention. Vision Research 2000, 40(10), 1489–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Kremlácek, J.; Kuba, M.; Chlubnová, J.; Kubová, Z. Effect of stimulus localisation on motion-onset VEP. Vision Research 2004, 44(26), 2989–3000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Lasker, A. G.; Zee, D. S. Ocular motor abnormalities in Huntington's disease. Vision Research 1997, 37(24), 3639–3645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Laubrock, J.; Cajar, A.; Engbert, R. Control of fixation duration during scene viewing by interaction of foveal and peripheral processing. Journal of Vision 2013, 13(12). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Maxwell, S. E.; Lau, M. Y.; Howard, G. S. Is psychology suffering from a replication crisis? What does “failure to replicate” really mean? American Psychologist 2015, 70(6), 487–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Miles, W. R. The reaction time of the eye. Psychological Monographs 1936, 47(2), 268–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Najemnik, J.; Geisler, W. S. Simple summation rule for optimal fixation selection in visual search. Vision Research 2009, 49(10), 1286–1294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Nuthmann, A. Fixation durations in scene viewing: Modeling the effects of local image features, oculomotor parameters, and task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2017, 24(2), 370–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Nuthmann, A.; Henderson, J. M. Object-based attentional selection in scene viewing. Journal of Vision 2010, 10(8). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Nuthmann, A.; Smith, T. J.; Engbert, R.; Henderson, J. M. CRISP: a computational model of fixation durations in scene viewing. Psychological Review 2010, 117(2), 382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Parkhurst, D.; Law, K.; Niebur, E. Modeling the role of salience in the allocation of overt visual attention. Vision Research 2002, 42(1), 107–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Patel, S. S.; Jankovic, J.; Hood, A. J.; Jeter, C. B.; Sereno, A. B. Reflexive and volitional saccades: Biomarkers of Huntington disease severity and progression. Journal of the Neurological Sciences 2012, 313(1), 35–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Pitzalis, S.; Di Russo, F. Spatial anisotropy of saccadic latency in normal subjects and braindamaged patients. Cortex; a journal devoted to the study of the nervous system and behavior 2001, 37(4), 475–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Portin, K.; Vanni, S.; Virsu, V.; Hari, R. Stronger occipital cortical activation to lower than upper visual field stimuli Neuromagnetic recordings. Experimental Brain Research 1999, 124(3), 287–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Previc, F. H. Functional specialization in the lower and upper visual fields in humans: Its ecological origins and neurophysiological implications. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 1990, 13(3), 519–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Rao, R. P. N.; Zelinsky, G. J.; Hayhoe, M. M.; Ballard, D. H. Eye movements in iconic visual search. Vision Research 2002, 42(11), 1447–1463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Rayner, K. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 1998, 124(3), 372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Rayner, K. The 35th Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture: Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 2009, 62(8), 1457–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Schlykowa, L.; Hoffmann, K.-P.; Bremmer, F.; Thiele, A. Monkey saccadic latency and pursuit velocity show a preference for upward directions of target motion. Neuroreport: An International Journal for the Rapid Communication of Research in Neuroscience 1996, 7(2), 409–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Sheth, B. R.; Young, R. Two Visual Pathways in Primates Based on Sampling of Space: Exploitation and Exploration of Visual Information. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 2016, 10(37). [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Shrout, P. E.; Rodgers, J. Psychology, Science, and Knowledge Construction: Broadening Perspectives from the Replication Crisis. Annual Review of Psychology 69 2018, 487–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Silva, M. F.; Brascamp, J. W.; Ferreira, S.; CasteloBranco, M.; Dumoulin, S. O.; Harvey, B. M. Radial asymmetries in population receptive field size and cortical magnification factor in early visual cortex. NeuroImage 167 2018, 41–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Skrandies, W. The upper and lower visual field of man: Electrophysiological and functional differences. Progress in sensory physiology 8 1987, 2–93. [Google Scholar]
  47. Strauss, G. P.; Ossenfort, K. L.; Whearty, K. M. Reappraisal and Distraction Emotion Regulation Strategies Are Associated with Distinct Patterns of Visual Attention and Differing Levels of Cognitive Demand. PLoS One 2016, 11(11), e0162290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Tatler, B. W.; Vincent, B. T. The prominence of behavioural biases in eye guidance. Visual Cognition 2009, 17(6-7), 1029–1054. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Termsarasab, P.; Thammongkolchai, T.; Rucker, J. C.; Frucht, S. J. The diagnostic value of saccades in movement disorder patients: a practical guide and review. Journal of Clinical Movement Disorders 2015, 2(1), 14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Trukenbrod, H.; Engbert, R. ICAT: a computational model for the adaptive control of fixation durations. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 2014, 1–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Tzelepi, A.; Laskaris, N.; Amditis, A.; Kapoula, Z. Cortical activity preceding vertical saccades: A MEG study. Brain Research 1321 2010, 105–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Tzelepi, A.; Yang, Q.; Kapoula, Z. The effect of transcranial magnetic stimulation on the latencies of vertical saccades. Experimental Brain Research 2005, 164(1), 67–77. [Google Scholar]
  53. Woodworth, R. S. Experimental psychology; New York: H. Holt and Company, 1938. [Google Scholar]
  54. Zelinsky, G. J. A theory of eye movements during target acquisition. Psychological Review 2008, 115(4), 787. [Google Scholar]
  55. Zhou, W.; King, W. M. Attentional sensitivity and asymmetries of vertical saccade generation in monkey. Vision Research 2002, 42(6), 771–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Zhou, Y.; Yu, G.; Xuefei, Y.; Wu, S.; Zhang, M. Asymmetric representations of upper and lower visual fields in egocentric and allocentric references. Journal of Vision 2017, 17(1), 111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. An urban scene used in a scene viewing task, and a sample display with a target circle among ovals, used in visual search tasks included in the new eye movement data for the meta-analysis. The square in the visual search display depicts for the reader, the position of the target in this particular display.
Figure 1. An urban scene used in a scene viewing task, and a sample display with a target circle among ovals, used in visual search tasks included in the new eye movement data for the meta-analysis. The square in the visual search display depicts for the reader, the position of the target in this particular display.
Jemr 12 00067 g001
Figure 2. Standardized mean difference (as Hedge’s g) between PSFDs for up-directed and down-directed saccades. The sequence of datasets in the forest plot is the same as the sequence presented in Table 1. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. The sizes of the black squares reflect sample sizes. Pooled effect sizes are shown as diamonds for scene viewing and visual search (open), and overall (closed). The horizontal extent of the diamonds depict 95% confidence intervals. The pooled effect sizes indicate an asymmetry such that PSFDs are shorter for up-directed than down-directed saccades.
Figure 2. Standardized mean difference (as Hedge’s g) between PSFDs for up-directed and down-directed saccades. The sequence of datasets in the forest plot is the same as the sequence presented in Table 1. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals. The sizes of the black squares reflect sample sizes. Pooled effect sizes are shown as diamonds for scene viewing and visual search (open), and overall (closed). The horizontal extent of the diamonds depict 95% confidence intervals. The pooled effect sizes indicate an asymmetry such that PSFDs are shorter for up-directed than down-directed saccades.
Jemr 12 00067 g002
Table 1. Description of tasks included in the meta-analysis. Presaccadic Fixation Durations (PSFDs) for up-directed and down-directed saccades, and effect sizes (Hedge’s g) for paired samples comparisons.
Table 1. Description of tasks included in the meta-analysis. Presaccadic Fixation Durations (PSFDs) for up-directed and down-directed saccades, and effect sizes (Hedge’s g) for paired samples comparisons.
StudyCondition & stimuli in studySample SizePSFD up-
saccades (ms)
PSFD
downsaccade
(ms)
Difference in means
(ms)
Hedge’s gStd Err
Foulsham & Kingstone (2010)2-second viewing of natural scenes and fractals (encoding phase)20243.55269.1725.620.7020.242
Greene et al., (2014)Engaged viewing of ambiguous (Rorschach) inkblots.44327.28355.2527.971.0150.183
Greene’s lab
~
15-second viewing to rate the attractiveness of 9 urban scenes.20252.40270.7118.310.5150.230
Greene’s lab
~
15-second viewing to rate the secureness of 9 urban scenes.19252.98278.6325.650.4990.234
Greene’s lab
~
Engaged viewing of university webpages.8185.91227.4941.582.5390.708
Strauss’ lab
~
Passive viewing of IAPS unpleasant scenes.20272.89300.227.311.2040.287
Strauss’ lab
~
Passive viewing of IAPS unpleasant scenes.19265.02302.3637.341.5270.331
Strauss et al., (2016)5-second viewing of unpleasant IAPS scenes, while distracted by thoughts of unrelated neutral objects.25291.77314.8223.050.5520.209
Strauss et al., (2016)5-second viewing of unpleasant IAPS scenes, while reappraising them to be neutral.25282.27304.6622.390.7260.219
Brown & Greene, (2018)Visual search for low contrast square in a random gray-dot display.18268.08297.7729.691.2180.303
Brown & Greene, (2018)
Greene &
Visual search for low contrast square in a random red-dot display.
Visual search for low contrast
18


18
280.39


256.14
313.86


278.71
33.47


22.57
1.506


1.409
0.337


0.325
Brown, (2017)square in a random gray-dot display.
Greene et al., (2014)Visual search for low contrast checkerboard in a random gray-dot display.12213.84255.2741.431.5790.420
Greene et al., (2014)Visual search for low contrast square in a random gray-dot display.24243.18286.5243.341.8220.329
Greene et al., (2013)Monocular visual search for high contrast square in a random gray-dot display.12218.29227.829.53#0.5770.293
Greene et al., (2013)Monocular visual search for low contrast square in a random gray-dot display.12341.636624.400.6470.299
Greene et al., (2013)Visual search for high contrast square in a random gray-dot display.11206.92201.69-5.23#-0.1860.281
Greene et al., (2013)Visual search for low contrast square in a random gray-dot display.12305.56317.612.04#0.3060.276
Greene et al., (2010)Visual search of roadmaps.10242.13257.9115.78#0.5520.314
Greene’s lab
~
3-second visual search-white circles on blue background.15201.52220.4118.891.3130.342
Greene’s lab
~
3-second visual search-white circles on green background.15198.24224.1625.921.5530.374
Greene’s lab
~
3-second visual search-white circles on red background.15200.31220.9620.651.4110.355
Greene’s lab
~
Visual search for low contrast square in a random gray-dot display.15206.02234.4628.441.2960.340
Means 247.07272.2525.18
Notes. ~ New data. #Non-significant paired samples t. IAPS: International Affective Picture System https://csea.phhp.ufl.edu/media/iapsmessage.html.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Greene, H.H.; Brown, J.M.; Strauss, G.P. Shorter Fixation Durations for Up-Directed Saccades During Saccadic Exploration: A Meta-Analysis. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2019, 12, 1-12. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.8.5

AMA Style

Greene HH, Brown JM, Strauss GP. Shorter Fixation Durations for Up-Directed Saccades During Saccadic Exploration: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2019; 12(8):1-12. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.8.5

Chicago/Turabian Style

Greene, Harold H., James M. Brown, and Gregory P. Strauss. 2019. "Shorter Fixation Durations for Up-Directed Saccades During Saccadic Exploration: A Meta-Analysis" Journal of Eye Movement Research 12, no. 8: 1-12. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.8.5

APA Style

Greene, H. H., Brown, J. M., & Strauss, G. P. (2019). Shorter Fixation Durations for Up-Directed Saccades During Saccadic Exploration: A Meta-Analysis. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 12(8), 1-12. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.8.5

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop