The Snacking Chameleon: Psychological Proximity Increases Imitation of Food Intake Independently of Brand Choice
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Method
2.1. Participants and design
2.2. Materials and Procedure
3. Results
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Cruwys, T.; Bevelander, K.E.; Hermans, R.C.J. Social modeling of eating: A review of when and why social influence affects food intake and choice. Appetite 2015, 86, 3–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Herman, C.P.; Polivy, J. Normative influences on food intake. Physiol. Behav. 2005, 86, 762–772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bevelander, K.E.; Anschütz, D.J.; Engels, R.C.M.E. Social norms in food intake among normal-weight and overweight children. Appetite 2012, 58, 864–872. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bevelander, K.E.; Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A.; Anschütz, D.J.; Hermans, R.C.J.; Engels, R.C.M.E. Imitation of snack food intake among normal-weight and overweight children. Front. Psychol. 2013, 18, 949. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hermans, R.C.J.; Larsen, J.K.; Herman, C.P.; Engels, R.C.M.E. Effects of social modeling on young women’s nutrient-dense food intake. Appetite 2009, 53, 135–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McFerran, B.; Dahl, D.; Fitzsimons, G.J.; Morales, A.C. I’ll have what she’s having: Effects of social influence and body type on the food choices of others. J. Consum. Res. 2010, 36, 915–929. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Feeney, J.R.; Polivy, J.; Pliner, P.; Sullivan, M.D. Comparing live and remote models in eating conformity research. Eat. Behav. 2011, 12, 75–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldman, S.J.; Herman, C.P.; Polivy, J. Is the effect of a social model attenuated by hunger? Appetite 1991, 17, 129–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Florack, A.; Palcu, J.; Friese, M. The moderating role of regulatory focus on the social modeling of food intake. Appetite 2013, 69, 114–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnston, L. Behavioral mimicry and stigmatization. Soc. Cogn. 2002, 20, 18–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robinson, E.; Benwell, H.; Higgs, S. Food intake norms increase and decrease snack food intake in a remote confederate study. Appetite 2013, 65, 20–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roth, D.A.; Herman, C.P.; Polivy, J.; Pliner, P. Self-presentational conflict in social eating situations. A normative perspective. Appetite 2001, 36, 165–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Herman, C.P.; Roth, D.A.; Polivy, J. Effects of the presence of others on food intake: A normative interpretation. Psychol. Bull. 2003, 129, 873–886. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hermans, R.C.J.; Lichtwarck-Aschoff, A.; Bevelander, K.E.; Herman, C.P.; Larsen, J.K.; Engels, R.C.M.E. Mimicry of food intake. The dynamic interplay between eating companions. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e31027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed][Green Version]
- Larsen, H.; Engels, C.M.E.; Souren, P.M.; Overbeek, G.J.; Granic, I. Peer influence in a micro-perspective: Imitation of alcoholic and non-alcoholic beverages. Addict. Behav. 2010, 35, 49–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koordeman, R.; Kuntsche, E.; Anschutz, D.J.; van Baaren, R.B.; Engels, R.C.M.E. Do we act upon what we see? Direct effects of alcohol cues in movies on young adults’ alcohol drinking. Alcohol Alcohol. 2011, 46, 393–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed][Green Version]
- Hansen, J.; Alves, H.; Trope, Y. Psychological distance reduces literal imitation: Evidence from an imitation-learning paradigm. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2016, 42, 320–330. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wessler, J.; Hansen, J. Temporal closeness promotes imitation of meaningful gestures in face-to-face communication. J. Nonverbal Behav. 2017, 41, 415–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liberman, N.; Trope, Y. The role of feasibility and desirability considerations in near and distant future decisions: A test of temporal construal theory. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 75, 5–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trope, Y.; Liberman, N. Temporal construal. Psychol. Rev. 2003, 110, 403–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trope, Y.; Liberman, N. Construal-level theory of psychological distance. Psychol. Rev. 2010, 117, 440–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vallacher, R.R.; Wegner, D.M. A theory of Action Identification; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Vallacher, R.R.; Wegner, D.M. What do people think they’re doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychol. Rev. 1987, 94, 3–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vallacher, R.R.; Wegner, D.M. Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1989, 57, 660–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wegner, D.M.; Vallacher, R.R. Action identification. In Handbook of Motivation and Cognition: Foundations of Social Behavior; Sorrentino, R.M., Higgins, E.T., Eds.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, UYSA, 1986; pp. 550–582. [Google Scholar]
- Fujita, K.; Henderson, M.D.; Eng, J.; Trope, Y.; Liberman, N. Spatial distance and mental construal of social events. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 278–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Genschow, O.; Hansen, J.; Wänke, M.; Trope, Y. Psychological distance modulates goal-based versus movement-based imitation. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 2019, 45, 1031–1048. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wessler, J.; Hansen, J. The effect of psychological distance on automatic goal contagion. Compr. Results Soc. Psychol. 2016, 1, 51–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Buchner, A.; Lang, A.-G. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: Tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav. Res. Methods 2009, 41, 1149–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Förster, J.; Friedman, R.S.; Liberman, N. Temporal construal effects on abstract and concrete thinking: Consequences for insight and creative cognition. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 87, 177–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hansen, J.; Rim, S.; Fiedler, K. Psychological distance and judgments of causal impact. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 49, 1184–1189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rim, S.; Hansen, J.; Trope, Y. What happens why? Psychological distance and focusing on causes versus consequences of events. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 104, 457–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hermans, R.C.J.; Engels, R.C.M.E.; Larsen, J.K.; Herman, C.P. Modeling of palatable food intake: The influence of quality of social interaction. Appetite 2009, 53, 801–804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vartanian, L.R.; Herman, C.P.; Polivy, J. Consumption stereotypes and impression management: How you are what you eat. Appetite 2007, 48, 265–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Codecheck, A.G. Laugenstangen &-Brezeln. Available online: https://www.codecheck.info/essen/knabbergebaeck/laugenstangen_brezeln.kat (accessed on 4 October 2019).
- Bischoff, C.; Reutner, L.; Hansen, J. Materials and data of the paper “The snacking chameleon: Psychological proximity increases imitation of food intake independently of brand choice”. Available online: https://osf.io/kta8c/ (accessed on 20 February 2020).
- Turner, J.C. Social Influence; Open University press: Milton Keynes, UK, 1991. [Google Scholar]
- Cruwys, T.; Platow, M.J.; Angullia, S.A.; Chang, J.M.; Diler, S.E.; Kirchner, J.L.; Lentfer, C.E.; Lim, Y.J.; Quarisa, A.; Tor, V.W.L.; et al. Modeling of food intake is moderated by salient psychological group membership. Appetite 2012, 58, 754–757. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De Castro, J.M. Family and friends produce greater social facilitation of food intake than other companions. Physiol. Behav. 1994, 56, 445–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hermans, R.C.J.; Salvy, S.-J.; Larsen, J.K.; Engels, R.C.M.E. Examining the effects of remote-video confederates on young women’s food intake. Eat. Behav. 2012, 13, 246–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Pliner, P.; Mann, N. Influence of social norms and palatability on amount consumed and food choice. Appetite 2004, 42, 227–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baskin, E.; Wakslak, C.J.; Trope, Y.; Novemsky, N. Why Feasibility Matters More to Gift Receivers than to Givers: A Construal-Level Approach to Gift Giving. J. Consum. Res. 2014, 41, 169–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Liviatan, I.; Trope, Y.; Liberman, N. Interpersonal Similarity as a Social Distance Dimension: Implications for Perception of Others’ Actions. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2008, 44, 1256–1269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Bischoff, C.; Reutner, L.; Hansen, J. The Snacking Chameleon: Psychological Proximity Increases Imitation of Food Intake Independently of Brand Choice. Foods 2020, 9, 228. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9020228
Bischoff C, Reutner L, Hansen J. The Snacking Chameleon: Psychological Proximity Increases Imitation of Food Intake Independently of Brand Choice. Foods. 2020; 9(2):228. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9020228
Chicago/Turabian StyleBischoff, Claudia, Leonie Reutner, and Jochim Hansen. 2020. "The Snacking Chameleon: Psychological Proximity Increases Imitation of Food Intake Independently of Brand Choice" Foods 9, no. 2: 228. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9020228
APA StyleBischoff, C., Reutner, L., & Hansen, J. (2020). The Snacking Chameleon: Psychological Proximity Increases Imitation of Food Intake Independently of Brand Choice. Foods, 9(2), 228. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9020228