Eye Tracking in Educational Science: Theoretical Frameworks and Research Agendas
Abstract
:Introduction
Instructional Design—adapting the environment to the learner’s abilities
Theories of human learning – the working memory perspective
- Seeking coherence of information. First and foremost it is crucial to avoid unnecessary information presented in instructional material, such as decorative pictures. As the learner tries to make sense out of every information given and integrate it with the other presented information and with own prior knowledge, irrelevant information will only unnecessarily consume cognitive capacities.
- Avoiding redundant information. The exact same information should not be given in different formats, because the learner tries to integrate all information with each other as well as with prior knowledge. This in turn costs cognitive capacities, which are not available for learning any more. One common ‘bad’ example is presenting a text on the slides and reading it out loud at the same time.
- Making use of multimedia. Even though the exact same information should not be presented in different modalities, preferable the same subject matter should be presented in different ways. For instance, an explanation of a car engine is easier to understand with an accompanying picture or animation.
- Making use of different modalities. To account for the dual-coding characteristics of working memory, instructional material should present related information in different modalities. For instance, a graph accompanied by an audio text instead of a written text.
- Avoiding split attention by seeking contiguity. Instructional material should present related information that needs to be integrated in closely, both in space and time. For instance, the legend of a graph should better be incorporated in the graph itself than presented on the side.
Testing learning theories in educational practice
Research agenda for Instructional Design theories
- In the latter example presented above (Ögren et al., 2016), we saw that the guidelines given, might need to be specified. Hence, it is crucial to test also the other guidelines for Instructional Design with eye tracking, but also under ecologically valid circumstances (i.e., actual learning material with real students).
- In the first example above (Jarodzka, Janssen, et al., 2015), we saw that even if assuming that those guidelines are appropriate, some basic pre-assumptions of these theories might not be (e.g., that learners do their best to actively integrate material). Hence, it is crucial to test also these. In particular, the many assumptions about perceptual processes must be tested directly with eye tracking.
- The research discussed thus far considered cognitive processes. However, metacognitive processes are also crucial for learning (i.e., monitoring what I already can do what I still need to practice). However, too little research has been conducted on this important topic until now (Van Gog & Jarodzka, 2013).
- Finally, eye tacking research is conducted in laboratories where one participant at a time is tested under minimal disturbance. This has, however, nothing to do with educational practice. From social psychology research, we know that performing a task in the presence of others might be inhibiting, but also facilitating (Bond & Titus, 1983). Eye tracking research also shows effects of social presence on attention (Oliva, Niehorster, Jarodzka, & Holmqvist, in press; Richardson et al., 2012) Hence, future eye tracking research should investigate social effects on processes of learning, for instance within so-called digital classrooms.
Expertise development—scaffolding the learner to the environment
Theories of human learning—the long-term memory perspective
Research on visual expertise in information-rich environments
Research agenda for visual expertise research
- To which extent can we generalize these findings? We have seen that sometimes even slight changes in the task can lead to different outcomes (cf. air traffic control), while sometimes the changes go in the same direction (cf. pathology). Also, some findings that are found in one profession (e.g., experts become more similar in air traffic control) are not true for another profession (e.g., experts in marine zoology become more diverse). Hence, future research should consistently vary task characteristics and professions to understand, which aspects of visual expertise are generic and which domain-specific.
- A lot of research on visual expertise has been conducted on simplified tasks. This was largely due to technological restrictions of the eye tracking apparatuses and software. Research should not be hold back by technological obstacles, but rather feed their development. In particular two issues must be tackled to foster ecologically valid research on visual expertise. First, the detection of smooth pursuit to enable valid analysis of dynamic stimuli. Thereby, it is not enough to detect smooth pursuit with a stand-alone algorithm, but it must be implemented into existing analysis software, so it can be used in applied research as well. Second, more automated analyses for mobile eye tracking. Clearly, the truest way of analyzing visual expertise often requires real-world eye tracking. However, cumbersome manual analyses often hold researchers back.
- The presented research has shown how much we can benefit from methodological triangulation when investigating multifaceted concepts such as visual expertise. In a next step, research should directly link the analysis of verbal and eye tracking data. Only in this way it will be possible to make more concrete statements about the cognitive structures underlying these processes.
- Finally, it must become the ultimate aim of this research line to unravel the organization of knowledge and skills in long-term memory and how it develops with increasing expertise. Only then it is possible to draw meaningful conclusions from eye tracking data that go beyond superficial statements such as ‘experts had longer fixation durations’ that have virtually no meaning for professional or educational practice (Kok & Jarodzka, 2016).
Eye movement modeling examples: Bridging Instructional Design and expertise research
Theories of human learning – training visual aspects of expertise
Research on eye movement modeling examples
Research agenda for EMME
- The role of the task and the stimulus characteristics: The research on EMME covers a diversity of tasks (from insight problem solving, to performance only, to transfer and learning) and a diversity of stimuli (from simple line drawings to complex videos). A systematic variation and concrete description of these factors should shed more light into when EMME are effective. For instance, existing studies already indicate that the visual complexity of the task is crucial: Van Gog et al. (2009) used a task that could be executed without perceptual input and found negative effects of EMME on performance (Van Marlen, Van Wermeskerken, Jarodzka, & Van Gog, in press). Jarodzka et al. (2013) used a fish locomotion classification task where all relevant information was visual salient. EMME was in part helpful in this case. Jarodzka, Balslev, et al. (2012) used a pediatric neurology task, where the relevant information was transient and not salient. This is where EMME were most helpful.
- The role of the eye movement display design is an entirely understudied aspect. Apart from two studies (Jarodzka, Balslev, et al., 2012; Jarodzka et al., 2013), none has compared different designs directly even though these studies indicate that this might be a crucial success factor for EMME. Results showed that reducing information on a spotlight manner guides visual attention on EMME videos best. Also, the spotlight facilitates visual search on testing videos most. However, the interpretation of relevant features is only enhanced, if a holistic processing is possible during learning.
- Moreover, the role of didactizing the expert model, as we have done in our studies, has not been directly investigated. In fact, most studies provide hardly any description on how the model’s eye movements were collected. This is surprising as we know very well from research to which large extent experts and novices differ in their processing and how unlikely it thus is that forcing experts’ processes upon novices can hardly work.
- Finally, the EMME methodology could be embedded into well-established methods of expertise trainings. For instance, the 4C-ID training (Van Merriënboer & Kirschner, 2007) is an elaborated model to design a curriculum for complex tasks. It includes modeling episodes that might easily be filled in with EMME for specific visual tasks. Another example is deliberate practice (Ericsson, Krampe, & Tesch-Römer, 1993). This method involves a detailed study of own and others performance. If the task includes visual aspects, studying the eye movements of an expert (or one owns) might provide additional benefits.
Discussion
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Arthur, P.; Blom, D.; Khuu, S. Music sightreading expertise, visually disrupted score and eye movements. Journal of Eye Movement Research 2016, 9(7), 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Atkinson, R. C.; Shiffrin, R. M. Spence, K. W., Spence, J. T., Eds.; Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In The psychology of learning and motivation: Advances in research and theory; Academic Press, 1968; Vol. 2, pp. 89–192. [Google Scholar]
- Baddeley, A. D. Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology 2012, 63(1), 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Balslev, T.; Jarodzka, H.; Holmqvist, K.; De Grave, W. S.; Muijtjens, A.; Eika, B.; Scherpbier, A. Visual expertise in paediatric neurology. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology 2012, 16(2), 161–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bandura, A. Social learning theory; Prentice-Hall, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Bandura, A.; Ross, S. A.; Ross, D. Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology 1961, 63(3), 575–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Beck, M. R.; Trenchard, M.; van Lamsweerde, A.; Goldstein, R. R.; Lohrenz, M. Searching in clutter: Visual attention strategies of expert pilots. In Proceedings of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society annual meeting; Santa Monica, USA, Sage, September 2012; Vol. 56, No. 1, pp. 1411–1415. [Google Scholar]
- Bertram, R.; Kaakinen, J.; Bensch, F.; Helle, L.; Lantto, E.; Niemi, P.; Lundbom, N. Eye movements of radiologists reflect expertise in CT study interpretation: A potential tool to measure resident development. Radiology 2016, 281(3), 805–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bond, C. F.; Titus, L. J. Social facilitation: A meta-analysis of 241 studies. Psychological Bulletin 1983, 94(2), 265–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Boshuizen, H. P. A.; Schmidt, H. G. On the role of biomedical knowledge in clinical reasoning by experts, intermediates, and novices. Cognitive Science 1992, 16(2), 153–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chandler, P.; Sweller, J. Cognitive load theory and the format of instruction. Cognition and Instruction 1991, 8(4), 293–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chase, W. G.; Simon, H. A. W., *!!! REPLACE !!!*, G., Chase, Eds.; The mind’s eye in chess. In Visual information processing; Academic Press, 1973; pp. 215–281. [Google Scholar]
- Cherubini, M.; Nüssli, M. A.; Dillenbourg, P. This is it! Indicating and looking in collaborative work at distance. Journal of Eye Movement Research 2010, 3(5), 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Collins, A.; Brown, J. S.; Newman, S. E. L., *!!! REPLACE !!!*, B., Resnick, Eds.; Cognitive apprenticeship: Teaching the craft of reading, writing, and mathematics. In Cognition and instruction: Issues and gendas; Erlbaum, 1989; pp. 453–494. [Google Scholar]
- De Groot, A. D. Thought and choice in chess; Mouton, 1946/2008. [Google Scholar]
- Denzin, N. K. Triangulation 2.0. Journal of Mixed Methods Research 2012, 6(2), 80–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dorr, M.; Vig, E.; Gegenfurtner, K. R.; Martinetz, T.; Barth, E. Eye movement modelling and gaze guidance. Paper presented at the Fourth International Workshop on HumanComputer Conversation, Oxford, UK, October; 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Dwyer, F. M. Adapting media attributes for effective learning. Educational Technology 1976, 16(8), 7–13. [Google Scholar]
- Ericsson, K. A.; Charness, N.; Feltovich, P.; Hoffman, R. R. (Eds.) The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance; Cambridge University Press, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Ericsson, K. A.; Kintsch, W. Long-term working memory. Psychological Review 1995, 102(2), 211–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ericsson, K. A.; Krampe, R. T.; Tesch-Römer, C. The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review 1993, 100(3), 363–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ericsson, K. A.; Lehmann, A. C. Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaption to task constraints. Annual Reviews in Psychology 1996, 47(1), 273–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ericsson, K. A.; Simon, H. A. Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data; MIT Press, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Ericsson, K. A.; Smith, J. Ericsson, K. A., Smith, J., Eds.; Prospects and limits in the empirical study of expertise. In Towards a general theory of expertise: Prospects and limits (pp. 138); Cambridge University Press, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Freyhof, H.; Gruber, H.; Ziegler, A. Expertise and hierarchical knowledge representation in chess. Psychological Research 1992, 54(1), 32–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Groner, R., & Siegenthaler, E. (2009). Improving the usability of eLearning tools: The IFeL Multifunctional Analysis and its application in distance teaching. Proceedings of the ICDE/EADTU Conference in Maastricht, June 2009. Retrieved from: http://www.ou.nl/Docs/Campagnes/ICDE2009/P apers/Final_Paper_100Groner.pdf.
- Gruber, H. Wissensakquisition und Gedächtnisleistung in Abhängigkeit vom Expertisegrad; University of Munich, Chair of Education and Educational Psychology, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Hegarty, M. Mental animation: Inferring motion from static displays of mechanical systems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 1992, 18(5), 1084–1102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Holmqvist, K.; Nyström, M.; Andersson, R.; Dewhurst, R.; Jarodzka, H.; Van de Weijer, J. Eye tracking: A comprehensive guide to methods and measures; Oxford University Press, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Jaarsma, T.; Boshuizen, H. P. A.; Jarodzka, H.; Nap, M.; Verboon, P.; Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. Tracks to a medical diagnosis: expertise differences in visual problem solving. Applied Cognitive Psychology 2016, 30(3), 314–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jaarsma, T.; Jarodzka, H.; Nap, M.; Van Merriёnboer, J. J. G.; Boshuizen, H. P. A. Expertise differences under the microscope: processing histopathological slides. Medical Education 2014, 48(3), 292–300. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jaarsma, T.; Jarodzka, H.; Nap, M.; Van Merriënboer, J. J. G.; Boshuizen, H. P. A. Expertise in clinical pathology: Bridging the gap. Advances in Health Sciences Education 2015, 20(4), 1089–1106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacob, R.; Karn, K. S. Hyönä, J., Radach, R., Deubel, H., Eds.; Eye tracking in humancomputer interaction and usability research: Ready to deliver the promises. In The mind's eye: Cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research; Elsevier, 2003; pp. 573–605. [Google Scholar]
- Jarodzka, H.; Balslev, T.; Holmqvist, K.; Nyström, M.; Scheiter, K.; Gerjets, P.; Eika, B. Conveying clinical reasoning based on visual observation via eye-movement modelling examples. Instructional Science 2012, 40(5), 813–827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jarodzka, H.; Boshuizen, H. P. A.; Kirschner, P. A. Lanzer, P., Ed.; Cognitive skills in medical diagnosis and intervention. In Catheterbased cardiovascular interventions; Knowledgebased approach; Springer, 2012; pp. 69–86. [Google Scholar]
- Jarodzka, H.; Gouw, P.; Van Meeuwen, L.; BrandGruwel, S. Air traffic control: Visual expertise in a dynamic problem solving task. Paper presented at the 18th European Conference on Eye Movements, Vienna, Austria, August; 2015. [Google Scholar]
- Jarodzka, H.; Janssen, N.; Kirschner, P. A.; Erkens, G. Avoiding split attention in computerbased testing: Is neglecting additional information facilitative? British Journal of Educational Technology 2015, 46(4), 803–817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jarodzka, H.; Scheiter, K.; Gerjets, P.; Van Gog, T. In the eyes of the beholder: How experts and novices interpret dynamic stimuli. Learning and Instruction 2010, 20(2), 146–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jarodzka, H.; Van Gog, T.; Dorr, M.; Scheiter, K.; Gerjets, P. Learning to see: Guiding students’ attention via a model’s eye movements fosters learning. Learning and Instruction 2013, 25, 62–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jucks, R.; Schulte-Löbbert, P.; Bromme, R. Supporting experts’ written knowledge communication through reflective prompts on the use of specialist concepts. Journal of Psychology 2007, 215(4), 237–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirschner, P. A.; Sweller, J.; Clark, R. E. Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist 2006, 41(2), 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kok, E.; Jarodzka, H. Before your very eyes: The value and limitations of eye tracking in medical education. Medical Education 2016, 51, 114–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kok, E.; Jarodzka, H.; De Bruin, A. B. H.; BinAmir, H.; Robben, S. G. F.; Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. Systematic viewing in radiology: Seeing more, missing less? Advances in Health Science Education 2015, 21(1), 189–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krejtz, K.; Duchowski, A. T.; Krejtz, I.; Kopacz, A.; Chrząstowski-Wachtel, P. Gaze transitions when learning with multimedia. Journal of Eye Movement Research 2016, 9(1), 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lachner, A.; Jarodzka, H.; Nückles, M. What makes an expert teacher? Investigating teachers’ professional vision and discourse abilities. Instructional Science 2016, 44(3), 197–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leff, D. R.; James, D. R.; Orihuela-Espina, F.; Kwok, K.-W.; Sun, L. W.; Mylonas, G.; Yang, G.-Z. The impact of expert visual guidance on trainee visual search strategy, visual attention and motor skills. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2015, 9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lehmann, A. C.; Gruber, H. Ericsson, K. A., Charness, N., Feltovich, P., Hoffman, R. R., Eds.; Music. In The Cambridge handbook of expertise and expert performance; Cambridge University Press, 2006; pp. 457–470. [Google Scholar]
- Lilienthal, O. (1889). Der Vogelflug als Grundlage der Fliegekunst, Berlin, Germany. Retrieved from: https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Lilient halFliegekunst.png.
- Lin, J. J.; Lin, S. S. Tracking eye movements when solving geometry problems with handwriting devices. Journal of Eye Movement Research 2014, 7(1), 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lindow, S.; Fuchs, H. M.; Fürstenberg, A.; Kleber, J.; Schweppe, J.; Rummer, R. On the robustness of the modality effect: Attempting to replicate a basic finding. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie 2011, 25(4), 231–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Litchfield, D.; Ball, L. J. Using another’s gaze as an explicit aid to insight problem solving. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 2011, 64(3), 649–656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lowe, R. K. Animation and learning: Selective processing of information in dynamic graphics. Learning and Instruction 2003, 13(2), 157–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mason, L.; Tornatora, M. C.; Pluchino, P. Do fourth graders integrate text and picture in processing and learning from an illustrated science text? Evidence from eye-movement patterns. Computers & Education 2013, 60(1), 95–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayer, R. E. Multimedia learning; Cambridge University Press, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Mayer, R. E.; Moreno, R. Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist 2003, 38(1), 43–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCabe, D.; Castel, A. Seeing is believing: The effect of brain images on judgments of scientific reasoning. Cognition 2008, 107(1), 343–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McNamara, A.; Booth, T.; Sridharan, S.; Caffey, S.; Grimm, C.; Bailey, R. Directing gaze in narrative art. In Paper presented at the Proceedings of the ACM Symposium on Applied Perception, Los Angeles, USA, August; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Meltzoff, A. N.; Moore, M. K. Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 1977, 198(4312), 75–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miller, G. A. The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review 1956, 63(2), 81–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nyström, M.; Holmqvist, K. Semantic override of low-level features in image viewing both initially and overall. Journal of Eye Movement Research 2008, 2, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ögren, M.; Nyström, M.; Jarodzka, H. There's more to the multimedia effect than meets the eye: Is seeing pictures believing? Instructional Science 2016, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oliva, M., Niehorster, D., Jarodzka, H., & Holmqvist, K. (in press). Influence of coactors on saccadic and manual responses. i-Percetion.
- Paivio, A. Dual coding theory: Retrospect and current status. Canadian Journal of Psychology 1991, 45(3), 255–287. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Penttinen, M.; Huovinen, E. The early development of sight-reading skills in adulthood: A study of eye movements. Journal of Research in Music Education 2011, 59(2), 196–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Penttinen, M.; Huovinen, E.; Ylitalo, A.-K. Silent music reading: Amateur musicians’ visual processing and descriptive skill. Musicae Scientiae 2013, 17(2), 198–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Penttinen, M.; Huovinen, E.; Ylitalo, A.-K. Reading ahead: Adult music students’ eye movements in temporally controlled performances of a children’s song. International Journal of Music Education 2015, 33(1), 36–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 1998, 124(3), 372–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rayner, K. Eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 2009, 62(8), 1457–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reingold, E. M.; Charness, N.; Pomplun, M.; Stampe, D. M. Visual span in expert chess players: Evidence from eye movements. Psychological Science 2001, 12(1), 48–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reingold, E. M.; Sheridan, H. Liversedge, S. P., Gilchrist, I. D., Everling, S., Eds.; Eye movements and visual expertise in chess and medicine. In Oxford handbook of eye movements; Oxford University Press, 2011; pp. 523–550. [Google Scholar]
- Richardson, D. C.; Dale, R. Looking to understand: The coupling between speakers' and listeners’ eye movements and its relationship to discourse comprehension. Cognitive Science 2005, 29(6), 1045–1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Richardson, D. C.; Street, C. N. H.; Tan, J. Y. M.; Kirkham, N. Z.; Hoover, M.; GhaneCavanaugh, A. Joint perception: Gaze and social context. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 2012, 6, 194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rummer, R.; Schweppe, J.; Fürstenberg, A.; Scheiter, K.; Zindler, A. The perceptual basis of the modality effect in multimedia learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 2011, 17(2), 159–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schank, R. C.; Abelson, R. P. Scripts, plans, goals, and understanding: An inquiry into human knowledge structures; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt, H. G.; Boshuizen, H. P. A. Evans, D. A., Patel, V. L., Eds.; Encapsulation of biomedical knowledge. In Advanced models of cognition for medical training and practice; Springer Verlag, 1992; pp. 265–282. [Google Scholar]
- Schnotz, W.; Lowe, R. K. Lowe, R. K., Schnotz, W., Eds.; A unified view of learning from animated and static graphics. In Learning with animation: Research and design implications; Cambridge University Press, 2008; pp. 304–356. [Google Scholar]
- Skuballa, I. T.; Fortunski, C.; Renkl, A. An eye movement pre-training fosters the comprehension of processes and functions in technical systems. Frontiers in Psychology 2015, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Skuballa, I. T.; Schwonke, R.; Renkl, A. Learning from narrated animations with different support procedures: Working memory capacity matters. Applied Cognitive Psychology 2012, 26(6), 840–847. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sridharan, S.; Bailey, R.; McNamara, A.; Grimm, C. Subtle gaze manipulation for improved mammography training. In Proceedings of the Symposium on Eye Tracking Research and Applications, Santa Barbara, USA, March; 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Stofer, K.; Che, X. Comparing experts and novices on scaffolded data visualizations using eye-tracking. Journal of Eye Movement Research 2014, 7(5), 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J.; Van Merriënboer, J. J. G.; Paas, F. Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychological Review 1998, 10(3), 251–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thurmond, V. A. The point of triangulation. Journal of Nursing Scholarship 2001, 33(3), 253–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van der Gijp, A.; Ravesloot, C.; Jarodzka, H.; Van der Schaaf, M.; Van der Schaaf, I.; Van Schaik, J.; Ten Cate, T. J. How visual search relates to visual diagnostic performance: a narrative systematic review of eye-tracking research in radiology. Advances in Health Sciences Education 2016, 1–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Gog, T.; Jarodzka, H. Azevedo, R., Aleven, V., Eds.; Eye tracking as a tool to study and enhance (meta-)cognitive processes in computer-based learning environments. In International handbook of metacognition and learning technologies; Springer Science+Business Media, 2013; pp. 143–156. [Google Scholar]
- Van Gog, T.; Jarodzka, H.; Scheiter, K.; Gerjets, P.; Paas, F. Attention guidance during example study via the model’s eye movements. Computers in Human Behavior 2009, 25(3), 785–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Gog, T.; Paas, F.; Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. Process-oriented worked examples: Improving transfer performance through enhanced understanding. Instructional Science 2004, 32(1/2), 83–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Gog, T.; Paas, F.; Van Merriënboer, J.; Witte, P. Uncovering the problem-solving process: Cued retrospective reporting versus concurrent and retrospective reporting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 2005, 11(4), 237–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Lehn, K. Cognitive skill acquisition. Annual Review of Psychology 1996, 47, 513–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Van Marlen, T.; Van Wermeskerken, M.; Jarodzka, H.; Van Gog, T. Showing a model’s eye movements in examples does not improve performance of problem-solving tasks. Computers in Human Behavior in press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Meeuwen, L. W.; Jarodzka, H.; Brand-Gruwel, S.; Kirschner, P. A.; De Bock, J. J. P. R.; Van Merriënboer, J. J. G. Identification of effective visual problem solving strategies in a complex visual domain. Learning and Instruction 2014, 32, 10–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Merriënboer, J. J. G.; Kirschner, P. A. Ten steps to complex learning; Erlbaum, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Wolff, C. E.; Jarodzka, H.; Van den Bogert, N.; Boshuizen, H. P. A. Teacher vision: Comparing expert and novice teachers’ perception of problematic classroom management scenes. Instructional Science 2016, 44(3), 243–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yarbus, A. L. Eye movements and vision; Plenum, 1967. [Google Scholar]
Copyright © 2024. This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Share and Cite
Halszka, J.; Holmqvist, K.; Gruber, H. Eye Tracking in Educational Science: Theoretical Frameworks and Research Agendas. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2017, 10, 1-18. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.10.1.3
Halszka J, Holmqvist K, Gruber H. Eye Tracking in Educational Science: Theoretical Frameworks and Research Agendas. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2017; 10(1):1-18. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.10.1.3
Chicago/Turabian StyleHalszka, Jarodzka, Kenneth Holmqvist, and Hans Gruber. 2017. "Eye Tracking in Educational Science: Theoretical Frameworks and Research Agendas" Journal of Eye Movement Research 10, no. 1: 1-18. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.10.1.3
APA StyleHalszka, J., Holmqvist, K., & Gruber, H. (2017). Eye Tracking in Educational Science: Theoretical Frameworks and Research Agendas. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 10(1), 1-18. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.10.1.3