Next Article in Journal
The Application of Eye-Tracking in Music Research
Previous Article in Journal
When Words and Graphs Move the Eyes: The Processing of Multimodal Causal Relations
 
 
Journal of Eye Movement Research is published by MDPI from Volume 18 Issue 1 (2025). Previous articles were published by another publisher in Open Access under a CC-BY (or CC-BY-NC-ND) licence, and they are hosted by MDPI on mdpi.com as a courtesy and upon agreement with Bern Open Publishing (BOP).
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Dynamics of Eye Movements Under Time Varying Stimuli

by
Verica Radisavljevic-Gajic
Villanova University, Villanova, PA, USA
J. Eye Mov. Res. 2018, 11(1), 1-7; https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.11.1.6
Submission received: 23 September 2017 / Published: 13 June 2018

Abstract

:
In this paper we study the pure-slow and pure-fast dynamics of the disparity convergence of the eye movements second-order linear dynamic mathematical model under time varying stimuli. Performing simulation of the isolated pure-slow and pure-fast dynamics, it has been observed that the pure-fast component corresponding to the eye angular velocity displays abrupt and very fast changes in a very broad range of values. The result obtained is specific for the considered second-order mathematical model that does not include any saturation elements nor time-delay elements. The importance of presented results is in their mathematical simplicity and exactness. More complex mathematical models can be built starting with the presented pure-slow and pure-fast first-order models by appropriately adding saturation and time-delay elements independently to the identified isolated pure-slow and pure-fast first-order models.

Introduction

Studying dynamics of eye movements plays an important role in the development of various eye therapies (Alvarez, 2015) and provides useful information about understanding of neurological processes and the human brain function, (Kennard & Leigh, 2008; Leigh & Zee, 2006). Modeling of the disparity convergence has been studied in several papers (Alvarez, Bhavsar, Semmlow, Bergen, & Pedrono, 2005; Alvarez, Semmlow, & Pedrono, 2005; Alvarez, Semmlow, & Yuan, 1998; Alvarez, Semmlow, Yuan, & Munoz, 1999; Horng, Semmlow, Hung, & Ciuffreda, 1998; Hung, 1998; Hung, Semmlow, & Ciufferda, 1986; Jiang, Hung, & Ciuffreda, 2002; Khosroyani & Hung, 2002). In some studies (Alvarez, Jaswal, Gohel, & Biswal, 2014; Kim, Vicci, Granger-Donetti, & Alvarez, 2011; Lee, Semmlow, & Alvarez, 2012; Radisavljevic-Gajic, 2006) different problem formulations are used. Some of the papers have observed experimentally and analytically the presence of the slow and fast eye movement dynamics, (Alvarez et al., 1998; Hung et al., 1986; Jiang et al., 2002; Khosroyani & Hung, 2002; Lee et al., 2012; Radisavljevic-Gajic, 2006). The analytical observation was made using the corresponding secondorder mathematical model (Alvarez et al., 1999; Horng et al., 1998).
This paper is a continuation of our previous paper (Radisavljevic-Gajic, 2006), originally done for the constant eye stimuli using the second-order dynamic mathematical model derived in Alvarez et al. (1999). For the model of Alvarez et al. (1999), we perform exact mathematical analysis with the goal to isolate the slow and fast components, and present simulation results for the case of time varying eye stimuli since they produce some interesting phenomena not previously observed for the case of constant eye stimuli (Radisavljevic-Gajic, 2006). The im-
Jemr 11 00006 i001Jemr 11 00006 i002Jemr 11 00006 i003
The simulation results of (15)–(16) and (21)–(22), assuming zero initial conditions, that is, x1(0) = 0 and x2(0) = 0, are presented in Figure 1, Figure 2, Figure 3 and Figure 4.

Discussion of the Obtained Simulation Results

The dynamic responses of the pure-slow xs(t) and pure-fast xf(t) variables in the time interval of 10 seconds are presented in Figure 1. It can be observed from this picture that the eye stimuli in the range of 10° to 30°, due to amplification at steady state as given by (20) generate the pure-fast component in the range from 4.4645 × 10° = 44.645° to 4.4645 × 30° = 133.9353°. The same figure shows that the pure-slow component remains in the same range as the input signal, that is, from 10° to 30°, due to the fact that the pure-slow subsystem steady state gain is Gs = 1.
The eye position in the original coordinates x1(t), and the eye original coordinates angular velocity (the time rate of the position change) x2(t) are plotted in Figure 2. It should be observed that the first peak of x2(t) is around 116°/s and that the follow up peaks are around 79°/s. This is caused due to different initial conditions at t = 0 and t = 4,8. We started simulation with zero initial conditions, that is, for the first period the initial conditions are x1(0) = 0° and x2(0) = 0°. For the second period, the initial conditions obtained from formulas (21) and (22) are non-zero and given by Jemr 11 00006 i004
In addition, due to the input signal decrease from 30° to 10°, the fast component takes a large negative value of ≈−67°/s. Hence, during the half period of two seconds, the eye angular velocity changes very drastically, from positive 79°/s to negative ≈−67°/s, producing the absolute angular velocity change of ≈146°/s.
The slow variable (eye position) x1(t) and its pureslow and pure-fast components are presented in Figure 3. Due to the fact that the slow variable is dominated by its pure-slow component and that it has a negligible contribution of the pure-fast component, the figure shows that practically x1(t) ≈ x1s(t), could have been also verified analytically using formula (21).
Much more interesting situation is with the fast variable x2(t) that represents the eye angular velocity, see Figure 4. It can be seen from this figure that its pure-fast component x2f(t) very quickly, in several milliseconds, reaches steady state with a maximum value of around x 2 f max = 140°/s. When the stimuli changes instantly from 30° to 10°, the variable x2f(t) drops within several milliseconds to a little bit below x 2 f min = 50°/sec. On the other hand, the pure-slow component x2s(t) goes in the opposite direction and reaches in less than a second x 2 s min = −130°/s. These two components form x2(t) = x2s(t)+ x2f(t) and together produce at steady state ≈10°/s for the eye angular velocity. Without the pure-slow/pure-fast decomposition, one would not be able to see these violent component in the disparity convergence of the eye movement dynamics.

Conclusions

It was shown that the fast component of the eye dynamics displays very fast and abrupt changes due to considered time varying stimuli as demonstrated in Figure 2 and Figure 4. The angular velocity, due to the change of the stimuli force of 20 degrees (from to 30° to 10°), displays large variations of more than 140°/s, as shown in Figure 4. This large change could have been restricted by introduction of a saturation element. However, that will lead to a new nonlinear mathematical model different than the linear second-order mathematical model considered in this paper. Such nonlinear models are not the subject of this paper, and they will be interesting for future research.

Ethics and Conflict of Interest

The author(s) declare(s) that the contents of the article are in agreement with the ethics described in http://biblio.unibe.ch/portale/elibrary/BOP/jemr/ethics.html and that there is no conflict of interest regarding the publication of this paper.

References

  1. Alvarez, T. L. 2015. A pilot study of disparity vergence and near dissociated phoria in convergence insufficiency patients before vs. after vergence therapy. Front Hum Neurosci 9: 419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Alvarez, T. L., M. Bhavsar, J. L. Semmlow, M. T. Bergen, and C. Pedrono. 2005. Short-term predictive changes in the dynamics of disparity vergence eye movements. Journal of Vision 5, 7: 4–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Alvarez, T. L., R. Jaswal, S. Gohel, and B. B. Biswal. 2014. Functional activity within the frontal eye fields, posterior parietal cortex, and cerebellar vermis significantly correlates to symmetrical vergence peak velocity: An ROI-based, fMRI study of vergence training. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience 8: 50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Alvarez, T. L., J. L. Semmlow, and C. Pedrono. 2005. Divergence eye movements are dependent on initial stimulus position. Vision Res 45, 14: 1847–1855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Alvarez, T. L., J. L. Semmlow, and W. Yuan. 1998. Closely Spaced, Fast Dynamic Movements in Disparity Vergence. Journal of Neurophysiology 79, 1: 37–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Alvarez, T. L., J. L. Semmlow, W. Yuan, and P. Munoz. 1999. Dynamic details of disparity convergence eye movements. Ann Biomed Eng 27, 3: 380–390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Chang, K. W. 1972. Singular Perturbations of a General Boundary Value Problem. SIAM Journal on Mathematical Analysis 3, 3: 520–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Gajic, Z. 2003. Linear Dynamic Systems and Signals. Prentice Hall/Pearson Education. [Google Scholar]
  9. Horng, J. L., J. L. Semmlow, G. K. Hung, and K. J. Ciuffreda. 1998. Initial component control in disparity vergence: A model-based study. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 45, 2: 249–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Hung, G. K. 1998. Dynamic model of the vergence eye movement system: Simulations using MATLAB/SIMULINK. Comput Methods Programs Biomed 55, 1: 59–68. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  11. Hung, G. K., J. L. Semmlow, and K. J. Ciufferda. 1986. A Dual-Mode Dynamic Model of the Vergence Eye Movement System. IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering BME-33, 11: 1021–1028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Jiang, B.-c., G. K. Hung, and K. J. Ciuffreda. 2002. Models of Vergence and AccommodationVergence Interactions. In Models of the Visual System. Edited by G. K. Hung and K. J. Ciuffreda. Boston, MA: Springer US, pp. 341–384. [Google Scholar]
  13. Kennard, C., and R. J. Leigh. 2008. Using Eye Movement as an Experimental Probe of Brain Function. Vol. 171. [Google Scholar]
  14. Khosroyani, M., and G. K. Hung. 2002. A dual-mode dynamic model of the human accommodation system. Bull Math Biol 64, 2: 285–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Kim, E. H., V. R. Vicci, B. Granger-Donetti, and T. L. Alvarez. 2011. Short-term adaptations of the dynamic disparity vergence and phoria systems. Exp Brain Res 212, 2: 267–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Kokotović, P., H. K. Khali, and J. O’Reilly. 1986. Singular Perturbation Methods in Control: Analysis and Design. Society for Industrial and Applied Mathematics. [Google Scholar]
  17. Lee, Y. Y., J. L. Semmlow, and T. L. Alvarez. 2012. Assessment of Dual-Mode and SwitchedChannel Models with Experimental Vergence Responses. 5, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Leigh, R. J., and D. S. Zee. 2006. The Neurology of Eye Movements. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  19. Radisavljevic-Gajic, V. 2006. Slow-fast decoupling of the disparity convergence eye movements dynamics. Ann Biomed Eng 34, 2: 310–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. The responses of the pure-slow xs(t) and pure-fast xf(t) variables in the interval of 10 seconds assuming zero initial conditions. It can be observed from this picture that the eye stimuli in the range of 10° to 30° generate the pure-fast component in the range from 44.6° to 133.9°. The figure shows also that the pure-slow component remains in the same range as the input signal, that is, from 10° to 30°.
