Next Article in Journal
Research on the Effects of the High Temperature and Humidity Environment on Human Comfort in Coal Mine Emergency Refuge System
Previous Article in Journal
Identifying Safety Training Resource Needs in the Cattle Feeding Industry in the Midwestern United States
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Adolescents’ Mobile Phone Use While Crossing the Road

School of Psychology, University of Lincoln, Brayford Way, Brayford Pool, Lincoln LN6 7TS, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Safety 2019, 5(2), 27; https://doi.org/10.3390/safety5020027
Submission received: 18 March 2019 / Revised: 17 April 2019 / Accepted: 23 April 2019 / Published: 1 May 2019

Abstract

:
Phones and other portable technology can be a distraction for pedestrians, affecting their ability to cross a road safely. This study focused on adolescents and investigated whether using a phone distracts attention while crossing the road. A field observation outside a secondary school in the north of England was carried out over a four-week period in 2018 with permission from the school. Observations included recording what accessories the pedestrian was carrying (phone, headphones or another electronic device) and their associated action (whether they were holding the device, speaking into a phone, looking at it, holding it to their ear or interacting with it manually). We observed whether the pedestrian looked (or failed to look) left and right before crossing the road, whether they crossed when the pedestrian light was on green or red, and whether they crossed within the cross-walk. We found that 31.37% of road crossings were made by adolescents with a phone or other device. They looked left and right before crossing less frequently when they had an electronic device with them, when looking at the screen and when texting or swiping. In conclusion, the safety of adolescent pedestrians is affected by mobile phones and music players.

1. Introduction

Pedestrian deaths and injuries are a global problem. The World Health Organization [1] reported that 23% of the 1.35 million global road traffic deaths in 2016 were pedestrians. In the UK, 25% of all road deaths reported in 2014 were pedestrians [2]. Also, 28.65% of pedestrian casualties in 2017 were to those younger than 17 years of age [2,3]. In cases where the pedestrian was noted as a contributory factor to a traffic collision, ‘the pedestrian failed to look properly’ was the most frequent cause of pedestrian death or injury, accounting for 49% of all such cases. In addition, the pedestrian failing to look properly was a contributory factor to road accidents in 39% of cases in which the pedestrian was not injured [4]. The pedestrian failing to look properly may be caused by distractions. A growing cause of distraction for pedestrians is the use of mobile technology, such as mobile phones, smartphones, and music players. Research has established that mobile phones can cause a dangerous distraction to drivers [5,6,7] and legislation against using a hand-held phone while driving exists in 139 countries [1]. However, the distractions of mobile phones to other road users, such as pedestrians, has not received as much research attention.
Safely negotiating traffic as a pedestrian requires a range of cognitive and perceptual skills including visual search, auditory localisation and attention. Attention in pedestrian contexts involves the ability to focus on relevant information, sustaining attention to traffic, switching attention, and ignoring distractions. Phones and other portable technology can be a distraction for pedestrians, affecting their safety. Mobile phone use was cited as an increasing cause of injuries requiring hospital treatment, especially for males and for pedestrians younger than 31 years [8]. For example, rates of injuries specifically due to pedestrians’ mobile phone use increased steadily from 0.36% in 2005 to 3.67% in 2010 [8], indicating that rates multiplied by ten in only five years. Field studies of pedestrian behaviour in several countries (e.g., Australia, Serbia, USA) have found that pedestrians who were texting or talking on a phone looked for traffic less frequently than non-users and this occurred more frequently among women than men [9,10,11,12]. Studies using an immersive laboratory road-crossing simulation found that pedestrians walked slower when talking on a mobile phone [13], paid less attention to traffic, allowed less time to cross before an oncoming vehicle and had more road traffic hits and near misses when using a mobile phone [14,15,16].
Most of the above research involved observing the behaviour of adults. Distraction may affect adolescents more than adults due to brain development. Adolescence is a period of substantial brain development that affects attention and other executive functions as well as social behaviour [17,18,19]. For example, a gradual increase in attention capacity and processing speed through adolescence [20] could affect the ability to inhibit distractions in a pedestrian environment. Given that mobile phones may be sources of distraction and adolescents are frequent users of mobile technology [21], more research on this age group of pedestrians is needed.
Mobile phone ownership is common among UK adolescents with estimates of more than 75% of 12–15-year-olds owning a phone [21,22]. Also, UK mobile phone use was found to be highest among 16–24-year-olds [23]. However, there is a scarcity of data available on whether phones are used by young people when negotiating road traffic. Field observations of pedestrians crossing the road have found varying rates of mobile phone usage. For example, researchers have observed that 8% of pedestrians they observed crossing an urban intersection were using a digital device, and 9% were using earbuds/headphones [24]. Also, 27.8% of pedestrians were observed crossing at intersections on a ‘Walk’ signal and 42% crossing on a ‘Do not Walk’ signal when wearing headphones, talking on a mobile phone or looking down at an electronic device [25]. However, neither of these studies reported the estimated age group of the pedestrians that they observed. Phones may be used by young people more often than older people in traffic. For example, cyclists aged 12–17 years have been reported to use hand-held electronic apparatus, including phones, more often than those aged 35–50 years around moving vehicles [26]. Less is known about young people’s phone use when walking among road traffic.
The aim of this research was to investigate adolescent pedestrian behaviour when using portable electronic devices, such as a mobile phone. We focused on adolescents because they are considered to engage in riskier behaviour than other age groups [18], they are regular users of mobile phones and electronic technology, they may be susceptible to distractions, and they are within the high-risk age group for road traffic injuries. The first phase of data collection aimed to ascertain how many adolescents crossed the road with a phone or another electronic device. This involved a simple frequency count. The aim of the second phase of data collection was to conduct a more detailed observation of road-crossing behaviour in relation to using a mobile phone or another device.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Site

Permission was obtained to observe school students crossing a road near their school. The crossing site was a signalised (‘Pelican’) crossing with traffic lights to stop vehicles and displayed a red or green person to indicate when the pedestrian should or should not cross (see Figure 1). The UK government Department for Transport advice for crossing a road at a signalised pedestrian crossing is in the form of a series of rules. To the best of our knowledge, it is not illegal to cross on a red (do not walk) signal, but it is illegal to loiter on any form of crossing. Traffic travelled in two directions. The road had several lanes for mixed traffic. Two lanes were for general vehicle use, one lane was reserved for public transport, such as buses and taxis, and another lane was reserved for bicycles. The school was located in a large city in the north of England. The age range of students attending the school was that of high school students in the UK, between 11 and 18 years. The most recent UK Office for Standards in Education (Ofsted) report graded the school as outstanding. It also indicated that there was a higher than average number of students from ‘financially deprived backgrounds’. The report described the school as a multicultural community. This is fairly typical of urban schools in the UK and in this respect we considered the sample to be representative of UK urban adolescents. Observations were done in the morning before school opening time and in the afternoon at school closing time over a four-week period in late spring to early summer in 2018. The majority of observations were made in dry weather conditions (sunny conditions = 42.8%, cloudy conditions = 39.6%, rain = 2.0%), and temperatures varied between 8.5 °C and 27 °C over this period.

2.2. Sample

2.2.1. Phase 1 Observation

A total of 3442 road crossings by school students were observed for two days each week for four weeks.

2.2.2. Phase 2 Observation

A total of 795 road crossings by school students were observed for two days each week for four weeks in 2018. Phase 2 data were collected on different occasions to phase 1, so in this respect, they were independent of phase 1 data. For comparison purposes, we attempted to more closely match the number of pedestrians with and without a phone. Consequently, 369 road crossings in phase 2 were made with a phone or other electronic device visible and 426 were without a device visible.

2.3. Procedure

Ethical approval was obtained from the University of Lincoln Research and Ethics Committee, and the British Psychological Society’s ethical guidelines were followed. Permission was obtained from the school to conduct the observation near school premises. Before commencing the observation, pilot studies were carried out by two observers for refining the coding categories and for calculating observer reliability analyses. The two observers agreed on 89% of road crossings. Subsequently, following further pilot studies, observations were carried out by a trained observer standing near the kerb edge facing the pedestrian.
For the first phase of data collection (phase 1), we were interested in how many adolescents crossed the road with a phone or another electronic device. For phase 1, observations involved a basic frequency count of adolescent pedestrians crossing the road. Two categories were used: (1) pedestrians carrying an electronic device (mobile phone, headphones or earplugs) visible to the observer, (2) no device visible to the observer. Observation for each pedestrian began when the pedestrian stepped off the pavement into the road and ended when they left the road and stepped onto the pavement.
As the aim for phase 2 was to observe specific pedestrian behaviours when using a mobile phone or other device, behaviours were observed as follows. Safe road-crossing behaviours observed were (1) looking left and right before crossing (indicated by head movements), (2) crossing on a green person, (3) crossing within the cross-walk and taking the shortest route, not diagonally. Technology use was categorised as (1) mobile phone or other device visible to the observer, (2) no device, (3) holding device, (4) holding device to ear, (5) texting/swiping, (6) looking at device. Pedestrian characteristics noted were gender, and whether the pedestrian crossed alone or with others. Crossing with others was defined as crossing together, looking at or talking to each other. Also, a member of the school staff stood outside the school near to the crossing point at school closing time for 15 min. Whether this adult was present, the time of day, and weather conditions were noted.

2.4. Data Analysis

We treated the data as observations of independent crossing events rather than observations of individual pedestrians. For phase 1 data, frequencies and percentages were calculated to ascertain the amount of road crossings with or without a mobile device. For phase 2 data, chi-square tests were used to analyse associations between technology use and road crossing behaviours [27]. To avoid Type 1 errors, Bonferroni corrections were applied to p values where multiple chi-square tests were calculated (Table 1, Table 2 and Table 3).

3. Results

3.1. Phase 1 Observation

A total of 1080 pedestrian road crossings were made with an electronic device visible to the observer (31.37%) and 2362 without an electronic device (68.62%).

3.2. Phase 2 Observation

Out of the total of 795 pedestrians observed crossing in phase 2, 378 (47.55%) were male pedestrians and 417 (52.45%) were female pedestrians, and 65.40% were observed in the morning between 8:00 and 8:45 am and 34.6% were observed between 3:05 and 3:40 pm. Approximately half of the road crossings were made by pedestrians who looked left and right before crossing (51.40%), 65.3% by pedestrians who crossed on the ‘green person’ and 57.6% by those who walked straight across the road within the cross-walk. Also, 369 (46.41%) road crossings observed in phase 2 were made with a phone or other electronic device visible and 426 (53.58%) were without a device visible. More females than males were observed with a device (female n = 242, 58.03% of female pedestrian road crossings; male n = 127, 33.59% of male pedestrian road crossings), holding a device (female n = 172, 41.25%; male n = 67, 17.72%), texting/swiping (female n = 37, 8.87%; male n = 20, 5.29%) and using headphones (female n = 138, 33.09%; male n = 84, 22.22%). More females than males failed to look left and right before crossing (female n = 217, 52.04%; male n = 169, 44.71%). Table 1, Table 2, Table 3, Table 4 and Table 5 present the data from the phase 2 observations.

3.2.1. Effects of Electronic Devices on Pedestrian Behaviour

As can be seen in Table 1, road crossings made by pedestrians with a phone or another device more frequently involved failing to look left and right before crossing than those without a device. Also, those holding a phone or engaging with the device by looking at the screen or texting/swiping were subsequently less likely to look left and right before crossing. However, other behaviours with a mobile phone such as speaking and/or listening (either by holding the phone to the ear or using headphones) did not affect looking left and right. Also, although road crossings made by females were less likely to involve looking left and right before crossing, there were no statistically significant gender differences with regards to the association between mobile phone-use behaviour and looking behaviour.
Table 2 shows that the majority of pedestrians crossed on a green (walk) signal; however, for those who crossed on a red (do not walk) signal, most did not have a mobile phone or other device with them, were not holding, speaking or looking at the screen. All other associations were nonsignificant. There was no statistically significant association between crossing within the cross-walk and phone/device use (Table 3).

3.2.2. Effects of Other People and Time of Day on Road-Crossing Behaviours

Compared to when crossing the road with peers, those crossing the road alone were more frequently observed looking left and right before crossing, crossing on a green signal and walking straight across the road within the cross-walk. However, only looking left and right was approaching significance (p = 0.053). Differences for crossing on green and crossing by the shortest route were statistically nonsignificant (see Table 4). The presence of an adult school staff member in the afternoons did not affect looking behaviour or crossing within the cross-walk, but significantly increased the frequency of crossings on a green (walk) signal (see Table 4).
Pedestrians crossing the road in the morning more frequently looked in both directions before crossing than those crossing in the afternoon after school who looked less frequently (p = 0.007). Although the majority of road crossings were with pedestrians crossing on the green person signal, a higher proportion of road crossings were made during the red person signal in the morning (39.62% crossed on red, 60.38% on green) than in the afternoon (25.5% crossed on red, 74.5% on green) (p < 0.001). Also, although the majority walked within the cross-walk, a higher proportion did so in the afternoon (81.03% within the crosswalk, 19.07% outside) than in the morning (61.08% within, 38.02% outside). The morning period was divided into three time periods according to whether it was comfortably before expected school arrival time (before 8:15 am), during the peak arrival time (8:15–8:30 am), or when the school gates were about to be closed and the student would be late for school (after 8:30 am). We expected that pedestrians would take more risks when they were about to be late for school. There was a relatively higher frequency of failing to look left and right before crossing after 8:30 am, although this did not reach statistical significance. This did not occur at the earlier or peak arrival times (see Table 5). The majority of road crossings were made by pedestrians who crossed on green at all three time periods. Significantly more road crossings were outside the cross-walk than inside after 8:30 a.m. This did not occur at the earlier arrival times.

4. Discussion

Almost a third of road crossings by school students on the route to and from school were made while using a mobile phone or music player. They were observed holding and interacting with these devices in a variety of ways while crossing, including speaking, texting or swiping, as well as listening. Regardless of whether a phone or device was in use, unsafe pedestrian behaviours were frequently observed. Mobile phones and other portable digital devices distracted adolescents’ looking behaviour at the roadside, especially when visual attention to the device was required. This supports previous observations of adults and young people [12], as well as the results of pedestrian simulator studies [28]. The detrimental effects of visual distractions (looking, texting and swiping behaviours) compared to auditory distractions (listening, using headphones) supports the results of previous research on visual and auditory distractions among young people [29]. For example, [29] found that adolescents were more sensitive to distractions than adults and that stimuli were more distracting for adolescents when in the same modality as the task. Similarly, in our study, visual attention to the mobile phone affected looking left and right, but listening behaviour did not.
When compared to previous research [24], our results indicated that adolescents used mobile technology more frequently than adults while interacting with road traffic. Compared to 31.37% of adolescents observed in our study, [24] observed 17% of pedestrians in total using portable technology while crossing a road. The rates of unsafe behaviours while using phones and other electronic devices were similar to previous studies. For example, [25] reported that 42% of pedestrians they observed crossing at intersections on a ‘Do not Walk’ signal were wearing headphones, talking on a mobile phone or looking down at an electronic device. In our study, 35.87% of pedestrians crossing on a ‘do not walk’ signal were observed with a phone or other device. Also, the majority of road crossings were on a green (walk) signal. However, most road crossings on a red (do not walk) signal did not involve the pedestrian having or using a mobile phone. Research with adult pedestrians has found similar results for crossing on a ‘walk’ signal. For example, [11] noted that most pedestrians obeyed the lights, and that distracting behaviours, including texting and using a phone, were not associated with obeying lights. It is not clear why safer behaviour is associated with mobile phones for this behaviour category; consequently, further research is needed to investigate possible reasons. Perhaps pedestrians tend to cross on red due to impatience when waiting for the light to change. If this is the case, then having a phone on hand may give the pedestrian something to do while waiting and, thus, reduce impatient behaviour.
Gender differences have been reported in young pedestrians’ injury rates and unsafe behaviour [8,30] with boys having higher injury rates than girls. Consequently, we were interested in exploring gender differences in phone use and related pedestrian behaviours. Our results found that rates of electronic device use were proportionately higher for female than male pedestrians, with 58.03% of female pedestrians observed with electronic devices compared to 33.59% of males. However, the rates of unsafe pedestrian behaviour in relation to technology use were similar for males and females. This does not support the gender differences noted in field studies of adult pedestrian behaviour [9,10,11,12].
The effect of the presence of other people at the roadside depended on who the other person was. The presence of an adult school staff member did not affect looking behaviour or crossing within the cross-walk but did increase the frequency of crossings on a green (walk) signal. Although our findings for the presence of other adolescents did not reach statistical significance, those crossing the road alone were more frequently observed looking left and right before crossing than those crossing with peers. According to Steinberg’s dual processing model of adolescent risk taking [19], adolescents would be expected to display more unsafe behaviour when interacting with peers. As peers can have a positive as well as a negative effect on adolescent risk taking [31], more detailed observations of the type of peer interactions are necessary.
Time of day affected safe pedestrian behaviour. Looking left and right was proportionately more frequent in the morning on the way to school and failing to look was more frequent in the afternoon on the way home from school. This reduction in careful looking behaviour might be due to the effect of tiredness at the end of the school day. The role of tiredness in rates of unintentional injuries among adolescents has been noted by previous researchers in several countries, such as China, Korea and the USA [32,33,34].
However, some of the after-school crossings were safer than morning crossings. For example, although the majority of pedestrians crossed on the green person signal, a higher proportion of road crossings were made during the red person signal in the morning than in the afternoon. Also, although the majority walked within the cross-walk, a higher proportion did so in the afternoon than in the morning. In this respect, our results support previous research of more unsafe behaviours by Chinese urban child pedestrians in the morning than the afternoon [35]. However, our results may have been affected by the presence of school staff members for part of the time in the afternoon. For example, students might be more likely to comply with crossing on green and crossing within the cross-walk in the presence of school staff.
We observed another time of day effect in the mornings. Those who were late to school in the morning (after 8:30 am) crossed within the cross-walk significantly less frequently and they less often looked left and right before crossing the road. This might be related to inattention to safety concerns while hurrying to get to school.
Limitations of the study were that observations were conducted at only one type of pedestrian crossing site at one school location. Also, the road had a slight curve, which may have affected pedestrian behaviour. Further research is needed on adolescent pedestrian behaviour and mobile phone distractions at other types of sites, particularly those with no traffic controls. More observations at different times of day would be beneficial, particularly in the afternoons when students are more likely to be exiting school en masse. As we did not film these young pedestrians for ethical reasons, we made fewer observations in the afternoons because the majority of students left the school premises at the same time making live observations more difficult. In contrast, arrival times were more staggered in the mornings. Also, observations were overt, and the researcher was clearly in view of the pedestrians, which could have affected their behaviour.
A further limitation resulting from the restrictions on filming young pedestrians is that it is possible that the same pedestrian was observed on more than one occasion. Consequently, our data should be interpreted as observations of crossing events rather than the behaviour of individual pedestrians. Although fairly detailed information was obtained about how the adolescent was using the phone or another electronic device, information about safe and unsafe pedestrian behaviour was limited to three categories of behaviour. Other behaviours that could be added to the observation would be waiting time and whether the pedestrian stopped at the kerb before crossing. As the school had a high proportion of students from low-income families, it is possible that our results represent a conservative estimate of adolescent pedestrian phone usage.
In conclusion, adolescent pedestrians are frequent users of mobile phones and music players when crossing the road and this affects their looking behaviour.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, A.B., L.A., P.G. and K.P.; methodology, A.B., L.A., P.G. and K.P.; formal analysis, A.B. and K.P.; investigation, A.B. and K.P.; writing—original draft preparation, A.B. and K.P.; writing—review and editing, L.A. and P.G.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. World Health Organization. Global Status Report on Road Safety; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  2. Department for Transport. Reported Road Casualties in Great Britain: Main Results 2014. Department for Transport: London, UK, 2015. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/463045/rrcgb2014-01.pdf (accessed on 5 March 2019).
  3. Department for Transport. Table RAS30002. Reported Casualties by Road User Type, Age and Severity: Great Britain. Department for Transport: London, UK, 2017. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/755690/ras30002.ods (accessed on 5 March 2019).
  4. Department for Transport. Table RAS50004. Reported Accidents Involving Pedestrians with Contributory Factors, Great Britain, Latest Available Year. Department for Transport: London, 2017. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/743098/ras50004.ods (accessed on 5 March 2019).
  5. Caird, J.K.; Willness, C.R.; Steel, P.; Scialfa, C. A meta-analysis of the effects of cell phones on driver performance. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2008, 40, 1282–1293. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  6. Haigney, D.; Westerman, S.J. Mobile (cellular) phone use and driving: A critical review of research methodology. Ergonomics 2001, 44, 132–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. McCartt, A.T.; Hellinga, L.A.; Bratiman, K.A. Cell Phones and Driving: Review of Research. J. Traffic Inj. Prev. 2006, 7, 89–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Nasar, J.L.; Troyer, D. Pedestrian injuries due to mobile phone use in public places. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2013, 57, 91–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Hatfield, J.; Murphy, S. The Effects of Mobile Phone Use on Pedestrian Crossing Behaviour at signalised and unsignalised intersections. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2007, 39, 197–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Pešić, D.; Antić, B.; Glavić, D.; Milenković, M. The effects of mobile phone use on pedestrian crossing behaviour at unsignalized intersections–Models for predicting unsafe pedestrian behaviour. Saf. Sci. 2016, 82, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Thompson, L.L.; Rivara, F.P.; Ayyagari, R.C.; Ebel, B.E. Impact of social and technological distraction on pedestrian crossing behaviour: An observational study. Inj. Prev. 2013, 19, 232–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Wells, H.L.; McClure, L.A.; Porter, B.E.; Schwebel, D.C. Distracted pedestrian behavior on two urban college campuses. J. Community Health 2018, 43, 96–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Neider, M.B.; Gaspar, J.G.; McCarley, J.S.; Crowell, J.A.; Kaczmarski, H.; Kramer, A.F. Walking and talking: Dual-task effects on street crossing behavior in older adults. Psychol. Aging 2011, 26, 260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Stavrinos, D.; Byington, K.W.; Schwebel, D.C. Effect of cell phone distraction on pediatric pedestrian injury risk. Pediatrics 2009, 123, e179–e185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Stavrinos, D.; Byington, K.W.; Schwebel, D.C. Distracted walking: Cell phones increase injury risk for college pedestrians. J. Saf. Res. 2011, 42, 101–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Stavrinos, D.; Pope, C.N.; Shen, J.; Schwebel, D.C. Distracted walking, bicycling, and driving: Systematic review and meta-analysis of mobile technology and youth crash risk. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 118–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Blakemore, S.J.; Choudhury, S. Development of the adolescent brain: Implications for executive function and social cognition. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2006, 47, 296–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Steinberg, L. Risk taking in adolescence: New perspectives from brain and behavioral science. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2007, 16, 55–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Steinberg, L. A dual systems model of adolescent risk-taking. Dev. Psychobiol. 2010, 52, 216–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Anderson, V.A.; Anderson, P.; Northam, E.; Jacobs, R.; Catroppa, C. Development of executive functions through late childhood and adolescence in an Australian sample. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2001, 20, 385–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Mireku, M.O.; Mueller, W.; Fleming, C.; Chang, I.; Dumontheil, I.; Thomas, M.S.C.; Röösli, M.; Toledano, M.B. Total recall in the SCAMP cohort: Validation of self-reported mobile phone use in the smartphone era. Environ. Res. 2018, 161, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Ofcom. Children and Parents: Media Use and Attitudes Report. 2015. Available online: http://stakeholders.ofcom.org.uk/binaries/research/media-literacy/children-parents-nov-15/childrens_parents_nov2015.pdf (accessed on 5 March 2019).
  23. Ofcom. Adults’ Media Use and Attitudes Report. April 2018. Available online: https://www.ofcom.org.uk/__data/assets/pdf_file/0011/113222/Adults-Media-Use-and-Attitudes-Report-2018.pdf (accessed on 4 March 2019).
  24. Violano, P.; Roney, L.; Bechtel, K. The incidence of pedestrian distraction at urban intersections after implementation of a Streets Smarts campaign. Inj. Epidemiol. 2015, 2, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Basch, C.H.; Ethan, D.; Zybert, P.; Basch, C.E. Pedestrian behavior at five dangerous and busy Manhattan intersections. J. Community Health 2015, 40, 789–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Goldenbeld, C.; Houtenbos, M.; Ehlers, E.; De Waard, D. The use and risk of portable electronic devices while cycling among different age groups. J. Saf. Res. 2012, 43, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Baswail, A.; Allinson, L.; Goddard, P.; Pfeffer, K. Adolescents’ Road-Crossing Behaviour and Mobile Phone Use (Dataset). 2019. Available online: http://[email protected] (accessed on 8 April 2019).
  28. Tapiro, H.; Oron-Gilad, T.; Parmet, Y. Cell phone conversations and child pedestrian’s crossing behavior; a simulator study. Saf. Sci. 2016, 89, 36–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Downing, H.C.; Barutchu, A.; Crewther, S.G. Developmental trends in the facilitation of multisensory objects with distractors. Front. Psychol. 2015, 5, 1559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Peden, M.; Scurfield, R.; Sleet, D.; Mohan, D.; Hyder, A.A.; Jarawan, E.; Mathers, C. World Report on Child Injury Prevention; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  31. Pfeffer, K.; Hunter, E. The effects of peer influence on adolescent pedestrian road-crossing decisions. Traffic Inj. Prev. 2013, 14, 434–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Lam, L.T.; Yang, L. Short duration of sleep and unintentional injuries among adolescents in China. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2007, 166, 1053–1058. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Kim, S.Y.; Sim, S.; Kim, S.G.; Choi, H.G. Sleep deprivation is associated with bicycle accidents and slip and fall injuries in Korean adolescents. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0135753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Wheaton, A.G.; Olsen, E.O.; Miller, G.F.; Croft, J.B. Sleep duration and injury-related risk behaviors among high school students—United States, 2007–2013. MMWR Morb. Mortal. Wkly. Rep. 2016, 65, 337–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Schwebel, D.C.; Wu, Y.; Swanson, M.; Cheng, P.; Ning, P.; Cheng, X.; Hu, G. Child pedestrian street-crossing behaviors outside a primary school: Developing observational methodologies and data from a case study in Changsha, China. J. Transp. Health 2018, 8, 283–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Views of the road crossing site.
Figure 1. Views of the road crossing site.
Safety 05 00027 g001
Table 1. Use of electronic devices in relation to road-crossing behaviours—looked before crossing (number of road crossings).
Table 1. Use of electronic devices in relation to road-crossing behaviours—looked before crossing (number of road crossings).
Phone/Device UseLooked Left and Right before CrossingDid Not Look Left and Right before Crossingχ2
Phone/other device visible 16520412.490 *
No phone or other device 244182
Holding phone 9214722.953 *
Not holding phone317239
Speaking into phone 12 160.857
Not speaking into phone397370
Looking at screen 26 488.691 *
Not looking at screen383338
Texting/swiping  19 388.065 *
Not texting/ swiping390348
Holding device to ear  7 196.471
Not holding device to ear402367
Head/ear phones1101120.444
No head/ear phones299274
Table 2. Use of electronic devices in relation to road-crossing behaviours—walk signal (number of road crossings).
Table 2. Use of electronic devices in relation to road-crossing behaviours—walk signal (number of road crossings).
Phone/Device UseCrossed on Green (Walk Signal)Crossed on Red (Do Not Walk Signal)χ2
Phone/other device visible 270 9918.904 *
No phone or other device 249177
Holding phone179 6013.932 *
Not holding phone340216
Speaking into phone 25  37.377 *
Not speaking into phone494273
Looking at screen 60 148.985 *
Not looking at screen459262
Texting/swiping  45 125.058
Not texting/ swiping474264
Holding device to ear 21  52.844
Not holding device to ear498271
Head/ear phones158 644.712
No head/ear phones361212
Table 3. Use of electronic devices in relation to road-crossing behaviours—cross-walk (number of road crossings).
Table 3. Use of electronic devices in relation to road-crossing behaviours—cross-walk (number of road crossings).
Phone/Device UseCross within the Cross-Walk Crossed outside the Cross-Walk χ2
Phone/other device visible 2181510.608
No phone or other device 240186
Holding phone1361030.070
Not holding phone322234
Speaking into phone 18 100.530
Not speaking into phone440327
Looking at screen 46 280.692
Not looking at screen412309
Texting/swiping  36 210.774
Not texting/ swiping422316
Holding device to ear 14 120.156
Not holding device to ear444325
Head/ear phones137 852.122
No head/ear phones321252
For Table 1, Table 2 and Table 3, Bonferroni corrections were applied (p = 0.05 / 7 = 0.007) due to multiple phone-use comparisons (7) made for each type of road-crossing behaviour, and * indicates significance at p = 0.007.
Table 4. Effects of other people on road-crossing behaviours (number of road crossings).
Table 4. Effects of other people on road-crossing behaviours (number of road crossings).
Looked Left and Right before CrossingDid Not Look Left and Right before Crossingχ2Crossed on Green (Walk Signal)Crossed on Red (Do Not Walk Signal)χ2Crossed within the Cross-Walk Crossed outside the Cross-Walk χ2
Student alone3142733.7593762111.4943322541.113
Student with peers 95113 14365 12681
School staff present ** 63770.444110304.167 *71690.043
No school staff present ** 3957 6432 5046
* p < 0.05, ** pm only.
Table 5. Safe behaviour associated with time of day (number of road crossings) * p < 0.01.
Table 5. Safe behaviour associated with time of day (number of road crossings) * p < 0.01.
Time of DayLooked Left and Right before CrossingDid Not Look Left and Right before Crossingχ2Crossed on Green (Walk Signal)Crossed on Red (Do Not Walk Signal)χ2Crossed within the Cross-Walk Crossed outside the Cross-Walk χ2
Going to school (am) 2862347.599 *31420615.915 *3172036.914 *
Return home (pm)123 152 20570 141134
Going to school before 8:1538372.94347 283.23357189.225 *
Going to school 8:15 to 8:30224164 227161 224164
Going to school after 8:302349 3514 3249
* p < 0.01.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Baswail, A.; Allinson, L.; Goddard, P.; Pfeffer, K. Adolescents’ Mobile Phone Use While Crossing the Road. Safety 2019, 5, 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/safety5020027

AMA Style

Baswail A, Allinson L, Goddard P, Pfeffer K. Adolescents’ Mobile Phone Use While Crossing the Road. Safety. 2019; 5(2):27. https://doi.org/10.3390/safety5020027

Chicago/Turabian Style

Baswail, Amal, Lesley Allinson, Paul Goddard, and Karen Pfeffer. 2019. "Adolescents’ Mobile Phone Use While Crossing the Road" Safety 5, no. 2: 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/safety5020027

APA Style

Baswail, A., Allinson, L., Goddard, P., & Pfeffer, K. (2019). Adolescents’ Mobile Phone Use While Crossing the Road. Safety, 5(2), 27. https://doi.org/10.3390/safety5020027

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop