Next Article in Journal
Improving the Flavor of Fermented Sausage by Increasing Its Bacterial Quality via Inoculation with Lactobacillus plantarum MSZ2 and Staphylococcus xylosus YCC3
Next Article in Special Issue
Correction: Endrizzi et al. Relationships between Intensity and Liking for Chemosensory Stimuli in Food Models: A Large-Scale Consumer Segmentation. Foods 2022, 11, 5
Previous Article in Journal
Effects of κ-Carrageenan and Guar Gum on the Rheological Properties and Microstructure of Phycocyanin Gel
Previous Article in Special Issue
Relationships between Intensity and Liking for Chemosensory Stimuli in Food Models: A Large-Scale Consumer Segmentation
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sensory Capacities and Eating Behavior: Intriguing Results from a Large Cohort of Italian Individuals

1
Institute for Maternal and Child Health—IRCCS, Burlo Garofolo, 34127 Trieste, Italy
2
Department of Medicine, Surgery and Health Sciences, University of Trieste, 34149 Trieste, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Foods 2022, 11(5), 735; https://doi.org/10.3390/foods11050735
Submission received: 30 December 2021 / Revised: 25 February 2022 / Accepted: 28 February 2022 / Published: 2 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Chemosensory Perception in Food)

Abstract

:
Eating behavior (EB) is a complex system influenced by many factors, but an undisputed role is played by the senses. In this work, we examined the effect of the sensory capacities on EB in 1152 Italian adult individuals. After administering a questionnaire on EB and assessing sensory performance through standard audiometric, olfactory, and taste tests, the prevalence of reduced sensory capacities (RSCs) and the correlation with selected risk factors were calculated. Regression models, structural equation modelling, and conditional recursive partitioning were used to investigate the relationship between variables. Around 70% of the subjects show reduced capacities in at least one sense, with taste being the most prevalent (55.21%). Male sex, aging, and low educational level are risk factors for RSCs. The increased number of senses with reduced capacities is a predictor of diminished food adventurousness and lower liking for vegetables, fish, and alcoholic beverages, while reduced capacities (RCs) in taste is a predictor of lower liking for alcoholic beverages and sweets. Overall, in addition to providing an overall picture of RSCs in Italian samples, our study reveals the association of RSCs with EB variables. This finding could have a relevant role in influencing individuals’ dietary habits and, therefore, health status.

1. Introduction

Eating behavior (EB) is a complex system encompassing food preferences, feeding practices, dieting, and eating-related problems including obesity, eating disorders, and feeding disorders. Food liking and food adventurousness have been shown to play a crucial role in EB, affecting food choices and intake and impacting individuals’ well-being [1]. Food liking have been described as an individual’s reported degree of liking for specific foods and beverages without regard to food intake per se [2]. It has been shown that food liking might be linked to food choices and are a proxy for reported intakes [3,4,5]. Additionally, studies have reported that food liking assessment may be a valid and feasible measure for studying the relationship between dietary behavior and health outcomes [6]. As an example, the liking for vegetables is correlated with the intake of vegetables, and it is well known that regular consumption of vegetables is linked with health benefits [7]. As regards food adventurousness (i.e., the willingness to try novel and unfamiliar foods), studies found out that the most food adventurous subjects tend to prefer a wide variety of foods, while individuals avoiding new food experiences (for example, neophobic children and adults) showed a lack of dietary variety [7,8,9,10].
Reduced taste, hearing, and smell capacities play a crucial role in EB, affecting nutrition and the perceiving of some foods [11]. For example, olfactory dysfunction affects both general perception and individual preferences of odors [12], with an impact on food selection, nutrition, and therefore health [13,14]. As a consequence, it might lead to weight loss and malnutrition, especially in the elderly [15,16]. As regards hearing loss (HL), it has been associated with low intakes of fat, protein [17], and reduced sensory input from textural cues, such as crispiness and crunchiness, leading to an increased risk of malnutrition in the elderly [18]. A lowered acuity or presence of taste loss can lead to poor appetite, inappropriate food choices, and decreased energy consumption, and it predisposes to a higher risk of developing cardiovascular diseases, overweight/obesity, and other diseases [19,20].
Based on these pieces of evidence, it is clear that reduced sensory capacities (RSCs) pose a significant threat to public health, seriously compromising the quality of life with direct consequences for health and safety and making their prevention of public interest [21,22,23,24]. Furthermore, recent findings showed that multiple RSCs exacerbate the effect of a single one [25,26], highlighting the importance of investigating reduced multisensory capacity. Several studies focused on defining the prevalence, the risk factors (e.g., age, sex, smoking habit, alcohol consumption, social status, education level, etc.), and the impact on health of single and multiple RSCs in individuals aged 40 years or more [22,27,28,29]. However, little is known about these variables and the effect of single or multiple RSCs in an entire adult population [30]. Although many studies in the literature suggest that RSCs are relatively common, there is still a lack of consensus on the prevalence of such disorders in population-based studies. In this light, the availability of phenotypic data from large cohorts of samples may offer exceptional opportunities to increase our knowledge of RSC’s prevalence and etiopathogenesis.
In this work, we analyzed a cohort of 1152 Italian adult individuals from isolated communities for which EB (food liking and food adventurousness) is available together with sensory data for hearing, smell, and taste, and personal information. The aims were to: (1) define the prevalence of single and multiple RSCs, (2) investigate the role of possible risk factors, and (3) evaluate the impact of RSCs on food liking and adventurousness.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Here, we analyzed part of the data collected between 2014 and 2015 from two Italian populations—Friuli Venezia Giulia (FVG), located in North-Eastern Italy, and Val Borbera (VBI), located in North-Western Italy—as a part of a research program aimed at the identification of genes and variants associated with common disorders. Inhabitants of these regions were invited to participate by public advertisements through local authorities, televisions and newspapers, and local physicians and mailings. Meetings were organized to present the project and its aims. No inclusion/exclusion criteria were applied during the recruitment phase. All the individuals, recruited as volunteers, were investigated for a large number of pathologies and phenotypes, including food habits [31] and sensory abilities on hearing, smell, and taste [32,33,34]. All parameters were systematically collected by professional and trained staff according to standardized protocols; participants were also required to fill in a questionnaire on health-related topics, including diet, lifestyle, and physical activity. All participants gave written informed consent, and the ethical committees of IRCCS-Burlo Garofolo and IRCCS-San Raffaele approved the study.
Information for a total of ~3000 individuals was collected. In this study, we applied the following inclusion criteria: (1) participation in the questionnaire about food liking and EB, (2) availability of complete information about all the three sensory phenotypes (hearing, smell, and taste), (3) availability of complete personal information (sex, age, educational attainment, smoking habits, and alcohol consumption), (4) absence of pathologies or conditions that could cause damage to senses (cancers, neurodegenerative diseases, ictus, etc.), (5) age ≥ 18 years.

2.2. Data Collection

For each participant, all tests and questionnaires were carried out on the same day.

2.2.1. Personal and Lifestyle Characteristics

Demographic, lifestyle information, and living habits, such as cigarette smoking (current smoker/no smoker), educational attainment, and alcohol consumption (in grams/day), were collected for each participant using a standard questionnaire.

2.2.2. Sensory Measurements and Reduced Sensory Capacities Definition

As regards hearing ability, all tests were performed using standard audiometers. Participants underwent pure-tone audiometry, tympanogram, and acoustic reflex testing in both ears. Measurements were all obtained after any acoustically obstructing wax had been removed. Different frequencies were measured: 0.25, 0.5, 1, 2, 4, and 8 kHz. For the statistical analysis, a new trait was created based on the mean frequency thresholds of 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz, following the Bureau International d’Audiophonologie (BIAP) recommendation (http://www.biap.org/en/recommendations/65-ct-2-classification-des-surdites, accessed on: 14 December 2020). An individual was considered to have a reduced hearing capacity if the average threshold was >20.
Regarding smell, the participants were instructed about the smell testing, and they carried out the test in a dedicated room. Twelve odorants in 12 commercially available felt-tip pens (“Sniffin’ Sticks” Burghart GmbH, Wedel, Germany) [35] were presented to each participant. The olfactory test consisted of odor identification with 4-alternative forced choices. The odorants were orange, leather, cinnamon, mint, banana, lemon, licorice, coffee, clove, pineapple, rose, and fish. The number of wrong answers was counted, and reduced smell capacity was defined as the number of errors ≥ 3, similar to other studies [30,36].
Taste responsiveness was determined using the Burghart filter paper method [37]. The tastes were sweet (sucrose, 0.2 g/mL), sour (citric acid, 0.165 g/mL), salty (NaCl, 1.0 mol/L), and bitter (quinine hydrochloride, 0.0024 g/mL). Participants were tested in a dedicated room without odors or visual or auditory distractions; they were instructed by an expert administrator and were asked not to eat or brush their teeth beforehand. Specifically, each participant placed the paper on his/her tongue and had to recognize the correct taste among a list of four descriptors: sweet, bitter, salt, and sour. A possible choice was also “I do not perceive any taste”. After tasting each paper, the participant rinsed his/her mouth with 10 mL of bottled mineral water (room temperature). The number of errors was counted, including as an error the answer “I do not perceive any taste”. Reduced taste capacity was defined as the number of errors ≥ 1, in agreement with other studies [28,30,36].
Each sensory capacity variable was coded as a binary trait considering the presence (1) and absence (0) of a reduced threshold. The number of the senses with reduced capacities was coded as an ordinal variable.

2.2.3. Eating Behavior

Liking of about 100 foods and beverages was assessed through a questionnaire. For each food, the participants answered the question “How much do you like it?” and they rated the preference on a 9-point scale ranging from “dislike extremely” (score 1) to “like extremely” (score 9) [38]. The liking groups were defined as the average of likings given by each individual for specific foods, and seven groups were taken into account as already described in [31]: alcoholic beverages, cheeses, fish, fruit, meat, sweet foods, and vegetables.
Food adventurousness was assessed as explained in [31]. Briefly, each subject answered the question: “How often do you try unfamiliar foods?” The response categories were: “never”, “rarely”, “some of the time”, “often”, and “very often”, categorized for the statistical analysis as numbers from zero to four.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were performed in R 3.6.2. (www.r-project.org, accessed on: 14 December 2020). The significance criterion was p ≤ 0.05.
Preliminary analysis conducted in each population separately (data not shown) revealed a similar trend, and we decided to merge all subjects in a unique dataset.
The prevalence of RSCs, calculated as a percentage (i.e., prevalence proportion), was described as a whole and according to sex and age groups.
The relationships between independent variables as possible risk factors and RSCs were assessed through logistic regression models for each sense function and through ordinal regression model (polr function, MASS R library) for the number of RSCs, which was coded as an ordinal variable from zero to three. The explanatory variables set included sex, age, education, smoking habits, and alcohol consumption. Age was considered in 10-year groups to capture better the effect. Education was used as a categorical variable: low (elementary/lower secondary) versus high (upper secondary/university). According to previous studies, alcohol consumption was classified into two categories (i.e., low and high) [30].
Multiple regression models were conducted to evaluate the possible effect of the number of RSCs on the EB phenotypes, i.e., food liking and food adventurousness in the entire population. Moreover, to better understand the complex interplay between variables, models were developed and tested with structural equation modelling (SEM, lavaan R package [39]). SEM [40] is a statistical technique that identifies the direct and indirect influences of variables. It is used when the response variable in one regression equation becomes a predictor in another. Potential confounders (sex, age, and educational level) were included in the models. Criteria for overall fit were chosen a priori: chi-square p-value non-significant (χ2 p > 0.05), confirmatory fit index (CFI) ≥ 0.92, Tucker–Lewis Index (TLI) > 0.87, and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) < 0.05 [41]. Finally, to evaluate the significance of the mediated model, the Sobel test was used [42]. In the statistical analysis, food adventurousness was considered as a numeric score ranging from 0 (“never”) to 4 (“very often”) and studied as a quantitative trait.
Conditional inference tree analysis was used to verify the effect of the number of RSCs on EB variables, in combination with the information on impairment in taste, hearing, and smell. Indeed, this method is not affected by multicollinearity and could be used also in presence of high correlations between independent variables, such as sensory capacity variables in our sample. We used the ctree function of R’s Partykit package (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/partykit/vignettes/partykit.pdf, accessed on: 14 December 2020). This statistical method [43] performs recursive partitioning that allows the creation of a regression tree; the variable with the highest predictive power (the lowest p-value after Bonferroni correction) is represented as the first node in the decision tree, so two subgroups (I and II) are created. For subgroup I, the variable with the lowest p-value (if any) is taken as the second or third node. The same is done for subgroup II. The final model is based on the splitting variables in each node with the highest statistical significance [44].

3. Results

3.1. Sample Characteristics

A total of 1152 individuals (710 from Friuli Venezia Giulia (FVG) and 442 from Val Borbera (VBI)) aged 18–89 years (mean age 51.6 ± 16.6) were included in the study. Sample features, considering all the individuals and their sensory impairment status, are summarized in Table 1.
Table S1 shows, for the whole population and according to age groups, the mean and standard deviation of each sensory variable (mean frequency thresholds of 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz for hearing and the number of errors for both smell and taste) and for each EB variable (food adventurousness and food liking).
As regards smell, based on the number of correct answers in the Sniffin’s stick test [35], in our population, the number of normosmic individuals (≥11 correct answers) was 691 (60.0%), the number of hyposmics (between 8 and 10 correct answers) was 409 (35.5%), and the number of anosmics (≤7 correct answers) was 52 (4.5%).

3.2. Prevalence of Reduced Sensory Capacities and Factors Influencing Sensory Ability

Figure 1 shows a Venn diagram of the prevalence of RSCs in our populations.
Considering the whole population, 346 out of 1152 (30.03%) subjects did not show any sense with RC. One sense with RC was present in 40.88% of the sample (8.85% hearing, 29.08% taste, and 2.95% smell), two in 20.58% (11.81% hearing and taste, 5.82% taste and smell, 2.95% hearing and smell), and three in 8.51%. Taste RC was the most common (overall 55.21% and 29.08% as single sense), followed by hearing (overall 32.12% and 8.85% as single) and smell (overall 20.23% and 2.95% as single one). Taste plus hearing was the most frequent combination of two senses with RCs (11.81%), while the less frequent was hearing plus smell (2.95%). Table S2 shows the number and percentage of individuals presenting one sense with RC (i.e., in hearing, smell, or taste), two and three RSCs, in all samples and according to age groups. In all the age groups, the single RC in taste was the most frequent, except in 70+ people, in which the concurrent RCs in the three senses was the most frequent (35%).
The prevalence of hearing, smell, and taste RC in females, males, and overall samples by 10-year age groups is shown in Figure 2.
In both sexes, starting from the age of 30, the prevalence of hearing RC almost doubled every decade. In the vast majority (>80%), hearing RC was very common in the elderly (age 70+). From the age of 30–39, its prevalence in males was higher than in females, although this difference was statistically significant only in the age group 50–59 (χ2 test p-value 0.01). The prevalence of smell RC reached 20% in the decade 60–69 and almost 50% in the 70+ group. No significant differences were found between the sexes (χ2 test p-value > 0.05). Regarding taste, the prevalence ranged from 40.4% (women 30–39) to 79.5% (men 70+). The prevalence in males was always higher than females, and it was statistically significant in age groups 50–59 and later (χ2 test p-value < 0.01).
Figure 3 shows the prevalence of single/multiple RSCs divided by age groups in females and males.
The proportion of people without RSCs added to subjects with only one sense with RC was about 90% in the younger population (<40 years) and decreased to 35% and ~18% in women and men over 70. The proportion of single RC was about 40% in women and 50% in men up to the decade 50–59, after which it further decreased, with lower values for males but with no statistical differences between the sexes. Conversely, the percentage of two and three rose between age groups to 82% in 70+ males and 64.9% in 70+ females. The prevalence of three RSCs was higher in males, and the difference between genders was statistically significant for the age group 50–59 y (p-value < 0.01).
Table S3 shows the association of sex, age, low education, high alcohol consumption, and smoking with the different senses with RCs and their number.
As already observed in terms of prevalence, gender (i.e., male) was a significant risk factor for hearing and taste RCs (OR 1.78 and 1.9, respectively, p-value < 0.001) and for the overall number of senses with RCs (OR 1.91, p-value < 0.0001). All sensory abilities were affected by aging: a 10-year increase in aging produced an increased risk for taste (1.13), for smell (1.67), and hearing ability (3.08, p-values < 0.01). In addition, a 10-year increase in aging corresponded to an increase in the risk of 1.81 in the number of senses with RCs (p-value < 0.0001). Low education level was a risk factor for smell, taste (OR 1.62 and 1.48, respectively, p-values < 0.01), and the overall number of senses with RCs, with an OR of 1.67 (p-value < 0.0001). We found no significant influences from smoking and high alcohol consumption.

3.3. Effect of Reduced Sensory Capacities on Food Liking and Food Adventurousness

All the liking groups showed good reliability (Cronbach alpha ≥ 0.73 in FVG and VBI).
Table S4 shows the effect of the number of senses with RCs on liking groups and food adventurousness. A higher number of senses with RCs was a significant predictor of a lower preference for alcoholic beverages, fish, and vegetables (p-value 0.01, 0.04, and 0.009, respectively). Additionally, a higher number of senses with RCs corresponded to lower values of food adventurousness, but the statistical significance was at the threshold (p-value 0.05). Because all the liking groups were associated with food adventurousness (Table S3), the effect of the RSCs on the preference for alcoholic beverages, vegetables, and fish could be mediated by food adventurousness. Therefore, structural equation modelling (SEM) and Sobel tests were performed to examine the direct and mediated effect of variables on the liking groups. A direct effect of the number of senses with RCs on the liking groups emerged in complex models, with interaction with gender, age, and education level (Figure 4A–C). The Sobel test revealed that the effect of RSCs on food liking groups was also mediated by food adventurousness for alcoholic beverages (z = −3.38, p-value = 0.001), for fish (z = −4.72, p-value < 0.001), and for vegetables (z = −3.96, p-value < 0.001). The SEM models had good fit parameters (CFI = 0.998, TLI = 0.978, p-value chi-square = 0.134, RMSEA = 0.033 for alcoholic beverages; CFI = 0.998, TLI = 0.973, p-value chi-square = 0.134, RMSEA = 0.033 for fish; and CFI = 0.999, TLI = 0.995, p-value chi-square = 0.286, RMSEA = 0.015 for vegetables).
To better understand the role of the RSCs on EB measures, we applied conditional recursive partitioning. In Figure 5 are shown the binary trees obtained for food adventurousness and for the liking for alcoholic beverages and sweets in which the effects of the number of senses with RCs and single sense with reduced capacity were detected, while Figure S1 shows the binary trees obtained for the other food liking in which the effect of RSCs was not found.
Considering food adventurousness as a dependent variable (Figure 5A), we found that the analysis was able to divide the whole sample into two subgroups according to high/low education level (node 1, p-value < 0.001). The subgroup with high education level was further split according to the number of senses with RCs (node 2, cutoff 0, p-value = 0.015). Considering a boxplot that indicated the distribution of food adventurousness in each subgroup, we can conclude that the individuals with low education (node 5, n = 479) had lower values of the trait, followed by individuals with high education but at least one sense with reduced capacity (node 4, n = 412) and by individuals with high education and normal sensory capacity (node 3, n = 261).
As regards the liking for alcoholic beverages (Figure 5B), the reduced capacity for taste divided the males aged 44 years and above into two subsamples (node 9, p-value 0.043). Subjects with RC (node 11, n = 228) showed a lower liking compared with those without RC (node 10, n = 98). Other influencing variables were education level and food adventurousness.
Finally, only taste RC affected the liking for sweets (Figure 5C, node 3, p-value < 0.001). In particular, males with RC (node 5, n = 315) showed lower liking compared with those without (node 4, n = 181).

4. Discussion

Single and multiple RSCs play a critical role in EB and, in general, in the quality of life, being even more relevant in the elderly. In this work, we report the data on their prevalence and association with possible risk factors in a large Italian cohort and their link with EB.
Regarding the prevalence of RSCs in our population, taste RCs are the most frequent ones, followed by hearing and smell. About 70% of individuals had at least one sense with RC, consistent with the data already reported for other countries, such as Germany (with a prevalence of one or more RSCs in 73.6% of the subjects [30]). Conversely, some differences were found with the prevalence of a single sense with RC; in the German population, the hearing RC was slightly higher (43.9%), while that of taste was lower (20.3%) than our Italian data. Analyzing a population of approximately 3500 American subjects aged 40 and over, Liu et al. (2016) [27] found a prevalence of smell RC (13.5%) comparable with ours, while the percentage of taste RC was lower (17.3%). In another study of approximately 3000 American individuals, the results were comparable with ours for taste RC but lower for the remaining two senses [28]. In principle, the difference with other studies could be due to the population’s specific features (i.e., different age range, ethnicity, etc.) or to the different methodology used to assess each sense capacity. For example, the hearing ability was assessed by subjective means, such as interviews in Correia et al. (2016) [28], but by instrumentation (i.e., objective means such as audiometric evaluation) in Khil et al. (2015) [30] and our study. Instrumental data represent a more precise way of calculating hearing thresholds, taking into consideration several audiometric frequencies. Regarding the smell ability, despite being evaluated in all studies in a more harmonized way using the Sniffin’ Sticks test, the number of odorants tested varies greatly study by study, ranging from 8 [27] to 12 in our study. Finally, to measure taste ability, Correia and colleagues (2016) [28] used the same filter papers we utilized, while Khil (2015) [30] adopted spray bottles.
Regarding possible risk factors for RSCs, we found that male gender was associated with a higher prevalence of hearing, taste, and multiple RCs, in accordance with previous studies [27,28,30]. Sex differences might be due to genetic factors, lifestyle-related diseases, and different attention to health and work exposure [45]. As expected, we observed that, with aging, all the senses worsen, and the number of senses with RCs increases. Indeed, it is already known that the aging process is associated with a decline in sense function, and possible explanations could be physiological and anatomical changes during life and accumulations of specific lifestyles or exposures over time with aging [11,23,46]. We found associations of RSCs with a low educational level, a socio-economic indicator, and then with a proxy of a healthy lifestyle and different exposure to environmental risk factors. In this light, our results are consistent with Khil et al. (2015) [30], whereas other studies reported an association of low educational level only with smell but not taste [27]. The role of smoking habits and high alcohol consumption was largely debated but controversial [27,30], and our findings exclude any possible role of these two attitudes in the populations under investigation.
As regards the effect of RSCs on EB variables, our data show that a higher number of senses with RC was associated with lower food adventurousness and liking for some foods, such as alcoholic beverages, vegetables, and fish. In addition, by means of conditional recursive partitioning, we were able to confirm the effect of the number of senses with RC on food adventurousness and to highlight the effect of taste RC on liking for alcoholic beverages and sweets in particular subsamples of individuals. To our knowledge, no other studies considered the effect of concurrent RSCs on EB. As regards RC in taste, which we found specifically associated with the liking for alcoholic beverages and sweets, many researchers investigated the role of the capacity to perceive a single taste (i.e., bitter or sweetener) on liking but controversial results are present [47,48,49,50,51]. Studies have investigated the effect of a single sense with reduced capacity in food liking. For example, Zang and colleagues found that the liking for chocolate and peanut was lower in individuals with smell dysfunctions compared with controls [52].
Since food liking could be considered a measure of food intake [31,53] and therefore correlated with diet, our data further reinforce the role of RSCs in EB. A lower food adventurousness in the elderly has already been described as a sign of low dietary variety and quality [54,55]. Other studies showed that people with RSCs have a poor diet. For example, individuals with smell dysfunctions have a reduced preference for fruits and vegetables [56] or consume a low-quality diet [57]. Other studies showed that food neophobia (i.e., the reluctance to try novel foods, almost the opposite measure of food adventurousness) is associated with chemosensory abilities such as smell and taste [58], in line with our findings.
Furthermore, our work clearly further demonstrates how complex the relationship is between sensory abilities and diet-related phenotypes. This aspect suggests that many factors must be included in this kind of study and that appropriate statistical instruments—such as, for example, structural equation modelling or conditional recursive partitioning analysis—are needed.
The present study has some limitations, here addressed. First, it did not include data about food intake, and therefore, no direct effect of RSCs on consumption could be evaluated. In addition, it the difference between our hearing and chemosensory tests deserve to be mentioned. Indeed, the test we used to assess hearing capacity was a detection test, i.e., it measured the ability of participants to detect sounds from various frequencies. On the contrary, our taste and smell tests were forced-choice identification, i.e., they assessed the ability of the participants to identify different tastes and odors. The identification process involves the ability to perceive or detect sensory stimuli and other factors, such as verbal capacity and knowledge of the specific odorants and tastes. Indeed, chemosensory identifications rely on semantic and episodic memory functions and depend on encoding, retrieval, and verbal mediation processes [59,60,61,62]. In particular, since these processes are age-sensitive, an important factor underlying age-related differences in odor/taste identification can be the efficiency with which verbal representations of odors/tastes are retrieved [61,63]. Researchers have proposed specific tests for exploring the variability in chemosensory abilities within a population of older adults, including frail and dependent individuals [63,64]. In these experiments, taste capacity was measured by a detection test in which verbal ability and memory recovery were not required. These tests are not easy to implement in general population surveys such as our work. Still, it is crucial to consider these issues in future studies in order to obtain results for chemosensory tests that more comparable with hearing tests. Considering these aspects, we did not use the term “sensory deficit” or “sensory impairment” employed in other studies. We chose “reduced sensory capacity”, which allowed us to include the two dimensions assessed in our identification tests, i.e., the ability to detect the odorous/taste compounds and the ability to recover the name of the corresponding odor/taste.
Another limitation is the choice of the cut-offs used to determine the reduced sensory capacities. Indeed, there are no specific guidelines, and we relied on similar studies [28,30,36]. In addition, we observed a high prevalence of reduced capacity for taste in our results. This is probably due to the definition of reduced capacity being whether the participant identified three tastes out of four, as already proposed in other studies [28,30]. In addition, it deserves mentioning the phenome of “taste confusion”, i.e., the fact that many people misidentify the quality of taste stimuli, particularly evident for bitter and sour taste [65]. This aspect is due to age and other factors such as smoking and head trauma [65]. All these considerations give an idea of how difficult it is to determine the “true” prevalence of reduced sensory capacities, which is an issue that must be considered in future studies.

5. Conclusions

Overall, the present findings provide a unique picture of the prevalence of RSCs in a Caucasian population. The frequent presence of multiple RSCs (~30%) further strengthens the hypothesis that there could be a common pathophysiology among the senses as well as shared environmental and lifestyle risk factors [25,30,46].
Furthermore, for the first time, it was demonstrated that there is a correlation between an increased number of RSCs and a reduced food adventurousness and liking for specific foods, with possible consequences on the individuals’ dietary habits and, therefore, their health status. In this light, any intervention aimed at the preservation of sensory function is essential since it could have a huge impact not only on the overall quality of life but also on food preference and intake, thus impacting individual health status.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/foods11050735/s1, Table S1: mean and standard deviation of each sensory variable (mean frequency thresholds of 0.5, 1, 2, and 4 kHz for hearing and the number of errors for both smell and taste) and for each EB variable (food adventurousness and food liking), Table S2: number and percentage of individuals presenting one sense with reduced capacity (RSC) (i.e., in hearing, smell, or taste), two and three RSCs, according to age groups, Table S3: Factors associated with hearing, smell, and taste abilities (tested by logistic regression analysis) and the numbers of senses with reduced capacity (tested by ordinal logistic regression; the categories are the number of the senses with reduced capacity), Table S4: Results of linear regression models for assessing the relationship between RSC and eating behavior, Figure S1: Binary trees, computed by conditional recursive partitioning, of the effect of the number of senses with reduced capacity and RC in hearing, smell, and taste, together with gender, age, food adventurousness, and education level, on liking for meat (A), vegetables (B), cheeses (C), fruit (D), and fish I. Only gender, age, and food adventurousness affected the liking for these foods.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.P.C., G.G. and P.G.; methodology, M.P.C., G.G. and P.G.; formal analysis, M.P.C.; resources, P.G.; writing—original draft preparation, M.P.C., A.M. and G.G.; writing—review and editing, M.P.C., A.M., P.T., A.S., G.G. and P.G.; visualization, M.P.C., A.S. and P.T.; project administration, G.G.; funding acquisition, P.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by SENSAGING-Sensory decays and aging (D70-PRINSENSAGING-19: CUP J94I19000930006) to P.G.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Institute for Maternal and Child Health-I.R.C.C.S. “Burlo Garofolo” of Trieste (Italy) (2007 242/07) and by the Ethical committee of the “San Raffaele” Hospital and of the Piemonte Region.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy restrictions.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the staff and the study participants. The authors thank Martina Bradaschia for the English revision of the manuscript.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Birch, L.L. Development of food preferences. Annu. Rev. Nutr. 1999, 19, 41–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  2. de Mendonça, S.N.T.G.; Brandão, H.C.A.D.N.T.M.; Brandão, W.A.P.L.N.T.M.; Quintino, C.A.A.; De Francisco, A.; Teixeira, E. Food Preferences of Middle Aged and Elderly Subjects in a Brazilian City. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2013, 17, 130–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Drewnowski, A.; Hann, C. Food Preferences and Reported Frequencies of Food Consumption as Predictors of Current Diet in Young Women. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1999, 70, 28–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Pérez-Rodrigo, C.; Ribas, L.; Serra-Majem, L.; Aranceta, J. Food Preferences of Spanish Children and Young People: The EnKid Study. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2003, 57, S45–S48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Wądołowska, L.; Babicz-Zielińska, E.; Czarnocińska, J. Food Choice Models and Their Relation with Food Preferences and Eating Frequency in the Polish Population: POFPRES Study. Food Policy 2008, 33, 122–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Pallister, T.; Sharafi, M.; Lachance, G.; Pirastu, N.; Mohney, R.P.; MacGregor, A.; Feskens, E.J.M.; Duffy, V.; Spector, T.D.; Menni, C. Food Preference Patterns in a UK Twin Cohort. Twin Res. Hum. Genet. 2015, 18, 793–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Laureati, M.; Bergamaschi, V.; Pagliarini, E. School-Based Intervention with Children. Peer-Modeling, Reward and Repeated Exposure Reduce Food Neophobia and Increase Liking of Fruits and Vegetables. Appetite 2014, 83, 26–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Hazley, D.; McCarthy, S.N.; Stack, M.; Walton, J.; McNulty, B.A.; Flynn, A.; Kearney, J.M. Food Neophobia and Its Relationship with Dietary Variety and Quality in Irish Adults: Findings from a National Cross-Sectional Study. Appetite 2022, 169, 105859. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Jaeger, S.R.; Rasmussen, M.A.; Prescott, J. Relationships between Food Neophobia and Food Intake and Preferences: Findings from a Sample of New Zealand Adults. Appetite 2017, 116, 410–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Falciglia, G.A.; Couch, S.C.; Gribble, L.S.; Pabst, S.M.; Frank, R. Food Neophobia in Childhood Affects Dietary Variety. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2000, 100, 1474–1481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Mathieu, M.-E.; Reid, R.E.R.; King, N.A. Sensory Profile of Adults with Reduced Food Intake and the Potential Roles of Nutrition and Physical Activity Interventions. Adv. Nutr. 2019, 10, 1120–1125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Seow, Y.-X.; Ong, P.K.C.; Huang, D. Odor-Specific Loss of Smell Sensitivity with Age as Revealed by the Specific Sensitivity Test. Chem. Senses 2016, 41, 487–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Kong, I.G.; Kim, S.Y.; Kim, M.-S.; Park, B.; Kim, J.-H.; Choi, H.G. Olfactory Dysfunction Is Associated with the Intake of Macronutrients in Korean Adults. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0164495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Kershaw, J.C.; Mattes, R.D. Nutrition and Taste and Smell Dysfunction. World J. Otorhinolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2018, 4, 3–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Gopinath, B.; Sue, C.M.; Kifley, A.; Mitchell, P. The Association Between Olfactory Impairment and Total Mortality in Older Adults. J. Gerontol. Ser. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2012, 67A, 204–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  16. Boyce, J.M. Effects of Ageing on Smell and Taste. Postgrad. Med. J. 2006, 82, 239–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Kim, S.Y.; Sim, S.; Kim, H.-J.; Choi, H.G. Low-Fat and Low-Protein Diets Are Associated with Hearing Discomfort among the Elderly of Korea. Br. J. Nutr. 2015, 114, 1711–1717. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Schiffman, S.S. Critical Illness and Changes in Sensory Perception. Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2007, 66, 331–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Schiffman, S.S. Effects of Aging on the Human Taste System. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2009, 1170, 725–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Schiffman, S.; Graham, B. Taste and Smell Perception Affect Appetite and Immunity in the Elderly. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 2000, 54, S54–S63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kern, D.W.; Wroblewski, K.E.; Schumm, L.P.; Pinto, J.M.; McClintock, M.K. Field Survey Measures of Olfaction: The Olfactory Function Field Exam (OFFE). Field Methods 2014, 26, 421–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. Crews, J.E.; Campbell, V.A. Vision Impairment and Hearing Loss Among Community-Dwelling Older Americans: Implications for Health and Functioning. Am. J. Public Health 2004, 94, 823–829. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Murphy, C. Prevalence of Olfactory Impairment in Older Adults. JAMA 2002, 288, 2307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Welge-Lüssen, A. Ageing, Neurodegeneration, and Olfactory and Gustatory Loss. B-ENT 2009, 13 (Suppl. S5), 129–132. [Google Scholar]
  25. Chia, E.-M. Association Between Vision and Hearing Impairments and Their Combined Effects on Quality of Life. Arch Ophthalmol. 2006, 124, 1465. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Kiely, K.M.; Anstey, K.J.; Luszcz, M.A. Dual Sensory Loss and Depressive Symptoms: The Importance of Hearing, Daily Functioning, and Activity Engagement. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 837. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Liu, G.; Zong, G.; Doty, R.L.; Sun, Q. Prevalence and Risk Factors of Taste and Smell Impairment in a Nationwide Representative Sample of the US Population: A Cross-Sectional Study. BMJ Open 2016, 6, e013246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Correia, C.; Lopez, K.J.; Wroblewski, K.E.; Huisingh-Scheetz, M.; Kern, D.W.; Chen, R.C.; Schumm, L.P.; Dale, W.; McClintock, M.K.; Pinto, J.M. Global Sensory Impairment in Older Adults in the United States. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2016, 64, 306–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Pinto, J.M.; Wroblewski, K.E.; Kern, D.W.; Schumm, L.P.; McClintock, M.K. Olfactory Dysfunction Predicts 5-Year Mortality in Older Adults. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e107541. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Khil, L.; Wellmann, J.; Berger, K. Determinants of Single and Multiple Sensory Impairments in an Urban Population. Otolaryngol. Head Neck Surg. 2015, 153, 364–371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Concas, M.P.; Catamo, E.; Biino, G.; Toniolo, D.; Gasparini, P.; Robino, A. Factors Associated with Food Liking and Their Relationship with Metabolic Traits in Italian Cohorts. Food Qual. Prefer. 2019, 75, 64–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Vuckovic, D.; Mezzavilla, M.; Cocca, M.; Morgan, A.; Brumat, M.; Catamo, E.; Concas, M.P.; Biino, G.; Franzè, A.; Ambrosetti, U.; et al. Whole-Genome Sequencing Reveals New Insights into Age-Related Hearing Loss: Cumulative Effects, Pleiotropy and the Role of Selection. Eur. J. Hum. Genet. 2018, 26, 1167–1179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Concas, M.P.; Cocca, M.; Francescatto, M.; Battistuzzi, T.; Spedicati, B.; Feresin, A.; Morgan, A.; Gasparini, P.; Girotto, G. The Role of Knockout Olfactory Receptor Genes in Odor Discrimination. Genes 2021, 12, 631. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Catamo, E.; Tornese, G.; Concas, M.P.; Gasparini, P.; Robino, A. Differences in Taste and Smell Perception between Type 2 Diabetes Mellitus Patients and Healthy Controls. Nutr. Metab. Cardiovasc. Dis. 2021, 31, 193–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Hummel, T.; Sekinger, B.; Wolf, S.R.; Pauli, E.; Kobal, G. ‘Sniffin’ Sticks’: Olfactory Performance Assessed by the Combined Testing of Odour Identification, Odor Discrimination and Olfactory Threshold. Chem. Senses 1997, 22, 39–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Vennemann, M.M.; Hummel, T.; Berger, K. The Association between Smoking and Smell and Taste Impairment in the General Population. J. Neurol. 2008, 255, 1121–1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Zhao, L.; Kirkmeyer, S.V.; Tepper, B.J. A Paper Screening Test to Assess Genetic Taste Sensitivity to 6-n-Propylthiouracil. Physiol. Behav. 2003, 78, 625–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Pirastu, N.; Kooyman, M.; Traglia, M.; Robino, A.; Willems, S.M.; Pistis, G.; Amin, N.; Sala, C.; Karssen, L.C.; Van Duijn, C.; et al. A Genome-Wide Association Study in Isolated Populations Reveals New Genes Associated to Common Food Likings. Rev. Endocr. Metab. Disord. 2016, 17, 209–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Rosseel, Y. Lavaan: An R Package for Structural Equation Modeling. J. Stat. Softw. 2012, 48, 1–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Wright, S.S. Correlation and Causation. J. Agric. Res. 1921, 20, 557–585. [Google Scholar]
  41. Tabachnick. Using Multivariate Statistics; Pearson: Boston, MA, USA, 2001; Volume XXVI. [Google Scholar]
  42. Sobel, M.E. Asymptotic Confidence Intervals for Indirect Effects in Structural Equation Models. Sociol. Methodol. 1982, 13, 290–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Hothorn, T.; Hornik, K.; Zeileis, A. Unbiased Recursive Partitioning: A Conditional Inference Framework. J. Comput. Graph. Stat. 2006, 15, 651–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Strobl, C.; Malley, J.; Tutz, G. An Introduction to Recursive Partitioning: Rationale, Application, and Characteristics of Classification and Regression Trees, Bagging, and Random Forests. Psychol. Methods 2009, 14, 323–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Yoshinaka, M.; Ikebe, K.; Uota, M.; Ogawa, T.; Okada, T.; Inomata, C.; Takeshita, H.; Mihara, Y.; Gondo, Y.; Masui, Y.; et al. Age and Sex Differences in the Taste Sensitivity of Young Adult, Young-Old and Old-Old Japanese: Age and Sex Differences in Taste Sense. Geriatr. Gerontol. Int. 2016, 16, 1281–1288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Michikawa, T.; Nishiwaki, Y.; Takebayashi, T. Are you conscious of any age-related taste impairment? prevalence of and factors associated with taste impairment in japan: Letters to the editor. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2011, 59, 951–953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Dinehart, M.E.; Hayes, J.E.; Bartoshuk, L.M.; Lanier, S.L.; Duffy, V.B. Bitter Taste Markers Explain Variability in Vegetable Sweetness, Bitterness, and Intake. Physiol. Behav. 2006, 87, 304–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Tepper, B.J. 6-n-Propylthiouracil: A Genetic Marker for Taste, with Implications for Food Preference and Dietary Habits. Am. J. Hum. Genet. 1998, 63, 1271–1276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Tepper, B.J.; Koelliker, Y.; Zhao, L.; Ullrich, N.V.; Lanzara, C.; D’Adamo, P.; Ferrara, A.; Ulivi, S.; Esposito, L.; Gasparini, P. Variation in the Bitter-Taste Receptor Gene TAS2R38, and Adiposity in a Genetically Isolated Population in Southern Italy. Obesity 2008, 16, 2289–2295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Feeney, E.; O’Brien, S.; Scannell, A.; Markey, A.; Gibney, E.R. Genetic Variation in Taste Perception: Does It Have a Role in Healthy Eating? Proc. Nutr. Soc. 2011, 70, 135–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  51. Drewnowski, A.; Henderson, S.A.; Cockroft, J.E. Genetic Sensitivity to 6-n-Propylthiouracil Has No Influence on Dietary Patterns, Body Mass Indexes, or Plasma Lipid Profiles of Women. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2007, 107, 1340–1348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Zang, Y.; Han, P.; Burghardt, S.; Knaapila, A.; Schriever, V.; Hummel, T. Influence of olfactory dysfunction on the perception of food. Eur. Arch. Otorhinolaryngol. 2019, 276, 2811–2817. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  53. Duffy, V.B.; Lanier, S.A.; Hutchins, H.L.; Pescatello, L.S.; Johnson, M.K.; Bartoshuk, L.M. Food Preference Questionnaire as a Screening Tool for Assessing Dietary Risk of Cardiovascular Disease within Health Risk Appraisals. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2007, 107, 237–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Knaapila, A.J.; Sandell, M.A.; Vaarno, J.; Hoppu, U.; Puolimatka, T.; Kaljonen, A.; Lagström, H. Food Neophobia Associates with Lower Dietary Quality and Higher BMI in Finnish Adults. Public Health Nutr. 2015, 18, 2161–2171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Tuorila, H.; Lähteenmäki, L.; Pohjalainen, L.; Lotti, L. Food Neophobia among the Finns and Related Responses to Familiar and Unfamiliar Foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 2001, 12, 29–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Duffy, V.B.; Backstrand, J.R.; Ferris, A.M. Olfactory Dysfunction and Related Nutritional Risk in Free-Living, Elderly Women. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1995, 95, 879–884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Gopinath, B.; Russell, J.; Sue, C.M.; Flood, V.M.; Burlutsky, G.; Mitchell, P. Olfactory Impairment in Older Adults Is Associated with Poorer Diet Quality over 5 Years. Eur. J. Nutr. 2016, 55, 1081–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Wildes, J.E.; Zucker, N.L.; Marcus, M.D. Picky eating in adults: Results of a web-based survey. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 2012, 45, 575–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Hultsch, D.F.; Dixon, R.A. Learning and Memory in Aging. In Handbook of the Psychology of Aging; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1990; pp. 258–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Kausler, D.H. Experimental Psychology, Cognition, and Human Aging; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Larsson, M.; Bäckman, L. Semantic Activation and Episodic Odor Recognition in Young and Older Adults. Psychol. Aging 1993, 8, 582–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Daniel, T.A.; Katz, J.S. Primacy and Recency Effects for Taste. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 2018, 44, 399–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Mojet, J. Taste Perception with Age: Generic or Specific Losses in Supra-Threshold Intensities of Five Taste Qualities? Chem. Senses 2003, 28, 397–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Sulmont-Rosse, C.; Maitre, I.; Amand, M.; Symoneaux, R.; Van Wymelbeke, V.; Caumon, E.; Tavares, J.; Issanchou, S. Evidence for Different Patterns of Chemosensory Alterations in the Elderly Population: Impact of Age Versus Dependency. Chem. Senses 2015, 40, 153–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  65. Doty, R.L.; Chen, J.H.; Overend, J. Taste Quality Confusions: Influences of Age, Smoking, PTC Taster Status, and Other Subject Characteristics. Perception 2017, 46, 257–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Venn diagram of the prevalence of reduced sensory capacities (RSCs). The graph shows the distribution of RSCs in our populations, highlighting the prevalence of single and multiple RSCs. H-RC, T-RC, S-RC = reduced capacity in hearing, taste, and smell, respectively.
Figure 1. Venn diagram of the prevalence of reduced sensory capacities (RSCs). The graph shows the distribution of RSCs in our populations, highlighting the prevalence of single and multiple RSCs. H-RC, T-RC, S-RC = reduced capacity in hearing, taste, and smell, respectively.
Foods 11 00735 g001
Figure 2. Prevalence of reduced capacity for hearing (A), smell (B), and taste (C) in the 10-year age groups for females (orange bars), males (yellow bars), and overall (green bars). The y-axis indicates the age groups, and the x-axis presents the prevalence as a percentage. The numbers above the bars are the prevalence.
Figure 2. Prevalence of reduced capacity for hearing (A), smell (B), and taste (C) in the 10-year age groups for females (orange bars), males (yellow bars), and overall (green bars). The y-axis indicates the age groups, and the x-axis presents the prevalence as a percentage. The numbers above the bars are the prevalence.
Foods 11 00735 g002
Figure 3. Prevalence of the number of senses with reduced capacities in males (A) and females (B) by 10-year age groups. The numbers above the bars are the prevalence. The colors indicate the number of senses with reduced capacities: the absence of sense with RC is indicated by the lighter color, the presence of three senses with RCs by the darkest.
Figure 3. Prevalence of the number of senses with reduced capacities in males (A) and females (B) by 10-year age groups. The numbers above the bars are the prevalence. The colors indicate the number of senses with reduced capacities: the absence of sense with RC is indicated by the lighter color, the presence of three senses with RCs by the darkest.
Foods 11 00735 g003
Figure 4. Structural equation models (Lavaan R package [39]) show the effect of the number of senses with reduced capacities on food adventurousness and liking for alcoholic beverages (A), fish (B), and vegetables (C). The models have a good fit (CFI = 0.998, TLI = 0.978, p-value chi-square = 0.134, RMSEA = 0.033 for alcoholic beverages; CFI = 0.998, TLI = 0.973, p-value chi-square = 0.134, RMSEA = 0.033 for fish; and CFI = 0.999, TLI = 0.995, p-value chi-square = 0.286, RMSEA = 0.015 for vegetables). Reported numbers are the effect of the independent variables on the dependent variables. * p value < 0.05, ** p-value < 0.001. y = years.
Figure 4. Structural equation models (Lavaan R package [39]) show the effect of the number of senses with reduced capacities on food adventurousness and liking for alcoholic beverages (A), fish (B), and vegetables (C). The models have a good fit (CFI = 0.998, TLI = 0.978, p-value chi-square = 0.134, RMSEA = 0.033 for alcoholic beverages; CFI = 0.998, TLI = 0.973, p-value chi-square = 0.134, RMSEA = 0.033 for fish; and CFI = 0.999, TLI = 0.995, p-value chi-square = 0.286, RMSEA = 0.015 for vegetables). Reported numbers are the effect of the independent variables on the dependent variables. * p value < 0.05, ** p-value < 0.001. y = years.
Foods 11 00735 g004
Figure 5. Binary trees, computed by conditional recursive partitioning, of the effect of the number of senses with reduced capacities and reduced capacity in hearing, smell, and taste, together with gender, age, food adventurousness, and education level, on food adventurousness (A), liking for alcoholic beverages (B), and liking for sweets (C).
Figure 5. Binary trees, computed by conditional recursive partitioning, of the effect of the number of senses with reduced capacities and reduced capacity in hearing, smell, and taste, together with gender, age, food adventurousness, and education level, on food adventurousness (A), liking for alcoholic beverages (B), and liking for sweets (C).
Foods 11 00735 g005
Table 1. Characteristics of study participants, considering all the individuals and according to sensory status (i.e., no sense with reduced capacity and at least one sense with reduced capacity). IQR = interquartile range; Food adv = food adventurousness.
Table 1. Characteristics of study participants, considering all the individuals and according to sensory status (i.e., no sense with reduced capacity and at least one sense with reduced capacity). IQR = interquartile range; Food adv = food adventurousness.
AllNo Sense with Reduced CapacityAt Least One Sense with Reduced Capacity
N (% males)1152 (43.1)346 (33.8)806 (47.0)
Age groups, N (%)
18–29145 (12.6)66 (19.1)79 (9.80)
30–39153 (13.3)81 (23.4)72 (8.93)
40–49194 (16.8)81 (23.4)113 (14.0)
50–59256 (22.2)71 (20.5)185 (23.0)
60–69224 (19.4)38 (11.0)186 (23.1)
70+180 (15.6)9 (2.60)171 (21.2)
Education, N (%)
Elementary170 (14.8)17 (4.91)153 (19.0)
Lower secondary309 (26.8)68 (19.7)241 (30.0)
Upper secondary516 (44.8)182 (52.6)334 (41.4)
University157 (13.6)79 (22.8)78 (9.68)
Current smokers, N (%)224 (19.4)80 (23.1)144 (17.9)
High alcohol consumers, N (%)298 (25.9)79 (22.8)219 (27.2)
Food adv, median (IQR)2.0 (1; 3)2.0 (1.3; 3.0)2.0 (1.0; 3.0)
Liking groups, median (IQR)
Alcoholic beverages4.8 (3.3; 6.2)5.0 (3.7; 6.5)4.8 (3.2; 6.2)
Cheeses6.3 (5.3; 7.0)6.3 (5.3; 7.2)6.2 (5.3; 7.0)
Fish6.7 (5.3; 7.5)6.8 (5.5; 7.5)6.5 (5.3; 7.3)
Fruit7.0 (6.3; 7.7)7.0 (6.4; 7.7)7.0 (6.3; 7.7)
Meat6.8 (6.1; 7.5)7.0 (6.0; 7.6)6.8 (6.1; 7.5)
Sweets6.4 (5.4; 7.3)6.6 (5.6; 7.3)6.4 (5.3; 7.3)
Vegetables6.7 (5.9; 7.5)6.8 (5.9; 7.6)6.7 (5.9; 7.5)
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Concas, M.P.; Morgan, A.; Tesolin, P.; Santin, A.; Girotto, G.; Gasparini, P. Sensory Capacities and Eating Behavior: Intriguing Results from a Large Cohort of Italian Individuals. Foods 2022, 11, 735. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods11050735

AMA Style

Concas MP, Morgan A, Tesolin P, Santin A, Girotto G, Gasparini P. Sensory Capacities and Eating Behavior: Intriguing Results from a Large Cohort of Italian Individuals. Foods. 2022; 11(5):735. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods11050735

Chicago/Turabian Style

Concas, Maria Pina, Anna Morgan, Paola Tesolin, Aurora Santin, Giorgia Girotto, and Paolo Gasparini. 2022. "Sensory Capacities and Eating Behavior: Intriguing Results from a Large Cohort of Italian Individuals" Foods 11, no. 5: 735. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods11050735

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop