Two Cognitive Dimensions of Students’ Mental Models in Science: Fidelity of Gestalt and Functional Fidelity
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- Further research is needed to explore the structure of students’ mental models in quantum physics beyond the context of the atomic hull.
- Empirical studies are needed to check the validity of the above-mentioned framework presented by Ubben and Heusler ([9], p. 1360) in quantum physics education and beyond. In this respect, confirmatory statistical methods are required.
- Finally, it would have to be clarified as to how and to what extent this framework describing the structure of mental models via the two dimensions Fidelity of Gestalt and Fidelity of Function could inform physics instruction or physics teacher education.
- On the one hand, quantum effects whose explanation requires a photon model, have no classical analogues. Hence, no classical framework is valid for the description of such effects.
- In contrast to further quantum concepts without classical analogy (e.g., quantum entanglement), the photon concept is still an integral element of international secondary school curricula [14]. Hence, the context of photons (on the level typically presented in secondary schools seems a suitable one for our study: since the participating secondary school students have been introduced to photons in their regular classes on quantum physics prior to our study, they may already have built their own mental models of the topic under investigation.
- In fact, this specific topic choice goes along with some limitations for our study which we reflect on in Section 7 of this article.
2. Theoretical Background on Mental Models
- some structural component (gestalt),
- some functional, abstract component it represents or codes (functionality),
- subjectivity of the mental model and being based on past experiences (individual).
- …circumvent having to ascribe a certain value to a gestalt and thus,
- …make the definition of a mental model more value-independent.
2.1. Educational Psychology Perspective
2.2. Neurological Perspective
3. Research Question
- As has been shown in Section 2, findings from educational psychology and neurology suggest these two primary dimensions in students’ mental models.
- The topics of the atomic shell and photons are different at first glance, but close in terms of content, e.g., when we think about electrons in the atomic shell that are quantum objects in the same way that photons are.
4. Methods
4.1. Study Design and Sample
4.2. Instrument
4.3. Data Analysis
5. Results
5.1. Descriptives and Item Correlations
5.2. Exploratory Factor Analysis Results
5.3. Interpretation of Exploratory Factor Analysis Results
- Item 1: In an interferometer the photon behaves like a particle and like a wave. It is none of them.This item can be associated with Functional Fidelity because of the key expression “behaves like” which necessarily refers to a functional aspect. Additionally, using a wave and a particle as possible options simultaneously is known to be low in Fidelity of Gestalt in some cases (cf. [50]), thus possibly shifting the attention to the functions.
- Item 4: I cannot make statements about the behavior of single photons inside the interferometer. I can only make statements about the statistical behavior of many identically prepared photons.This item can be associated with Functional Fidelity because of the key expression “behavior of single photons” which necessarily refers to a functional aspect. In addition to this, statistical behavior is addressed, which is functional as well.
- Item 5: The current position of a photon between source and detector is indeterminate in principle.This item can also be associated with Functional Fidelity because its emphasis does not lay on the single position but on how to determine the position, which is an interactive action between some entity and the photon, making this statement functional.
- Item 6: The current position of a photon between source and detector is not indeterminate in principle, but unknown to the experimenter.This item can be associated with Functional Fidelity because its emphasis does not lay on the single position but on how to determine the position, which is an interactive action between some entity and the photon, making this statement functional.
- Item 8: In quantum physics it is possible that a quantum object does not possess classically well-defined properties, such as position.This item can be associated with Functional Fidelity because of the key expression “does not possess classically well-defined properties” which necessarily refers to a functional aspect. Though many properties are functional, some of gestalt may be associated as well (like e.g., “shape”), which is consistent with the finding that this item loads onto the factor Fidelity of Gestalt as well (secondary loading , cf. Figure 2).
- Item 10: With sufficient knowledge of the initial conditions, it would be possible in classical physics to predict the outcome of a dice roll.This item can be associated with Functional Fidelity because the given statement fits to an abstract understanding of randomness in classical physics.
- Item 2: When the photon inside the interferometer moves towards the detector it takes a specific path, even if I cannot determine this path.This item can be associated with Fidelity of Gestalt because of the key expression “specific path” which necessarily refers to a gestalt aspect: one may think of a well-defined trajectory the photon follows. This seems to be counterintuitive, but paths have been found to mainly be associated with lines by students before [51], p. 191, making this finding indeed consistent with previous research.
- Item 3: The photon follows a specific path, regardless of whether I observe this path or not.This item can be associated with Fidelity of Gestalt because of the key expression “specific path” which necessarily refers to a gestalt aspect: one may think of a well-defined trajectory the photon follows. For the same reason as item 2, the categorization as addressing Fidelity of Gestalt is consistent.
- Item 7: No one can tell with certainty if a photon is transmitted or reflected at a beam splitter cube.Though at first glance, this item seems to be of functional nature by looking at the words “transmitted” and “reflected”, the main aspect appears to be the simultaneity of the actions, both of which can be imagined by different paths at once in one mental model. It is thereby the opposite of item 1, as not two gestalts are addressed that lead the focus on functions but two functions which bring the focus on the gestalt.
- Item 9: With sufficient knowledge of the initial conditions, it would be possible to predict if a single photon is transmitted or reflected at the beam splitter.This item can be associated with Fidelity of Gestalt because the prediction of a single photon’s behavior at the beam splitter cube is likely to go along with an imagination of a well-defined trajectory the photon follows (cf. the above argumentation for item 2).
6. Discussion
6.1. Functional Fidelity and Fidelity of Gestalt as Dimensions to Describe Students’ Mental Models of Photons
- Non-developed type: low degree of FG and FF.Example: Students of this type have no knowledge about the term photon and, thus, their imagination is non-developed.
- Architectural type: high degree of FG, low degree of FF.
- Dual type: high degree of FG, high degree of FF.Example: Students of this type describe photons as energy quanta of light in a very abstract way. However, whenever it comes to describing concrete experiments, these students fall back to a particle notion in a naïve sense (cf. [54]).
- Functional type: low degree of FG, high degree of FF.Example: Students of this type describe photons as energy quanta of light in a very abstract way (cf. [54]).
6.2. Describing the Structure of Students’ Mental Models via the Dimensions Fidelity of Gestalt and Functional Fidelity
7. Limitations
- In this project, we have adopted a questionnaire to elicit students’ mental models [44] of photons’ properties and behaviour, which is accepted in the quantum physics education research community and which has previously been used in several empirical studies on learning quantum physics (e.g., [8,9,45]). However, this instrument is not specifically based on our theoretical framework on mental models in quantum physics. This means that it was not developed with the objective of operationalising the two factors of Fidelity of Gestalt and Functional Fidelity of mental models by means of test items. Consequently, by developing a survey instrument to investigate students’ mental models in the quantum physics context that addresses the two dimensions of Fidelity of Gestalt and Functional Fidelity in specifically developed test items, we expect an even clearer manifestation of the two factors in student thinking. The development of such an instrument is part of future research.
- In this study, we investigated the extent to which the description of mental models from a rather theoretical perspective by means of Fidelity of Gestalt and Functional Fidelity in student thinking is possible in quantum physics beyond the atomic hull context [9]. In order to investigate this transferability, we have decided to survey the structure of learners’ mental models on a central topic that covers the characteristic features of quantum physics in a first step: photons’ properties and behavior. Certainly, this selection limits the generalizability of our empirical findings; in particular, from our findings, we cannot conclude that learners’ mental models of all quantum concepts can be described by these two dimensions. It is possible that for certain quantum concepts, further sub-dimensions of Gestalt or Functional Fidelity could be found in the learners’ thinking. This needs to be addressed in follow-up studies.
- We note, that in teaching quantum physics at the secondary school level it is widespread practice to transfer properties to photons that were previously studied in the context of electrons (cf. [14,53]). However, an isomorphic mapping between electrons and photons is not valid from a subject-specific point of view, and the problem of using an analogy between photons and electrons has already been highlighted in Ref. [63], p. 650: for example, a photon position operator does not exist and, hence, no position eigenstates either [64]. Instead, photons are assigned a meaning as elementary field mode excitations in quantum electrodynamics [65]. Therefore, the question arises whether our results regarding the structure of students’ thinking about photons are not actually consistent with the findings of [9] presented earlier for the atomic hull context, because electrons and photons are usually introduced in secondary school lessons in analogy to each other without regarding them as different concepts from a subject-specific point of view. In order to tackle this limitation, we can come back to neurological findings presented in Section 2 of this article that suggest that the cognitive dimensions (Fidelity of Gestalt and Functional Fidelity) in learners’ mental models of photons empirically uncovered in this article, indeed, appear to be deeply rooted in human thinking. Nonetheless, we acknowledge a need for further research in this context: for example, analyzing the mental models of learners who were introduced to quantum field theory might add value to the field.
- Furthermore, it is noteworthy, that we limited our study to secondary school students. Therefore, generalising our findings to learners from other educational backgrounds is not straightforward. However, the earlier work of [9] suggests that one might find a congruent structure of mental models in individuals from different backgrounds, differing only in the expressions of Fidelity of Gestalt and Functional Fidelity in student thinking.
8. Conclusions and Outlook
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Ke, J.-L.; Monk, M.; Duschl, R. Learning Introductory Quantum Physics: Sensori-motor experiences and mental models. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2005, 27, 1571–1594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Abhang, R.Y. Making introductory quantum physics understandable and interesting. Res. J. Sci. Educ. 2005, 10, 63–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Singh, C.; Marshman, E. A Review of student difficulties in upper-Level quantum mechanics. Phys. Rev. ST Phys. Educ. Res. 2015, 11, 020117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalkanis, G.; Hadzidaki, P.; Stavrou, D. An instructional model for a radical conceptual change towards quantum mechanics concepts. Sci. Educ. 2003, 87, 257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henriksen, E.K.; Angell, C.; Vistnes, A.I.; Bungum, B. What ist Light? Students’ Reflections on the Wave-Particle Duality of Light and the Nature of Physics. Sci. Educ. 2018, 27, 81–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Taber, K.S. Learning Quanta: Barriers to stimulating transitions in student understanding of orbital ideas. Sci. Educ. 2005, 89, 94–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bouchée, T.; de Putter-Smits, L.; Thurlings, M.; Pepin, B. Towards a better understanding of conceptual difficulties in introductory quantum physics courses. Stud. Sci. Educ. 2021, 2021, 1–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bitzenbauer, P. Effect of an introductory quantum physics course using experiments with heralded photons on preuniversity students’ conceptions about quantum physics. Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 2021, 17, 020103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ubben, M.; Heusler, S. Gestalt and Functionality as Independent Dimensions of Mental Models in Science. Res. Sci. Educ. 2021, 51, 1349–1363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Treagust, D.; Chittleborough, G.; Mamiala, T. Students’ understanding of the role of scientific models in learning science. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2002, 24, 357–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stefani, C.; Tsaparlis, G. Students’ levels of explanations, models, and misconceptions in basic quantum chemistry: A phenomenographic study. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2009, 46, 520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mashhadi, A.; Woolnough, B. Insights into students’ understanding of quantum physics: Visualizing quantum entities. Eur. J. Phys. 1999, 2, 511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sayer, R.; Maries, A.; Singh, C. A quantum interactive learning tutorial on the double-slit experiment to improve student understanding of quantum mechanics. Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 2017, 13, 010123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Stadermann, H.K.E.; van den Berg, E.; Goedhart, M.J. Analysis of secondary school quantum physics curricula of 15 different countries: Different perspectives on a challenging topic. Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 2019, 15, 010130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hentschel, K. Photons; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson-Laird, P.N. Mental Models: Towards a Cognitive Science of Language, Inferences, and Consciousness; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Gentner, D.; Stevens, A.L. Mental Models; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Craik, K. The Nature of Exploration; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1983. [Google Scholar]
- Horst, S. Cognitive Pluralism; The MIT Press: Cambridge, UK, 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Amin, T.G.; Smith, C.; Wiser, M. Student Conceptions and Conceptual Change: Three Overlapping Phases of Research. In Handbook of Research in Science Education; Lederman, N., Abell, S., Eds.; Chapter 4; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2014; Volume II. [Google Scholar]
- Mayer, R.E. Multimedia Learning; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Halpern, A.R.; Zatorre, R.J. When that tune runs through your head: A PET investigation of auditory imagery for familiar melodies. Cerebral Cortex 1999, 9, 697–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paivio, A. Imagery and Verbal Processes; Holt, Rinehart and Winston: New York, NY, USA, 1971. [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, J.J. The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems; Houghton Mifflin: Boston, MA, USA, 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, J.J. The theory of affordances. In Perceiving, Acting, and Knowing: Toward an Ecological Psychology; Shaw, R., Bransford, J., Eds.; Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Gibson, J.J. An Ecological Approach to Visual Perception; Lawrence Erlbaum: New York, NY, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
- Panksepp, J. Affective Neuroscience; Oxford: New York, NY, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Peterson, J.B. Maps of Meaning: The Architecture of Belief; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
- Dagar, V.; Yadav, A. Constructivism: A Paradigm for Teaching and Learning. Arts Soc. Sci. J. 2016, 7, 2–4. [Google Scholar]
- Carey, S. The Origin of Concepts; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Piaget, J. Play, Dreams and Imitation in Childhood; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 1951. [Google Scholar]
- Fixione, M.; Lieto, A. Representing Non Classical Concepts in Formal Ontologies: Prototypes and Exemplars. In New Challenges in Distributed Information Filtering and Retrieval; Lai, C., Semeraro, G., Vargiu, E., Eds.; Springer: Heidelberg, Germany, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Burge, T. Five theses on de re states and attitudes. In The Philosophy of David Kaplan; Almog, J., Leonardi, P., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
- Hubber, P. Year 12 students’ mental models of the nature of light. Res. Sci. Educ. 2006, 36, 419–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sherin, B. Common sense clarified: The role of intuitive knowledge in physics problem solving. Res. Sci. Teach. 2006, 43, 535–555. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levy, J.; Trevarthen, C. Metacontrol of Hemispheric Function in Human Split-Brain Patients. J. Exp. Psychol. Human Percept. Perform. 1976, 2, 299–312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gazzaniga, M.S. Forty-five years of split-brain research and still going strong. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2005, 6, 653–659. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roser, M.E.; Fugelsang, J.A.; Dunbar, K.N.; Corballis, P.M.; Gazzaniga, M.S. Dissociating processes supporting causal perception and causal inference in the brain. Neuropsychology 2005, 19, 591–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Gopnik, A.; Glymour, C.; Sobel, D.M.; Schulz, L.E.; Kushnir, T.; Danks, D. A Theory of Causal Learning in Children: Causal Maps and Bayes Nets. Psychol. Rev. 2004, 111, 3–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Scholl, B.J.; Tremoulet, P. Perceptual Causality and Animacy. Trends Cognit. Sci. 2000, 4, 299–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scholl, B.J.; Nakayama, K. Causal Capture: Contextual Effects on the Perception of Collision Events. Psychol. Sci. 2002, 13, 493–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ireson, G.A. multivariate analysis of undergraduate physics students’ conceptions of quantum phenomena. Eur. J. Phys. 1999, 20, 193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mashhadi, A.; Woolnough, B. Cognitive mapping of advanced level physics students’ conceptions of quantum physics. In Proceedings of the Conference on Educational Research (Australian Association for Research in Education), Singapore, 25–29 November 1996; Available online: https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED414195.pdf (accessed on 20 January 2022).
- Müller, R.; Wiesner, H. Teaching quantum mechanics on an introductory level. Am. J. Phys. 2002, 70, 200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Müller, R. Quantenphysik in der Schule; Logos: Berlin, Germany, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Marshman, E.; Singh, C. Investigating and improving student understanding of quantum mechanics in the context of single photon interference. Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 2017, 13, 010117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hemphill, J.F. Interpreting the Magnitudes of Correlation Coefficients. Am. Psych. 2003, 58, 78–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kaiser, H. The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 1960, 20, 141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brown, J.D. Choosing the right type of rotation in PCA and EFA. Shiken JALT Test. Eval. SIG Newsl. 2009, 13, 20. [Google Scholar]
- Bormann, M. Das Schülervorverständnis zu Elektronen. In Vorträge der DPG; Kuhn, W., Ed.; DPG: Gießen, Germany, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- Kawalkar, A.; Vijapurkar, J.D. What Do Cells Really Look Like? Children’s Resistance to Accepting a 3-D Model. Proc. EpiSTEME 2009, 3, 187–193. [Google Scholar]
- Ayene, M.; Kriek, J.; Damtie, B. Wave-particle duality and uncertainty principle: Phenomenographic categories of description of tertiary physics students’ depictions. Phys. Rev. ST Phys. Educ. Res. 2011, 7, 020113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krijtenburg-Lewerissa, K.; Pol, H.J.; Brinkman, A.; van Joolingen, W.R. Insights into teaching quantum mechanics in secondary and lower undergraduate education. Phys. Rev. Phys. Educ. Res. 2017, 13, 010109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bitzenbauer, P.; Meyn, J.-P. Towards types of students’ conceptions about photons: Results of an interview study. In Physics Teacher Education—What Matters? Bonello, C., Marks, J.B., Galea, A., Eds.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2022. [Google Scholar]
- Fedele, B.; Michelini, M.; Stefanel, A. 5–10 year old pupils explore magnetic phenomena in Cognitive Laboratory (CLOE). In Cresils; Pint, R., Couso, D., Eds.; University of Udine: Udine, Italy, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Greca, I.M.; Moreira, M.A. The kinds of mental representations - models, propositions and images—Used by college physics students regarding the concept of field. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1997, 19, 711–724. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saglam, M.; Millar, R. Upper High School Students’ Understanding of Electromagnetism. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2006, 28, 543–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guisasol, J.; Almud, J.M.; Zubimed, J.L. Difficulties in Learning the Introductory Magnetic Field Theory in the First Years of University. Sci. Educ. 2004, 88, 443–464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Galili, I.; Hazan, A. Learners’ knowledge in optics: Interpretation, structure and analysis. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2000, 22, 57–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Djanette, B.; Fouad, C. Determination of university students’ misconceptions about light using concept maps. Procedia—Soc. Behav. Sci. 2014, 152, 582–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Goldberg, F.M.; McDermott, L.C. Student difficulties in understanding image formation by a plane mirror. Phys. Teach. 1986, 24, 472–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heywood, D.S. Primary trainee teachers’ learning and teaching about light: Some pedagogic implications for initial teacher training. Int. J. Sci. Teach. 2005, 27, 1447–1475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strnad, J. Photons in introductory quantum physics. Am. J. Phys. 1986, 54, 650. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bialynicki-Birula, I.; Bialynicka-Birula, Z. Why photons cannot be sharply localized. Phys. Rev. A 2009, 79, 032112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hobson, A. There are no particles, there are only fields. Am. J. Phys. 2013, 81, 211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
No. | Item |
---|---|
1 | In an interferometer, the photon behaves like a particle and like a wave. It is none of them. |
2 | When the photon inside the interferometer moves towards the detector it takes a specific path, even if I cannot determine this path. |
3 | The photon follows a specific path, regardless of whether I observe this path or not. |
4 | I cannot make statements about the behavior of single photons inside the interferometer. I can only make statements about the statistical behavior of many identically prepared photons. |
5 | The current position of a photon between source and detector is indeterminate in principle. |
6 | The current position of a photon between source and detector is not indeterminate in principle, but unknown to the experimenter. |
7 | No one can tell with certainty if a photon is transmitted or reflected at a beam splitter cube. |
8 | In quantum physics, it is possible that a quantum object does not possess classically well-defined properties, such as position. |
9 | With sufficient knowledge of the initial conditions, it would be possible to predict if a single photon is transmitted or reflected at the beam splitter. |
10 | With sufficient knowledge of the initial conditions, it would be possible in classical physics to predict the outcome of a dice roll. |
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | 1.00 | −0.15 | 0.29 | 0.17 | −0.32 | 0.13 | 0.38 | −0.13 | 0.36 | |
2 | 1.00 | 0.26 | −0.12 | −0.10 | 0.19 | |||||
3 | −0.15 | 0.26 | 1.00 | −0.26 | −0.18 | 0.33 | −0.23 | |||
4 | 0.29 | 1.00 | 0.29 | −0.17 | 0.24 | −0.16 | 0.23 | |||
5 | 0.17 | 0.29 | 1.00 | −0.43 | 0.13 | 0.38 | −0.12 | 0.29 | ||
6 | −0.32 | −0.17 | −0.43 | 1.00 | −0.34 | 0.21 | −0.25 | |||
7 | 0.13 | −0.12 | −0.26 | 0.13 | 1.00 | 0.17 | −0.40 | 0.20 | ||
8 | 0.38 | −0.10 | −0.18 | 0.24 | 0.38 | −0.34 | 0.17 | 1.00 | −0.38 | 0.40 |
9 | −0.13 | 0.19 | 0.33 | −0.16 | −0.12 | 0.21 | −0.40 | −0.38 | 1.00 | −0.39 |
10 | 0.36 | −0.23 | 0.23 | 0.29 | −0.25 | 0.20 | 0.40 | −0.39 | 1.00 |
No. | Item | Mean () | + | ∘ | − |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | In an interferometer, the photon behaves like a particle and like a wave. It is none of them. | ||||
2 | When the photon inside the interferometer moves towards the detector it takes a specific path, even if I cannot determine this path. | ||||
3 | The photon follows a specific path, regardless of whether I observe this path or not. | ||||
4 | I cannot make statements about the behavior of single photons inside the interferometer. I can only make statements about the statistical behavior of many identically prepared photons. | ||||
5 | The current position of a photon between source and detector is indeterminate in principle. | ||||
6 | The current position of a photon between source and detector is not indeterminate in principle, but unknown to the experimenter. | ||||
7 | No one can tell with certainty if a photon is transmitted or reflected at a beam splitter cube. | ||||
8 | In quantum physics, it is possible that a quantum object does not possess classically well-defined properties, such as position. | ||||
9 | With sufficient knowledge of the initial conditions, it would be possible to predict if a single photon is transmitted or reflected at the beam splitter. | ||||
10 | With sufficient knowledge of the initial conditions, it would be possible in classical physics to predict the outcome of a dice roll. |
Item No. | Factor 1 | Factor 2 | |
---|---|---|---|
Factor 1: Functional Fidelity | 1 | 0.61 | −0.12 |
4 | 0.56 | 0.01 | |
5 | 0.70 | 0.04 | |
6 | −0.68 | −0.02 | |
8 | 0.67 | −0.28 | |
10 | 0.59 | −0.34 | |
Factor 2: Fidelity of Gestalt | 2 | 0.05 | 0.52 |
3 | −0.04 | 0.72 | |
7 | 0.10 | −0.65 | |
9 | −0.29 | −0.71 | |
Eigenvalues | 2.97 | 1.47 | |
%variance | 29.73 | 14.69 |
Present Study | Ubben and Heusler [9] | Original Item [44] | |
---|---|---|---|
Functional Fidelity | The current position of a photon between source and detector is indeterminate in principle. | The movement of an electron within the atomic hull can never be determined exactly. | The current position of an electron between source and screen is indeterminate in principle. |
Fidelity of Gestalt | When the photon inside the interferometer moves towards the detector it takes a specific path, even if I cannot determine this path. | Electrons within the atomic hull move along set trajectories with high velocities around the core. | When the electron inside the double-slit experiment moves towards the screen it takes a specific path, even if I cannot determine this path. |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Ubben, M.S.; Bitzenbauer, P. Two Cognitive Dimensions of Students’ Mental Models in Science: Fidelity of Gestalt and Functional Fidelity. Educ. Sci. 2022, 12, 163. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12030163
Ubben MS, Bitzenbauer P. Two Cognitive Dimensions of Students’ Mental Models in Science: Fidelity of Gestalt and Functional Fidelity. Education Sciences. 2022; 12(3):163. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12030163
Chicago/Turabian StyleUbben, Malte S., and Philipp Bitzenbauer. 2022. "Two Cognitive Dimensions of Students’ Mental Models in Science: Fidelity of Gestalt and Functional Fidelity" Education Sciences 12, no. 3: 163. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12030163
APA StyleUbben, M. S., & Bitzenbauer, P. (2022). Two Cognitive Dimensions of Students’ Mental Models in Science: Fidelity of Gestalt and Functional Fidelity. Education Sciences, 12(3), 163. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci12030163