Next Article in Journal
Changes in Polyphenolics during Storage of Products Prepared with Freeze-Dried Wild Blueberry Powder
Next Article in Special Issue
The Effect of an Evaluative Label on Consumer Perception of Cheeses in Hungary
Previous Article in Journal
Gas Chromatography–Mass Spectrometry-Based Metabolite Profiling for the Assessment of Freshness in Gilthead Sea Bream (Sparus aurata)
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Snacking Chameleon: Psychological Proximity Increases Imitation of Food Intake Independently of Brand Choice
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Assessing the Influence of Visual-Taste Congruency on Perceived Sweetness and Product Liking in Immersive VR

1
Department of Food Science, University of Copenhagen, Rolighedsvej 26, 1958 Federiksberg C, Denmark
2
School of Design, Royal College of Art, Kensington Gore, London SW7 2EU, UK
3
Department of Psychology, University of Copenhagen, Oester Farimagsgade 2A, 1353 Copenhagen K, Denmark
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Foods 2020, 9(4), 465; https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040465
Submission received: 13 March 2020 / Revised: 27 March 2020 / Accepted: 31 March 2020 / Published: 9 April 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue How Visuals Affect Food Choice?)

Abstract

:
This study was designed to assess whether the combined effect of taste-congruent and incongruent extrinsic visual cues presented in virtual reality (VR) influences the perception of sweetness and product liking. Three VR environments (sweet-congruent, sweet-incongruent, and neutral) were created based on the evidence in existing literature. Participants tasted the same beverage in three VR environments and evaluated the environment and beverage liking, as well as perceived taste intensity (sweetness, sourness, and bitterness), congruency, comfort, and environment vividness. Frontal EEG alpha asymmetry (FAA) was also recorded as a complementary physiological measurement of overall liking. The results showed that the perceived sweetness of the beverage was significantly elevated in a sweet-congruent environment versus the other environments. Visual-taste congruency did not seem to have an effect on beverage liking and overall liking, whereas an increase in environment liking was found in the incongruent environment versus the other environments. These findings confirmed the significant influence of taste-specific visual cues on flavour perception, while the successful use of VR in the study provided insight into future applications of taste-specific VR environment in the modulation of flavour perception and sugar reduction.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Our senses are continuously stimulated while we eat, and our brain filters and organises sensory information into an ultimate perception of flavour. Vision, as one of the five basic senses, which also contributes to the formation of the final flavour perception [1]. A growing body of research shows that visual cues, both intrinsic and extrinsic, are able to influence acceptance and perceived flavour [2,3,4,5,6]. At the same time, congruency of different sensory inputs has also been shown to increase our recognition speed [7], as well as product liking [8,9,10,11,12,13,14]. In this study, using both subjective and physiological measurements, we focus on the influence of combined visual cues on the perception of sweetness, as well as product liking.

1.1. Influence of Visual Cues on Perception of Sweetness

Visual factors such as shape, colour, and visual texture have been shown to affect perceived intensity of sweetness. Roundness is usually associated with sweetness while angular shapes are associated with bitterness, saltiness, and sourness [3,5,15,16,17,18]. For instance, round plates and food representation have increased sweetness ratings by 17% [3] while round-shaped chocolates are rated 30% sweeter than angular-shaped chocolates [5]. Similar associations were also found for packaging typeface [19], packaging shape [20], and face shape [21]. Apart from shape, colour also has a role in sweetness perception. Intrinsic colour cues such as red/pink colouring have been found to increase the intensity of sweetness while black is linked to bitterness [4,22,23]. Similar effects have been observed with extrinsic colour cues such as the colour of the container [2] and colours in a dining environment [24]. Such an effect is coherent with evidence on people who systematically associate colours with specific tastes [17,25,26,27].
However, some studies posit that it is the contrast between food colour and background colour that changes perceived sweetness [28,29,30]. For example, a pink strawberry mousse was rated 15% sweeter when placed on a white plate than when placed on a black plate [29]. Moreover, visual texture also plays a role in taste perception [31], and surface texture has long been shown to influence taste and hedonic evaluation [32,33,34,35]. Biscuits have been rated sweeter when served on a smooth and shiny plate than when served on a rough and grainy plate [33]. Furthermore, angular surface patterns have enhanced perceived bitterness as compared with round surface patterns [32]. Although evidence regarding the influence of visual texture on flavour is relatively scarce, it could potentially be similar to the evidence from the aforementioned studies on the influence of tactile feedback on flavour perception. In the current study, we adapted evidence gathered in the literature to investigate how combined visual cues in VR environments influence perceived intensity of sweetness, as well as beverage liking.

1.2. Application of VR in Sensory and Consumer Research

With advances in immersive technologies such as mixed VR, augmented reality (AR), and mixed reality, increasing numbers of researchers have started to explore applications of these technologies in sensory and consumer studies [36,37,38,39]. Although conducting studies in natural and real-life settings tends to increase test validity and consumer engagement, it can be tricky and costly [40,41,42]. VR is able to create a sense of immersion that evokes feelings of presence and involvement which in turn make VR a cost-effective alternative to conducting studies in real-life settings [43]. Additionally, contextual factors can be more easily standardised in these immersive environments as compared to natural settings [44,45]. Beyond such applications in research, immersive technology is developing at an extraordinary speed in its commercial use. Total spending on AR and VR products and services will rocket, according to a compound annual growth rate of 113.2% projected for the period from 2017 to 2021 [46]. This rapid growth of immersive technology means that research insights acquired today can be readily adapted and utilised in people’s homes and businesses to empower consumer experiences and to change behaviours. For these reasons, we chose VR as a medium capable of presenting all visual cues to the participants in our experiment.

