Syntactic Gender Agreement Processing on Direct-Object Clitics by Spanish-Speaking Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Evidence from ERP
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Phenomena
1.1.1. Null Objects
- Spanish (Simon-Cerejido and Gutiérrez-Clellen [5] p. 336)
- 2.
- Bulgarian (Ivanov [6] p. 196)
- 3.
- Greek (Marinis [7], p. 11)
- 4.
- Pronominal Null Object Context (Grégoire, 1; 11.22)
- 5.
- Non-pronominal Null Object Context (Grégoire, 2; 5.13)
1.1.2. Agreement Errors
1.2. Interface Delay, Interface Deficit and Definites
- 6.
- Anaphoric Context
- 7.
- Deictic Context
1.3. The Unique Checking Constraint (UCC), the Computational Complexity Hypothesis (CCH), Interface Deficit and Clitic Agreement Errors
1.4. The Surface Hypothesis and Clitic Agreement in DLD
1.5. Clitic Agreement Errors in Spanish DLD
1.6. Event-Related Potentials and Agreement in Children with DLD
1.7. Summary and Research Questions
- Do child Spanish-speakers show the LAN-type effect we expect for gender agreement errors?
- If so, is this effect less pronounced for children diagnosed with DLD than it is for typically developing children of the same age?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Procedures
2.2.1. Stimuli
- 8.
- El papá lo filmó.
2.2.2. Event-Related Potentials Recording
2.3. Data Analysis
Statistical Analysis
3. Results
3.1. ERP Time Window 250–500 ms
3.2. ERP Time Window 500–850 ms
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
References
- Castilla, A.P.; Pérez-Leroux, A.T. Omissions and Substitutions in Spanish Object Clitics: Developmental Optionality as a Property of the Representational System. Lang. Acquis. 2010, 17, 2–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wexler, K.; Gavarro, A.; Torrens, V. Feature Checking and Object Clitic Omission in Child Catalan and Spanish. In Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory 2002. Selected Papers from ‘Going Romance’, Groningen, 28–30 November 2002; Bok-Bennema, R., Hollebrandse, B., Kampers-Manhe, B., Sleeman, P., Eds.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2004; pp. 253–268. [Google Scholar]
- Jackson-Maldonado, D.; Maldonado, R. Grammaticality differences between Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment and their typically developing peers. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2017, 52, 750–765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pérez-Leroux, A.T.; Castilla, A.P.; Brunner, J. General and specific effects of lexicon in grammar: Determiner and object pronoun omissions in child Spanish. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2012, 55, 313–327. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Simon-Cereijido, G.; Gutiérrez-Clellen, V. Spontaneous language markers of Spanish language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2007, 28, 317–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ivanov, I. L1 Acquisition of Bulgarian Object Clitics: Unique Checking Constraint or Failure to Mark Referentiality. In BUCLD 32 Proceedings; Chan, H., Jacob, H., Kapia, E., Eds.; Cascadilla Press: Somerville, MA, USA, 2008; pp. 189–200. [Google Scholar]
- Marinis, T. The Acquisition of Clitic Objects in Modern Greek: Single Clitics, Clitic Doubling, Clitic Left Dislocation. ZAS Pap. Linguist. 2000, 15, 1–23. [Google Scholar]
- Pirvulescu, M.; Roberge, Y. Licit and illicit null objects in L1 French. In Theoretical and Experimental Approaches to Romance Linguistics; Gess, R., Rubin, E., Eds.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Nethererlands, 2005; pp. 197–212. [Google Scholar]
- MacWhinney, B. The CHILDES Project; Lawrence Erlbaum: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Landa, M.A. Conditions on Null Objects in Basque Spanish and Their Relation to Leísmo and Clitic Doubling. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Choi, J.K. [-Person] direct object drop: The genetic cause of a syntactic feature in Paraguayan Spanish. Hispania 2000, 83, 531–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yépez, M.V. Direct Object Clitics in Quiteño Spanish. Ph.D. Thesis, Cornell, Ithaca, NY, USA, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Schwenter, S.A. Null Objects across South America. In Selected Proceedings of the 8th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium; Face, T.L., Klee, C., Eds.; Cascadilla Proceedings Project: Somerville, MA, USA, 2006; pp. 23–36. [Google Scholar]
- Costa, J.; Lobo, M. Clitic omission, null objects or both in the acquisition of European Portuguese? In Romance Languages and Linguistic Theory; Baauw, S., Drijkoningen, F., Pinto, M., Eds.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2007; pp. 59–71. [Google Scholar]
- Wang, Q.; Lillo-Martin, D.; Best, C.; Levitt, A. Null subject vs. null object: Some evidence from the acquisition of Chinese and English. Lang. Acquis. 1992, 2, 221–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stalnaker, R. Pragmatic Presuppositions. In Semantics and Philosophy; Munitz, M., Unger, D., Eds.; New York University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1974; pp. 197–219. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, C. Demonstratives as definites. In Information Sharing: Reference and Presupposition in Language Generation and Interpretation; van Deemter, K., Kibble, R., Eds.; CSLI Press—Stanford: Palo Alto, CA, USA, 2002; pp. 89–196. [Google Scholar]
- Roberts, C. Uniqueness in Definite Noun Phrases. Linguist. Philos. 2003, 26, 287–350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wexler, K. Very Early Parameter Setting and the Unique Checking Constraint: A New Explanation of the Optional Infinitive Stage. Lingua 1998, 106, 23–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bittner, M. Time and Modality without Tense or Modals. In Tense Across Languages; Musan, R., Rathert, M., Eds.; Niemeyer: Tübingen, Germany, 2011; pp. 147–188. [Google Scholar]
- Reichenbach, H. Elements of Symbolic Logic; MacMillan: New York, NY, USA, 1947. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, R.W. A First Language: The Early Stages; Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1973. [Google Scholar]
- de Villiers, J.G.; de Villiers, P.A. A crosssectional study of the acquisition of grammatical morphemes. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 1973, 2, 267–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hernández-Pina, R. Teorías Psicosociolingüísticas y su Aplicación a la Adquisición del Español Como Lengua Materna; Siglo XXI: Madrid, España, 1984. [Google Scholar]
- Wimmer, H.; Perner, J. Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 1983, 13, 103–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron-Cohen, S. Mindblindness: An. Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Avrutin, S. Psycholinguistic Investigations in the Theory of Reference. Ph.D. Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA, 1994. [Google Scholar]
- Schaeffer, J. The Acquisition of Direct Object Scrambling and Clitic Placement; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2000. [Google Scholar]
- Onishi, K.; Baillargeon, R. Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science 2005, 308, 255–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Woodward, A.L.; Sommerville, J.A.; Gerson, S.; Henderson, A.M.E.; Buresh, J. The emergence of intention attribution in infancy. In The Psychology of Learning and Motivation: Vol 51; Ross, B.H., Ed.; Elsevier Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 2009; pp. 187–222. [Google Scholar]
- Baker, N.D.; Greenfield, P.M. The Development of New and Old Information in Young Children’s Early Language. Lang. Sci. 1988, 10, 3–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grinstead, J. Subjects and Interface Delay in Child Spanish and Catalan. Language 2004, 80, 40–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grinstead, J.; Spinner, P. The clausal left periphery in child Spanish and German. Probus 2009, 21, 51–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grinstead, J.; De la Mora, J.; Pratt, A.; Flores, B. Temporal Interface Delay and Root Nonfinite Verbs in Spanish-Speaking Children with Specific Language Impairment: Evidence from the Grammaticality Choice Task. In Hispanic Child Languages: Typical and Impaired Development; Grinstead, J., Ed.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2009; pp. 239–263. [Google Scholar]
- Grinstead, J.; Baron, A.; Vega-Mendoza, M.; De la Mora, J.; Cantú-Sánchez, M.; Flores, B. Tense marking and spontaneous speech measures in Spanish specific language impairment: A discriminant function analysis. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2013, 56, 352–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, N. Questions of Form and Interpretation. Linguist. Anal. 1975, 1, 75–109. [Google Scholar]
- Jackendoff, R. Consciouness and the Computational Mind; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1987. [Google Scholar]
- Jackendoff, R. The Architecture of the Language Faculty; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Grinstead, J.; Lintz, P.; Vega-Mendoza, M.; De la Mora, J.; Cantú-Sánchez, M.; Flores-Avalos, B. Evidence of optional infinitive verbs in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking children with SLI. Lingua 2014, 140, 52–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rice, M.L.; Wexler, K.; Hershberger, S. Tense over Time: The Longitudinal Course of Tense Acquisition in Children with Specific Language Impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1998, 41, 1412–1431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rice, M.L.; Wexler, K.; Redmond, S.M. Grammaticality Judgments of an Extended Optional Infinitive Grammar: Evidence from English-Speaking Children with Specific Language Impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1999, 42, 943–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, N. The Minimalist Program; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Grinstead, J.; Cantu-Sanchez, M.; Flores-Avalos, B. Canonical and Epenthetic Plural Marking in Spanish-Speaking Children with Specific Language Impairment. Lang. Acquis. 2008, 15, 329–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bedore, L.; Leonard, L. “Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with SLI”: Erratum. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2002, 45, 1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bedore, L.; Leonard, L. Verb inflections and noun phrase morphology in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2005, 26, 195–225. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Picallo, M.C. Gender and Number in Romance. Lingue Ling. 2008, 7, 47–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jakubowicz, C.; Nash, L. Functional Categories and Syntactic Operations in (Ab)normal Language Acquisition. Brain Lang. 2001, 77, 321–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anderson, R.T.; Souto, S.M. The use of articles by monolingual Puerto Rican Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2005, 26, 621–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Restrepo, M.A.; Gutierrez-Clellen, V.F. Article Use in Spanish-Speaking Children with Specific Language Impairment. J. Child Lang. 2001, 28, 433–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grinstead, J.; Lintz, P.; Pratt, A.; Vega-Mendoza, M.; De la Mora, J.; Cantú-Sánchez, M.; Flores-Avalos, B. Null Subject Licensing in Monolingual Spanish SLI: A Discriminant Function Analysis. Languages 2018, 3, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Leonard, L.B.; Eyer, J.A.; Bedore, L.M.; Grela, B.G. Three Accounts of the Grammatical Morpheme Difficulties of English-Speaking Children with Specific Language Impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1997, 40, 741–753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rice, M.; Wexler, K. Test. of Early Grammatical Impairment-the Examiner’s Manual; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Jakubowicz, C.; Roulet, L. Do French-Speaking Children with SLI Present a Selective Deficit on Tense? In BUCLD Proceedings 28; Brugos, A., Micciulla, L., Smith, C.E., Eds.; Cascadilla Press: Somerville, MA, USA, 2004; pp. 256–266. [Google Scholar]
- Wexler, K.; Schaeffer, J.; Bol, G. Verbal Syntax and Morphology in Typically Developing Dutch Children and Children with SLI: How Developmental Data Can Play an Important Role in Morphological Theory. Syntax 2004, 7, 148–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leonard, L.B.; Dromi, E.; Adam, G.; Zadunaisky-Ehrlich, S. Tense and Finiteness in the Speech of Children with Specific Language Impairment Acquiring Hebrew. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2000, 35, 319–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- De la Mora, J.; Paradis, J.; Grinstead, J.; Flores, B.; Cantú, M. Object Clitics in Spanish Speaking Children with and without SLI. In Symposium for Research on Child Language Disorders; University of Wisconsin: Madison, AL, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- De la Mora, J. Direct Object Clitics and Determiners in Spanish Specific Language Impairment. Master’s Thesis, University of Alberta, Edmonton, AB, Canada, 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Leonard, L. Language learnability and specific language impairment in children. Appl. Psycholinguist. 1989, 10, 179–202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Aguilar-Mediavilla, E.; Sanz-Torrent, M.; Serra-Raventós, M. Influence of phonology on morpho-syntax in Romance languages in children with Specific Language Impairment (SLI). Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2007, 42, 325–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Oetting, J.B. Language-Impaired and Normally Developing Children’s Acquisition of English Plural. Ph.D. Thesis, University of Kansas, Kansas, KS, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Rice, M.L.; Oetting, J.B. Morphological Deficits of Children with SLI: Evaluation of Number Marking and Agreement. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 1993, 36, 1249–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rice, M.L.; Wexler, K. Toward Tense as a Clinical Marker of Specific Language Impairment in English-Speaking Children. J. Speech Hear. Res. 1996, 39, 1239–1257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Svirsky, M.A.; Stallings, L.M.; Lento, C.L.; Ying, E.; Leonard, L.B. Grammatical Morphologic Development in Pediatric Cochlear Implant Users May Be Affected by the Perceptual Prominence of the Relevant Markers. Ann. Otol. Rhinol. Laryngol. 2002, 189, 109–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Merino, B. Language development in normal and language handicapped Spanish-speaking children. Hisp. J. Behav. Sci. 1983, 5, 379–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morgan, G.; Restrepo, M.A.; Auza, A. Varaibility in the grammatical profiles of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. In Hispanic Child Languages: Typical and Impaired Development; Grinstead, J., Ed.; John Benjamins: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2009; pp. 283–302. [Google Scholar]
- Bedore, L.; Leonard, L. Grammatical Morphology Deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2001, 44, 905–924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacobson, P.; Schwartz, R. Morphology in incipient bilingual Spanish-speaking preschool children with specific language impairment. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2002, 23, 23–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacobson, P. The effects of language impairment on the use of direct object pronouns and verb inflections in heritage Spanish speakers: A look at attrition, incomplete acquisition and maintenance. Biling-Lang. Cogn. 2012, 15, 22–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bosch, L.; Serra, M. Grammatical Morphology Deficits of Spanish-Speaking Children with Specific Language Impairment. Amst. Ser. Child Lang. Dev. 1997, 6, 33–45. [Google Scholar]
- Luck, S. An introduction to the Event-Related Potential Technique; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Silva-Pereyra, J.; Gutiérrez-Sigut, E.; Carreiras, M. An ERP study of coreference in Spanish: Semantic and grammatical gender cues. Psychophysiology 2012, 49, 1401–1411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Barber, H.; Carreiras, M. Grammatical Gender and Number Agreement in Spanish: An ERP Comparison. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 137–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Molinaro, N.; Barber, H.; Carreiras, M. Grammatical agreement processing in reading: ERP findings and future directions. Cortex 2001, 47, 908–930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bornkessdel-Schlesewsky, I.; Schlesewsky, M. The extended argument dependency model: A neurocognitive approach to sentence comprehension across language. Psychol. Rev. 2006, 113, 787–821. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Friederici, A.; Weissenborn, J. Mapping sentence form onto meaning: The syntax–semantic interface. Brain Res. 2007, 1146, 50–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Friederici, A.; Hahne, A.; Saddy, D. Distinct neurophysiological patterns reflecting aspects of syntactic complexity and syntactic repair. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 2002, 31, 45–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaan, E.; Swaab, T. Electrophysiological evidence for serial sentence processing: A comparison between non-preferred and ungrammatical continuations. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2003, 17, 621–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hahne, A.; Eckstein, K.; Friederici, A. Brain signatures of syntactic and semantic processes during children’s language development. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2004, 16, 1302–1318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Oberecker, R.; Friedrich, M.; Friederici, A. Neural correlates of syntactic processing in two-year-olds. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005, 17, 1667–1678. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Silva-Pereyra, J.; Rivera-Gaxiola, M.; Kuhl, P.K. An event-related brain potential study of sentence comprehension in preschoolers: Semantic and morphosyntactic processing. Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res. 2005, 23, 247–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Weber-Fox, C.; Leonard, L.; Wray, A.; Tomblin, J.B. Electrophysiological correlates of rapid auditory and linguistic processing in adolescents with specific language impairment. Brain Lang. 2010, 115, 162–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sabisch, B.; Hahne, A.; Glass, E.; von Suchodoletz, W.; Friederici, A. Children with specific language impairment: The role of prosodic processes in explaining difficulties in processing syntactic information. Brain Res. 2009, 1261, 37–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fonteneau, E.; van der Lely, H.K. Electrical brain responses in language-impaired children reveal grammar-specific deficits. PLoS ONE 2008, 3, e1832. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Epstein, B.; Hestvik, A.; Shafer, V.; Schwartz, R. ERPs reveal atypical processing of subject versus object Wh-questions in children with specific language impairment. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2013, 48, 351–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brown, L.; Sherbenou, R.J.; Johnsen, S.K. Test. de Inteligencia No-Verbal TONI-2 [Test. of Nonverbal Intelligence 2]; TEA Press: Madrid, España, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Bishop, D.V.; Snowling, M.J.; Thompson, P.A.; Greenhalgh, T.; CATALISE Consortium. Phase 2 of CATALISE: A multinational and multidisciplinary Delphi consensus study of problems with language development: Terminology. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2017, 58, 1068–1080. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Puyuelo, M.; Renom, J.; Solanas, A.; Wiig, E. Evaluación de Lenguaje BLOC Screening [Language Assessment BLOC]. Manual de Usuario; Editorial Masson: Barcelona, España, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Puyuelo, M.; Salavera, C.; Wiig, E. Aportaciones a la evaluación del lenguaje mediante BLOC-C y BLOC-SR [Usefulness of the BLOC-C and BLOC-SR tests in language assessment]. Rev. Logop. Foniatría Audiol. 2013, 33, 83–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Alva, E.; Hernández-Padilla, E. La Producción del Lenguaje de Niños Mexicanos. Un Estudio Transversal de Niños de 5 a 12 años [Language Production of Mexican Children. A Cross-Sectional Study of Children Aged 5 to 12 Years]; Editorial UNAM: Mexico City, Mexico, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Alva, E.; Arias-Trejo, N.; Mazón, N.; Mejía, I.; Hernández-Padilla, E.; Carrión, R.; Pérez, B. ¿Cómo Usan los Niños las Palabras? [How do Children Use Words?]; Editorial UNAM: CDMX, México, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Restrepo, M.A.; Gorin, J.; Gray, S. Screening Spanish-speaking children for language impairment: Results from a scale development grant. In Inaugural Bilingual Research Conference; University of Houston: Houston, TX, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
- Kapantzoglou, M.; Thompson, M.; Gray, S.; Restrepo, M.A. Assessing measurement invariance for Spanish sentence repetition and morphology elicitation tasks. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2016, 59, 254–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gray, S. The relationship between phonological memory, receptive vocabulary, and fast mapping in young children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2006, 49, 955–969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nash, M.; Donaldson, M. Word Learning in Children with Vocabulary Deficits. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2005, 48, 439–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spaulding, T.J.; Hosmer, S.; Schechtman, C. Investigating the interchangeability and diagnostic utility of the PPVT-III and PPVT-IV for children with and without SLI. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2013, 15, 453–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Montgomery, J.W. Verbal working memory in sentence comprehension in children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2000, 43, 293–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conti-Ramsden, G. Processing and linguistic markers in young children with specific language impairment. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2003, 46, 1029–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gathercole, S. Non-word repetition and word learning: The nature of the relationship. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2006, 27, 513–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vugs, B.; Knoors, H.; Cuperus, J.; Hendriks, M.; Verhoeven, L. Interactions between working memory and language in young children with specific language impairment (SLI). Child Neuropsychol. 2016, 22, 955–978. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Noonan, N.; Redmond, S.M.; Archibald, L. Contributions of Children’s Linguistic and Working Memory Proficiencies to Their Judgments of Grammaticality. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2014, 57, 979–989. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Russell, B. On Denoting. Mind 1905, 66, 479–493. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heim, I. Articles and Definiteness. In Semantics: An International Handbook of Contemporary Research; Stechow, A.V., Wunderlich, D., Eds.; De Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 1991. [Google Scholar]
Study | Age (Years Old) | Method | % Agreement Errors | Different from Controls? |
---|---|---|---|---|
Monolingual Spanish | ||||
Merino [64] | 5–8 | Elicited Production | 25% | Yes |
De la Mora [57] | 5.3 | Elicited Production–Prompted Response | 17% | Yes |
Morgan et al. [65] | 5.3 | Cloze Test | 11% | Yes a |
Jackson-Maldonado and Maldonado [3] | 7.3 | Frog Story–Story Retell | Mean number of errors = 1.58 | No |
Bilingual Spanish-English | ||||
Bedore and Leonard [44,66] | 3;11–5;6 | Elicited Production | 38% b | Yes |
Bedore and Leonard [45] | 3;11–5;6 | Spontaneous Production | 8% c | Yes |
Jacobson and Schwartz [67] | 4;7 | Elicited Production | 63% feminine 12% masculine | Yes |
Jacobson [68] | Lower grades—7.2 years old Higher grades—10.9 years old | Elicited Production | 48% d 46% e 33% f 32% g | Yes Yes Yes No |
Bilingual Spanish-Catalan | ||||
Bosch and Serra [69] | 7;6 | Spontaneous Production | 18.25% | No |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Roa-Rojas, P.; Grinstead, J.; Silva-Pereyra, J.; Fernández, T.; Rodríguez-Camacho, M. Syntactic Gender Agreement Processing on Direct-Object Clitics by Spanish-Speaking Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Evidence from ERP. Children 2021, 8, 175. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8030175
Roa-Rojas P, Grinstead J, Silva-Pereyra J, Fernández T, Rodríguez-Camacho M. Syntactic Gender Agreement Processing on Direct-Object Clitics by Spanish-Speaking Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Evidence from ERP. Children. 2021; 8(3):175. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8030175
Chicago/Turabian StyleRoa-Rojas, Paloma, John Grinstead, Juan Silva-Pereyra, Thalía Fernández, and Mario Rodríguez-Camacho. 2021. "Syntactic Gender Agreement Processing on Direct-Object Clitics by Spanish-Speaking Children with Developmental Language Disorder: Evidence from ERP" Children 8, no. 3: 175. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8030175