Next Article in Journal
Clinical Presentation and Management of a Dinutuximab Beta Extravasation in a Patient with Neuroblastoma
Next Article in Special Issue
Depressive Symptomatology and Parenting Stress: Influence on the Social-Emotional Development of Pre-Schoolers in Chile
Previous Article in Journal
Familial and Genetic Influences on the Common Pediatric Primary Pain Disorders: A Twin Family Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Using the Very Short Form of the Children’s Behavior Questionnaire for Spanish-Speaking Populations in Low- and Middle-Income Countries: A Psychometric Analysis of Dichotomized Variables
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

How Living in Vulnerable Conditions Undermines Cognitive Development: Evidence from the Pediatric Population of Guatemala

by
Joaquín A. Ibáñez-Alfonso
1,2,
Rosalba Company-Córdoba
1,2,
Claudia García de la Cadena
3,
Antonio Sianes
4,* and
Ian Craig Simpson
1
1
Department of Psychology, Human Neuroscience Lab, Universidad Loyola Andalucía, 41704 Sevilla, Spain
2
ETEA Foundation, Development Institute of Universidad Loyola Andalucía, 14004 Córdoba, Spain
3
Department of Neuropsychology, Universidad del Valle de Guatemala, Guatemala 01015, Guatemala
4
Research Institute on Policies for Social Transformation, Universidad Loyola Andalucía, 14004 Córdoba, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2021, 8(2), 90; https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020090
Submission received: 1 December 2020 / Revised: 15 January 2021 / Accepted: 22 January 2021 / Published: 29 January 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Child Development in Latin America)

Abstract

:
Low-socioeconomic backgrounds represent a risk factor for children’s cognitive development and well-being. Evidence from many studies highlights that cognitive processes may be adversely affected by vulnerable contexts. The aim of this study was to determine if living in vulnerable conditions affects childhood cognitive development. To achieve this, we assessed the performance of a sample of 347 Guatemalan children and adolescents aged from 6 to 17 years (M = 10.8, SD = 3) in a series of 10 neuropsychological tasks recently standardized for the pediatric population of this country. Two-fifths of the sample (41.5%) could be considered to have vulnerable backgrounds, coming from families with low-socioeconomic status or having had a high exposure to violence. As expected, results showed lower scores in language and attention for the vulnerable group. However, contrary to expectations, consistent systematic differences were not found in the executive function tasks. Vulnerable children obtained lower scores in cognitive flexibility compared to the non-vulnerable group, but higher scores in inhibition and problem-solving tasks. These results suggest the importance of developing pediatric standards of cognitive performance that take environmental vulnerable conditions into consideration. These findings, one of the first obtained in the Guatemalan population, also provide relevant information for specific educational interventions and public health policies which will enhance vulnerable children and adolescent cognitive development.

1. Introduction

Half of the world’s at-risk population due to multidimensional poverty are children [1]. Living in vulnerable conditions affects these children’s future opportunities from very early years by lessening their developmental potential. Perhaps surprisingly, childhood poverty is not only a problem for children in developing countries. An important number of children in developed countries live with insufficient resources to guarantee their proper physical, psychological, and social development. One consequence of poverty is malnutrition, which prevents the adequate development of one in three children around the world [2]. Given the global repercussions of poverty and its associated negative consequences, the United Nations (U.N.) has initiated diverse actions to combat this problem including adopting the 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development [3]. The 2030 Agenda contains 17 Sustainable Development Goals (SDGs), including such objectives as eradicating poverty and hunger, and to improve the quality of education globally [4]. By proposing the involvement of governments in the implementation of these initiatives, the idea is to ensure sufficient economic and social resources are made available to fulfill those goals. Hence, the SDGs represent an opportunity to improve the lives of these children and to provide equal life opportunities. To achieve the SDGs, a shift in the way academic research is undertaken is needed due to the fact that the current assessment and reward systems used to evaluate academic performance and research worth often do not take into account the importance of transformative research designed to improve societal conditions [5]. Such a shift could lead to the creation and implementation of a whole new set of policies and interventions which would allow the scientific community to actively participate in achieving the SDGs [6]. Our study embraces this commitment by studying how vulnerable contexts have implications for children’s development.

1.1. Cognitive Development Under Vulnerable Conditions

Johnson, Riis, and Noble [7] conducted an extensive literature review of the relationship between socioeconomic status and brain development, noting that an important number of studies have taken into account the implication that a lack of resources and exposure to toxins and stress have for a child’s brain function and structure. Special attention needs to be taken regarding the effect of exposure to stress in early years. Stress is usually defined as exposure to life events that requires a subsequent adaptive behavior [8]. Previous studies have described how stressors caused by poverty affect children and adolescent health status [9], with some authors describing these effects as disturbances in brain development [10,11]. These consequences are related to the activation and reaction of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal axis. This results in increased cortisol levels, which especially affect prefrontal areas that are involved in cognitive functions such as working memory, cognitive flexibility, and inhibition, along with more general functions, such as self-regulation cognitive and emotional processes. Those factors end up negatively affecting children’s health and psychological wellbeing as well as quality of life [12]. In turn, this results in difficulties for children in escaping the effects of these early negative influences, even once adulthood has been reached [13]. Given these relationships, it is unsurprising that diverse studies have shown the alterations in subcortical areas such as the hippocampus [14,15,16,17], the amygdala [14,16], along with other dorsolateral prefrontal cortex volumes [15] in children from low socioeconomic status (SES) backgrounds. These alterations can impact cognitive performance in a number of areas, including attention, memory, motor skills, language, and executive functions.
Attention allows the individual to be oriented towards and focused on a specific target. SES seems to be related with performance in attentional tasks. For example, a study that compared attention tasks in a group of 10–13-year-old (n = 141) children, and found that children from vulnerable backgrounds showed less attentional efficacy than children from nonvulnerable backgrounds [18]. One possible explanation for this finding is that children who live in chaotic households where nothing is predictable, become adapted to this context and consequently show less attentional selectivity [19]. The adaption to unpredictability could be a strategy to cope with the limited sense of control that they can experience on a daily basis. What are the consequences for low SES children whose attention is negatively impacted by their circumstances? Using selective attention properly allows the individual to be oriented to a specific goal in a given task. This capacity is important in early ages and is related with school performance, since without a proper control of this kind of attention, children could face problems when ignoring irrelevant items [20]. In this sense, low-SES children are inclined to attend similarly to targets and irrelevant stimuli, which leads to interference in their attentional performance compared with their counterparts [21]. Mezzacappa [22] evaluated the attentional capacity in children of 6 years of age from diverse socioeconomic backgrounds, finding that low-SES children tend to show less speed and accuracy in measures of attention. Taking this into account, the results of research focusing on attention suggest that economically disadvantaged children have greater problems in inhibiting irrelevant information in comparison with children from wealthier backgrounds [18,23,24]. Despite the probable negative consequences low SES has for attention in young children, there is some evidence that they may be able overcome these disadvantages at a later age. In their study, Lupien et al. [25], found that low-SES children at 6 years of age performed worse than their peers in selective attention tasks, but this tended to change with age as no significant differences were found between low-SES adolescents and their peers. Nevertheless, taking all of these results into account, the results suggest that economically disadvantaged children show a lower performance in attention-related tasks.
Memory is an important cognitive domain since it allows the individual to retain internal and external information and learn from experience. Previous studies suggest that low SES affects a variety of memory-related processes such as visual and verbal memory, in both episodic and semantic systems, up until the age of 9 [26]. Farah et al. [27] found that children from low-SES contexts performed significantly worse than matched controls in incidental learning tasks. These authors suggested that the results were due to the effects that stress causes in memory systems. In a study conducted with adolescents [28], procedural memory did not show significant relations with SES levels. Nevertheless, in the same study, higher socioeconomic indicators were related with better performances on working memory tasks [28].
In one study, using the type of school as an indicator of SES, children from private schools showed better performance in memory tasks of visual encoding and deferred evocation [29]. Burneo-Garcés et al. [30], studied verbal and visual memory performance using immediate recall, delayed recall, and recognition tasks. These authors found that children from medium SES backgrounds performed better than their lower SES background peers on verbal memory tasks that measure immediate and delayed recall as well as on recognition tasks. The medium SES children also performed better in visual memory tasks involving immediate recall, although there was no difference between the two groups in delayed recall or recognition tasks.
Motor skill refers to the ability to coordinate the movement required to complete an action. Some studies have examined the relationship between SES and components related with motor development. For example, in a study conducted with school-age children, SES was shown to have an impact on motor development scores [31]. Adding as a mediation variable the adequacy of household environment, assessed by the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME) inventory [32], these authors reported that the proportion of variance in motor development explained by SES increased from 9 to 13%. Other studies in which children who attended public and private schools were compared have shown that children attending public schools obtained lower scores in fine motor skills in comparison with their private school counterparts [33,34].
Language is the ability that allows an individual to communicate with others. The relationship between SES and children’s verbal language ability has been evaluated in several studies, with negative effects of socioeconomic status generally being reported [27,35,36]. Differences have also been found in young children’s writing skills [26] and vocabulary [37]. Some aspects of the home environment, such as the availability of resources in the home, as well as parental educational level, can impact receptive vocabulary in children at different ages [38]. Lack of resources and low parental education are often associated with low SES levels and children from low SES settings are, among other factors, exposed to less complex oral language [39], which could in turn affect performance in language-related tasks. As Fernald, Marchman, and Weisleder showed in their study [40], there are significant differences in language aspects such as processing efficiency and vocabulary learning in infants at 18 months of age depending on their socioeconomic status. A similar study from Ecuador [30] compared performance in verbal comprehension and verbal fluency tasks, finding that the medium SES group performed better than the lower SES group. In a study of older children, adolescents from diverse socioeconomic backgrounds exhibited lower scores in language-related tasks, especially in vocabulary tests [41]. Farah et al. [27] examined neurocognitive functioning using a battery of tests defined jointly by functional and anatomical criteria. For the language-based tasks, SES disparities were found in the perisylvian system, an area of the brain known to be involved in language processing. Thus, a neurological correlate of these SES related differences in language tasks could be supported by existing differences in the left perisylvian area.
Executive functions (EF) are known as a compendium of processes which allow the self-regulation of goal-directed behaviors. Due to their importance, these processes have been well described and studied in the literature [42]. Evidence has been found linking socioeconomic status and measures of intelligence [26,43], EF in general [44], and specific processes such as working memory and cognitive control [27,36] with children at social risk obtaining lower scores in EF-related tasks [27,45,46,47]. A recent study highlighted family functioning as a source that could explain the relation between SES and the difficulties found in executive function performance in children [48]. EF are composed by a cognitive process that allow the individual to solve new and complex tasks, therefore the significance of EF in school performance is evident, being a factor that has been shown to be a mediator between poverty and school achievement in a multiracial study [49] and in a study conducted with a sample of vulnerable children from Argentina [50]. Working memory (WM) is one of the EF cognitive processes most studied and is related to the ability to operate with short-term information. This process is commonly understood to be divided into the visuospatial sketchpad, episodic buffer, and the phonological loop. In one study, when comparing low SES children from urban and rural areas with their wealthier counterparts, Tine [51] observed that urban low SES children performed worse than wealthier children in visuospatial and phonological tasks, while low SES children from a rural area underperformed in visuospatial WM. Leonard et al. [28], assessed WM among other domains in economically disadvantaged adolescents and their peers, finding that lower SES negatively affects this domain. Moreover, experiencing poverty during childhood, impacts the visuospatial WM component in adults [52].

1.2. Guatemalan Context

Based on this previous evidence, it seems that growing up in a low-SES environment potentially creates negative impacts for these children from an early stage of development. Specifically, the lack of resources available to these children gives rise to difficulties in areas such as physical and psychological health, and academic success, among others. However, the majority of studies that have examined this issue have been undertaken in high-income countries, especially in the United States [24,51,53]. Consequently, there is a significant lack of similar studies that have been carried out in middle-to-low-income countries. In order to address this shortfall, in this study we assessed cognitive skills such as motor skills, memory, attention, language, and executive functions in a group children and adolescents from Guatemala.
Within the countries of the Central American isthmus, Guatemala is the largest country with approximately 13 million inhabitants. It has a complex history in which 25 cultural ethnic groups have converged, 22 of which are Mayans, along with one Xinca group, one Garífuna group, and one non-indigenous group. Thus, the country is extraordinarily influenced by the indigenous culture to which 43.8% of population belongs [54]. All of these different ethnic groups have to face many difficulties on a daily basis, such as discrimination and racism. Additionally, despite being the largest economy in the Central American region, with regards to the indicators of nutrition, health, violence exposure, education, employment, and the well-being of the population, there are huge gaps as many of these factors have not been adequately studied in this region. A significant source of vulnerability for Guatemalan children and adolescents are those factors related with the familiar socioeconomic status. Importantly, what is considered to be medium-low SES in Guatemala, would be seen as low SES or indications of a vulnerable population in countries with a larger economy and extended welfare. Hence, the proportion of children in Guatemala living in what would be considered low SES conditions in other countries is almost certainly higher than indicated by the official figures.
Despite the fact that the government of Guatemala has included factors such as access to education and poverty reduction as a priority in its National Development Plan [55], there is still a long way to go. Guatemalan children and adolescents experience different types of vulnerability depending on the area in which they reside. Being aware of the realities these children face in their daily lives can help in understanding how the different forms of vulnerability interfere with their proper psychological and cognitive development.
A study conducted by the Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations found that in 2014 around 25% of the rural population of Guatemala were living in conditions of extreme poverty, with a further 50% of the rural population were considered as suffering from poverty [56]. This fact means that rural children and adolescents find themselves in a vulnerable position from an early age. Malnutrition is a condition associated with poverty. Consequently, long term nutritional programs have been implemented in rural areas of Guatemala for children due to the importance of adequate nutrition in cognitive development [57,58]. Moreover, although children and adolescents from urban areas may suffer less from malnutrition, they nevertheless suffer from other hazards such as being witness to, or a victim of extreme violence, a factor which has also been shown to negatively affect children’s development. For example, witnessing intimate partner violence (defined as parental partner violence of a physical, emotional, or psychological nature [59]) in different stages of childhood and adolescence has been shown to have a negative effect on cognitive development [60]. Moreover, suffering from violence has been shown to have an impact on brain functions in a sample of Latin American preadolescents [61]. This sample showed higher cortisol levels and less activation of the right hemisphere areas involved in social perception as well as cognition. A more recent study examined the effects of exposure to violence along with food concern on the relationship between socioeconomic characteristics and mental health indicators in children and adolescents living in vulnerable conditions in Guatemala [12]. These authors found a moderating effect of exposure to violence on the relationship between socioeconomic characteristics and depression, anxiety, and health-related quality of life such that higher exposure to violence resulted in higher scores in depression and anxiety indicators. Moreover, in the same study adolescents showed higher depression and anxiety scores and lower health-related quality of life scores compared to children suggesting that these mental problems increase when children grow older. Food insecurity experienced by parents was not found to be a moderating variable between socioeconomic and mental health variables.

1.3. Aim of Study

The objective of the present study was to determine if living in vulnerable conditions negatively impacts the cognitive development of children and adolescents. To accomplish this, we compared the cognitive performance of vulnerable and non-vulnerable children living in Guatemala. To achieve a broad comparison, 10 different neuropsychological tests were chosen to encompass the most relevant cognitive processes: Motor skills, memory, attention, language, and executive functions. A second motivation for choosing these specific tests was that they had recently been standardized for the Guatemalan population. We hypothesized that there would be significant differences between the two groups, even after applying the same normative inclusion criteria. Lower scores for the vulnerable group were expected in all cognitive domains assessed.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 397 participants underwent a screening process for inclusion in the study. Inclusion criteria were: (a) Having Spanish as mother language or proficient as a second language, (b) having an Intelligence Quotient of ≥80, (c) scoring below the threshold scores in depression and anxiety questionnaires, (d) not having previous history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, and (e) not having a history of drug use. Based on these criteria, 50 participants were excluded from the study (42 from the vulnerable group, 8 from the non-vulnerable group). The inclusion criteria were chosen based on our research goal. We aimed to assess healthy children (for which we used the questionnaires) with no intellectual or mental health disturbances (for which we used the TONI-2, anxiety and depression questionnaires, respectively). Since our goal was to assess a pediatric population, we selected the age range from 6 to 17, taking 6 as the minimum age because we consider that from this age, children are able to complete the vast majority of neuropsychological tasks used in the study.
The vulnerable group was characterized by living in areas where extreme poverty and violence exposure were key difficulties in their lives. The non-vulnerable group was composed of participants from medium-low SES backgrounds whose environment was not especially characterized by these difficulties. In this regard, it is important to consider that our vulnerable and non-vulnerable groups were comprised of children from both low and medium-low SES backgrounds. Accordingly, in this study we did not set out to compare high-SES with low-SES participants. The final sample was composed of 347 Guatemalan children and adolescents aged from 6 to 17 years (M = 10.8, SD = 3.0). Within the whole sample, the vulnerable subsample was composed of 144 participants (71 girls, 49.3%) recruited from three schools of the educative institution Fe y Alegria (age M = 11.0, SD = 3.7). Two of the schools were based in rural areas of the Totonicapán department, which is located in the southwest of the country (91 participants, 63.2% of the vulnerable sample) while the third school was based in the suburban area of Guatemala City, in the district of Villanueva. Within the vulnerable sample, children recruited from the rural schools suffered from poverty conditions while children recruited from the urban school were routinely exposed to violence on a daily basis since the school was located in an area bordered by two competing criminal gangs. Thus, these schools were chosen because of the especially vulnerable conditions faced by students that experience a high exposure to violence and poverty. Within the vulnerable group, 75 rural participants (52.1% of the vulnerable group, 21.6% of the total sample) were bilinguals (Spanish-K’iche). The non-vulnerable group was composed of 203 participants (95 girls, 46.8%; age M = 10.7, SD = 2.5). These participants came from one private and one public school located in the capital, Guatemala City, where the children’s families had a medium socioeconomic status. The mean level of parental education (MLPE), calculated using the average number of years that the primary caregivers went to school, was 6.5 (SD = 4.5) for the vulnerable group, and 10.5 (SD = 4.1) for the non-vulnerable group. All participants gave their informed consent prior to participating in the study.

2.2. Instruments

The questionnaires and screening tests used to ensure that participants met our inclusion criteria for physical and mental health were the same as those used in a previous study conducted by Rivera & Arango-Lasprilla [62]. The 10 neuropsychological tasks used to assess the sample were chosen since they encompass the measurement of the most common cognitive processes: Motor skills, memory, attention, language, and executive functions. These neuropsychological tasks are included in Appendix A. Normative data for the Spanish-speaking pediatric population is available for all the following neuropsychological tests, including specific normative data for the Guatemalan pediatric population [63,64,65,66,67,68,69,70,71,72].

2.3. Procedure

In order to inform families about the study goals and procedure, the researchers met teachers and parents in each school. Parents were then asked if they would allow their children to participate and to sign the informed consent, as well as complete the sociodemographic, clinical, and language questionnaires. Children with consent from both groups were then pseudo-randomly selected for participation in the study as we implemented a constraint of trying to balance age and gender across the vulnerable and non-vulnerable groups.
Children were individually assessed for approximately 120 min in a well-illuminated room provided by each school. Children received a rest break when one hour of assessment was completed. The assessments were performed by trained psychologists. The screening tasks were administered in the same session and immediately corrected in order to determine if the participant met the inclusion criteria. Each neuropsychological test from the battery was administered in a different order, randomly assigned to each child. Some tasks could not be completed by some children because of different reasons: The child was unable to read, the numerical order was not internalized, or the participant refused to complete the task.

2.4. Data Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS version 26 [73]. First, missing values were coded to indicate the reason for the lack of information. Subsequently, outliers in the raw scores were modified to have a value of ±3 SD using the winsorinzing method [74].
All comparisons were done with bootstrapping using 95% confidence intervals and 1000 repetitions in order to control for possible bias due to the non-normality present in some of the distributions. Although the vulnerable group were slightly older (0.3 years, 95%CI −0.4, 1.0), this difference was not significant (t(235.5) < 1, p = 0.400). In contrast, the 4 year difference (95%CI −4.9, −3.1) in the mean level of parental education favoring the non-vulnerable group was significant (t(345) = −8.62, p < 0.0001). Chi square test revealed that gender distribution between groups was not significant, χ2 (1) = 0.212, p = 0.645.
Means and standards deviations were analyzed in all neuropsychological tasks. A series of independent samples t-test was conducted to evaluate the differences in scores between the vulnerable and non-vulnerable groups in the 10 neuropsychological tasks.

3. Results

The raw scores obtained by the vulnerable and non-vulnerable groups in the 10 neuropsychological tests along with the results of the independent samples t-test comparisons are described in Table 1, Table 2, Table 3 and Table 4. The raw scores obtained by age group and vulnerable condition are included in the Table 5.
The comparisons are shown in Table 1, Table 2, Table 3 and Table 4, ordered by the cognitive domain. For the motor skill and memory tasks, although all of the means were slightly higher for the non-vulnerable group, none of the differences were significant and all effect sizes were small (Cohen’s ds ≤ 0.15). Nonetheless, significant differences between the groups were found in other cognitive processes. With regards to attention, the non-vulnerable group performed better on both tasks. For the D2 concentration index, the size of the effect was medium-to-large (p = 0.001, d = 0.61) whereas for the SDMT (Symbol Digit Test) task, the effect was small-to-medium (p = 0.012, d = 0.31). Regarding language performance, the non-vulnerable group scored significantly higher in the majority of tests, with the largest effect sizes being for receptive vocabulary PPVT-III (p = 0.001, d = 0.87), and the language comprehension Token test (p = 0.001, d = 0.63). Measures of language expression also showed a general better performance by the non-vulnerable group. In the phonological verbal fluency series all differences were significant (ps < 0.05, Cohen’s ds between 0.22 and 0.38). For semantic fluency, the non-vulnerable group was superior in the animal category (p = 0.016, d = 0.29), but surprisingly, not in the fruit category (p = 0.922, d = 0.03). Results in executive function tasks were heterogeneous. In the TMT (Trail Making Test) task the vulnerable group needed significantly more time to complete both the A and the B series (ps = 0.001, ds > 0.75), which may be interpreted as a slower processing speed (A) and worse cognitive flexibility (B). In contrast, in the interference score of the Stroop task, which is related to the cognitive inhibition ability, the vulnerable group showed better performance (p = 0.001, d = 0.44), without significate differences in some automatic processes such as color naming (p = 0.506, d = 0.09). In the case of the M-WCST (Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test) task, vulnerable participants also exhibited better performance in terms of problem-solving ability (correct categories, p = 0.006, d = 0.33), although no difference in the flexibility measure was observed (perseverative errors, p = 0.765, d = 0.02).

4. Discussion

The purpose of this study was to determine if living in vulnerable conditions negatively impacts cognitive development. To achieve this, we used 10 neuropsychological tests recently standardized for the Guatemalan pediatric population to evaluate a broad range of cognitive abilities in two groups of children and adolescents. These 10 tests were chosen because they encompass the measurement of the most relevant cognitive processes: Motor skills, memory, attention, language, and executive functions. The vulnerable group was composed of children who are living in poverty conditions and/or are frequently exposed to violence in their daily lives. The non-vulnerable group was composed of children from medium-low SES backgrounds who rarely experience violent incidents or suffer extreme poverty in their daily lives.
As per our expectations, the vulnerable group underperformed in language- and attention-related tasks in comparison with their non-vulnerable counterparts. In contrast, results for the executive function tasks differed in some cases from those expected.
Regarding motor skills, no significant difference was found between the vulnerable and non-vulnerable groups on the ROCF (Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test) copy subtest. This result differs from previous studies which did report differences [33,34,75]. The differences between the results reported here and what has been found in previous studies could be due to the different way in which children are classified in terms of vulnerability. Previous studies have used the type of school (public vs. private) to classify participants [34,75], whereas in this study we used the socioeconomic characteristics of the area in which the schools were based in order to classify the children. Potentially, the different methods used to form the vulnerable and non-vulnerable groups mean that the results of the different studies are not directly comparable.
No differences were found when comparing vulnerable and non-vulnerable groups in terms of memory performance in the ROCF memory subtest and in verbal memory subtests. This result does not agree with previous findings as the economically disadvantaged group was expected to obtain lower scores [30]. A previous study which examined how early ages’ Socioeconomic Context (SEC) affects cognitive functions throughout an entire life span [76], showed that higher SECs in early ages and adulthood were related to better scores in verbal memory recognition. The literature regarding memory of participants with a background of poverty shows a link between lower family income and the hypo-reactivity of the cortisol response, which has been shown to be linked to associative memory in children [77]. The lack of participants from high-SES backgrounds, being our whole sample from medium-low and low SES, may have reduced the expected effects.
The vulnerable group in the present study showed poorer performance in attentional tasks in comparison with the non-vulnerable group, although the effect size was small to medium (SDMT, d = 0.31; D2 concentration index, d = 0.61). This is in accordance with previously reported findings which have also found differences [18,19,47]. This pattern of results has been replicated in sustained attention tasks involving infant participants [24,78]. In fact, low-SES has recently related to a higher risk of suffering from symptoms of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) [79]. In the D2 test, the concentration index offers information about the child’s ability to maintain attention and response accuracy for an extended time period. Thus, this index provides information about how selective and sustained attention function. The way in which contextual characteristics influence basic cognitive processes that are attention-related has been studied [22], and are in the line with the results reported here. Furthermore, the present results agree with those found in studies which have evaluated Event-related Potentials (ERPs) and Electroencephalographic (EEG) measures, since there seems to exist a clear neural correlate between SES and attention-related tasks [23,80]. This means that there is evidence of the neurophysiological substrate of the implications of SES differences in terms of attentional performance. Our results highlight the profound negative impact that attention development may suffer under vulnerable conditions, such as violence exposure and extreme poverty. The relation between attention and socioeconomic disadvantage has been of such importance that some authors have proposed specific interventions to diminish this developmental gap [81].
Regarding language, vulnerable children and adolescents achieved a lower performance than their non-vulnerable counterparts, in the majority of language tasks. Other authors have also observed a similar impact of SES on language [27,47,82]. Our results for receptive vocabulary, as assessed using the PPVT-III, agree with previous studies which have reported lower scores in the case of low-SES children on different language measures such as semantic processing [36], receptive vocabulary [38], language comprehension tasks [30,41], and fluency [83]. The receptive vocabulary results in the present study warrant further comment. The difference between means of 23 points favoring the non-vulnerable group is a large effect (Cohen’s d = 0.87). When interpreting the data, it is important to consider that the vulnerable group had two factors that could be related with their low scores in language tasks: Vulnerability and the large percentage of bilinguals included in the sample. The way in which the bilingualism impacts language tasks has been studied, and in general, bilingualism tends to be associated with reduced language scores in children (for a review, see Bialystok) [84]. Moreover, there are studies that have considered the impact of both bilingualism and a low-SES background, finding that each factor independently has a negative impact on children’s linguistic performance [85]. Furthermore, language performance has been studied in indigenous minorities in comparison with the non-indigenous population in a sample of Australian school students, with lower scores in receptive vocabulary obtained by the indigenous group [86]. Thus, it plausible that in cases where vulnerability and minority languages converge, the development of the majority language is especially at risk.
One curious result from the language tasks was the conflicting findings in the two semantic fluency tasks. While the non-vulnerable children generated more words in the animal category (p = 0.016, d = 0.29), there was a null effect in the fruit category (p = 0.922, d = 0.03). The first point to note here is that the mean difference in the animal category was small, being just 1.3 words, so in a practical sense, the two groups are not too dissimilar. Nevertheless, we offer the following tentative explanation for the different pattern of results. These two tasks were carried out in Spanish. As previously noted, there were a large number of bilinguals in the vulnerable group. Given the importance of food in general to the lives of these children and their families, it is possible that the bilingual children have learned the names for fruit in both their native language and Spanish and were thus able to generate as many fruit names as their non-vulnerable counterparts. In contrast, the children in the vulnerable group may not have been exposed to as many non-native Guatemalan animals (via reading, personal experience, parental tuition, etc.) as their non-vulnerable counterparts, and such were not as capable of generating as large a range of animal names in the Spanish language as the children in the non-vulnerable group.
In regard to executive functioning, results in this study contrast in some cases with previous literature that links vulnerability conditions with worse EF [27,36,45,87]. Although our results in the TMT were congruent with previous evidence, the interpretation of results obtained in the Stroop test and in the M-WCST is especially challenging. For example, a previous study conducted in Ecuador with medium and low-SES participants focusing on cognitive inhibition tasks found that performance in resistance to interference was better for vulnerable children [30]. Although this result was the opposite of that expected by these authors, it is congruent with the results reported here. We offer the following account as a possible explanation for these results. The use of this test is recommended for children from 6 years old because simple word reading skills should already be achieved by this age. However, this does not occur in all contexts. School milestones are reached at different ages depending on the country and the characteristics of the study population. In this case, the fact that children from vulnerable backgrounds may not be as proficient in reading as their non-vulnerable counterparts could be reducing the word-color reading interference effect. Thus, a possible explanation for this result may be that an interference effect is not present in these children because their low levels of literacy do not give rise to such an effect using this literacy-based task. This account may also explain why an almost significant difference was found in the word naming task (p = 0.089, d = 0.22, mean difference of 4.3 words favoring the non-vulnerable group), but not in the color naming task (p = 0.506, d = 0.09) despite the fact that both are considered to be measures of automatic processes. While the former task involves reading, the latter task does not, requiring participants to simply recognize and name colors. Consequently, based on these results, we would recommend the use of alternative tasks that avoid word and number reading to test cognitive inhibition in vulnerable pediatric population. One possible alternative is the inhibition task included in the NEPSY-II battery [88].
Similarly, in the TMT series, children should be able to complete this task by the age of 6, but some from our vulnerable sample were not able to. In such cases, children did not have the number system internalized and, in others, the alphabet. Given that these tasks involve the tracing of number and letter forms, it is plausible that the higher response times in both the TMT series in our vulnerable group was due to the fact that some children in this group were less familiar with the stimuli they were meant to produce compared to their counterparts in the non-vulnerable group. Although the differences found between groups in the two TMT tasks are large effect sizes (TMT-A, d = 1.03; TMT-B, d = 0.75). Nonetheless, due to the limitations previously described, these results should be interpreted with caution. In the M-WCST subtest, something similar to the Stroop results occurred as we expected to find significantly lower scores in the vulnerable group. However, somewhat surprisingly, the vulnerable group significantly outperformed the non-vulnerable group in the problem-solving task and differences in perseverative errors were non-significant. At this point, it is important to take into consideration the high rates of bilingualism found in our vulnerable group. There are studies that have analyzed the implications of bilingualism in low-SES children core processes related with the executive functions such as inhibition and shifting [89], or working memory tasks [90], among others. Bilingualism helps to enhance some cognitive processes independent of the children’s level of SES [91]. This leads us to think about the possible benefit of speaking more than one language for children that live in disadvantaged conditions. Studying a sample of 6-year-old children and taking into account both SES and the bilingual condition of the participants, Calvo and Bialystok [92] found that although children from middle-class backgrounds obtained better scores than lower-SES counterparts, the bilinguals outperformed monolinguals in executive function tasks but not in language-related tasks, independent of the SES level. These authors concluded that the influence of bilingualism and SES are both significant as well as independent predictors of children’s cognitive functioning. Additionally, in a sample of Latino low-SES preschoolers, White and Greenfield [93] compared bilinguals (Spanish-English) and monolinguals, finding that bilinguals performed better than their peers in executive function tasks. Similar conclusions have been obtained in a study with adolescents [94]. All of these results lead us to argue that bilingual conditions could compensate the adverse effects that living in vulnerable conditions causes in the children and adolescents assessed.
An important point to note about the present study is that we did not set out to compare high-SES with low-SES participants. However, we would expect to find significant differences in the vast majority of cognitive tasks if such a comparison was done. Even though the sample differs in terms of violence exposure and the grade of socioeconomic difficulties, the participants’ cognitive performance has shown to be especially different in those cognitive tasks that require cognitive processes to be more affected by extreme low-SES. Moreover, the urban educational center included in the vulnerable group is located on the outskirts of Guatemala City in an area which borders two rival criminal gangs. Consequently, the children and adolescents who attend this school were exposed to community violence on a daily basis. Future studies should consider assessing this factor as children in our non-vulnerable sample may also have been experiencing exposure to violence.
These findings, one of the first obtained in the Guatemalan pediatric population, can be a key factor to keep in mind when designing cognitive assessment protocols and intervention programs for children and adolescents who live in similar vulnerable settings. Considering the importance that attentional, linguistic, and executive processes have in academic performance, our results contribute to understanding the impact that vulnerability conditions may add in these children and adolescent cognitive development and school trajectories. This unequal situation represents a real threat for their present and future psychosocial adjustment and well-being [12], and we believe that these findings could provide relevant information for specific educational interventions and public health policies to enhance vulnerable children and adolescent cognitive development and quality of life.