1.3. Objective Biometric Measurement of Liking

Conventional sensory and consumer trials usually rely on subjective self-reported responses and the measurement of product liking (i.e., hedonic rating) is mainly based on intrinsically individual differences in perception and liking [47]. It is helpful to use a second type of measurement to provide further evidence. Nowadays, there is a growing interest in applying biometric measurements such as eye tracking, electroencephalography (EEG), and facial recognition in sensory and consumer studies, therefore, adding an objective complement to measures of sensory perception [39,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56]. Among these biometric instruments, EEG is a common non-invasive neuroimaging technique that records electrical activity along the scalp. It records voltage fluctuations caused by ionic current flow among neurons, reflecting brain responses during different tasks [57]. The EEG signal spectral power difference in the alpha frequency band (8 to 13 Hz) between the left and right prefrontal cortex, also known as frontal alpha asymmetry (FAA), is a neurophysiological correlate to the positive effect [58,59]. According to the approach-withdrawal model [60], right-frontal cortical regions facilitate withdrawal motivation, while the left-frontal cortical regions mediate approach motivation [61,62]. This means that individuals with greater right-frontal cortical activation tend to exhibit withdrawal-related behaviour while individuals with greater left frontal cortical activation exhibit approach-related behaviour [59,61]. Therefore, it is possible to use the asymmetry index as an indicator of the appreciation/avoidance tendency. In sensory and consumer studies, FAA was used to study perceived pleasantness towards sensory stimulus [63,64]. In this study, we recorded EEG data for participants drinking a sweet beverage in ”sweet”, ”bitter”, and ”neutral” VR environments. We compared the FAA at electrodes F3 and F4 during tastings in three different VR environments to assess individuals’ affinity complementary to self-reported hedonic ratings obtained in a survey.
The main focus of this study is to investigate individual perception of sweetness in virtual environments simulating visual-taste congruency and incongruency. The first aim of the study is to assess whether a visual-taste congruent environment can lead to increased perceived intensity in sweetness. The second aim is to investigate whether congruent environments increase product liking. On the basis of previous studies, our hypothesis was that perceived intensity of sweetness, as well as beverage liking, would be greater in a sweet-congruent environment than in an incongruent or neutral environment.

2. Materials and Methods

A total of 41 participants (17 males and 24 females, mean age 27.73 ± 2.12 years) were recruited using both direct and online recruitment procedures in Denmark. The selection criteria were as follows: right-handed healthy individuals with normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders and no synaesthesia. The current study adhered strictly to the Declaration of Helsinki and ethical approval was obtained from the local ethics committee (at University of Copenhagen). All individuals gave written informed consent and a six-digit identification code was assigned to each individual. Data collection was handled according to the General Data Protection Regulation (GDPR).

2.1. Experimental Procedures

The duration of the experiment was about 1 hour in each case; all participants were tested individually (Figure 1). Each was asked to read and sign a consent form immediately upon arrival, and then the researcher briefly explained the experimental procedures, potential risks, and individual rights. They were told that the study investigated consumer preference for three types of berry-flavoured beverages with similar ingredient compositions. Next, participants were (after scalp cleaning procedures) outfitted with an EEG headset. Optimisation of data recording quality was done before a palate cleanse using water to remove any taste residues. Afterwards, participants entered the first VR environment and had the opportunity to freely explore the environment for 150 s (exploration phase). This was followed by a tasting phase, where participants drank one of the test beverages via a plastic straw while being immersed in the VR environment for 150 s and their brain signals were recorded (Figure S1). During the tasting phase, subjects were instructed to sit calmly and avoid unnecessary movements to prevent movement artefacts. A verbal reminder was given by the researcher 30 s before the end of the tasting session. After removing the VR headset, participants filled in a five minute questionnaire regarding the test beverage and environment using a pen while a palate cleanse was performed (Survey a). Altogether, the exploration phase, the tasting phase and the questionnaire lasted for 10 min. Next, the 10 min stimulus and test survey phase were repeated twice in a different environment each time. In total, three VR environments were presented to the participants, in a random order generated by an algorithm. At the end of this process, each participant completed a short questionnaire regarding demographic information, self-reported health state, sweetness liking and preference, as well as VR familiarity (Survey b). Each participant, upon completion, was compensated with a gift set, valued at approximately 100 Danish crowns.

2.2. Stimulus

2.2.1. Taste Stimuli and Palate Cleanser

All taste stimuli were the same in this study. In each VR environment, participants received 100 mL of test beverage contained in a white plastic cup and consumed the beverage using a plastic straw. Participants were asked to finish drinking the test beverage (100 mL) if possible. They were also given the option to recap the taste of the test beverage before the end of the tasting session. Concentrated grenadine syrup (SAS, Teisseire, France) was diluted with water at a 1:7 ratio based on the producer’s recommendation and a pilot tasting session. Table S1 shows the nutritional content of the taste stimulus. The test beverage was freshly made for each test day under hygienic conditions and mixed thoroughly before administration. The 100 mL amounts, in their plastic cups, were placed in the test room one hour prior to the experiment and the room temperature was kept constant at 22 °C to ensure maximum taste sensitivity in the 20 to 30 °C temperature range [65,66]. Palate cleansing was performed to remove any residue before and between taste samples to minimise carryover effects [67,68,69]. Water, one of the most effective palate cleansers for sweetness [70], was used in this study. Participants rinsed their mouths with water before each tasting in order to re-establish a baseline oral environment.

2.2.2. Visual Stimuli

Three VR environments were used as visual stimuli. According to the existing literature, sweet-congruent visual cues such as round shapes, pink/red-shaded colours, and soft/smooth visual textures were incorporated into the design of the sweet environment. Meanwhile, bitter-congruent visual cues such as angular shapes, black/grey-shaded colours, and rough/grainy textures were incorporated into the incongruent/bitter environment, and the neutral environment was designed based on the test room itself, with a plain white background (Figure 2). Models were built using Rhinoceros three-dimensional (3D) software (Rhino 6.0) and rendered using Unity software (Unity, version 2019.2.13). With slight modifications, all three environments were designed based on the size of the test room (length = 3.6 m, width = 2.7 m, and height = 2.55 m), while the furniture and decorations in each environment were also adjusted for similarity in size, scale, and spatial arrangement. Android application packages (APK) of the VR environments were made freely available through three QR codes (Figure S2). Participants experienced the VR contexts through a Samsung Gear VR headset powered by Samsung Galaxy S7 smartphone. All three VR environmental designs were verified through a pilot questionnaire to ensure visual-taste association.

2.3. Questionnaires

2.3.1. Pilot Questionnaires

The taste association of all the visual stimuli was verified via an online questionnaire (Survey monkey) with a total number of 138 participants (69 males and 69 females, mean age 34.20 ± 13.82 years). Participants were asked to associate three designed environments (in two-dimensional (2D) format) with sweetness, bitterness, sourness, and saltiness. They also rated the liking of three environments using a nine-point hedonic scale.

2.3.2. Experiment Questionnaires

Two questionnaires were given to the participants during the experiment. The first questionnaire (for survey a) was given immediately following every tasting session, using a nine-point scale with −4 = “not at all”, 0 = “neutral”, and +4 = “extremely”. We measured the levels of comfort, taste intensity (sweetness, sourness and bitterness), beverage and environment liking, visual-taste congruence, and environment vividness (Table S4). The second questionnaire (for Survey b) was given at the end of the experiment, capturing demographic information, self-reported health state, exercise frequency, sweet food consumption frequency, sweet food liking, and VR familiarity (Table S5). Self-reported states of health, along with frequencies for exercise and sweet food consumption, were registered on a five-point scale, whereas sweet food liking and VR familiarity were measured on a seven-point scale.

2.4. EEG Recording

Neurophysiological data was acquired using a wireless nine-channel headset (ABM B-Alert X10) with a sampling rate of 256 Hz and electrodes positioned (in reference to both left and right mastoids) at POz, Fz, Cz, F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, and P4. The headset was connected via Bluetooth to a Windows 10 computer. Data collection was carried out using the iMotion research software platform. Channel impedance was checked throughout the experiment (X-Series Basic, version 3.2.5.0) to ensure good impedance, and only those individual experiments which yielded valid data for electrodes F3 and F4 were accepted as input for this study. Three sets of data were excluded from the study due to bad signal-to-noise ratio.
Offline data processing was performed using a Matlab EEGLAB toolbox [71]. Baseline correction was conducted throughout the recordings to reduce potential electrode drift. Excessive noises, with amplitudes below or above 150 μV, were excluded. Independent component analysis (ICA) was performed to remove ocular artefacts using the EEGLAB toolbox. FAA was assessed based on published recommendations [61,72]. In brief, power values in the alpha range (8–13 Hz) were extracted from the F3 and F4 channels. An asymmetry score was calculated by subtracting the F4 values from the F3 values and dividing by the sum for the two electrodes (formula below). One thing to be noted is that there is an inverse relationship between alpha power and cortical activity, which means decreased alpha power reflects increased regional cortical activity [72]. Therefore, a more negative FAA value would indicate higher activation of the left frontal regions (greater liking).
log ( F 3 ) log ( F 4 ) log ( F 3 ) + log ( F 4 )

2.5. Data Analysis

We used SPSS (version 24, IBM Corp, Armank, NY, USA) for statistical analysis. A one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was followed by a post hoc by Tukey’s test, conducted to assess differences in perceived taste (sweetness, bitterness, and sourness), beverage and environment liking, visual-taste congruency, comfort, and environment vividness, as well as the difference in FAA index (objective overall liking measure) in three different settings. Pearson correlation coefficients were used to assess interrelations for congruency and liking (beverage, environment, and overall liking). Gender difference in all the liking attributes was interpreted using the Mann–Whitney U test. All p-values below 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

3. Results

3.1. Visual-Taste Association and Liking of Three VR Environments (Pilot Survey)

The association between tastes and designed environment and liking was investigated in a pilot survey (n = 138). The sweet environment was 81.2% associated with sweetness while the bitter environment was mainly associated with bitterness (66.0%) and least associated with sweetness (5.1%). The neutral environment had a relatively balanced association with four different tastes (Figure 3). This result confirms the designed VR environments as sweet-congruent, bitter-congruent, and neutral. The average liking of sweet, bitter, and neutral environments was 4.8 (SD = 1.7), 4.4 (SD = 2.1), and 4.4 (SD = 1.5), respectively (Table S3). No significant difference was observed.

3.2. Demographic Characteristics and Preferences

Demographic characteristics and preferences of participants are shown in Table S2. The average familiarity with VR headsets was 4.7/1.3 (mean/SD), corresponding to “familiarity”. The male participants were found to be more familiar with VR headsets than the females (p < 0.05). The average self-rated health status was “good” (4.4/0.5, mean/SD) while the average exercise frequency was “a few times per week” (3.7/0.8, mean/SD). Average consumption frequency of sweet food/beverage was “a few times per week” (3.5/0.9, mean/SD) while the average liking of sweet foods/beverages was “like slightly” (4.7/1.2, mean/SD).

3.3. Visual-Taste Congruency on Perceived Taste

Figure 4 illustrates the rating of visual-taste congruency, as well as the influence of each visual environment (sweet, bitter, and neutral) on sweetness intensity. The rating of congruency was the highest in the sweet VR environment (1.9/0.3, mean/SEM) while that in the bitter environment was the lowest (−0.83/0.4, mean/SEM). The rating for sweetness was the greatest in the sweet-congruent environment (2.4/0.2, mean/SEM) while it was the lowest in the incongruent environment (1.3/0.2, mean/SEM). ANOVA analysis also revealed significant effects of the visual environment on the intensity of perceived sweetness (F (2, 120) = 9.391, p = 0.0002) with an effect size (η2 = 0.135) classified as large (Table 1) [60]. Consistent with the above results, the post hoc Tukey’s test on congruency and perceived sweetness ratings showed that the sweet environment had yielded significantly higher ratings than the bitter environment (Table 2). There was no observed effect on perceived bitterness or sourness.
Environments that do not share a letter are significantly different from each other (p < 0.05) in the post hoc Tukey tests.

3.4. Visual-Taste Congruency on Liking

To obtain an overview of the interrelations among visual-taste congruency and all the liking responses, Pearson correlation coefficients were generated for congruency and three liking variants in the three different VR environments (Table 3). A positive correlation between congruency on liking and environment liking was observed in all three VR environments, but not for beverage liking or overall liking. Furthermore, a positive correlation was found between environment liking and beverage liking in all three environments. As expected, FAA was negatively correlated to the other three variants, because FAA showed more negative values to indicate higher liking. However, no significance was found. To assess the difference of liking level among the three environments, the ANOVA analysis showed a significant effect of visual environment on environment liking (F (2, 120) = 6.490, p = 0.0021, and η2 = 0.043). However, no effect was observed in beverage liking and overall liking (FAA).

3.5. Gender Difference on Liking

Although no significant difference was observed in environment liking using our pilot questionnaire, a significant difference was shown in environment liking in the experiment questionnaire. Additionally, the gender ratio in the experiment was relatively imbalanced as compared with that of the pilot survey. We suspected that there could be a gender-influenced difference in preferences, therefore, we investigated gender differences in the environment, beverage, and overall liking using the Mann–Whitney U test (Table 4). Gender difference did not appear in the hedonic evaluation or the sweetness rating, across all three environments (p > 0.10).

4. Discussion

Overall, the results of the study reveal that visual-taste congruency has a significant influence on the perceived intensity of sweetness. The same beverage was rated significantly sweeter in a sweet-congruent environment than in an incongruent (bitter) or neutral environment. A maximum 13% increase in sweetness perception (1.15 points) on a nine-point scale was revealed when participants were immersed in a sweet-congruent VR environment. This result is consistent with a growing body of literature on visual sweetness enhancement [2,3,5,24,73]. With the global obesity crisis closely related to excessive sugar consumption [74,75,76], it is important to build psychological models that enhance individuals’ sweetness perception to help reduce sugar levels in foods and beverages.
Unexpectedly, we did not observe any influence of visual-taste congruency on product liking. In theory, however, mixed results have been shown in the existing literature on the influence of taste-specific visual cues on flavour perception. Some studies have indicated a significant influence of taste-congruent visual cues on product liking [2,14,24] while other studies have not reported this result [5,77]. Additionally, a recent study showed the existence of crossmodal correspondences between physical warmth and light colours [78]. This type of temperature-visual congruency increases consumer preferences only for light-coloured goods under comfortable conditions, which indicates that the level of comfort could play a role in the influence of congruency on product liking. Reflecting on this issue, it is possible that the VR environments presented in the current study were not comfortable enough to increase product liking, as the average comfort rating for the three environments lies was either “slightly comfortable” or “moderately comfortable”. Interestingly, significant results have been shown in environment liking but not found in pilot testing where respondents rated 2D pictures of three VR environments. A maximum 12% increase in environment liking (1.1 points) on a nine-point scale was reported when immersed in a congruent VR environment. We initially suspected it was gender difference that caused this shift of liking, since some of the female participants explicitly reported extreme liking of the sweet environment and the gender ratio was less balanced in the experiment population as compared with that of the pilot population. However, this factor was later eliminated. As a positive correlation was found between environment liking and beverage liking, it seemingly indicates a complementary effect between taste and visual cues on consumer preferences. It is also possible that the 3D VR experience is more real and attractive than 2D images, meaning that the participants like it better through stronger overall affect. An increase of 13% in overall environment liking (1.13 point) was observed after switching to the same nine-point scale. No significant difference was observed in overall liking (FAA), since the results of beverage affinity counteracted the results of environment affinity.
This is the first time that combined taste-specific visual cues were shown to influence sweetness perception in a VR environment. As VR and other immersive technologies become less expensive and easier to use, they can be used as a flexible interface to change eating environments anywhere and anytime. Thus, they could reduce people’s sugar intake by enhancing sweetness perception without compromising the overall taste experience. The results from the current study provide insight into the future design of taste-congruent VR environments. In future, it would be interesting to see if the addition of other sensory-congruent cues (e.g., auditory, tactile, and olfactory) could further modulate individual flavour perception in immersive environments. For example, taste-specific speech sounds [79] and tactile cues [80] can be easily incorporated into immersive environments to influence flavour perception.
The present study has several limitations stemming from the fact that the EEG was recorded while participants tasted the beverage in the VR environment. This means that the resulting FAA values provided an overall indication of the approach/withdrawal tendency towards the tested product and environment, but was unable to specifically represent a response towards environment or beverage. It is possible to obtain such values by recording brain activity separately while being in a VR environment only, or during tasting only. However, this is difficult to fulfil due to logistic reasons. Moreover, the VR environment could be more interactive and real with motion sensors added to the setup. For example, participants could reach for the cup holding the beverage as represented in the environment instead of blindly receiving the beverage from the researcher. This would require more advanced apparatus to power the software as the current equipment (Samsung gear VR powered by Samsung S7) was unable to do so.

5. Conclusions

The current study found that visual-taste congruency could exert a significant influence on an individual’s perception of sweetness but not on product liking. This result provides insight into the design of future taste-congruent environments that could enhance certain flavour perceptions and ultimately change people’s eating behaviour. Immersive technology such as VR could be a useful medium to deliver the above designs in a flexible and cost-effective manner.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2304-8158/9/4/465/s1, Figure S1: Photo of a participant tasting test beverage during VR exposure, Figure S2: QR codes for APK files of three VR environments, Table S1: Nutritional content of taste stimulus used in the experiment, Table S2: Background information of experiment participants (n = 41) depicted as mean (SD) or n, Table S3: Ratings of environment liking (9-point hedonic scale) in pilot survey (n = 138) depicted as mean (SD), Table S4: Questions and choices in experiment survey a, Table S5: Questions and choices in experiment survey b.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, Y.C., A.X.H. and F.J.A.P.-C.; methodology, Y.C., G.M. and F.J.A.P.-C.; software, Y.C., A.X.H. and G.M.; validation, Y.C., A.X.H. and I.F.; formal analysis, Y.C.; investigation, Y.C. and I.F.; resources, Y.C. and A.X.H.; data curation, Y.C. and I.F.; writing—original draft preparation, Y.C.; writing—review and editing, Y.C., A.X.H., I.F., G.M. and F.J.A.P.-C.; visualization, Y.C.; supervision, G.M. and F.J.A.P.-C.; project administration, Y.C.; funding acquisition, Y.C. and F.J.A.P.-C.; All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the FoodSMART project (European Union’s Horizon 2020 Programme), grant number 643999. First author Y.C. is currently supported by the Chinese Scholarship Council.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Annemarie Olsen and Wender Bredie for commenting on the study design as well as Belinda Lange, Sanne Sansolios and Gordon Lucas for technical support.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Spence, C. Multisensory flavour perception. Curr. Biol. 2013, 23, R365–R369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. Carvalho, F.M.; Spence, C. Cup colour influences consumers’ expectations and experience on tasting specialty coffee. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 75, 157–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Fairhurst, M.T.; Pritchard, D.; Ospina, D.; Deroy, O. Bouba-Kiki in the plate: Combining crossmodal correspondences to change flavour experience. Flavour 2015, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Hidaka, S.; Shimoda, K. Investigation of the Effects of Color on Judgments of Sweetness Using a Taste Adaptation Method. Multisens. Res. 2014, 27, 189–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Wang, Q.; Carvalho, F.R.; Persoone, D.; Spence, C. Assessing the effect of shape on the evaluation of expected and actual chocolate flavour. Flavour 2017, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  6. Wang, Q.J.; Mielby, L.A.; Junge, J.Y.; Bertelsen, A.S.; Kidmose, U.; Spence, C.; Byrne, D.V. The Role of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Sensory Factors in Sweetness Perception of Food and Beverages: A Review. Foods 2019, 8, 211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Hershenson, M. Reaction time as a measure of intersensory facilitation. J. Exp. Psychol. 1962, 63, 289–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Krishna, A.; Elder, R.S.; Caldara, C. Feminine to smell but masculine to touch? Multisensory congruence and its effect on the aesthetic experience. J. Consum. Psychol. 2010, 20, 410–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Labroo, A.A.; Dhar, R.; Schwarz, N. Of Frog Wines and Frowning Watches: Semantic Priming, Perceptual Fluency, and Brand Evaluation. J. Consum. Res. 2008, 34, 819–831. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Wang, Q.; Spence, C. Assessing the Effect of Musical Congruency on Wine Tasting in a Live Performance Setting. i-Perception 2015, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Hagtvedt, H.; Brasel, S.A. Cross-Modal Communication: Sound Frequency Influences Consumer Responses to Color Lightness. J. Mark. Res. 2016, 53, 551–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Velasco, C.; Adams, C.; Petit, O.; Spence, C. On the localization of tastes and tasty products in 2D space. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 71, 438–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Nouchi, R.; Kawashima, R.; Sugiura, M. A Sweet Voice: The Influence of Cross-Modal Correspondences Between Taste and Vocal Pitch on Advertising Effectiveness. Multisens. Res. 2019, 32, 401–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Spence, C.; Velasco, C.; Knöferle, K. A large sample study on the influence of the multisensory environment on the wine drinking experience. Flavour 2015, 3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Velasco, C.; Woods, A.T.; Marks, L.E.; Cheok, A.D.; Spence, C. The semantic basis of taste-shape associations. PeerJ 2016, 4, e1644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Velasco, C.; Woods, A.T.; Deroy, O.; Spence, C. Hedonic mediation of the crossmodal correspondence between taste and shape. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 41, 151–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Wan, X.; Woods, A.T.; van Den Bosch, J.J.F.; McKenzie, K.J.; Velasco, C.; Spence, C. Cross-cultural differences in crossmodal correspondences between basic tastes and visual features. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Woods, A.T.; Spence, C.; Butcher, N.; Deroy, O. Fast Lemons and Sour Boulders: Testing Crossmodal Correspondences Using an Internet-Based Testing Methodology. i-Perception 2013, 4, 365–379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Velasco, C.; Woods, A.T.; Hyndman, S.; Spence, C. The Taste of Typeface. i-Perception 2015, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Velasco, C.; Salgado-Montejo, A.; Marmolejo-Ramos, F.; Spence, C. Predictive packaging design: Tasting shapes, typefaces, names, and sounds. Food Qual. Prefer. 2014, 34, 88–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Nouchi, R.; Kawashima, R.; Sugiura, M. Round Faces Are Associated with Sweet Foods: The Role of Crossmodal Correspondence in Social Perception. Foods 2019, 8, 103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Clydesdale, F.M.; Gover, R.; Fugardi, C. The effect of color on thirst quenching, sweetness, acceptability and flavor intensity in fruit punch flavored beverages. J. Food Qual. 1992, 15, 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Spence, C.; Wan, X.; Woods, A.; Velasco, C.; Deng, J.; Youssef, J.; Deroy, O. On tasty colours and colourful tastes? Assessing, explaining, and utilizing crossmodal correspondences between colours and basic tastes. Flavour 2015, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Oberfeld, D.; Hecht, H.; Allendorf, U.; Wickelmaier, F. Ambient lighting modifies the flavor of wine. J. Sens. Stud. 2009, 24, 797–832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Spence, C. On the psychological impact of food colour. Flavour 2015, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. O’Mahony, M. Gustatory Responses to Nongustatory Stimuli. Perception 1983, 12, 627–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Tomasik-Krótki, J.; Strojny, J. Scaling of sensory impressions. J. Sens. Stud. 2008, 23, 251–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Piqueras Fiszman, B.; Giboreau, A.; Spence, C. Assessing the influence of the color of the plate on the perception of a complex food in a restaurant setting. Flavour 2013, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Piqueras-Fiszman, B.; Alcaide, J.; Roura, E.; Spence, C. Is it the plate or is it the food? Assessing the influence of the color (black or white) and shape of the plate on the perception of the food placed on it. Food Qual. Prefer. 2012, 24, 205–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Harrar, V.; Spence, C. The taste of cutlery: How the taste of food is affected by the weight, size, shape, and colour of the cutlery used to eat it. Flavour 2013, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Okajima, K.; Spence, C. Effects of Visual Food Texture on Taste Perception. i-Perception 2011, 2, 966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Van Rompay, T.J.L.; Finger, F.; Saakes, D.; Fenko, A. “See me, feel me”: Effects of 3D-printed surface patterns on beverage evaluation. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 62, 332–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Biggs, L.; Juravle, G.; Spence, C. Haptic exploration of plateware alters the perceived texture and taste of food. Food Qual. Prefer. 2016, 50, 129–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Slocombe, B.G.; Carmichael, D.A.; Simner, J. Cross-modal tactile–taste interactions in food evaluations. Neuropsychologia 2016, 88, 58–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Tu, Y.; Yang, Z.; Ma, C. Touching tastes: The haptic perception transfer of liquid food packaging materials. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 39, 124–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Spence, C.; Carvalho, F.M. The coffee drinking experience: Product extrinsic (atmospheric) influences on taste and choice. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Stelick, A.; Penano, A.G.; Riak, A.C.; Dando, R. Dynamic Context Sensory Testing—A Proof of Concept Study Bringing Virtual Reality to the Sensory Booth. J. Food Sci. 2018, 83, 2047–2051. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Sinesio, F.; Moneta, E.; Porcherot, C.; Abbà, S.; Dreyfuss, L.; Guillamet, K.; Bruyninckx, S.; Laporte, C.; Henneberg, S.; McEwan, J.A. Do immersive techniques help to capture consumer reality? Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 77, 123–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Pennanen, K.; Närväinen, J.; Vanhatalo, S.; Raisamo, R.; Sozer, N. Effect of virtual eating environment on consumers’ evaluations of healthy and unhealthy snacks. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Boutrolle, I.; Delarue, J. Studying Meals in the Home and in the Laboratory. In Meals in Science and Practice; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2009; pp. 128–165. [Google Scholar]
  41. King, S.C.; Weber, A.J.; Meiselman, H.L.; Lv, N. The effect of meal situation, social interaction, physical environment and choice on food acceptability. Food Qual. Prefer. 2004, 15, 645–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Meiselman, H.L.; Johnson, J.L.; Reeve, W.; Crouch, J.E. Demonstrations of the influence of the eating environment on food acceptance. Appetite 2000, 35, 231–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Higgins, E.T.; Scholer, A.A. Engaging the consumer: The science and art of the value creation process. J. Consum. Psychol. 2009, 19, 100–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Bangcuyo, R.G.; Smith, K.J.; Zumach, J.L.; Pierce, A.M.; Guttman, G.A.; Simons, C.T. The use of immersive technologies to improve consumer testing: The role of ecological validity, context and engagement in evaluating coffee. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 41, 84–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Sinesio, F.; Saba, A.; Peparaio, M.; Saggia Civitelli, E.; Paoletti, F.; Moneta, E. Capturing consumer perception of vegetable freshness in a simulated real-life taste situation. Food Res. Int. 2018, 105, 764–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Corporation, I.D. Worldwide Augmented and Virtual Reality Spending Guide. Available online: https://www.idc.com/getdoc.jsp?containerId=IDC_P34919 (accessed on 13 March 2020).
  47. Kemp, S.E. Sensory Evaluation: A Practical Handbook; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  48. Brown, C.; Randolph, A.; Burkhalter, J. The Story of Taste: Using EEGs and Self-Reports to Understand Consumer Choice. Kennesaw J. Undergrad. Res. 2012, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Yoto, A.; Sato, K.; Yokogoshi, H. Correlations between Central Nervous Activity and Subjective Evaluation of Beverage. Int. J. Affect. Eng. 2013, 12, 45–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Manippa, V.; van der Laan, L.N.; Brancucci, A.; Smeets, P.A.M. Health body priming and food choice: An eye tracking study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 72, 116–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Gunaratne, T.M.; Fuentes, S.; Gunaratne, N.M.; Torrico, D.D.; Gonzalez Viejo, C.; Dunshea, F.R. Physiological Responses to Basic Tastes for Sensory Evaluation of Chocolate Using Biometric Techniques. Foods 2019, 8, 243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. De Wijk, R.A.; Kaneko, D.; Dijksterhuis, G.B.; van Zoggel, M.; Schiona, I.; Visalli, M.; Zandstra, E.H. Food perception and emotion measured over time in-lab and in-home. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 75, 170–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Nouchi, R.; Kawashima, R.; Sugiura, M. Anxiety increases visual attention to hedonic foods: A preliminary eye-tracking study on the impact of the interplay between integral and incidental affect on foods. Appetite 2019, 137, 218–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Seo, H.-S.; Iannilli, E.; Hummel, C.; Okazaki, Y.; Buschhüter, D.; Gerber, J.; Krammer, G.; Lengerich, B.; Hummel, T. A salty-congruent odor enhances saltiness: Functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2013, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Nouchi, R.; Kawashima, R.; Sugiura, M. Tastiness but not healthfulness captures automatic visual attention: Preliminary evidence from an eye-tracking study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 64, 148–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Gere, A.; Danner, L.; Dürrschmid, K.; Kókai, Z.; Sipos, L.; Huzsvai, L.; Kovács, S. Structure of presented stimuli influences gazing behavior and choice. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 83, 103915. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Teplan, M. Fundamental of EEG Measurement. Meas. Sci. Rev. 2002, 2, 1–11. [Google Scholar]
  58. Davidson, R.J.; Ekman, P.; Saron, C.D.; Senulis, J.A.; Friesen, W.V. Approach-withdrawal and cerebral asymmetry: Emotional expression and brain physiology: I. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1990, 58, 330–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Harmon-Jones, E.; Gable, P.A.; Peterson, C.K. The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: A review and update. Biol. Psychol. 2010, 84, 451–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Davidson, R.J. Anterior cerebral asymmetry and the nature of emotion. Brain Cogn. 1992, 20, 125–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Coan, J.A.; Allen, J.J.B. Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biol. Psychol. 2004, 67, 7–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Silberman, E.K.; Weingartner, H. Hemispheric lateralization of functions related to emotion. Brain Cogn. 1986, 5, 322–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Di Flumeri, G.; Herrero, M.; Trettel, A.; Cherubino, P.; Maglione, A.; Moneta, E.; Peparaio, M.; Babiloni, F. EEG Frontal Asymmetry Related to Pleasantness of Olfactory Stimuli in Young Subjects; Springer: Belrin, Germany, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  64. Modica, E.; Cartocci, G.; Rossi, D.; Martinez Levy, A.C.; Cherubino, P.; Maglione, A.G.; Di Flumeri, G.; Mancini, M.; Montanari, M.; Perrotta, D.; et al. Neurophysiological Responses to Different Product Experiences. Comput. Intell. Neurosci. 2018, 2018, 9616301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. McBurney, D.H.; Collings, V.B.; Glanz, L.M. Temperature dependence of human taste responses. Physiol. Behav. 1973, 11, 89–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Talavera, K.; Ninomiya, Y.; Winkel, C.; Voets, T.; Nilius, B. Influence of temperature on taste perception. Cell Mol. Life Sci. 2007, 64, 377–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Johnson, E.A.; Vickers, Z. The effectiveness of palate cleansing strategies for evaluating the bitterness of caffeine in cream cheese. Food Qual. Prefer. 2004, 15, 311–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Delwiche, J.; O’Mahony, M. Flavour discrimination: An extension of thurstonian ‘Paradoxes’ to the tetrad method. Food Qual. Prefer. 1996, 7, 1–5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Vickers, Z.; Morris, E.E.; Savaria, M. Effectiveness of oral cleansers for evaluating sourness. J. Sens. Stud. 2008, 23, 526–532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Lucak, C.; Delwiche, J. Efficacy of Various Palate Cleansers with Representative Foods. Chemosens. Percept. 2009, 2, 32–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Delorme, A.; Makeig, S. EEGLAB: An open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 2004, 134, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Allen, J.J.B.; Coan, J.A.; Nazarian, M. Issues and assumptions on the road from raw signals to metrics of frontal EEG asymmetry in emotion. Biol. Psychol. 2004, 67, 183–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Ross, C.F.; Bohlscheid, J.; Weller, K. Influence of Visual Masking Technique on the Assessment of 2 Red Wines by Trained and Consumer Assessors. J. Food Sci. 2008, 73, S279–S285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Morenga, L.T.; Mallard, S.; Mann, J. Dietary sugars and body weight: Systematic review and meta-analyses of randomised controlled trials and cohort studies. BMJ 2013, 346, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Drewnowski, A. The real contribution of added sugars and fats to obesity. Epidemiol. Rev. 2007, 29, 160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Rippe, J.; Angelopoulos, T. Sugars, obesity, and cardiovascular disease: Results from recent randomized control trials. Eur. J. Nutr. 2016, 55 (Suppl. S2), 45–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  77. Wang, Q.J.; Mielby, L.A.; Thybo, A.K.; Bertelsen, A.S.; Kidmose, U.; Spence, C.; Byrne, D.V. Sweeter together? Assessing the combined influence of product-related and contextual factors on perceived sweetness of fruit beverages. J. Sens. Stud. 2019, 34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Nouchi, R.; Kawashima, R.; Sugiura, M. Light colors and comfortable warmth: Crossmodal correspondences between thermal sensations and color lightness influence consumer behavior. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 72, 45–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Motoki, K.; Saito, T.; Park, J.; Velasco, C.; Spence, C.; Sugiura, M. Tasting names: Systematic investigations of taste-speech sounds associations. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Pramudya, R.C.; Choudhury, D.; Zou, M.; Seo, H.-S. “Bitter Touch”: Cross-modal associations between hand-feel touch and gustatory cues in the context of coffee consumption experience. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 83, 103914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Overall experimental procedures.
Figure 1. Overall experimental procedures.
Foods 09 00465 g001
Figure 2. Three VR environments used in the study. (A) Sweet environment; (B) Bitter environment; and (C) Neutral environment.
Figure 2. Three VR environments used in the study. (A) Sweet environment; (B) Bitter environment; and (C) Neutral environment.
Foods 09 00465 g002
Figure 3. Association between four tastes and (A) sweet environment; (B) bitter environment; and (C) neutral environment (in terms of %).
Figure 3. Association between four tastes and (A) sweet environment; (B) bitter environment; and (C) neutral environment (in terms of %).
Foods 09 00465 g003
Figure 4. Participants’ rating on (A) visual-taste congruency and (B) intensity of sweetness, sourness, and bitterness in three VR environments (mean ± SEM). * indicates p < 0.05.
Figure 4. Participants’ rating on (A) visual-taste congruency and (B) intensity of sweetness, sourness, and bitterness in three VR environments (mean ± SEM). * indicates p < 0.05.
Foods 09 00465 g004
Table 1. Effect of visual-taste congruency on perceived tastes, liking, congruency, comfort, and environment vividness in terms of degrees of freedom, F value, p-value, and effect size.
Table 1. Effect of visual-taste congruency on perceived tastes, liking, congruency, comfort, and environment vividness in terms of degrees of freedom, F value, p-value, and effect size.
dfFp-Valueη2
Sweetness29.391<0.00050.135
Bitterness 20.1550.8570.003
Sourness20.0020.998<0.0005
Beverage liking20.5630.5710.009
Environment liking26.4900.0730.043
FAA20.5630.5710.009
Congruency218.530<0.00050.236
Vividness22.2400.1110.036
Comfort21.0410.3560.017
Significant effects (p < 0.05) are shown in bold.
Table 2. Average ratings with standard deviation and standard error per visual environment.
Table 2. Average ratings with standard deviation and standard error per visual environment.
ENVIRONMENTMEAN(SD)SEMCOMPARISON
SWEETNESSSweet2.42(1.31)0.21A
Bitter1.27(1.34)0.21B
Neutral1.61(1.12)0.17AB
BITTERNESSSweet−3.32(1.29)0.2A
Bitter−3.17(1.12)0.17A
Neutral−3.24(1.16)0.18A
SOURNESSSweet−2.07(2.07)0.32A
Bitter−2.05(1.90)0.29A
Neutral−2.05(2.12)0.33A
BEVERAGE LIKINGSweet1.29(1.72)0.27A
Bitter0.9(1.77)0.28A
Neutral1.12(1.53)0.24A
ENVIRONMENT LIKINGSweet1.39 (1.63)0.25A
Bitter0.29 (1.60)0.25B
Neutral0.34 (1.44)0.23AB
FAASweet−0.01(0.07)0.01A
Bitter0.01(0.07)0.01A
Neutral0.00(0.07)0.01A
CONGRUENCYSweet1.90(1.90)0.31A
Bitter−0.83(2.40)0.37B
Neutral−0.10(1.89)0.29B
VIVIDNESSSweet1.39(1.75)0.27A
Bitter0.80(1.78)0.28A
Neutral0.59(1.82)0.28A
COMFORTSweet2.39(1.24)0.19A
Bitter2.07(1.69)0.27A
Neutral1.93(1.49)0.23A
Table 3. Pearson correlation coefficients between environment liking, beverage liking, overall liking (FAA), and congruency in (A) the sweet environment, (B) the bitter environment, and (C) the neutral environment.
Table 3. Pearson correlation coefficients between environment liking, beverage liking, overall liking (FAA), and congruency in (A) the sweet environment, (B) the bitter environment, and (C) the neutral environment.
(A) Sweet environment
CongruencyBeverage LikingEnvironment LikingFAA
Congruency1.00.120.47 **−0.01
Beverage liking-1.00.30 *−0.08
Environment liking--1.0−0.34
FAA---1.0
(B) Bitter environment
CongruencyBeverage LikingEnvironment LikingFAA
Congruency1.00.250.45 **−0.24
Beverage liking-1.00.40 *−0.21
Environment liking--1.0−0.1
FAA---1.0
(C) Neutral environment
CongruencyBeverage LikingEnvironment LikingFAA
Congruency1.00.160.18 *−0.14
Beverage liking-1.00.31 *−0.38
Environment liking--1.0−0.17
* Indicates significance at the 0.05 level and ** indicates significance at the 0.01 level.
Table 4. Gender difference between environment liking, beverage liking, and perceived sweetness intensity in (A) the sweet environment, (B) the bitter environment and (C) the neutral environment.
Table 4. Gender difference between environment liking, beverage liking, and perceived sweetness intensity in (A) the sweet environment, (B) the bitter environment and (C) the neutral environment.
(A) Sweet environment
Environment likingBeverage likingSweetness
Male1(−0.5,2)2(1,2.5)2(2,4)
Female2(0,3)1(0,3)3(2,3)
Z−1.515−0.890−0.027
P0.1300.3730.978
(B) Bitter environment
Environment likingBeverage likingSweetness
Male0(−1,0.5)1(−0.5,3)1(1,2)
Female0(−1,2)1(0,2)1.5(1,2)
Z−0.860−0.526−0.125
P0.3900.5990.901
(C) Neutral environment
Environment likingBeverage likingSweetness
Male1(−1,2)2(1,2)1(−1,2)
Female0(0,1)2(1,2.75)0(0,1)
Z−0.464−0.411−0.464
P0.6420.6810.642

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Chen, Y.; Huang, A.X.; Faber, I.; Makransky, G.; Perez-Cueto, F.J.A. Assessing the Influence of Visual-Taste Congruency on Perceived Sweetness and Product Liking in Immersive VR. Foods 2020, 9, 465. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040465

AMA Style

Chen Y, Huang AX, Faber I, Makransky G, Perez-Cueto FJA. Assessing the Influence of Visual-Taste Congruency on Perceived Sweetness and Product Liking in Immersive VR. Foods. 2020; 9(4):465. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040465

Chicago/Turabian Style

Chen, Yang, Arya Xinran Huang, Ilona Faber, Guido Makransky, and Federico J. A. Perez-Cueto. 2020. "Assessing the Influence of Visual-Taste Congruency on Perceived Sweetness and Product Liking in Immersive VR" Foods 9, no. 4: 465. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040465

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop