Microsaccades Reflect the Dynamics of Misdirected Attention in Magic
Abstract
:Introduction
Microsaccades as an index of covert attention
Method
Participants
Stimuli
Apparatus
Procedure
Eye Movement Analyses
Results
Task Accuracy
Microsaccades during constrained viewing
Microsaccades during free-viewing
Discussion
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Barnhart, A. S., and S. D. Goldinger. 2014. Blinded by magic: Eye-movements reveal the misdirection of attention. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barnhart, A. S., M. J. Ehlert, S. D. Goldinger, and A. D. Mackey. 2018. Cross-modal attentional entrainment: Insights from magicians. In Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. Advance online publication. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beanland, V., and K. Pammer. 2010. Looking without seeing or seeing without looking? Eye movements in sustained inattentional blindness. Vision Research 50: 977–988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boyer, T. W., and M. Wang. 2018. Direct gaze, eye movements, and covert and overt social attention processes. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics 80: 1654–1659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Betta, E., and M. Turatto. 2006. Are you ready? I can tell by looking at your microsaccades. Neuroreport 17: 1001–1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cavina-Pratesi, C., G. Kuhn, M. Ietswaart, and A. D. Milner. 2011. The magic grasp: Motor expertise in deception. PLoS ONE 6: e16568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cornsweet, T. N. 1956. Determination of the stimuli for involuntary drifts and saccadic eye movements. Journal of the Optical Society of America 46: 987–993. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Costela, F. M., J. Otero-Millan, M. B. McCamy, S. L. Macknik, X. G. Troncoso, A. N. Jazi, S. M. Crook, and S. Martinez-Conde. 2014. Fixational eye movement correction of blink-induced gaze position errors. PLoS One 9: e110889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cui, J., J. Otero-Millan, S. L. Macknik, M. King, and S. Martinez-Conde. 2011. Social misdirection fails to enhance a magic illusion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5: 103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Danek, A. H., T. Fraps, A. von Müller, B. Grothe, and M. Öllinger. 2014. It’s a kind of magic--What self-reports can reveal about the phenomenology of insight problem solving. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ditchburn, R. W., and B. L. Ginsborg. 1952. Vision with a stabilized retinal image. Nature 170: 36–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eayrs, J., and N. Lavie. 2018. Establishing individual differences in perceptual capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 44: 1240–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Engbert, R. 2006. Microsaccades: A microcosm for research on oculomotor control, attention, and visual perception. Progress in Brain Research 154: 177–192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Engbert, R., and R. Kliegl. 2003. Microsaccades uncover the orientation of covert attention. Vision Research 43: 1035–1045. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Engbert, R., and K. Mergenthaler. 2006. Microsaccades are triggered by low retinal image slip. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 103: 7192–7197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hafed, Z. M., and J. J. Clark. 2002. Microsaccades as an overt measure of covert attention shifts. Vision Research 42: 2533–2545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hafed, Z. M., L. P. Lovejoy, and R. J. Krauzlis. 2011. Modulation of microsaccades in monkey during a covert visual attention task. The Journal of Neuroscience 31: 15219–15230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hall, L., T. Strandberg, P. Pärnamets, A. Lind, B. Tärning, and P. Johansson. 2013. How the polls can be both spot on and dead wrong: Using choice blindness to shift political attitudes and voter intentions. PLoS ONE 8: e60554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hergovich, A., K. Gröbl, and C.-C. Carbon. 2011. The paddle move commonly used in magic tricks as a ameans for analysing the perceptual limits of combined motion trajectories. Perception 40: 358–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Horowitz, T. S., E. M. Fine, D. E. Fencsik, S. Yurgenson, and J. M. Wolfe. 2007. Fixational eye movements are not an index of covert attention. Psychological Science 18: 356–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johansson, P., L. Hall, S. Sikström, and A. Olsson. 2005. Failure to detect mismatches between intention and outcome in a simple decision task. Science 310: 116–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhn, G., A. A. Amlani, and R. A. Rensink. 2008. Towards a science of magic. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 12: 349–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kuhn, G., and L. M. Martinez. 2012. Misdirection-Past, present, and the future. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5: 172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kuhn, G., and B. W. Tatler. 2005. Magic and fixation: Now you don’t see it, now you do. Perception 34: 1155–1161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kuhn, G., B. W. Tatler, J. M. Findlay, and G. G. Cole. 2008. Misdirection in magic: Implications for the relationship between eye gaze and attention. Visual Cognition 16: 391–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lamont, P., and J. M. Henderson. 2008. More attention and greater awareness in the scientific study of magic. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 10: 871–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lamont, P., J. M. Henderson, and T. J. Smith. 2010. Where science and magic meet: The illusion of a "science of magic". Review of General Psychology 14: 16–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laubrock, J., R. Engbert, and R. Kliegl. 2005. Microsaccade dynamics during covert attention. Vision Research 45: 721–730. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laubrock, J., R. Engbert, M. Rolfs, and R. Kliegl. 2007. Microsaccades are an index of covert attention: Commentary on Horowitz, Fine, Fencsik, Yurgenson, and Wolfe (2007). Psychological Science 18: 364–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laubrock, J., R. Kliegl, M. Rolfs, and R. Engbert. 2010. When do microsaccades follow spatial attention? Attention. Perception, & Psychophysics 72: 683–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macknik, S. L., M. King, J. Randi, A. Robbins, Teller, Thompson J., and S. Martinez-Conde. 2008. Attention and awareness in stage magic: Turning tricks into research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9: 871–879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Macknik, S. L., S. Martinez-Conde, and S. Blakeslee. 2010. Sleights of mind: What the neuroscience of magic reveals about our everyday deceptions. New York: Henry Holt & Co. [Google Scholar]
- Martinez-Conde, S., and S. L. Macknik. 2007. Mind tricks. Nature 448: 414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martinez-Conde, S., and S. L. Macknik. 2008. Magic and the brain. Scientific American 299: 72–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martinez-Conde, S., S. L. Macknik, X. G. Troncoso, and T. A. Dyar. 2006. Microsaccades counteract visual fading during fixation. Neuron 49: 297–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Martinez-Conde, S., S. L. Macknik, X. G. Troncoso, and D. H. Hubel. 2009. Microsaccades: A neurophysiological analysis. Trends in Neurosciences 32: 463–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martinez-Conde, S., J. Otero-Millan, and S. L. Macknik. 2013. The impact of microsaccades on vision: Towards a unified theory of saccadic function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 14: 83–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCamy, M. B., Collins, N., Otero-Millan, J., Al-Kalbani, M., Macknik, S. L., Coakley, D.,... Martinez-Conde, S. 2013. Simultaneous recordings of ocular microtremor and microsaccades with a piezoelectric sensor and a video-oculography system. PeerJ 1: e14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCamy, M. B., A. N. Jazi, J. Otero-Millan, S. L. Macknik, and S. Martinez-Conde. 2013. The effects of fixation target size and luminance on microsaccades and square-wave jerks. PeerJ 1: e9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCamy, M. B., S. L. Macknik, and S. Martinez-Conde. 2014. Different fixational eye movements mediate the prevention and the reversal of visual fading. Journal of Physiology 592: 4381–4394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCamy, M. B., J. Otero-Millan, L. L. Di Stasi, S. L. Macknik, and S. Martinez-Conde. 2014. Highly informative natural scene regions increase microsaccade production during visual scanning. Journal of Neuroscience 34: 2956–2966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melloni, L., C. M. Schwiedrzik, E. Rodriguez, and W. Singer. 2009. (Micro)Saccades, corollary activity and cortical oscillations. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 239–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Memmert, D. 2006. The effects of eye movements, age, and expertise on inattentional blindness. Consciousness and Cognition 15: 620–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Olson, J. A., A. A. Amlani, A. Raz, and R. A. Rensink. 2015. Influencing choice without awareness. Consciousness and Cognition 37: 225–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Olson, J. A., M. Landry, K. Appourchaux, and A. Raz. 2016. Simulated thought insertion: Influencing the sense of agency using deception and magic. Consciousness and Cognition 43: 11–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Otero-Millan, J., S. L. Macknik, R. E. Langston, and S. Martinez-Conde. 2013. An oculomotor continuum from exploration to fixation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 110: 6175–6180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Otero-Millan, J., S. L. Macknik, A. Robbins, M. McCamy, and S. Martinez-Conde. 2011. Stronger misdirection in curved than in straight motion. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 5: 133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pastukhov, A., and J. Braun. 2010. Rare but precious: Microsaccades are highly informative about attentional allocation. Vision Research 50: 1173–1184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Phillips, F., M. B. Natter, and E. J. Egan. 2015. Magically deceptive biological motion--the French Drop sleight. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quian Quiroga, R. 2016. Magic and cognitive neuroscience. Current Biology 26: R390–R394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rieiro, H., S. Martinez-Conde, and S. L. Macknik. 2013. Perceptual elements in Penn & Teller’s "Cups and Balls "magic trick. PeerJ 1: e19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rensink, R. A., and G. Kuhn. 2015. The possibility of a science of magic. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 1576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rolfs, M., J. Laubrock, and R. Kliegl. 2006. Shortening and prolongation of saccade latencies following microsaccades. Experimental Brain Research 169: 369–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rucci, M., R. Iovin, M. Poletti, and F. Santini. 2007. Miniature eye movements enhance fine spatial detail. Nature 447: 851–855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shalom, D. E., M. G. de Sousa Serro, M. Giaconia, L. M. Martinez, A. Rieznik, and M. Sigman. 2013. Choosing in freedom or forced to choose? Introspective blindness to psychological forcing in stage-magic. PLoS ONE 8: e58254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegenthaler, E., Costela, F. M., McCamy, M. B., Di Stasi, L. L., Otero-Millan, J., Sonderegger, A.,…Martinez-Conde, S. 2014. Task difficulty in mental arithmetic affects microsaccadic rates and magnitudes. European Journal of Neuroscience 39: 287–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, T. J. 2015. The role of audience participation and task relevance on change detection during a card trick. Frontiers in Psychology 6: 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smith, T. J., P. Lamont, and J. M. Henderson. 2012. The penny drops: Change blindness at fixation. Perception 41: 489–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomas, C., and A. Didierjean. 2016. Magicians fix your mind: How unlikely solutions block obvious ones. Cognition 154: 169–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomas, C., A. Didierjean, F. Maquestiaux, and P. Gygax. 2015. Does magic offer a cryptozoology ground for psychology? Review of General Psychology 19: 117–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Troncoso, X. G., S. L. Macknik, J. Otero-Millan, and S. Martinez-Conde. 2008. Microsaccades drive illusory motion in the Enigma illusion. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 105: 16033–16038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valsecchi, M., E. Betta, and M. Turatto. 2007. Visual oddballs induce prolonged microsaccadic inhibition. Experimental Brain Research 177: 196–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- van Dam, L. C., and R. van Ee. 2005. The role of (micro)saccades and blinks in perceptual bistability from slant rivalry. Vision Research 45: 2417–2435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wilson, K., and C. C. French. 2014. Magic and memory: Using conjuring to explore the effects of suggestion, social influence, and paranormal belief on eyewitness testimony for an ostensibly paranormal event. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yuval-Greenberg, S., E. P. Merriam, and D. J. Heeger. 2014. Spontaneous microsaccades reflect shifts in covert attention. Journal of Neuroscience 34: 13693–13700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Copyright © 2019. This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Share and Cite
Barnhart, A.S.; Costela, F.M.; Martinez-Conde, S.; Macknik, S.L.; Goldinger, S.D. Microsaccades Reflect the Dynamics of Misdirected Attention in Magic. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2019, 12, 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.6.7
Barnhart AS, Costela FM, Martinez-Conde S, Macknik SL, Goldinger SD. Microsaccades Reflect the Dynamics of Misdirected Attention in Magic. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2019; 12(6):1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.6.7
Chicago/Turabian StyleBarnhart, Anthony S., Francisco M. Costela, Susana Martinez-Conde, Stephen L. Macknik, and Stephen D. Goldinger. 2019. "Microsaccades Reflect the Dynamics of Misdirected Attention in Magic" Journal of Eye Movement Research 12, no. 6: 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.6.7
APA StyleBarnhart, A. S., Costela, F. M., Martinez-Conde, S., Macknik, S. L., & Goldinger, S. D. (2019). Microsaccades Reflect the Dynamics of Misdirected Attention in Magic. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 12(6), 1-14. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.6.7