Figure 1. The responses of the pure-slow xs(t) and pure-fast xf(t) variables in the interval of 10 seconds assuming zero initial conditions. It can be observed from this picture that the eye stimuli in the range of 10° to 30° generate the pure-fast component in the range from 44.6° to 133.9°. The figure shows also that the pure-slow component remains in the same range as the input signal, that is, from 10° to 30°.
Jemr 11 00006 g001
Figure 2. The variables x1(t) and x2(t) as functions of time. It can be observed that the first peak of x2(t) is around 116°/s and that the follow up peaks are around 79°/s. This is caused due to different initial conditions at t = 0 and t = 4. It was shown in the paper that x1(4) = 9.4 and x2(4) = 2.6°/s. Due to the input signal decrease from 30° to 10°, the fast variable takes a large negative value of ≈−67°/s. During the half period of two seconds x2(t) changes very drastically, from positive 79°/s to negative ≈−67°/s, producing the absolute change of ≈−146°/s.
Figure 2. The variables x1(t) and x2(t) as functions of time. It can be observed that the first peak of x2(t) is around 116°/s and that the follow up peaks are around 79°/s. This is caused due to different initial conditions at t = 0 and t = 4. It was shown in the paper that x1(4) = 9.4 and x2(4) = 2.6°/s. Due to the input signal decrease from 30° to 10°, the fast variable takes a large negative value of ≈−67°/s. During the half period of two seconds x2(t) changes very drastically, from positive 79°/s to negative ≈−67°/s, producing the absolute change of ≈−146°/s.
Jemr 11 00006 g002
Figure 3. Eye position x1(t) and its pure-slow and pure-fast components. Due to the fact that the slow variable is dominated by its pure-slow components and that it has a negligible contribution of the pure-fast component, the figure shows that practically x1(t) ≈ x1s(t), which was also verified analytically in formula (21).
Figure 3. Eye position x1(t) and its pure-slow and pure-fast components. Due to the fact that the slow variable is dominated by its pure-slow components and that it has a negligible contribution of the pure-fast component, the figure shows that practically x1(t) ≈ x1s(t), which was also verified analytically in formula (21).
Jemr 11 00006 g003
Figure 4. Eye angular velocity x2(t) as a function of time. It can be seen that its pure-fast component x2f(t) very quickly, in several milliseconds, reaches steady state with a very high value of around x 2 f max = 140°/s. When the stimuli changes instantly from 30° to 10°, x2f(t) drops within several milliseconds to a little bit below x 2 f min = 50°/sec. The pure-slow component x2s(t) goes in the opposite direction and reaches in less than a second x 2 s min = −130°/s. These two components form x2(t) = x2s(t) + x2f (t)and together produce at steady state ≈10°/s. Without the pure-slow/pure-fast decomposition, one would not be able to see these violent components of the eye movement dynamics.
Figure 4. Eye angular velocity x2(t) as a function of time. It can be seen that its pure-fast component x2f(t) very quickly, in several milliseconds, reaches steady state with a very high value of around x 2 f max = 140°/s. When the stimuli changes instantly from 30° to 10°, x2f(t) drops within several milliseconds to a little bit below x 2 f min = 50°/sec. The pure-slow component x2s(t) goes in the opposite direction and reaches in less than a second x 2 s min = −130°/s. These two components form x2(t) = x2s(t) + x2f (t)and together produce at steady state ≈10°/s. Without the pure-slow/pure-fast decomposition, one would not be able to see these violent components of the eye movement dynamics.
Jemr 11 00006 g004

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Radisavljevic-Gajic, V. Dynamics of Eye Movements Under Time Varying Stimuli. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2018, 11, 1-7. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.11.1.6

AMA Style

Radisavljevic-Gajic V. Dynamics of Eye Movements Under Time Varying Stimuli. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2018; 11(1):1-7. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.11.1.6

Chicago/Turabian Style

Radisavljevic-Gajic, Verica. 2018. "Dynamics of Eye Movements Under Time Varying Stimuli" Journal of Eye Movement Research 11, no. 1: 1-7. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.11.1.6

APA Style

Radisavljevic-Gajic, V. (2018). Dynamics of Eye Movements Under Time Varying Stimuli. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 11(1), 1-7. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.11.1.6

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop