One Page of Text: Eye Movements During Regular and Thorough Reading, Skimming, and Spell Checking
Abstract
:Introduction
Previous Research
The Current Study
Terms and Classifications
Hypotheses
Methods
Participants
Apparatus
Design and Stimuli
- (1)
- What is described as a danger for animals and plants alike?
- To be killed.
- To be poisoned.
- To be eaten.
- To be injured.
- None of the above.
Procedure
Data Analysis
Results
Comprehension Scores
Trial Duration
Fixations and Saccades
Total Reading Times per Character for Lines of Text and for Individual Words
Total Number of Visits
Discussion
Thorough Reading
Skimming
Spell Checking
Conclusions
Ethics and Conflict of Interest
Acknowledgments
Appendix A. Example Stimuli Text in English (Including Line Breaks)
References
- Abbott, M. J., B. Angele, Y. D. Ahn, and K. Rayner. 2015. Skipping syntactically illegal the previews: The role of predictability. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 41, 6: 1703–1714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baker, L., and L. C. Beall. 2009. Metacognitive processes and reading comprehension. Edited by S. E. Israel and G. G. Duffy. In Handbook of research on reading comprehension. Routledge: pp. 373–388. [Google Scholar]
- Bicknell, K., and R. Levy. 2012. Word predictability and frequency effects in a rational model of reading. In Proceedings of the 34th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Conference. Vol. 1, pp. 126–131. [Google Scholar]
- Biedert, R., J. Hees, A. Dengel, and G. Buscher. 2012. A robust realtime reading-skimming classifier. In Proceedings of the Symposium on Eye Tracking Research and Applications. Vol. 1, pp. 123–130. [Google Scholar]
- Bohan, J., H. Leuthold, Y. Hijikata, and A. J. Sanford. 2012. The processing of good-fit semantic anomalies: An ERP investigation. Neuropsychologia 50, 14: 3174–3184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boucheix, J. M., and R. K. Lowe. 2010. An eye tracking comparison of external pointing cues and internal continuous cues in learning with complex animations. Learning and Instruction 20, 2: 123–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brysbaert, M., D. Drieghe, and F. Vitu. 2005. Edited by G. Underwood. Word skipping: Implications for theories of eye movement control in reading. In Cognitive processes in eye guidance. Oxford University Press: pp. 53–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Canham, M., and M. Hegarty. 2010. Effects of knowledge and display design on comprehension of complex graphics. Learning and Instruction 20, 2: 155–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Koning, B. B., H. K. Tabbers, R. M. J. P. Rikers, and F. Paas. 2010. Attention guidance in learning from a complex animation: Seeing is understanding? Learning and Instruction 20, 2: 111–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duggan, G. B., and S. J. Payne. 2009. Text skimming: The process and effectiveness of foraging through text under time pressure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 15, 3: 228–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duggan, G. B., and S. J. Payne. 2011. Skim reading by satisficing: Evidence from eye tracking. In Proceedings of the SIGCHI Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems. ACM: pp. 1141–1150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eitel, A., and T. Kühl. 2016. Effects of disfluency and test expectancy on learning with text. Metacognition and Learning 11, 1: 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fernández, G., D. E. Shalom, R. Kliegl, and M. Sigman. 2014. Eye movements during reading proverbs and regular sentences: The incoming word predictability effect. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29, 3: 260–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferreira, F., and P. E. Engelhardt. 2009. Good enough language processing: A satisficing approach. In Proceedings of the 31st Annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Edited by N. Taatgen, H. Rijn, J. Nerbonne and L. Schomaker. pp. 413–418. Available online: http://141.14.165.6/CogSci09/papers/75/paper75.pdf.
- Ferreira, F., and N. D. Patson. 2007. The “good enough” approach to language comprehension. Language and Linguistics Compass 1, 1–2: 71–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foraker, S., and G. L. Murphy. 2012. Polysemy in sentence comprehension: Effects of meaning dominance. Journal of Memory and Language 67, 4: 407–425. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gao, J., and H. Suzuki. 2005. Long distance dependency in language modeling: An empirical study. In Natural language processing. Springer: pp. 396–405. [Google Scholar]
- Gersten, R., L. S. Fuchs, J. P. Williams, and S. Baker. 2001. Teaching reading comprehension strategies to students with learning disabilities: A review of research. Review of Educational Research 71, 2: 279–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Huey, E. B. 1908. The psychology and pedagogy of reading. The Macmillan Company. [Google Scholar]
- Hyönä, J. 2010. The use of eye movements in the study of multimedia learning. Learning and Instruction 20, 2: 172–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hyönä, J., R. F. Lorch, and J. K. Kaakinen. 2002. Individual differences in reading to summarize expository text: Evidence from eye fixation patterns. Journal of Educational Psychology 94, 1: 44–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hyönä, J., R. F. Lorch, and M. Rinck. 2003. Edited by R. Radach, J. Hyönä and H. Deubel. Eye movement measures to study global text processing. In The mind’s eye: Cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research. Elsevier Science: pp. 313–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hyönä, J., and P. Niemi. 1990. Eye movements during repeated reading of a text. Acta Psychologica 73: 259–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hyönä, J., and A.-M. Nurminen. 2006. Do adult readers know how they read? Evidence from eye movement patterns and verbal reports. British Journal of Psychology 97, 1: 31–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jarodzka, H., and S. Brand-Gruwel. 2017. Tracking the reading eye: Towards a model of real-world reading. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning 33, 3: 193–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jarodzka, H., K. Scheiter, P. Gerjets, and T. van Gog. 2010. In the eyes of the beholder: How experts and novices interpret dynamic stimuli. Learning and Instruction 20, 2: 146–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaakinen, J. K., and J. Hyönä. 2010. Task effects on eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 36, 6: 1561–1566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, A., and L. Osterhout. 2005. The independence of combinatory semantic processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Journal of Memory and Language 52, 2: 205–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kintsch, W. 1998. Comprehension: A paradigm for cognition. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lou, Y., Y. Liu, J. K. Kaakinen, and X. Li. 2017. Using support vector machines to identify literacy skills: Evidence from eye movements. Behavior Research Methods 49, 3: 887–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Magliano, J. P., A. C. Graesser, L. A. Eymard, K. Haberlandt, and B. Gholson. 1993. Locus of interpretive and inference processes during text comprehension: A comparison of gaze durations and word reading times. Cognition 19, 3: 704–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayer, R. E. 2010. Unique contributions of eye-tracking research to the study of learning with graphics. Learning and Instruction 20, 2: 167–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Development Core Team. 2015. R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version 3.2.2). R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Available online: http://www.r-project.org/.
- Rayner, K. 1998. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 124, 3: 372–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K. 2009. Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62, 8: 1457–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K., and S. A. Duffy. 1986. Lexical complexity and fixation times in reading: Effects of word frequency, verb complexity, and lexical ambiguity. Memory & Cognition 14, 3: 191–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K., E. R. Schotter, M. E. Masson, M. C. Potter, and R. Treiman. 2016. So much to read, so little time: How do we read, and can speed reading help? Psychological Science in the Public Interest 17, 1: 4–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K., T. Warren, B. J. Juhasz, and S. P. Liversedge. 2004. The effect of plausibility on eye movements in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 30, 6: 1290–1301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sadoski, M. 1999. Comprehending comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly 34, 4: 493–500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanders, T. J. M., and M. A. Gernsbacher. 2004. Accessibility in text and discourse processing. Discourse Processes 37, 2: 79–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanford, A. J., and P. Sturt. 2002. Depth of processing in language comprehension: Not noticing the evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6, 9: 382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmidt-Weigand, F., A. Kohnert, and U. Glowalla. 2010. A closer look at split visual attention in system-and self-paced instruction in multimedia learning. Learning and Instruction 20, 2: 100–110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schotter, E. R., K. Bicknell, I. Howard, R. Levy, and K. Rayner. 2014. Task effects reveal cognitive flexibility responding to frequency and predictability: Evidence from eye movements in reading and proofreading. Cognition 131, 1: 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schotter, E. R., R. Tran, and K. Rayner. 2014. Don’t believe what you read (only once): Comprehension is supported by regressions during reading. Psychological Science 25, 6: 1218–1226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Simola, J., J. Salojärvi, and I. Kojo. 2008. Using hidden Markov model to uncover processing states from eye movements in information search tasks. Cognitive Systems Research 9, 4: 237–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strukelj, A., K. Scheiter, M. Nyström, and K. Holmqvist. 2016. Exploring the lack of a disfluency effect: Evidence from eye movements. Metacognition and Learning 11, 1: 71–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tinker, M. A. 1927. Eye movement duration, pause duration, and reading time. Psychological Review 35, 5: 385–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trauzettel-Klosinski, S., and K. Dietz. 2012. Standardized assessment of reading performance: The new international reading speed texts IReST. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 53, 9: 5452–5461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vasishth, S., K. Suckow, R. L. Lewis, and S. Kern. 2010. Short-term forgetting in sentence comprehension: Crosslinguistic evidence from verb-final structures. Language and Cognitive Processes 25, 4: 533–567. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vitu, F. 2011. Edited by S. P. Liversedge, I. D. Gilchrist and S. Everling. On the role of visual and oculomotor processes in reading. In The Oxford handbook of eye movements. Oxford University Press: pp. 731–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, S. J., K. L. Warrington, V. A. McGowan, and K. B. Paterson. 2015. Eye movements during reading and topic scanning: Effects of word frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 41, 1: 233–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilkinson, S. C., W. Reader, and S. J. Payne. 2012. Adaptive browsing: Sensitivity to time pressure and task difficulty. International Journal of Human Computer Studies 70, 1: 14–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wotschack, C. 2009. Eye movements in reading strategies: How reading strategies modulate effects of distributed processing and oculomotor control. Universitätsverlag Potsdam. [Google Scholar]
Hypothesis compared to regular reading | Hypothesized effects compared to regular reading | |
Thorough reading | Better comprehension of material | Higher comprehension scores |
No difference in processing on words for all fixations | Average fixation durations similar to regular reading | |
No difference in distances between two subsequent fixations during reading | Saccade amplitudes similar to regular reading | |
More rereading of previously read text | Higher proportion of vertical saccades | |
More regressions | Higher proportion of leftward saccades | |
More deliberate reading during entire trial | Longer total reading times, higher number of visits on words | |
Reading is very similar to regular reading when investigating one entire trial | Reading pattern is similar to regular reading during entire trial | |
Skimming | Reduced comprehension of material | Lower comprehension scores |
Lower amounts of processing on words for all fixations | Shorter average fixation durations | |
Larger distances between two subsequent fixations during reading | Larger saccade amplitudes | |
Less rereading of previously read text | Lower proportion of vertical saccades | |
Fewer regressions | Lower proportion of leftward saccades | |
Less deliberate reading during entire trial | Shorter total reading times, lower number of visits on words | |
Uniform reading pattern across the entire text | Total reading times and number of visits are similar over the entire text | |
Spell checking | Reduced comprehension of material | Lower comprehension scores |
Higher amounts of processing on words for all fixations | Longer average fixation durations | |
Smaller distances between two subsequent fixations during reading | Smaller saccade amplitudes | |
More pronounced frequency effect | Even longer first fixation durations and first-pass reading times for less frequent compared to more frequent words | |
More pronounced word length effect | Even longer total reading times for longer compared to shorter words | |
No differences in rereading of previously read text | Proportion of vertical saccades similar to regular reading | |
More regressions | Higher proportion of leftward saccades | |
More deliberate reading during entire trial | Longer total reading times, higher number of visits on words | |
Uniform reading pattern across the entire text | Total reading times and number of visits are similar over the entire text |
(a) | ||
Vertical saccades | ||
% | PP +/− | |
Regular reading | 14.92 | - |
Thorough reading | 15.53 * | 0.61 * |
Skimming | 12.79 *** | −2.13 *** |
Spell checking | 16.94 *** | 2.02 *** |
(b) | ||
Leftward saccades | ||
% | PP +/− | |
Regular reading | 23.37 | - |
Thorough reading | 24.17 | 0.80 |
Skimming | 21.25 *** | −2.02 *** |
Spell checking | 24.45 * | 1.08 * |
Estimate | SE | df | t-value | p-value | |
Thorough reading | |||||
0.2334 | 0.0729 | 100 | 3.202 | .00183 | ** |
Skimming | |||||
−0.6218 | 0.0868 | 49 | −7.162 | .00000 | *** |
Spell checking | |||||
0.1789 | 0.0879 | 75 | 2.034 | .04550 | * |
Line number | |||||
0.0037 | 0.0042 | 6415 | 0.865 | .38717 | |
Thorough reading: Line number | |||||
0.0003 | 0.0060 | 6422 | 0.047 | .96261 | |
Skimming: Line number | |||||
0.0117 | 0.0061 | 6431 | 1.920 | .05489 | . |
Spell checking: Line number | |||||
0.0148 | 0.0060 | 6413 | 2.472 | .01347 | * |
Estimate | SE | df | t-value | p-value | |
Thorough reading | |||||
0.1181 | 0.0390 | 35 | 3.026 | .00461 | ** |
Skimming | |||||
−0.3571 | 0.0394 | 74 | −9.050 | .00000 | *** |
Spell checking | |||||
0.06.686 | 0.0550 | 33 | 1.215 | .23317 | |
Word number | |||||
−0.0011 | 0.0002 | 36490 | −5.249 | .00000 | *** |
Thorough reading: Word number | |||||
0.0004 | 0.0003 | 36440 | 1.511 | .13082 | |
Skimming: Word number | |||||
0.0008 | 0.0003 | 36120 | 2.532 | .01134 | * |
Spell checking: Word number | |||||
0.0016 | 0.0003 | 36720 | 5.583 | .00000 | *** |
Estimate | SE | df | t-value | p-value | |
Thorough reading | |||||
0.2536 | 0.0755 | 26 | 3.358 | .00247 | ** |
Skimming | |||||
−0.6153 | 0.0730 | 34 | −8.431 | .00000 | *** |
Spell checking | |||||
0.0661 | 0.0986 | 37 | 0.671 | .50675 | |
Word number | |||||
−0.0021 | 0.0003 | 59640 | −7.460 | .00000 | *** |
Thorough reading: Word number | |||||
0.0007 | 0.0004 | 59640 | 1.780 | .07502 | . |
Skimming: Word number | |||||
0.0024 | 0.0004 | 59640 | 5.812 | .00000 | *** |
Spell checking: Word number | |||||
0.0018 | 0.0004 | 59640 | 4.362 | .00001 | *** |
Behavior compared to regular reading | Measured effects compared to regular reading | |
Thorough reading | Better comprehension of material | Higher comprehension scores |
No difference in processing on words for all fixations | Average fixation durations similar to regular reading | |
Larger distances between two subsequent fixations during reading | Larger saccade amplitudes similar to regular reading | |
More rereading of previously read text | Higher proportion of vertical saccades | |
No difference in regressions on the current line of text | No difference in proportionof leftwardsaccades | |
More deliberate reading during entire trial | Longer total reading times, higher number of visits on words | |
Reading behavior is similar to regular reading when investigating fixations durations on words in one entire trial | Reading pattern is similar to regular reading during entire trial | |
Skimming | Worse comprehension of material | Lower comprehension scores |
Lower amounts of processing on words for all fixations | Shorter average fixation durations | |
Larger distances between two subsequent fixations during reading | Larger saccade amplitudes | |
Less rereading of previously read text | Lower proportion of vertical saccades | |
Fewer regressions | Lower proportion of leftward saccades | |
Less deliberate reading during entire trial | Shorter total reading times, lower number of visits on words | |
Uniform reading pattern across the entire text | Total reading times and number of visits are similar over the entire text | |
Spell checking | Worse comprehension of material | Lower comprehension scores |
Higher amounts of processing on words for all fixations | Longer average fixation durations | |
Smaller distances between two subsequent fixations during reading | Smaller saccade amplitudes | |
More pronounced frequency effect | Even longer first fixation durations and first-pass reading times for less frequent compared to more frequent words | |
More pronounced word length effect | Even longer total reading times for longer compared to shorter words | |
More rereading of previously read text | Higher proportion of vertical saccades | |
More regressions | Higher proportion of leftward saccades | |
More deliberate reading during entire trial | Longer total reading times, higher number of visits on words | |
Uniform reading pattern across the entire text | Total reading times and number of visits are similar over the entire text |
Copyright © 2018. This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Share and Cite
Strukelj, A.; Niehorster, D.C. One Page of Text: Eye Movements During Regular and Thorough Reading, Skimming, and Spell Checking. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2018, 11, 1-22. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.11.1.1
Strukelj A, Niehorster DC. One Page of Text: Eye Movements During Regular and Thorough Reading, Skimming, and Spell Checking. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2018; 11(1):1-22. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.11.1.1
Chicago/Turabian StyleStrukelj, Alexander, and Diederick C. Niehorster. 2018. "One Page of Text: Eye Movements During Regular and Thorough Reading, Skimming, and Spell Checking" Journal of Eye Movement Research 11, no. 1: 1-22. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.11.1.1
APA StyleStrukelj, A., & Niehorster, D. C. (2018). One Page of Text: Eye Movements During Regular and Thorough Reading, Skimming, and Spell Checking. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 11(1), 1-22. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.11.1.1