Limitations and Future Directions

Advances in this field should be directed along the lines of in-depth knowledge of how the factors associated with poverty affect cognitive development and what interventions could be effective in reducing these negative effects. Studying which cultural and socioeconomic factors associated with the vulnerability are present in both developed and developing countries may facilitate understanding of this issue. Moreover, given that cognitive development may be negatively affected by cases where children and adolescents live in vulnerability conditions, we consider it desirable to create proper adjusted scales aimed at these populations as existing scales created using only children from non-vulnerable samples are not suitable for use in these populations. The field of pediatric neuropsychology would greatly benefit from having inclusive standards that would enable the proper assessment of children and adolescents taking into account their idiosyncrasies. Regarding advice for other similar studies, we would recommend the use of the 10 neuropsychological tasks employed in this study, with the possible exception of those that rely on the influence of reading competency to assess some cognitive process. The reason for the caution regarding tests containing a linguistic component is that these types of tests could be limiting for children who, despite having a normal cognitive ability, could be unfairly assessed as exhibiting poor cognitive performance simply due to having underdeveloped reading skills (e.g., a child with an adequate inhibition ability would obtain excessively low scores in the Stroop task if they did not have an adequate reading ability).
Regarding limitations, it is important to consider that the data collected via the sociodemographic questionnaires could be influenced by social desirability. We cannot be certain that the information provided by parents regarding income and years of their own education is accurate or has been overestimated by some respondents. In relation to the executive function tests (e.g., in TMT and Stroop tests), we concluded that these results should be interpreted with caution due to the high number of children who did not complete those tasks successfully due to low reading proficiency. It is important to consider that children of the same age, but from different countries or areas, can achieve educational milestones at different developmental stages. As explained above, we recommend the use of neuropsychological tasks with a minimum reading component, as it may influence the cognitive assessment in this kind of pediatric population. Another limitation concerns the number of participants, which is insufficient for an epidemiological study and may directly affect external validity. Additionally, only participants from low- and medium-low SES backgrounds were assessed in this study. Consequently, future studies should try to increase the sample size and include participants from high SES backgrounds to improve the strength of these findings.
The long duration of the assessment process could be another weak issue in this study since fatigue during the assessment process may have influenced the performance of some children. Nevertheless, we included all 10 instruments as we wanted as broad a coverage as possible and, to lessen this potential limitation, participants could take a break during the assessment whenever they felt tired. Furthermore, as the order in which the tests were administered was randomized, this ensures that possible effects due to fatigue did not systematically influence the results of any one task—had the tests been applied in a fixed order, the results of the last tests may have been less reliable. Moreover, while the inclusion of 10 tests did result in a 2 h testing session, other researchers do not necessarily have to include all of these instruments and can instead choose just the instruments required to satisfy their research goals. A further reason for including a wide variety of instruments is that researchers from poor countries may be limited in terms of access to neuropsychological tests. By including a broad selection in the present study, we increase the chances that the information presented here will be useful for other researchers. So, for example, we included two assessments of attention, the Symbol Digit Modalities Test, SDMT and the D2 Concentration Endurance Test. Both tests revealed a significant difference between the vulnerable and non-vulnerable groups. Thus, future researchers could choose to just include one of these tests based on availability and the specific characteristics of their research. Finally, we would also like to highlight the relevance of longitudinal studies to increase our knowledge about the developmental trajectory of the abilities assessed in the present study. This knowledge would enable intervention programs to be better adjusted to a children and adolescent developmental stage.

5. Conclusions

In this study we aimed to promote inclusive research by evaluating the cognitive performance of a profile of children usually forgotten about in neuropsychological assessment, specifically, children and adolescents living in vulnerable conditions. To the best of our knowledge, few previous studies have focused on vulnerable populations, and this is especially true of Guatemala, which has a large pediatric indigenous population. To date, pediatric neuropsychology has scarcely represented ethnic and vulnerable minorities when assessing normative cognitive development. The sample assessed in this study was characterized for presenting different types of vulnerability such as exposure to violence and extreme poverty. These are our main conclusions:
  • Vulnerable children and adolescents underperformed in attention, language, and some subtests of executive functions tasks, in comparison with non-vulnerable participants;
  • Effect sizes were considerably large in the case of the PPVT-III language task and both forms of TMT executive function task, suggesting the need for specific normative data designed for children and adolescents from these backgrounds;
  • There were no significant differences between vulnerable and non-vulnerable participants in terms of performance in motor skills and memory tasks;
  • Bilingualism of a high percentage of the vulnerable sample may have played an important role in the obtained results, especially in executive functions’ tasks;
  • Vulnerability may not be a risk factor for the development of all cognitive domains, since vulnerable participants obtained equal or betters scores in some subtests;
  • In future studies, it would be important to consider the reading ability of participants and to further study how bilingualism may interfere in the cognitive assessment of vulnerable populations.
These conclusions should be considered when creating intervention programs that aim to diminish the impact that vulnerability causes in children and adolescent lives. Knowing which cognitive functions are more affected by vulnerability might favor the creation and implementation of public policies related with education that work in the advantage of millions of girls and boys from both developing and developed countries.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.A.I.-A., R.C.-C.; methodology, J.A.I.-A., R.C.-C.; formal analysis, R.C.-C.; investigation, C.G.d.l.C., J.A.I.-A., R.C.-C.; resources, C.G.d.l.C., J.A.I.-A.; data curation, J.A.I.-A., R.C.-C.; writing—original draft preparation, R.C.-C.; writing—review and editing, A.S., C.G.d.l.C., I.C.S., J.A.I.-A., R.C.-C.; visualization, J.A.I.-A.; supervision, A.S., C.G.d.l.C., I.C.S., J.A.I.-A.; project administration, J.A.I.-A., R.C.-C.; funding acquisition, J.A.I.-A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Agencia Andaluza de Cooperación Internacional para el Desarrollo, Junta de Andalucía Goverment (Spain), under the project “Estatus socioeconómico y desarrollo cognitivo en la infancia y la adolescencia: herramientas de evaluación innovadoras para poblaciones vulnerables. El caso de Guatemala”, grant number 0INN007/2017, and by the Universidad Loyola Andalucía funds.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Andalusian government on 6 April 2019 (code be443f9fda0f49abcfe25cbda85ce6e5e3444776). The study was also approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Universidad del Valle de Guatemala.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to the sensitivity of data collected from underage participants.

Acknowledgments

This study was made in collaboration with the educative foundation Fe y Alegria Guatemala. We would like to thank Karen Avendaño Martínez and Luis Alberto López Moratalla for their support and to the whole staff, families, and participants from the schools involved in the project. We are grateful to Jose Manuel Moreno from the Entreculturas association, and to Ma José Vázquez and Michela Accerenzi from the ETEA Foundation, Development Institute of the Universidad Loyola Andalucía, for their administrative and technical support.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

Appendix A.1. Motor Skills and Memory Tests

Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, ROCF [95]. The ROCF is a meaningless geometric composition whereby the participant is asked to both copy (copy subtest) as well as reproduce after three minutes without the figure being available for visual inspection (memory subtest). The cognitive abilities assessed include constructive praxis, visuospatial perception, memory, and planning. The maximum score is 36 in each subtest. This task has been shown to have good reliability in pediatric populations in both subtests; Cronbach’s α = 0.83 in the copy subtest and 0.78 in the memory subtest [96]. Convergent validity has been supported with other tests that assess constructs such as visuospatial [97] and constructional ability [98]. The application of this test took approximately 10 min.
Learning and verbal memory test, TAMVI-I [64]. The test consists of three parts, (1) verbal repetition, which serves as the learning phase, (2) delayed memory, and (3) visual recognition. In the learning phase the evaluator read a word list which contains 12 words from three categories: Clothing, furniture, and body parts. The list was repeated 4 times, and the participants were required to repeat the list each time. Prior to commencing the verbal repetition tasks, participants were informed that there would be a subsequent memory test of these items later in the session. Thirty minutes later, a delayed memory task was applied. Finally, there was a recognition task in which the participants saw a list of words and had to indicate which of these were in the initial task. The maximum score in the first subtest is 48, with 12 being the maximum score in the other two subtests. The TAMVI-I has shown good test-retest reliability (Cronbach’s α > 0.85). The convergent validity of the memory task scores of the Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure and the phonological verbal fluency test has shown coefficients scores greater than 0.295 in recent studies [64]. The administration of this task took 10 min approximately.

Appendix A.2. Attention Tests

Symbol Digit Modalities Test, SDMT [99]. This test is used to detect neurological dysfunctions in children and adults, as well as to detect difficulties in reading skills in children. This task consists of replacing figures with other numbers by using a previously learned key. The final score is the total number of correct substitutions made in 90 s. The maximum score is 110. It can be administered either verbally or in written form, although here only the written option was applied. The SDMT has shown very good test-retest reliability (ICC = 0.80) [99] and its convergent validity has also shown to be good with other cognitive measure tests [100]. The symbol digit modalities test assesses neurocognitive functions such as attention, speed processing, or working memory. This task has a duration of 2 min.
Concentration Endurance Test, D2 [101]. The D2 assesses speed and amount of performance, quality of performance, and the relationship between speed and accuracy of attention. The D2 can be applied to children, adolescents, and adults. The test has 14 lines with 47 items in each one. The participant has to mark the three items that the examiner previously showed. The participants had 20 s to complete each line. Although the instrument has different scores (e.g., total responses, total correct responses, omission and commission errors, effectiveness of the performance, and concentration index), in this study only the concentration index was used due to its relevance to the research objectives. The D2 has shown good psychometric properties with values of internal consistency for Cronbach’s α of approximately 0.95 and a validity coefficient of Cronbach’s α = 0.47 [102,103]. A study conducted with premature and born-at-term babies using the Spanish adaptation values of approximately Cronbach’s α of 0.90 were reported [104]. Administration of this test takes around 10 min.

Appendix A.3. Language Tests

Peabody picture vocabulary test, PPVT-III [105]. The PPVT-III is used to assess the receptive vocabulary and can be used as a screening method to have a quick view of verbal attitude in individuals aged 2 to 90 years. The tool contains 192 items in total, each one consisting of a sheet with 4 black and white illustrations. The task is to select the illustration which best fits the meaning of a word verbally presented by the examiner. Test administration begins with age-appropriate items. This test has shown good internal consistency [106] and good concurrent validity [107]. The original validation study indicated a Cronbach’s α > 0.90 [108]. The maximum possible score is 192. The required time for the assessment is about 12 min.
Shortened version of Token Test, TOKEN [109]. The reduced version of a Token test allows the assessor to measure the number of understood commands without redundancy. The tool includes 20 tokens in five colors, two sizes, and two shapes, and the answer document. This reduced tool has 36 items of increasing difficulty, which are divided into six parts. Those items are orders that a child has to complete. The maximum score in this task is 36. Its construct and concurrent validity has been shown to be well assessed with children [110] and the computerized version also has good psychometric properties [111]. The internal reliability as assessed by the Kunder-Richardson coefficient has been reported as 0.92 [112]. Test-retest reliability has shown values of 0.75 [62]. Its administration takes around 10 min.
Phonological and Semantic Verbal Fluency Tests [113]. In this study, the test was administered and scored following Olabarrieta-Landa et al.’s [69] guidelines. The test includes two different verbal fluency modalities: Phonological and semantic. The number of words emitted in each category in 60 s is measured and is considered a controlled verbal production test. In the phonological subtest, children had to say as many words as they know starting with /f/, /a/, /s/. In the semantical subtest, children had to produce words belonging to two categories: Animals and fruits. This test has shown acceptable levels of test-retest reliability in previous studies [114], being its test-retest values from 0.78 to 0.87 in both phonological and sematic subtests [62]. Its administration takes around 8 min.

Appendix A.4. Executive Function Tests

Trail making test, TMT A-B [115]. The TMT is a very popular diagnostic tool used with diverse types of cerebral dysfunction. The participant has to “connect” dots in a logical order with the TMT-A being comprised of numbers and the TMT-B comprised of numbers and letters. TMT-A is used for children who are below 15 years of age and the TMT-B for those who are above this age. The score for this instrument is of the time that the participant needed to complete the task. Both forms of TMT have shown good reliability and validity scores [116,117]. Both forms, A and B has shown test-retest values of 0.68 and 0.76 respectively [62]. The time needed for the administration in the case of the TMT test is normally between 5 and 10 min.
Modified Wisconsin Sorting Card Test, M-WCST [118]. The goal of M-WCST, is to assess abilities as the cognitive flexibility. Cognitive processes such as working memory, attentional flexibility, response inhibition, and other aspects of executive function play an important role in this task. This version has a total of 48 response cards and four key cards. The examiner takes the first category chosen by the participant as correct. If in the second time the chosen category is different from the first one, it is accepted as correct. The examiner informs if the election is correct or not and when the rule has changed at the sixth correct classification. The test ends up when six categories are correctly classified or until the participant has used all cards, that is, the maximum number of correct categories that can be obtained is six. The WCST has shown great reliability in perseverative and non-perseverative error scores (between 0.91 and 0.96) [119] and good test-retest reliability [120], showing test-retest values of 0.80 in the correct categories score [62]. The administration of this test takes around 15 min.
The Stroop color and Word test [121]. This test is composed of three pages with a total of 100 items in each one. The first part has “red”, “green”, and “blue” written on it in black ink. The second part has 100 elements “xxxx” inked with different colors (red, green, or blue). The last part has the same number of elements but this time they are color words inked with mixed colors. The score is the number of words the participant can read in 45 s for each trial. Using the three types of scores, it is possible to calculate an inhibition score. Psychometric properties of this test has been shown to be good in terms of validity, have a reliability of around 0.70 in all subtests [122]. This test provides a score which indicates how well the participant is able to resist interference. Its administration time is approximately 10 min.

References

  1. United Nations. Programa de las Naciones Unidas para el Desarrollo. Available online: http://hdr.undp.org/en/2019-MPI (accessed on 20 April 2020).
  2. United Nations International Children’s Emergency Fund UNICEF. The changing face of malnutrition. Available online: https://features.unicef.org/state-of-the-worlds-children-2019-nutrition/ (accessed on 6 March 2020).
  3. United Nations General Assembly. Transforming our world: The 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development. Available online: https://sustainabledevelopment.un.org/post2015/transformingourworld (accessed on 20 April 2020).
  4. United Nations. The Sustainable Development Agenda—United Nations Sustainable Development. Available online: https://www.un.org/sustainabledevelopment/development-agenda/ (accessed on 6 March 2020).
  5. Saric, J.; Blaettler, D.; Bonfoh, B.; Hostettler, S.; Jimenez, E.; Kiteme, B.; Koné, I.; Lys, J.-A.; Masanja, H.; Steinger, E.; et al. Leveraging research partnerships to achieve the 2030 Agenda: Experiences from North-South cooperation. GAIA Ecol. Perspect. Sci. Soc. 2019, 28, 143–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Schneider, F.; Kläy, A.; Zimmermann, A.B.; Buser, T.; Ingalls, M.; Messerli, P. How can science support the 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development? Four tasks to tackle the normative dimension of sustainability. Sustain. Sci. 2019, 14, 1593–1604. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Johnson, S.B.; Riis, J.L.; Noble, K.G. State of the Art Review: Poverty and the Developing Brain. Pediatrics 2016, 137, e20153075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Selye, H. Stress and the general adaptation syndrome. Br. Med. J. 1950, 1, 1383–1392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Santiago, C.D.; Wolff, B.C.; Wadsworth, M.E. The Stress of Growing up Poor: Pathways to Compromised Development for Low-Income Children and Adolescents. In Handbook of Stress: Causes, Effects and Control; Heidenreich, P., Pruter, I., Eds.; Nova Science Publishers Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2009; pp. 229–260. [Google Scholar]
  10. Shonkoff, J.P.; Garner, A.S. The lifelong effects of early childhood adversity and toxic stress. Pediatrics 2012, 129, e232–e246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  11. Blair, C.; Raver, C.C. Poverty, Stress, and Brain Development: New Directions for Prevention and Intervention. Acad. Pediatr. 2016, 16 (Suppl. 3), S30–S36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Company-Córdoba, R.; Gómez-Baya, D.; López-Gaviño, F.; Ibáñez-Alfonso, J.A. Mental Health, Quality of Life and Violence Exposure in Low-Socioeconomic Status Children and Adolescents of Guatemala. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 7620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Evans, G.W.; Kim, P. Multiple risk exposure as a potential explanatory mechanism for the socioeconomic status-health gradient. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2010, 1186, 174–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Luby, J.; Belden, A.; Botteron, K.; Marrus, N.; Harms, M.P.; Babb, C.; Nishino, T.; Barch, D. The effects of poverty on childhood brain development: The mediating effect of caregiving and stressful life events. JAMA Pediatr. 2013, 167, 1135–1142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Mackey, A.P.; Finn, A.S.; Leonard, J.A.; Jacoby-Senghor, D.S.; West, M.R.; Gabrieli, C.F.O.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Neuroanatomical correlates of the income-achievement gap. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 26, 925–933. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Noble, K.G.; Houston, S.M.; Kan, E.; Sowell, E.R. Neural correlates of socioeconomic status in the developing human brain. Dev. Sci. 2012, 15, 516–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Yu, Q.; Daugherty, A.M.; Anderson, D.M.; Nishimura, M.; Brush, D.; Hardwick, A.; Lacey, W.; Raz, S.; Ofen, N. Socioeconomic status and hippocampal volume in children and young adults. Dev. Sci. 2018, 21, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Ison, M.S.; Greco, C.; Korzeniowski, C.; Morelato, G. Selective Attention: A comparative study on Argentine students from different socioeconomic contexts. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 2015, 13, 343–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Schibli, K.; Wong, K.; Hedayati, N.; D’Angiulli, A. Attending, learning, and socioeconomic disadvantage: Developmental cognitive and social neuroscience of resilience and vulnerability. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2017, 1396, 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Steinmayr, R.; Ziegler, M.; Träuble, B. Do intelligence and sustained attention interact in predicting academic achievement? Learn. Individ. Differ. 2010, 20, 14–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hackman, D.A.; Farah, M.J. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends Cog. Sci. 2009, 13, 65–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  22. Mezzacappa, E. Alerting, Orienting, and Executive Attention: Developmental Properties and Sociodemographic Correlates in an Epidemiological Sample of Young, Urban Children. Child Dev. 2004, 75, 1373–1386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Stevens, C.; Lauinger, B.; Neville, H. Differences in the neural mechanisms of selective attention in children from different socioeconomic backgrounds: An event-related brain potential study. Dev. Sci. 2009, 12, 634–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Clearfield, M.W.; Jedd, K.E. The Effects of Socio-Economic Status on Infant Attention. Infant Child Dev. 2013, 22, 53–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Lupien, S.J.; King, S.; Meaney, M.J. Can poverty get under your skin? Basal cortisol levels and cognitive function in children from low and high socioeconomic status. Dev. Psychopathol. 2001, 13, 653–676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Da Rosa Piccolo, L.; Arteche, A.X.; Fonseca, R.P.; Grassi-Oliveira, R.; Salles, J.F. Influence of family socioeconomic status on IQ, language, memory and executive functions of Brazilian children. Psicol. Reflex. Crit. 2016, 29, 23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Farah, M.J.; Shera, D.M.; Savage, J.H.; Betancourt, L.; Giannetta, J.M.; Brodsky, N.L.; Malmud, E.K.; Hurt, H. Childhood poverty: Specific associations with neurocognitive development. Brain Res. 2006, 1110, 166–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Leonard, J.A.; Mackey, A.P.; Finn, A.S.; Gabrieli, J.D.E. Differential effects of socioeconomic status on working and procedural memory systems. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Matute-Villaseñor, E.; Sanz, A.; Díaz, E.; Roselli, M.; Ardila, A. Influencia del nivel educativo de los padres, el tipo de escuela y el sexo en el desarrollo de la atención y la memoria. Rev. Lationam Psicol. 2009, 41, 257–276. [Google Scholar]
  30. Burneo-Garcés, C.; Cruz-Quintana, F.; Pérez-García, M.; Fernández-Alcántara, M.; Fasfous, A.; Pérez-Marfil, M.N. Interaction between Socioeconomic Status and Cognitive Development in Children Aged 7, 9, and 11 Years: A Cross-Sectional Study. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2019, 44, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ferreira, L.; Godinez, I.; Gabbard, C.; Vieira, J.L.L.; Caçola, P. Motor development in school-age children is associated with the home environment including socioeconomic status. Child Care Health Dev. 2018, 44, 801–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Caldwell, B.M.; Bradley, R.H. Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment: Administration Manual; Family & Human Dynamics Research Institute, Arizona State University: Tempe, AZ, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  33. Bobbio, T.G.; Morcillo, A.M.; Filho, A.D.A.B.; Gonçalves, V.M.G. Factors Associated with Inadequate Fine Motor Skills in Brazilian Students of Different Socioeconomic Status. Percept. Mot. Skills 2007, 105 (Suppl. 3), 1187–1195. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Durán, E.; Pinillos-Patiño, Y.; Herazo-Beltrán, Y.; Galeano-Muñoz, L.; Prieto, E. Determinantes del desempeño psicomotor en escolares de Barranquilla, Colombia. Rev. Salud Pública 2017, 19, 297–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Musso, M. Executive functions: A study about the impact of the poverty on executive performance. Interdisciplinaria 2010, 27, 95–110. [Google Scholar]
  36. Noble, K.; Mccandliss, B.; Farah, M. Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Dev. Sci. 2007, 10, 464–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Hoff, E.; Tian, C. Socioeconomic status and cultural influences on language. J. Commun. Disord. 2005, 38, 271–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Reynolds, S.A.; Andersen, C.; Behrman, J.; Singh, A.; Stein, A.D.; Benny, L.; Crookston, B.T.; Cueto, S.; Dearden, K.; Georgiadis, A.; et al. Disparities in children’s vocabulary and height in relation to household wealth and parental schooling: A longitudinal study in four low- and middle-income countries. SSM Popul. Health 2017, 3, 767–786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Huttenlocher, J.; Vasilyeva, M.; Waterfall, H.R.; Vevea, J.L.; Hedges, L.V. The varieties of speech to young children. Dev. Psychol. 2007, 43, 1062–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Fernald, A.; Marchman, V.A.; Weisleder, A. SES differences in language processing skill and vocabulary are evident at 18 months. Dev. Sci. 2013, 16, 234–248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Spencer, S.; Clegg, J.; Stackhouse, J. Language and disadvantage: A comparison of the language abilities of adolescents from two different socioeconomic areas. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2012, 47, 274–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar]
  43. Stumm, S.V.; Plomin, R. Intelligence Socioeconomic status and the growth of intelligence from infancy through adolescence. Intelligence 2015, 48, 30–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Last, B.S.; Lawson, G.M.; Breiner, K.; Steinberg, L.; Farah, J. Childhood socioeconomic status and executive function in childhood and beyond. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0202964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Arán-Filippetti, V. Funciones ejecutivas en niños escolarizados. Av. Psicol. Latinoam. 2011, 29, 98–113. [Google Scholar]
  46. Arán-filippetti, V.; De Minzi, C.R. A Structural Analysis of Executive Functions and Socioeconomic Status in School-Age Children: Cognitive Factors as Effect Mediators. J. Genet. Psychol. 2012, 173, 393–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Noble, K.G.; Norman, M.F.; Farah, M.J. Neurocognitive correlates of socioeconomic status in kindergarten children. Dev. Sci. 2005, 8, 74–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Vrantsidis, D.M.; Clark, C.A.C.; Chevalier, N.; Espy, K.A.; Wiebe, S.A. Socioeconomic status and executive function in early childhood: Exploring proximal mechanisms. Dev. Sci. 2020, 23, e12917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Lawson, G.M.; Farah, M.J. Executive Function as a Mediator between SES and Academic Achievement throughout Childhood. Int. J. Behav. Dev. 2017, 41, 94–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  50. Korzeniowski, C.; Cupani, M.; Ison, M.; Difabio, H. School performance and poverty: The mediating role of executive functions. EJREP 2016, 14, 474–494. [Google Scholar]
  51. Tine, M. Working Memory Differences between Children Living in Rural and Urban Poverty. J. Cog. Dev. 2013, 15, 599–613. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Duval, E.; Garfinkel, S.; Swain, J.; Evans, G.; Blackburn, E.; Angstadt, M.; Sripada, C.S.; Liberzon, I. Childhood Poverty is Associated with Altered Hippocampal Function and Visuospatial Memory in Adulthood. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2016, 23, 39–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Briceno, A.C.L.; De Feyter, J.J.; Winsler, A. The school readiness of children born to low-income, adolescent Latinas in Miami. Am. J. Orthopsychiatry 2013, 83, 430–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. International Work Group for Indigenous Affairs. Guatemala—IWGIA—International Work Group for Indigenous Affairs. Available online: https://www.iwgia.org/en/guatemala.html (accessed on 17 April 2020).
  55. El Informe de Desarrollo Humano 2014 del PNUD Sugiere la Creación de un Fondo Monetario Regional Para Reducir la Vulnerabilidad Financiera, El PNUD en Guatemala. Available online: https://www.gt.undp.org/content/guatemala/es/home/presscenter/pressreleases/2014/07/24/el-informe-de-desarrollo-humano-2014-del-pnud-sugiere-la-creaci-n-de-un-fondo-monetario-regional-para-reducir-la-vulnerabilidad-financiera-.html (accessed on 21 October 2020).
  56. Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations. Overview of Rural Poverty in Latin America and the Caribbean: Solutions for Eliminating Rural Poverty in the 21 st Century. Available online: http://www.fao.org/3/ca2275en/CA2275EN.pdf (accessed on 21 October 2020).
  57. Freeman, H.E.; Klein, R.E.; Kagan, J.; Yarbrough, C. Relations between nutrition and cognition in rural Guatemala. Am. J. Public Health 1977, 67, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Freeman, H.E.; Klein, R.E.; Townsend, J.W.; Lechtig, A. Nutrition and Cognitive Development among Rural Guatemalan Children. Am. J. Public Health 1980, 70, 1277–1285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Carlson, B. Children Exposed to Intimate Partner Violence: Research Findings and Implications for Intervention. Trauma Violence Abus. 2000, 1, 321–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Mueller, I.; Tronick, E. Early Life Exposure to Violence: Developmental Consequences on Brain and Behavior. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Buchweitz, A.; Azeredo De, L.A.; Sanvicente-vieira, B.; Cará, V.M.; Esper, N.B.; Soder, R.B.; da Costa, J.C.; Portuguez, M.W.; Franco, A.R.; Grassi-Oliveira, R. Violence and Latin-American preadolescents: A study of social brain function and cortisol levels. Dev. Sci. 2019, 22, e12799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Rivera, D.; Arango-Lasprilla, J.C. Methodology for the development of normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric populations. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 581–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  63. Arango-Lasprilla, J.C.; Rivera, D.; Ertl, M.M.; Muñoz-Mancilla, J.M.; García-Guerrero, C.E.; Rodriguez-Irizarry, W.; Arelis, A.A.; Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y.; Nevado, M.B.; Coto, M.V.; et al. Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure—Copy and immediate recall (3 minutes): Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric populations. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 593–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Rivera, D.; Olabarrieta-Landa, L.; Rabago- Barajas, B.V.; Irías-Escher, M.J.; Saracostti-Schwartzman, M.; Ferrer-Cascales, R.; Martín-Lobo, P.; Calderón, J.; Uriarte, M.A.; Guia, A.P.; et al. Newly developed Learning and Verbal Memory Test (TAMV-I): Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 695–706. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Arango-Lasprilla, J.C.; Rivera, D.; Trapp, S.; Jiménez-Pérez, C.; Hernández-Carrillo, C.L.; Pohlenz-Amador, S.; Ver-gara-Moragues, E.; Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y.; Rodriguez-Irizarry, W.; De La Cadena, C.G.; et al. Symbol Digit Modalities Test: Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 639–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Rivera, D.; Salinas, C.; Ramos-Usuga, D.; Delgado-Mejía, I.D.; Vasallo-Key, Y.; Hernández-Agurcia, G.P.; Vásquez, J.V.; García-Guerrero, C.; De La Cadena, C.G.; Barajas, B.R.; et al. Concentration Endurance Test (d2): Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 661–671. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Olabarrieta-Landa, L.; Rivera, D.; Ibáñez-Alfonso, J.A.; Albaladejo-Blázquez, N.; Martín-Lobo, P.; Delgado-Mejía, I.D.; Lara, L.; Barajas, B.R.; Salgado, A.R.; Quispe, L.P.; et al. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III: Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 687–694. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Olabarrieta-Landa, L.; Rivera, D.; Rodríguez-Lorenzana, A.; Pohlenz-Amador, S.; García-Guerrero, C.E.; Padilla-López, A.; Sánchez-SanSegundo, M.; Velázquez-Cardoso, J.; Marante, J.D.; Caparros-Gonzalez, R.; et al. Shortened Version of the Token Test: Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. Neurorehabilitation 2017, 41, 649–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Olabarrieta-Landa, L.; Rivera, D.; Lara, L.; Rute-Pérez, S.; Rodríguez-Lorenzana, A.; Galarza-del-Angel, J.; Guia, A.P.; Ferrer-Cascales, R.; Velázquez-Cardoso, J.; Varillas, A.C.; et al. Verbal fluency tests: Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 673–686. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Arango-Lasprilla, J.C.; Rivera, D.; Ramos-Usuga, D.; Vergara-Moragues, E.; Montero-López, E.; Adana-Díaz, L.A.; Arelis, A.A.; García-Guerrero, C.; De La Cadena, C.G.; Espezúa, X.L.; et al. Trail Making Test: Normative data for the Latin American Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 627–637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Arango-Lasprilla, J.C.; Rivera, D.; Nicholls, E.; Aguayo-Arelis, A.; García de la Cadena, C.; Peñalver-Guia, A.I.; Vergara-Moragues, E.; Rodriguez-Lorenzana, A.; Marín-Morales, A.; Soto-Añari, M.; et al. Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (M-WCST): Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 617–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Rivera, D.; Morlett-Paredes, A.; Peñalver-Guia, A.I.; Irías-Escher, M.J.; Soto-Añari, M.; Aguayo-Arelis, A.; Rute-Pérez, S.; Rodriguez-Lorenzana, A.; Rodríguez-Agudelo, Y.; Albaladejo-Blazquez, N.; et al. Stroop Color-Word Interference Test: Normative data for Spanish-speaking pediatric population. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 605–616. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  73. IBM Corps. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 26.0; IBM Corps: Armonk, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  74. Reifman, A.; Keyton, K. Winsorize. In Encyclopedia of Research Design; Salkind, N.J., Ed.; Sage: Thousand Oaks, CA, USA, 2010; pp. 1636–1638. [Google Scholar]
  75. Frey, P.D.; Pinelli, B. Visual Discrimination and Visuomotor Integration among Two Classes of Brazilian Children. Percept. Mot. Skills 1991, 72, 847–850. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Scott, A.B.; Reed, R.G.; Garcia-Willingham, N.E.; Lawrence, K.A.; Segerstrom, S.C. Lifespan Socioeconomic Context: Associations ith Cognitive Functioning in Later Life. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2019, 74, 113–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Raffington, L.; Prindle, J.; Keresztes, A.; Binder, J.; Heim, C.; Shing, Y.L. Blunted cortisol stress reactivity in low-income children relates to lower memory function. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2018, 90, 110–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Brandes-Aitken, A.; Braren, S.; Swingler, M.; Voegtline, K.; Blair, C. Sustained attention in infancy: A foundation for the development of multiple aspects of self-regulation for children in poverty. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2019, 184, 192–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Machlin, L.; McLaughlin, K.A.; Sheridan, M.A. Brain structure mediates the association between socioeconomic status and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Dev. Sci. 2020, 23, e12844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. D’Angiulli, A.; Lipina, S.J.; Olesinska, A. Explicit and implicit issues in the developmental cognitive neuroscience of social inequality. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2012, 6, 254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Schrieff-Elson, L.E.; Ockhuizen, J.R.H.; During, G.; Thomas, K.G.F. Attention-training with children from socioeconomically disadvantaged backgrounds in Cape Town. J. Child Adolesc. Ment. Health 2017, 29, 147–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Schwab, J.F.; Lew-Williams, C. Language learning, socioeconomic status, and child-directed speech. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Cogn. Sci. 2016, 7, 264–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Arán-Filippetti, V. Fluidez verbal según tipo de tarea, intervalo de tiempo y estrato socioeconómico, en niños escolarizados. An. Psicol. 2011, 27, 816–826. [Google Scholar]
  84. Bialystok, E. Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Biling Lang. Cogn. 2009, 12, 3–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Meir, N.; Armon-lotem, S. Independent and Combined Effects of Socioeconomic Status (SES) and Bilingualism on Children’s Vocabulary and Verbal Short-Term Memory. Front. Psychol. 2017, 8, 1442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  86. Pearce, W.M.; Flanagan, K. Language abilities of Indigenous and non-Indigenous Australian children from low socioeconomic backgrounds in their first year of school. Int. J. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2019, 21, 212–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Hackman, D.A.; Gallop, R.; Evans, G.W.; Farah, M.J. Socioeconomic status and executive function: Developmental trajectories and mediation. Dev. Sci. 2015, 18, 686–702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Korkman, M.; Kirk, U.; Kemp, S. NEPSY—Second Edition (NEPSY-II), 2nd ed.; Harcourt Assessment: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
  89. Hartanto, A.; Toh, W.X.; Yang, H. Bilingualism Narrows Socioeconomic Disparities in Executive Functions and Self-Regulatory Behaviors during Early Childhood: Evidence from the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study. Child Dev. 2019, 90, 1215–1235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Blom, E.; Küntay, A.C.; Messer, M.; Verhagen, J.; Leseman, P. The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish–Dutch children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 2014, 128, 105–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  91. Engel de Abreu, P.; Cruz-Santos, A.; Tourinho, C.; Martin, R.; Bialystok, E. Bilingualism Enriches the Poor: Enhanced Cognitive Control in Low-Income Minority Children. Psychol. Sci. 2012, 23, 1364–1371. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Calvo, A.; Bialystok, E. Independent effects of bilingualism and socioeconomic status on language ability and executive functioning. Cognition 2014, 130, 278–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. White, L.J.; Greenfield, D.B. Executive functioning in Spanish- and English-speaking Head Start preschoolers. Dev. Sci. 2017, 20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Krizman, J.; Skoe, E.; Kraus, N. Bilingual enhancements have no socioeconomic boundaries. Dev. Sci. 2016, 19, 881–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Rey, A. REY: Test de Copia y de Reproducción de Memoria de Figuras Geométricas Complejas; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  96. Cortés, S.; Galindo, J.F.; Villa, M.G.; Salvador, C.J. La Figura Compleja de Rey para niños: Propiedades psicométricas. Salud Ment. 1997, 20, 17–20. [Google Scholar]
  97. Hooper, H.E. Hooper Visual Organization Test (HVOT); Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
  98. Wechsler, D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Revised; Harcourt Brace Jovanovich, Inc.: Toronto, ON, Canada, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  99. Smith, A. Manual de Test de Símbolos y Dígitos SDMT. Publicaciones de Psicología Aplicada; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  100. Strober, L.; DeLuca, J.; Benedict, R.H.; Jacobs, A.; Cohen, J.A.; Chiaravalloti, N.; Hudson, L.D.; Rudick, R.A.; LaRocca, N.G. Symbol Digit Modalities Test: A valid clinical trial endpoint for measuring cognition in multiple sclerosis. Mult. Scler. 2019, 25, 1781–1790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Brickenkamp, R. Manual del Test de Atención d2; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  102. Bates, M.E.; Lemay, E.P.J. The d2 Test of attention: Construct validity and extensions in scoring techniques. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2004, 10, 392–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Brickenkamp, R.; Schmidt-Atzert, L.; Liepmann, D.; Zillmer, E. d2- R Test di Attenzione Concentrata [d2-R Test of Sustained Attention]; Hogrefe: Firenze, Italy, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  104. Cuesta-Izquierdo, M.; de Iscar-Pérez, M.J.; Begega-Losa, M.A.; Mendez-López, M.; Alvarez-Pérez, L.; Solís, G.; Fernández Colomer, M.B.; Arias Perez, J.L. Psychometric properties of the d2 selective attention test in a sample of premature and born-at-term babies. Psicothema 2007, 19, 706–710. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  105. Dunn, L.M.; Dunn, L.M.; Arribas, D. PPVT-III Peabody. Test de Vocabulario en Imágenes, 2nd ed.; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  106. Williams, K.T.; Wang, J. Technical References to the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Third Edition (PPVT-III), 3rd ed.; American Guidance Service: Circle Pines, MN, USA, 1997. [Google Scholar]
  107. Hayward, D.V.; Stewart, G.E.; Phillips, L.M.; Norris, S.P.; Lovell, M.A. Test review: Peabody picture vocabulary test–III (PPVT-III). Available online: http://www.uofaweb.ualberta.ca/elementaryed/ccrl.cfm (accessed on 10 October 2020).
  108. Henninger, N. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test. In Encyclopedia of Clinical Neuropsychology; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  109. De Renzi, E.; Faglioni, P. Development of a shortened version of the Token Test. Cortex 1978, 14, 41–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Gallardo, G.; Guàrdia, J.; Villaseñor, T.; McNeil, M.R. Psychometric data for the revised token test in normally developing Mexican children ages 4–12 years. Arch Clin NeuroPsychol. Off J. Natl. Acad. Neuropsychol. 2011, 26, 225–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  111. McNeil, M.R.; Pratt, S.R.; Szuminsky, N.; Sung, J.E.; Fossett, T.R.D.; Fassbinder, W.; Lim, K.Y. Reliability and validity of the computerized Revised Token Test: Comparison of reading and listening versions in persons with and without aphasia. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2015, 58, 311–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Strauss, E.; Sherman, E.; Spreen, O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests. Administration, Norms and Commentary, 3rd ed.; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  113. Benton, A.L.; Hamsher, K.D. Multilingual Aphasia Examination; AJA Associates: Iowa City, IA, USA, 1989. [Google Scholar]
  114. Preston, J.L. Speech and literacy: The connection and the relevance to clinical populations. In Speech Disorders: Causes, Treatment and Social Effects; Harrison, A.E., Ed.; Languages and Linguistics Series: York, UK, 2010; pp. 43–73. [Google Scholar]
  115. Reitan, R.M.; Wolfson, D. The Halstead-Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation; Neuropsychology Press: Tucson, AZ, USA, 1985. [Google Scholar]
  116. Arbuthnott, K.; Frank, J. Trail making test, part B as a measure of executive control: Validation using a set-switching paradigm. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2000, 22, 518–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. Giovagnoli, A.R.; Mascheroni, S.; Simoncelli, M.; Laiacona, M.; Capitani, E. Trail Making Test: Normative values from287 normal adult controls. Ital. J. Neurol. Sci. 1996, 17, 305–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Schretlen, D.J. Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test: M-WCST; Professional Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  119. Heaton, R.K. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources: Odessa, FL, USA, 1981. [Google Scholar]
  120. Ozonoff, S.; Mcevoy, R.E. A longitudinal-study of executive function and theory of mind development in autism. Dev. Psychopathol. 1994, 6, 415–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Golden, C.J. Manual de Test de Colores y Palabras. Publicaciones de Psicología Aplicada; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, España, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  122. Barreto, L.C.R.; Pulido, N.C.; Roa, C.A.P. Psychometric properties of the stroop color-word test in non-pathological colombian population. Univ. Psychol. 2016, 15, 255–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Table 1. Motor skills and memory tests raw scores and independent samples t-test results.
Table 1. Motor skills and memory tests raw scores and independent samples t-test results.
VulnerableNon-Vulnerable
TasksnM (SD)nM (SD)dft StatisticCIpd
ROCF
Copy14329.2 (8.3)20130.1 (6.2)249.5−1.1[2.5, 0.6]0.2890.13
Memory14318.2 (9.8)20118.7 (7.2)245.5−0.5[−2.3, 1.4]0.5870.06
TAMV-I
Verbal learning14430.6 (8.0)20331.7 (7.3)345−1.4[−3.0, 0.5]0.1890.15
Memory delay1448.8 (2.7)2039.1 (2.1)260.2−1.2[−0.8, 0.2]0.2070.13
Recognition14411.1 (1.4)20311.2 (1.3)345−1.1[−0.4, 0.1]0.2760.07
ROCF: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; TAMV-I: Learning and Verbal Memory Test.
Table 2. Attention tests raw scores and independent samples t-test results.
Table 2. Attention tests raw scores and independent samples t-test results.
VulnerableNon-Vulnerable
TasksnM (SD)nM (SD)dft StatisticCIpd
SDMT13730.3 (14.8)20334.3 (11.4)241.12−2.6[−6.9, −0.9]0.0120.31
D2-CON13793.3 (52.7)193121.3 (40.7)244.49−5.2[−38.1, −17.1]0.0010.61
SDMT: Symbol Digit Test; D2-CON: Concentration Endurance Test (D2) Concentration Index.
Table 3. Language tests raw scores and independent samples t-test results.
Table 3. Language tests raw scores and independent samples t-test results.
VulnerableNon-Vulnerable
TasksnM (SD)nM (SD)dft StatisticCIpd
PPVT-III14385.9 (30.0)199109.2 (24.6)267.3−7.6[−29.2, −17.3]0.0010.87
Token14429 (3.8)20331 (2.7)238.4−5.4[−2.7, −1.3]0.0010.63
Verbal fluency
/f/1435.7 (4.1)2037.1 (3.3)261.1−3.2[−2.1, −0.5]0.0020.38
/s/1436.9 (4.0)2037.7 (3.4)274.3−2.0[−1.6, 0.0]0.0480.22
/a/1437.0 (3.9)2037.8 (3.3)278.2−2.0[−1.6, 0.0]0.0510.23
Animals14413.7 (4.8)20315(4.4)345−2.5[−2.3, −0.3]0.0160.29
Fruits14410.8 (3.2)20310.7 (2.9)3450.1[−0.7, 0.7]0.9220.03
PPVT-III: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Third edition; Token: Shortened version of the Token Test.
Table 4. Executive functions tests raw scores and independent samples t-test results.
Table 4. Executive functions tests raw scores and independent samples t-test results.
VulnerableNon-Vulnerable
TasksnM (SD)nM (SD)dftCIpd
Stroop
Word11470.1 (23.7)19074.4 (16.6)180.2−1.7[−9.2, 0.4]0.0890.22
Color11453.6 (14.2)19052.4 (12.5)214.90.7[−2.1, 4.3]0.5060.09
Word-color11431.5 (8)19029 (9.3)3022.4[0.4, 4.5]0.0140.28
Interference1141.7 (7.5)190−1.5 (7.1)3023.6[1.6, 4.9]0.0010.44
TMT
A13948 (22.6)20330.7 (11.5)186.98.3[13.5, 21.6]0.0011.03
B12080.2 (36.8)20357.9 (24.9)184.45.9[15.3, 29.3]0.0010.75
M-WCST
Correct cat.1434.6 (1.8)2024 (1.8)3433.0[0.2, 0.9]0.0060.33
Pers. errors1434.3 (4.8)2024.2 (3.8)3430.3[−0.8, 1.2]0.7650.02
Total errors14312.9 (10.6)20215.2 (9.2)343−2.2[−4.4, −0.2]0.0290.24
Stroop: Stroop Color-word interference test; TMT: Trail Making Test; M-WCST: Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; Correct cat.: Correct categories; Pers. Errors.: Perseverative errors.
Table 5. Raw scores by age group and vulnerability condition.
Table 5. Raw scores by age group and vulnerability condition.
Children (6–11 Years)Adolescents (12–17 Years)
TasksVulnerableNon-VulnerableVulnerableNon-Vulnerable
nM (SD)nM (SD)nM (SD)nM (SD)
ROCF
Copy8625.33 (8.69)12828.60 (6.69)5735.09 (1.29)7332.76 (4.09)
Memory8613.67 (8.70)12816.26 (6.41)5725.13 (6.83)7323.09 (6.27)
TAMV-I
Verbal learning8728.32 (8.32)12930.54 (7.71)5733.95 (6.14)7433.81 (6.16)
Memory delay877.76 (2.79)1298.78 (2.23)5710.33 (1.39)749.66 (1.66)
Recognition8710.64 (1.63)12911.01 (1.42)5711.70 (0.53)7411.58 (0.89)
SDMT8021 (10.58)12928.94 (9.29)5743.37 (8.66)7443.59 (8.39)
D2- CON8062.59 (36.65)122105.26 (34.44)57136.39 (40.20)71148.96 (35.65)
PPVT-III8669.81 (22.76)12899.01 (21.28)57110.07 (22.47)71127.51 (18.90)
Token8727.70 (4.01)12930.34 (2.69)5731.05 (2.41)7432.21 (2.17)
Verbal fluency
/f/863.64 (3.04)1296.09 (2.88)578.89 (3.51)748.77 (3.30)
/s/865.15 (3.24)1296.77 (3.03)579.58 (3.6)749.45 (3.43)
/a/865.48 (3.45)1296.78 (2.93)579.37 (3.16)749.64 (3.24)
Animals8711.76 (4.11)12913.29 (3.55)5716.68 (4.11)7417.91 (4.16)
Fruits879.36 (3.14)1299.82 (2.41)5712.88 (1.98)7412.27 (2.98)
Stroop
Word5754.42 (21.95)12068.59 (15.64)5785.82 (12.04)7084.34 (13.14)
Color5744.32 (10.53)12048.61 (11.27)5762.82 (11.11)7058.97 (11.96)
Word-color5727.14 (6.87)12026.32 (8.14)5735.88 (6.63)7033.63 (9.36)
Interference573.41 (7.63)120−1.88 (7.22)57−0.97 (6.99)70−0.78 (6.88)
TMT
A8251.98 (24.84)12932.47 (11.25)5742.30 (17.56)7427.49 (11.18)
B6381.80 (40.56)12963.13 (25.63)5778.41 (32.33)7448.72 (20.79)
M-WCST
Correct cat.864.06 (1.88)1283.61 (1.71)575.44 (1.13)744.76 (1.69)
Pers. errors865.83 (5.46)1284.98 (3.99)572.07 (2.30)742.78 (2.95)
Total errors8616.29 (11.14)12817.59 (8.73)577.84 (7.23)7411.18 (8.65)
ROCF: Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test; TAMV-I: Learning and Verbal Memory Test; SDMT: Symbol Digit Test; D2-CON: Concentration Endurance Test (D2) Concentration Index; PPVT-III: Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, Third edition; Token: Shortened version of the Token Test; Stroop: Stroop Color-word interference test; TMT: Trail Making Test; M-WCST: Modified Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; Correct cat.: Correct categories; Pers. Errors.: Perseverative errors.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ibáñez-Alfonso, J.A.; Company-Córdoba, R.; García de la Cadena, C.; Sianes, A.; Simpson, I.C. How Living in Vulnerable Conditions Undermines Cognitive Development: Evidence from the Pediatric Population of Guatemala. Children 2021, 8, 90. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020090

AMA Style

Ibáñez-Alfonso JA, Company-Córdoba R, García de la Cadena C, Sianes A, Simpson IC. How Living in Vulnerable Conditions Undermines Cognitive Development: Evidence from the Pediatric Population of Guatemala. Children. 2021; 8(2):90. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020090

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ibáñez-Alfonso, Joaquín A., Rosalba Company-Córdoba, Claudia García de la Cadena, Antonio Sianes, and Ian Craig Simpson. 2021. "How Living in Vulnerable Conditions Undermines Cognitive Development: Evidence from the Pediatric Population of Guatemala" Children 8, no. 2: 90. https://doi.org/10.3390/children8020090

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop