How Living in Vulnerable Conditions Undermines Cognitive Development: Evidence from the Pediatric Population of Guatemala
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Cognitive Development Under Vulnerable Conditions
1.2. Guatemalan Context
1.3. Aim of Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Instruments
2.3. Procedure
2.4. Data Analysis
3. Results
4. Discussion
Limitations and Future Directions
5. Conclusions
- Vulnerable children and adolescents underperformed in attention, language, and some subtests of executive functions tasks, in comparison with non-vulnerable participants;
- Effect sizes were considerably large in the case of the PPVT-III language task and both forms of TMT executive function task, suggesting the need for specific normative data designed for children and adolescents from these backgrounds;
- There were no significant differences between vulnerable and non-vulnerable participants in terms of performance in motor skills and memory tasks;
- Bilingualism of a high percentage of the vulnerable sample may have played an important role in the obtained results, especially in executive functions’ tasks;
- Vulnerability may not be a risk factor for the development of all cognitive domains, since vulnerable participants obtained equal or betters scores in some subtests;
- In future studies, it would be important to consider the reading ability of participants and to further study how bilingualism may interfere in the cognitive assessment of vulnerable populations.
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Appendix A.1. Motor Skills and Memory Tests
Appendix A.2. Attention Tests
Appendix A.3. Language Tests
Appendix A.4. Executive Function Tests
References
- United Nations. Programa de las Naciones Unidas para el Desarrollo. Available online: http://hdr.undp.org/en/2019-MPI (accessed on 20 April 2020).
- United Nations International Children’s Emergency Fund UNICEF. The changing face of malnutrition. Available online: https://features.unicef.org/state-of-the-worlds-children-2019-nutrition/ (accessed on 6 March 2020).
- United Nations General Assembly. Transforming our world: The 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development. Available online: https://sustainabledevelopment.un.org/post2015/transformingourworld (accessed on 20 April 2020).
- United Nations. The Sustainable Development Agenda—United Nations Sustainable Development. Available online: https://www.un.org/sustainabledevelopment/development-agenda/ (accessed on 6 March 2020).
- Saric, J.; Blaettler, D.; Bonfoh, B.; Hostettler, S.; Jimenez, E.; Kiteme, B.; Koné, I.; Lys, J.-A.; Masanja, H.; Steinger, E.; et al. Leveraging research partnerships to achieve the 2030 Agenda: Experiences from North-South cooperation. GAIA Ecol. Perspect. Sci. Soc. 2019, 28, 143–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schneider, F.; Kläy, A.; Zimmermann, A.B.; Buser, T.; Ingalls, M.; Messerli, P. How can science support the 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development? Four tasks to tackle the normative dimension of sustainability. Sustain. Sci. 2019, 14, 1593–1604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Johnson, S.B.; Riis, J.L.; Noble, K.G. State of the Art Review: Poverty and the Developing Brain. Pediatrics 2016, 137, e20153075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Selye, H. Stress and the general adaptation syndrome. Br. Med. J. 1950, 1, 1383–1392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Santiago, C.D.; Wolff, B.C.; Wadsworth, M.E. The Stress of Growing up Poor: Pathways to Compromised Development for Low-Income Children and Adolescents. In Handbook of Stress: Causes, Effects and Control; Heidenreich, P., Pruter, I., Eds.; Nova Science Publishers Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 229–260. [Google Scholar]
- Shonkoff, J.P.; Garner, A.S. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics 2012, 129, e232–e246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Blair, C.; Raver, C.C. Poverty, Stress, and Brain Development: New Directions for Prevention and Intervention. Acad. Pediatr. 2016, 16 (Suppl. 3), S30–S36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Company-Córdoba, R.; Gómez-Baya, D.; López-Gaviño, F.; Ibáñez-Alfonso, J.A. Mental Health, Quality of Life and Violence Exposure in Low-Socioeconomic Status Children and Adolescents of Guatemala. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, G.W.; Kim, P. Multiple risk exposure as a potential explanatory mechanism for the socioeconomic status-health gradient. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2010, 1186, 174–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Luby, J.; Belden, A.; Botteron, K.; Marrus, N.; Harms, M.P.; Babb, C.; Nishino, T.; Barch, D. The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: The mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatr. 2013, 167, 1135–1142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mackey, A.P.; Finn, A.S.; Leonard, J.A.; Jacoby-Senghor, D.S.; West, M.R.; Gabrieli, C.F.O.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Neuroanatomical correlates of the income-achievement gap. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 26, 925–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Noble, K.G.; Houston, S.M.; Kan, E.; Sowell, E.R. Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Dev. Sci. 2012, 15, 516–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yu, Q.; Daugherty, A.M.; Anderson, D.M.; Nishimura, M.; Brush, D.; Hardwick, A.; Lacey, W.; Raz, S.; Ofen, N. Socioeconomic status and hippocampal volume in children and young adults. Dev. Sci. 2018, 21, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ison, M.S.; Greco, C.; Korzeniowski, C.; Morelato, G. Selective Attention: A comparative study on Argentine students from different socioeconomic contexts. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 13, 343–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schibli, K.; Wong, K.; Hedayati, N.; D’Angiulli, A. Attending, learning, and socioeconomic disadvantage: Developmental cognitive and social neuroscience of resilience and vulnerability. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2017, 1396, 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steinmayr, R.; Ziegler, M.; Träuble, B. Do intelligence and sustained attention interact in predicting academic achievement? Learn. Individ. Differ. 2010, 20, 14–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hackman, D.A.; Farah, M.J. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends Cog. Sci. 2009, 13, 65–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Mezzacappa, E. Alerting, Orienting, and Executive Attention: Developmental Properties and Sociodemographic Correlates in an Epidemiological Sample of Young, Urban Children. Child Dev. 2004, 75, 1373–1386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stevens, C.; Lauinger, B.; Neville, H. Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: An event-related brain potential study. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 634–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Clearfield, M.W.; Jedd, K.E. The Effects of Socio-Economic Status on Infant Attention. Infant Child Dev. 2013, 22, 53–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lupien, S.J.; King, S.; Meaney, M.J. Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Dev. Psychopathol. 2001, 13, 653–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Da Rosa Piccolo, L.; Arteche, A.X.; Fonseca, R.P.; Grassi-Oliveira, R.; Salles, J.F. Influence of family socioeconomic status on IQ, language, memory and executive functions of Brazilian children. Psicol. Reflex. Crit. 2016, 29, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Farah, M.J.; Shera, D.M.; Savage, J.H.; Betancourt, L.; Giannetta, J.M.; Brodsky, N.L.; Malmud, E.K.; Hurt, H. Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Res. 2006, 1110, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Leonard, J.A.; Mackey, A.P.; Finn, A.S.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Differential effects of socioeconomic status on working and procedural memory systems. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Matute-Villaseñor, E.; Sanz, A.; Díaz, E.; Roselli, M.; Ardila, A. Influencia del nivel educativo de los padres, el tipo de escuela y el sexo en el desarrollo de la atención y la memoria. Rev. Lationam Psicol. 2009, 41, 257–276. [Google Scholar]
- Burneo-Garcés, C.; Cruz-Quintana, F.; Pérez-García, M.; Fernández-Alcántara, M.; Fasfous, A.; Pérez-Marfil, M.N. Interaction between Socioeconomic Status and Cognitive Development in Children Aged 7, 9, and 11 Years: A Cross-Sectional Study. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2019, 44, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferreira, L.; Godinez, I.; Gabbard, C.; Vieira, J.L.L.; Caçola, P. Motor development in school-age children is associated with the home environment including socioeconomic status. Child Care Health Dev. 2018, 44, 801–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caldwell, B.M.; Bradley, R.H. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment: Administration Manual; Family & Human Dynamics Research Institute, Arizona State University: Tempe, AZ, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Bobbio, T.G.; Morcillo, A.M.; Filho, A.D.A.B.; Gonçalves, V.M.G. Factors Associated with Inadequate Fine Motor Skills in Brazilian Students of Different Socioeconomic Status. Percept. Mot. Skills 2007, 105 (Suppl. 3), 1187–1195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Durán, E.; Pinillos-Patiño, Y.; Herazo-Beltrán, Y.; Galeano-Muñoz, L.; Prieto, E. Determinantes del desempeño psicomotor en escolares de Barranquilla, Colombia. Rev. Salud Pública 2017, 19, 297–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Musso, M. Executive functions: A study about the impact of the poverty on executive performance. Interdisciplinaria 2010, 27, 95–110. [Google Scholar]
- Noble, K.; Mccandliss, B.; Farah, M. Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, 464–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, E.; Tian, C. Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. J. Commun. Disord. 2005, 38, 271–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Reynolds, S.A.; Andersen, C.; Behrman, J.; Singh, A.; Stein, A.D.; Benny, L.; Crookston, B.T.; Cueto, S.; Dearden, K.; Georgiadis, A.; et al. Disparities in children’s vocabulary and height in relation to household wealth and parental schooling: A longitudinal study in four low- and middle-income countries. SSM Popul. Health 2017, 3, 767–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Huttenlocher, J.; Vasilyeva, M.; Waterfall, H.R.; Vevea, J.L.; Hedges, L.V. The varieties of speech to young children. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 43, 1062–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fernald, A.; Marchman, V.A.; Weisleder, A. SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Dev. Sci. 2013, 16, 234–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Spencer, S.; Clegg, J.; Stackhouse, J. Language and disadvantage: A comparison of the language abilities of adolescents from two different socioeconomic areas. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2012, 47, 274–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar]
- Stumm, S.V.; Plomin, R. Intelligence Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence. Intelligence 2015, 48, 30–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Last, B.S.; Lawson, G.M.; Breiner, K.; Steinberg, L.; Farah, J. Childhood socioeconomic status and executive function in childhood and beyond. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0202964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arán-Filippetti, V. Funciones ejecutivas en niños escolarizados. Av. Psicol. Latinoam. 2011, 29, 98–113. [Google Scholar]
- Arán-filippetti, V.; De Minzi, C.R. A Structural Analysis of Executive Functions and Socioeconomic Status in School-Age Children: Cognitive Factors as Effect Mediators. J. Genet. Psychol. 2012, 173, 393–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Noble, K.G.; Norman, M.F.; Farah, M.J. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Dev. Sci. 2005, 8, 74–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vrantsidis, D.M.; Clark, C.A.C.; Chevalier, N.; Espy, K.A.; Wiebe, S.A. Socioeconomic status and executive function in early childhood: Exploring proximal mechanisms. Dev. Sci. 2020, 23, e12917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lawson, G.M.; Farah, M.J. Executive Function as a Mediator between SES and Academic Achievement throughout Childhood. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2017, 41, 94–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Korzeniowski, C.; Cupani, M.; Ison, M.; Difabio, H. School performance and poverty: The mediating role of executive functions. EJREP 2016, 14, 474–494. [Google Scholar]
- Tine, M. Working Memory Differences between Children Living in Rural and Urban Poverty. J. Cog. Dev. 2013, 15, 599–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duval, E.; Garfinkel, S.; Swain, J.; Evans, G.; Blackburn, E.; Angstadt, M.; Sripada, C.S.; Liberzon, I. Childhood Poverty is Associated with Altered Hippocampal Function and Visuospatial Memory in Adulthood. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 23, 39–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Briceno, A.C.L.; De Feyter, J.J.; Winsler, A. The school readiness of children born to low-income, adolescent Latinas in Miami. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 2013, 83, 430–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- International Work Group for Indigenous Affairs. Guatemala—IWGIA—International Work Group for Indigenous Affairs. Available online: https://www.iwgia.org/en/guatemala.html (accessed on 17 April 2020).
- El Informe de Desarrollo Humano 2014 del PNUD Sugiere la Creación de un Fondo Monetario Regional Para Reducir la Vulnerabilidad Financiera, El PNUD en Guatemala. Available online: https://www.gt.undp.org/content/guatemala/es/home/presscenter/pressreleases/2014/07/24/el-informe-de-desarrollo-humano-2014-del-pnud-sugiere-la-creaci-n-de-un-fondo-monetario-regional-para-reducir-la-vulnerabilidad-financiera-.html (accessed on 21 October 2020).
- Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations. Overview of Rural Poverty in Latin America and the Caribbean: Solutions for Eliminating Rural Poverty in the 21 st Century. Available online: http://www.fao.org/3/ca2275en/CA2275EN.pdf (accessed on 21 October 2020).
- Freeman, H.E.; Klein, R.E.; Kagan, J.; Yarbrough, C. Relations between nutrition and cognition in rural Guatemala. Am. J. Public Health 1977, 67, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Freeman, H.E.; Klein, R.E.; Townsend, J.W.; Lechtig, A. Nutrition and Cognitive Development among Rural Guatemalan Children. Am. J. Public Health 1980, 70, 1277–1285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Carlson, B. Children Exposed to Intimate Partner Violence: Research Findings and Implications for Intervention. Trauma Violence Abus. 2000, 1, 321–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mueller, I.; Tronick, E. Early Life Exposure to Violence: Developmental Consequences on Brain and Behavior. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Buchweitz, A.; Azeredo De, L.A.; Sanvicente-vieira, B.; Cará, V.M.; Esper, N.B.; Soder, R.B.; da Costa, J.C.; Portuguez, M.W.; Franco, A.R.; Grassi-Oliveira, R. Violence and Latin-American preadolescents: A study of social brain function and cortisol levels. Dev. Sci. 2019, 22, e12799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rivera, D.; Arango-Lasprilla, J.C. Methodology for the development of normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric populations. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 581–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Arango-Lasprilla, J.C.; Rivera, D.; Ertl, M.M.; Muñoz-Mancilla, J.M.; García-Guerrero, C.E.; Rodriguez-Irizarry, W.; Arelis, A.A.; Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y.; Nevado, M.B.; Coto, M.V.; et al. Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure—Copy and immediate recall (3 minutes): Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric populations. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 593–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rivera, D.; Olabarrieta-Landa, L.; Rabago- Barajas, B.V.; Irías-Escher, M.J.; Saracostti-Schwartzman, M.; Ferrer-Cascales, R.; Martín-Lobo, P.; Calderón, J.; Uriarte, M.A.; Guia, A.P.; et al. Newly developed Learning and Verbal Memory Test (TAMV-I): Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 695–706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Arango-Lasprilla, J.C.; Rivera, D.; Trapp, S.; Jiménez-Pérez, C.; Hernández-Carrillo, C.L.; Pohlenz-Amador, S.; Ver-gara-Moragues, E.; Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y.; Rodriguez-Irizarry, W.; De La Cadena, C.G.; et al. Symbol Digit Modalities Test: Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 639–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rivera, D.; Salinas, C.; Ramos-Usuga, D.; Delgado-Mejía, I.D.; Vasallo-Key, Y.; Hernández-Agurcia, G.P.; Vásquez, J.V.; García-Guerrero, C.; De La Cadena, C.G.; Barajas, B.R.; et al. Concentration Endurance Test (d2): Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 661–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Olabarrieta-Landa, L.; Rivera, D.; Ibáñez-Alfonso, J.A.; Albaladejo-Blázquez, N.; Martín-Lobo, P.; Delgado-Mejía, I.D.; Lara, L.; Barajas, B.R.; Salgado, A.R.; Quispe, L.P.; et al. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III: Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 687–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Olabarrieta-Landa, L.; Rivera, D.; Rodríguez-Lorenzana, A.; Pohlenz-Amador, S.; García-Guerrero, C.E.; Padilla-López, A.; Sánchez-SanSegundo, M.; Velázquez-Cardoso, J.; Marante, J.D.; Caparros-Gonzalez, R.; et al. Shortened Version of the Token Test: Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. Neurorehabilitation 2017, 41, 649–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Olabarrieta-Landa, L.; Rivera, D.; Lara, L.; Rute-Pérez, S.; Rodríguez-Lorenzana, A.; Galarza-del-Angel, J.; Guia, A.P.; Ferrer-Cascales, R.; Velázquez-Cardoso, J.; Varillas, A.C.; et al. Verbal fluency tests: Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 673–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Arango-Lasprilla, J.C.; Rivera, D.; Ramos-Usuga, D.; Vergara-Moragues, E.; Montero-López, E.; Adana-Díaz, L.A.; Arelis, A.A.; García-Guerrero, C.; De La Cadena, C.G.; Espezúa, X.L.; et al. Trail Making Test: Normative data for the Latin American Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 627–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Arango-Lasprilla, J.C.; Rivera, D.; Nicholls, E.; Aguayo-Arelis, A.; García de la Cadena, C.; Peñalver-Guia, A.I.; Vergara-Moragues, E.; Rodriguez-Lorenzana, A.; Marín-Morales, A.; Soto-Añari, M.; et al. Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (M-WCST): Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 617–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Rivera, D.; Morlett-Paredes, A.; Peñalver-Guia, A.I.; Irías-Escher, M.J.; Soto-Añari, M.; Aguayo-Arelis, A.; Rute-Pérez, S.; Rodriguez-Lorenzana, A.; Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y.; Albaladejo-Blazquez, N.; et al. Stroop Color-Word Interference Test: Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 605–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- IBM Corps. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 26.0; IBM Corps: Armonk, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Reifman, A.; Keyton, K. Winsorize. In Encyclopedia of Research Design; Salkind, N.J., Ed.; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2010; pp. 1636–1638. [Google Scholar]
- Frey, P.D.; Pinelli, B. Visual Discrimination and Visuomotor Integration among Two Classes of Brazilian Children. Percept. Mot. Skills 1991, 72, 847–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Scott, A.B.; Reed, R.G.; Garcia-Willingham, N.E.; Lawrence, K.A.; Segerstrom, S.C. Lifespan Socioeconomic Context: Associations ith Cognitive Functioning in Later Life. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2019, 74, 113–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Raffington, L.; Prindle, J.; Keresztes, A.; Binder, J.; Heim, C.; Shing, Y.L. Blunted cortisol stress reactivity in low-income children relates to lower memory function. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2018, 90, 110–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brandes-Aitken, A.; Braren, S.; Swingler, M.; Voegtline, K.; Blair, C. Sustained attention in infancy: A foundation for the development of multiple aspects of self-regulation for children in poverty. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2019, 184, 192–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Machlin, L.; McLaughlin, K.A.; Sheridan, M.A. Brain structure mediates the association between socioeconomic status and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Sci. 2020, 23, e12844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- D’Angiulli, A.; Lipina, S.J.; Olesinska, A. Explicit and implicit issues in the developmental cognitive neuroscience of social inequality. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schrieff-Elson, L.E.; Ockhuizen, J.R.H.; During, G.; Thomas, K.G.F. Attention-training with children from socioeconomically disadvantaged backgrounds in Cape Town. J. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 2017, 29, 147–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwab, J.F.; Lew-Williams, C. Language learning, socioeconomic status, and child-directed speech. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2016, 7, 264–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arán-Filippetti, V. Fluidez verbal según tipo de tarea, intervalo de tiempo y estrato socioeconómico, en niños escolarizados. An. Psicol. 2011, 27, 816–826. [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, E. Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Biling Lang. Cogn. 2009, 12, 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Meir, N.; Armon-lotem, S. Independent and Combined Effects of Socioeconomic Status (SES) and Bilingualism on Children’s Vocabulary and Verbal Short-Term Memory. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Pearce, W.M.; Flanagan, K. Language abilities of Indigenous and non-Indigenous Australian children from low socioeconomic backgrounds in their first year of school. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2019, 21, 212–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hackman, D.A.; Gallop, R.; Evans, G.W.; Farah, M.J. Socioeconomic status and executive function: Developmental trajectories and mediation. Dev. Sci. 2015, 18, 686–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Korkman, M.; Kirk, U.; Kemp, S. NEPSY—Second Edition (NEPSY-II), 2nd ed.; Harcourt Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Hartanto, A.; Toh, W.X.; Yang, H. Bilingualism Narrows Socioeconomic Disparities in Executive Functions and Self-Regulatory Behaviors during Early Childhood: Evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study. Child Dev. 2019, 90, 1215–1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blom, E.; Küntay, A.C.; Messer, M.; Verhagen, J.; Leseman, P. The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish–Dutch children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014, 128, 105–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Engel de Abreu, P.; Cruz-Santos, A.; Tourinho, C.; Martin, R.; Bialystok, E. Bilingualism Enriches the Poor: Enhanced Cognitive Control in Low-Income Minority Children. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 1364–1371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Calvo, A.; Bialystok, E. Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition 2014, 130, 278–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- White, L.J.; Greenfield, D.B. Executive functioning in Spanish- and English-speaking Head Start preschoolers. Dev. Sci. 2017, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krizman, J.; Skoe, E.; Kraus, N. Bilingual enhancements have no socioeconomic boundaries. Dev. Sci. 2016, 19, 881–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rey, A. REY: Test de Copia y de Reproducción de Memoria de Figuras Geométricas Complejas; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Cortés, S.; Galindo, J.F.; Villa, M.G.; Salvador, C.J. La Figura Compleja de Rey para niños: Propiedades psicométricas. Salud Ment. 1997, 20, 17–20. [Google Scholar]
- Hooper, H.E. Hooper Visual Organization Test (HVOT); Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler, D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised; Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.: Toronto, ON, Canada, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Smith, A. Manual de Test de Símbolos y Dígitos SDMT. Publicaciones de Psicología Aplicada; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Strober, L.; DeLuca, J.; Benedict, R.H.; Jacobs, A.; Cohen, J.A.; Chiaravalloti, N.; Hudson, L.D.; Rudick, R.A.; LaRocca, N.G. Symbol Digit Modalities Test: A valid clinical trial endpoint for measuring cognition in multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. 2019, 25, 1781–1790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brickenkamp, R. Manual del Test de Atención d2; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Bates, M.E.; Lemay, E.P.J. The d2 Test of attention: Construct validity and extensions in scoring techniques. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2004, 10, 392–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brickenkamp, R.; Schmidt-Atzert, L.; Liepmann, D.; Zillmer, E. d2- R Test di Attenzione Concentrata [d2-R Test of Sustained Attention]; Hogrefe: Firenze, Italy, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Cuesta-Izquierdo, M.; de Iscar-Pérez, M.J.; Begega-Losa, M.A.; Mendez-López, M.; Alvarez-Pérez, L.; Solís, G.; Fernández Colomer, M.B.; Arias Perez, J.L. Psychometric properties of the d2 selective attention test in a sample of premature and born-at-term babies. Psicothema 2007, 19, 706–710. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Dunn, L.M.; Dunn, L.M.; Arribas, D. PPVT-III Peabody. Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes, 2nd ed.; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Williams, K.T.; Wang, J. Technical References to the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition (PPVT-III), 3rd ed.; American Guidance Service: Circle Pines, MN, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- Hayward, D.V.; Stewart, G.E.; Phillips, L.M.; Norris, S.P.; Lovell, M.A. Test review: Peabody picture vocabulary test–III (PPVT-III). Available online: http://www.uofaweb.ualberta.ca/elementaryed/ccrl.cfm (accessed on 10 October 2020).
- Henninger, N. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- De Renzi, E.; Faglioni, P. Development of a shortened version of the Token Test. Cortex 1978, 14, 41–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallardo, G.; Guàrdia, J.; Villaseñor, T.; McNeil, M.R. Psychometric data for the revised token test in normally developing Mexican children ages 4–12 years. Arch Clin NeuroPsychol. Off J. Natl. Acad. Neuropsychol. 2011, 26, 225–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McNeil, M.R.; Pratt, S.R.; Szuminsky, N.; Sung, J.E.; Fossett, T.R.D.; Fassbinder, W.; Lim, K.Y. Reliability and validity of the computerized Revised Token Test: Comparison of reading and listening versions in persons with and without aphasia. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2015, 58, 311–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Strauss, E.; Sherman, E.; Spreen, O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. Administration, Norms and Commentary, 3rd ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Benton, A.L.; Hamsher, K.D. Multilingual Aphasia Examination; AJA Associates: Iowa City, IA, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
- Preston, J.L. Speech and literacy: The connection and the relevance to clinical populations. In Speech Disorders: Causes, Treatment and Social Effects; Harrison, A.E., Ed.; Languages and Linguistics Series: York, UK, 2010; pp. 43–73. [Google Scholar]
- Reitan, R.M.; Wolfson, D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation; Neuropsychology Press: Tucson, AZ, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
- Arbuthnott, K.; Frank, J. Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: Validation using a set-switching paradigm. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2000, 22, 518–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Giovagnoli, A.R.; Mascheroni, S.; Simoncelli, M.; Laiacona, M.; Capitani, E. Trail Making Test: Normative values from287 normal adult controls. Ital. J. Neurol. Sci. 1996, 17, 305–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schretlen, D.J. Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: M-WCST; Professional Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Heaton, R.K. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
- Ozonoff, S.; Mcevoy, R.E. A longitudinal-study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 1994, 6, 415–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Golden, C.J. Manual de Test de Colores y Palabras. Publicaciones de Psicología Aplicada; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, España, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Barreto, L.C.R.; Pulido, N.C.; Roa, C.A.P. Psychometric properties of the stroop color-word test in non-pathological colombian population. Univ. Psychol. 2016, 15, 255–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Vulnerable | Non-Vulnerable | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Tasks | n | M (SD) | n | M (SD) | df | t Statistic | CI | p | d |
ROCF | |||||||||
Copy | 143 | 29.2 (8.3) | 201 | 30.1 (6.2) | 249.5 | −1.1 | [2.5, 0.6] | 0.289 | 0.13 |
Memory | 143 | 18.2 (9.8) | 201 | 18.7 (7.2) | 245.5 | −0.5 | [−2.3, 1.4] | 0.587 | 0.06 |
TAMV-I | |||||||||
Verbal learning | 144 | 30.6 (8.0) | 203 | 31.7 (7.3) | 345 | −1.4 | [−3.0, 0.5] | 0.189 | 0.15 |
Memory delay | 144 | 8.8 (2.7) | 203 | 9.1 (2.1) | 260.2 | −1.2 | [−0.8, 0.2] | 0.207 | 0.13 |
Recognition | 144 | 11.1 (1.4) | 203 | 11.2 (1.3) | 345 | −1.1 | [−0.4, 0.1] | 0.276 | 0.07 |
Vulnerable | Non-Vulnerable | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Tasks | n | M (SD) | n | M (SD) | df | t Statistic | CI | p | d |
SDMT | 137 | 30.3 (14.8) | 203 | 34.3 (11.4) | 241.12 | −2.6 | [−6.9, −0.9] | 0.012 | 0.31 |
D2-CON | 137 | 93.3 (52.7) | 193 | 121.3 (40.7) | 244.49 | −5.2 | [−38.1, −17.1] | 0.001 | 0.61 |
Vulnerable | Non-Vulnerable | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Tasks | n | M (SD) | n | M (SD) | df | t Statistic | CI | p | d |
PPVT-III | 143 | 85.9 (30.0) | 199 | 109.2 (24.6) | 267.3 | −7.6 | [−29.2, −17.3] | 0.001 | 0.87 |
Token | 144 | 29 (3.8) | 203 | 31 (2.7) | 238.4 | −5.4 | [−2.7, −1.3] | 0.001 | 0.63 |
Verbal fluency | |||||||||
/f/ | 143 | 5.7 (4.1) | 203 | 7.1 (3.3) | 261.1 | −3.2 | [−2.1, −0.5] | 0.002 | 0.38 |
/s/ | 143 | 6.9 (4.0) | 203 | 7.7 (3.4) | 274.3 | −2.0 | [−1.6, 0.0] | 0.048 | 0.22 |
/a/ | 143 | 7.0 (3.9) | 203 | 7.8 (3.3) | 278.2 | −2.0 | [−1.6, 0.0] | 0.051 | 0.23 |
Animals | 144 | 13.7 (4.8) | 203 | 15(4.4) | 345 | −2.5 | [−2.3, −0.3] | 0.016 | 0.29 |
Fruits | 144 | 10.8 (3.2) | 203 | 10.7 (2.9) | 345 | 0.1 | [−0.7, 0.7] | 0.922 | 0.03 |
Vulnerable | Non-Vulnerable | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Tasks | n | M (SD) | n | M (SD) | df | t | CI | p | d |
Stroop | |||||||||
Word | 114 | 70.1 (23.7) | 190 | 74.4 (16.6) | 180.2 | −1.7 | [−9.2, 0.4] | 0.089 | 0.22 |
Color | 114 | 53.6 (14.2) | 190 | 52.4 (12.5) | 214.9 | 0.7 | [−2.1, 4.3] | 0.506 | 0.09 |
Word-color | 114 | 31.5 (8) | 190 | 29 (9.3) | 302 | 2.4 | [0.4, 4.5] | 0.014 | 0.28 |
Interference | 114 | 1.7 (7.5) | 190 | −1.5 (7.1) | 302 | 3.6 | [1.6, 4.9] | 0.001 | 0.44 |
TMT | |||||||||
A | 139 | 48 (22.6) | 203 | 30.7 (11.5) | 186.9 | 8.3 | [13.5, 21.6] | 0.001 | 1.03 |
B | 120 | 80.2 (36.8) | 203 | 57.9 (24.9) | 184.4 | 5.9 | [15.3, 29.3] | 0.001 | 0.75 |
M-WCST | |||||||||
Correct cat. | 143 | 4.6 (1.8) | 202 | 4 (1.8) | 343 | 3.0 | [0.2, 0.9] | 0.006 | 0.33 |
Pers. errors | 143 | 4.3 (4.8) | 202 | 4.2 (3.8) | 343 | 0.3 | [−0.8, 1.2] | 0.765 | 0.02 |
Total errors | 143 | 12.9 (10.6) | 202 | 15.2 (9.2) | 343 | −2.2 | [−4.4, −0.2] | 0.029 | 0.24 |
Children (6–11 Years) | Adolescents (12–17 Years) | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Tasks | Vulnerable | Non-Vulnerable | Vulnerable | Non-Vulnerable | ||||
n | M (SD) | n | M (SD) | n | M (SD) | n | M (SD) | |
ROCF | ||||||||
Copy | 86 | 25.33 (8.69) | 128 | 28.60 (6.69) | 57 | 35.09 (1.29) | 73 | 32.76 (4.09) |
Memory | 86 | 13.67 (8.70) | 128 | 16.26 (6.41) | 57 | 25.13 (6.83) | 73 | 23.09 (6.27) |
TAMV-I | ||||||||
Verbal learning | 87 | 28.32 (8.32) | 129 | 30.54 (7.71) | 57 | 33.95 (6.14) | 74 | 33.81 (6.16) |
Memory delay | 87 | 7.76 (2.79) | 129 | 8.78 (2.23) | 57 | 10.33 (1.39) | 74 | 9.66 (1.66) |
Recognition | 87 | 10.64 (1.63) | 129 | 11.01 (1.42) | 57 | 11.70 (0.53) | 74 | 11.58 (0.89) |
SDMT | 80 | 21 (10.58) | 129 | 28.94 (9.29) | 57 | 43.37 (8.66) | 74 | 43.59 (8.39) |
D2- CON | 80 | 62.59 (36.65) | 122 | 105.26 (34.44) | 57 | 136.39 (40.20) | 71 | 148.96 (35.65) |
PPVT-III | 86 | 69.81 (22.76) | 128 | 99.01 (21.28) | 57 | 110.07 (22.47) | 71 | 127.51 (18.90) |
Token | 87 | 27.70 (4.01) | 129 | 30.34 (2.69) | 57 | 31.05 (2.41) | 74 | 32.21 (2.17) |
Verbal fluency | ||||||||
/f/ | 86 | 3.64 (3.04) | 129 | 6.09 (2.88) | 57 | 8.89 (3.51) | 74 | 8.77 (3.30) |
/s/ | 86 | 5.15 (3.24) | 129 | 6.77 (3.03) | 57 | 9.58 (3.6) | 74 | 9.45 (3.43) |
/a/ | 86 | 5.48 (3.45) | 129 | 6.78 (2.93) | 57 | 9.37 (3.16) | 74 | 9.64 (3.24) |
Animals | 87 | 11.76 (4.11) | 129 | 13.29 (3.55) | 57 | 16.68 (4.11) | 74 | 17.91 (4.16) |
Fruits | 87 | 9.36 (3.14) | 129 | 9.82 (2.41) | 57 | 12.88 (1.98) | 74 | 12.27 (2.98) |
Stroop | ||||||||
Word | 57 | 54.42 (21.95) | 120 | 68.59 (15.64) | 57 | 85.82 (12.04) | 70 | 84.34 (13.14) |
Color | 57 | 44.32 (10.53) | 120 | 48.61 (11.27) | 57 | 62.82 (11.11) | 70 | 58.97 (11.96) |
Word-color | 57 | 27.14 (6.87) | 120 | 26.32 (8.14) | 57 | 35.88 (6.63) | 70 | 33.63 (9.36) |
Interference | 57 | 3.41 (7.63) | 120 | −1.88 (7.22) | 57 | −0.97 (6.99) | 70 | −0.78 (6.88) |
TMT | ||||||||
A | 82 | 51.98 (24.84) | 129 | 32.47 (11.25) | 57 | 42.30 (17.56) | 74 | 27.49 (11.18) |
B | 63 | 81.80 (40.56) | 129 | 63.13 (25.63) | 57 | 78.41 (32.33) | 74 | 48.72 (20.79) |
M-WCST | ||||||||
Correct cat. | 86 | 4.06 (1.88) | 128 | 3.61 (1.71) | 57 | 5.44 (1.13) | 74 | 4.76 (1.69) |
Pers. errors | 86 | 5.83 (5.46) | 128 | 4.98 (3.99) | 57 | 2.07 (2.30) | 74 | 2.78 (2.95) |
Total errors | 86 | 16.29 (11.14) | 128 | 17.59 (8.73) | 57 | 7.84 (7.23) | 74 | 11.18 (8.65) |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2021 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Ibáñez-Alfonso, J.A.; Company-Córdoba, R.; García de la Cadena, C.; Sianes, A.; Simpson, I.C. How Living in Vulnerable Conditions Undermines Cognitive Development: Evidence from the Pediatric Population of Guatemala. Children 2021, 8, 90. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020090
Ibáñez-Alfonso JA, Company-Córdoba R, García de la Cadena C, Sianes A, Simpson IC. How Living in Vulnerable Conditions Undermines Cognitive Development: Evidence from the Pediatric Population of Guatemala. Children. 2021; 8(2):90. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020090
Chicago/Turabian StyleIbáñez-Alfonso, Joaquín A., Rosalba Company-Córdoba, Claudia García de la Cadena, Antonio Sianes, and Ian Craig Simpson. 2021. "How Living in Vulnerable Conditions Undermines Cognitive Development: Evidence from the Pediatric Population of Guatemala" Children 8, no. 2: 90. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020090
APA StyleIbáñez-Alfonso, J. A., Company-Córdoba, R., García de la Cadena, C., Sianes, A., & Simpson, I. C. (2021). How Living in Vulnerable Conditions Undermines Cognitive Development: Evidence from the Pediatric Population of Guatemala. Children, 8(2), 90. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020090