Prosodic Rephrasing and Violations of the Phase Impenetrability Condition
Abstract
:1. Introduction
(1) |
2. Background
2.1. Prosodic Structure Theory, as Applied to Turkish
(2) | intonation phrase | (ι) |
phonological phrase | (φ) | |
prosodic word | (ω) |
(3) |
(4) |
2.2. A PST Approach to Turkish Prosody That Is Not Phase-Based
(5) | a. | Match-Clause: | ForceILLP | intonational phrase (ι) |
b. | Match-Phrase: | syntactic phrase | phonological phrase (φ) | |
c. | Match-Word: | M-word | prosodic word (ω) |
(6) | a. | [ForceILLP Aynur [ForceILLP sınav-ı | geç-ti] | okul-u | bırak-mış.] | |
Aynur | exam-acc | pass-pst | school-acc | drop.out-evd | ||
‘Aynur, and she had passed the exam, has dropped out from the school.’ | ||||||
b. * | [ɩ Aynur [ɩ exam-acc pass+pst] school-acc drop.out-evd] | |||||
c. | [ɩ Aynur] [ɩ exam-acc pass+pst] [ɩ school-acc drop.out-evd] |
(7) | a. | [ForceILLP (vP(NPKitap) | oku-du-nv)] | |
book | read-pst-2sg | |||
‘You read a book.’ | ||||
b. * | [ɩ (φ (φ (ω | book)) | (ω read-pst-2sg))] | |
c. | [ɩ (φ (ω | book) | (ω read-pst-2sg))] |
(8) | a. | (NP(AP ıslak) | saçN) | [from (Güneş 2015, p. 38)] |
wet | hair | |||
b.* | (φ(φ (ω wet)) | (ω hair)) | ||
c. | (φ (ω wet) | (ω hair)) |
(9) | a. * | (φ (ω X) (ω Y)(ω Z)) | Bin-Max(φ) is violated |
b. | (φ (ω X) (ω Y Z)) | Bin-Max(φ) is satisfied | |
c. | (φ (ω X Y) (ωZ)) | Bin-Max(φ) is satisfied | |
d. | (φ (ω X Y Z)) | Bin-Max(φ) is satisfied |
(10) | Nevriye | araba-da | yağmurluğ-un-u | arı-yor. | (Güneş 2015, p. 110) |
Nevriye | car-loc | raincoat-3poss-acc | search-prog | ||
‘Nevriye is looking for her raincoat in the car.’ |
(11) | The syntax phrase marker and predicted prosodic structure of (10): |
(12) | [ι (φ (ω Nevrİye)) | (φ (ω arabada)) | (φ(ω yağmurluğ-un-u) | (ω arı-yor))] |
(13) | Aynur | kapıyı | araladı | kediler | dışarı | kaçtı |
Aynur | door.acc | open.pst | cat.pl | outside | escape.pst | |
‘Aynur opened the door, (and at that moment) the cats escaped.’ |
(14) | H- L H L H% |
[ɩ (φ (ω Aynur)) (φ(ω kapıyı) (ω araladı))] |
(15) |
2.3. A PST Approach to Turkish Prosody That Assumes the “Modular” PIC
(16) | Phase Impenetrability Condition (PIC) (Chomsky 2000, p. 108): |
In a phase α with head H, the domain of H is not accessible to operations outside α; only H and its edge are accessible to such operations. |
(17) | Maximal φ Condition |
A phonological phrase φ (…ω, etc.) can be no larger than a phase. |
(18) | Aynur | kapıyı | araladı | kediler | dışarı | kaçtı |
Aynur | door.acc | open.pst | cat.pl | outside | escape.pst | |
‘Aynur opened the door, (and at that moment) the cats escaped.’ |
3. PIC Violations in and across Prosodic Domains
3.1. Reprosodification in the Turkish vP Domain
(19) | A: | Davet sırasında ne olmuştu? | ||
‘What happened during the reception?’ | ||||
B: | Ziyaretçi-ler | Aynur-u | gör-müş-∅-tü-ler | |
visitors-pl | Aynur-acc | see-perf-cop-pst-3pl | ||
‘The visitors had seen Aynur.’ |
(20) | H- L H L L% | ||
[ι(φ(ω ziyaretçiler)) | (φ(ω Aynur-u) | (ω gör-müş-∅-tü-ler))] | |
visitors | Aynur-acc | see-perf-cop-pst-3pl | |
‘The visitors had seen Aynur.’ |
(21) | A: | Ziyaretçiler Aynur’u görmüş müydüler? |
‘Had the visitors seen Aynur?’ | ||
B: | Gör-müş-Ø-tü-ler. | |
See-perf-cop-pst-3pl | ||
‘(Yes, they) had seen (her).’ | ||
(22) | a. | H L L% |
[ι(φ(ω gör-müş) (ω -Ø-tü-ler))] | ||
b. | H-L% | |
* [ι(φ(ω gör-müş-Ø-tü-ler))] |
(23) | a. | [ForceP [TP subject [vP object participle-verb] copular-verb]] | syntax for (19B) |
b. | [ForceP ( | ||
syntax for (21B) |
(24) |
(25) | [ι(φ(ω ziyaretçiler)) (φ(φ(φ(ω Aynur-u)) (ω gör-müş)) (ω ∅-tü-ler))] |
(26) | a. | [ι(φ(ω ziyaretçiler)) (φ(ω Aynur-u gör-müş) (ω ∅-tü-ler))] |
b. | [ι(φ(ω ziyaretçiler)) (φ(ω Aynur-u) (ω gör-müş ∅-tü-ler))] |
(27) | a. | [ι(φ(φ(ω gör-müş)) (ω tü-ler))] |
b. | [ι(φ(ω görmüş) (ω tüler))] |
3.2. NP, AP, and CP Domains: Optional Variable Parse in Non-Final φs
(28) | Uzun | pelerin | giy-en | kadın | araba-yı | sor-du |
long | cape | wear-nom | woman | car-acc | ask-pst | |
‘The woman that is wearing a long cape asked about the car.’ |
(29) |
(30) | a. | [ι(φ(ω uzun) | (ωpelerin | giyen | kadın)) | (φ(ω arabayı) | (ω sordu.))] |
b. | [ι(φ(ωuzun | pelerİn) | (ω giyen | kadın)) | (φ(ω arabayı) | (ω sordu.))] | |
c. | [ι(φ(ω uzun | pelerİn | gİyen) | (ω kadın)) | (φ(ω arabayı) | (ω sordu.))] | |
d. | [ι(φ(ω uzun | pelerİn | gİyen | kadın)) | (φ(ω arabayı) | (ω sordu.))] | |
long | cape | wear.nom | woman | car.acc | ask.pst | ||
‘The woman who is wearing a long cape asked about the car.’ |
(31) | Prosodic realization of (28), according to PFPIC accounts |
(32) | Prosodic realization of the complex DP in (28), according to PFNOPIC account | ||||
(i) | Match (clause/XP/M-word with exponents) with ɩ/φ/ω:16 | ||||
(φ(φ(φ(φ(ω uzun)) | (ω pelerin)) | (ω giyen)) | (ω kadın)) | ||
long | cape | wear.nom | woman | ||
‘The woman who is wearing a long cape…’ |
(ii) | Reduce to repair recursively embedded φs (to satisfy Non-Rec(φ)): | ||||
((ω1 uzun) | (ω2 pelerin) | (ω3 giyen) | (ω4 kadın))φ-non-final |
(iii) | Repair Bin-Max(φ) violations (combine ω): | |||||
a. | (φ(ω uzun) | (ω pelerin | giyen | kadın)) | [ combine ω2, ω3 and ω4] | |
b. | (φ(ω uzun | pelerİn) | (ω giyen | kadın)) | [ combine ω1 and ω2/ω3 and ω4] | |
c. | (φ(ω uzun | pelerİn | gİyen) | (ω kadın)) | [ combine ω1, ω2 and ω3] | |
d. | (φ(ω uzun | pelerİn | gİyen | kadın)) | [ combine ω1, ω2, ω3 and ω4] | |
long | cape | wear-nom | woman | |||
‘Long cape wearing woman …’ |
3.3. Clause-Size Tunes and Overriding Clause-Internal Prosodic Constituency
(33) | A: | I hear Sue’s taking a course to become a driving instructor.18 |
B: | Sue!? | |
B′: | A driving instructor!? [multiword surprise-redundancy contour] | |
B’’: | Sue’s taking a course to become a driving instructor!? |
(34) | %LH-L | LH% | ||
↗↘baba-m-ı | arı-yacak-tı-m↗ | – ama gitmiş. | ||
father-1poss-acc | call-fut-past-1sg | – but he has left | ||
‘I was about to call my dad—but it turns out that he has left.’ |
(35) | Aynur | kapıyı | araladı | kediler | dışarı | kaçtı |
Aynur | door.acc | open.pst | cat.pl | outside | escape.pst | |
‘Aynur opened the door, (and at that moment) the cats escaped.’ |
(36) | %LH-L | LH% | [(35) with a tune] | |
[ι(φ(ω Aynur kapıyı araladı))] |
(37) | H- | L H | L | H% | [(35) without a tune] | |
[ι(φ(ω Aynur)) (φ (ω kapıyı) (ωaraladı))] |
4. Conclusions
“Note that the Modular PIC analysis may be falsified language-internally. If a particular phenomenon suggests that a phase head—say, v—lacks or is endowed with a PIC at PF, the PIC is expected to be lacking (or to be present) in all constructions involving the head and that phenomenon in this particular language.”
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
acc | Accusative case |
evd | Evidential |
pst | Past |
loc | Locative |
nom | Nominalizer |
poss | Possesive agreement |
prog | Progressive |
pl | Plural |
perf | Perfect |
cop | Copula |
fut | Future |
sg | Singular |
1 | Not all accounts that adopt a phase-based syntax–phonology interface also adopt the PIC-at-PF view. See Scheer (2011) and Newell (2017) for the separation of the two and the related literature. In a similar vein, most accounts that assume PIC-at-PF adopt a weakened version of PIC, in which only certain operations or certain phases (in certain languages) may show PIC-at-PF. For such weakened versions, e.g., for selective freezing effects of primary vs. secondary stress in English see Marvin (2002, 2011, 2013), for the Modular-PIC view see D’Alessandro and Scheer (2015), or for the PIC à la carte idea see Scheer (2011). We can list Samuels (2009, 2010, 2011, 2015), Richards (2006), and Faust (2014) as among the proponents of the stricter view of PIC-at-PF. Also see Section 2.3 of this paper for a more detailed presentation of the claims of some of these accounts. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
2 | For PST-based phonology literature see Nespor and Vogel (1986); Selkirk (1984, 1986, 1995a, 2011); McCawley (1968); Pierrehumbert and Beckman (1988); Selkirk and Tateishi (1988, 1991); Ghini (1993); Jun and Elordieta (1997); Féry (2010, 2017); Truckenbrodt (1995, 1999, 2012); Ishihara (2007, 2014); Itō and Mester (2013, 2022); and Cheng and Downing (2007, 2012, 2016). For a summary of syntax–prosody interface accounts that do and do not assume PST, see a recent overview by Bennett and Elfner (2019). | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
3 | Importantly, however, I reject the view that final stress is somehow underlyingly present on a morphosyntactic word, M, even when there are no acoustic cues of stress on M. I reject this view because it tends towards unfalsifiability and is incompatible with the emerging view that “final stress” is merely post-lexical ω-boundary assignment (e.g., see Özçelik 2012). More generally, I reject any notion of ‘abstract’ prosodic structure, i.e., an underlying metrical structure for finally stressed words. If there is no acoustic evidence for the presence of a prosodic domain D, then D is simply not part of the prosodic structure of a given speech string. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
4 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
5 | See Section 2.2 and Section 3 for some environments that trigger such mismatches, in which multiple morphosyntactic words are parsed as a single prosodic word. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
6 | Non-recursivity is a commonly observed property of some phonological grammars across different language groups (Selkirk 1995b; Truckenbrodt 1995, 1999; Hamlaoui and Szendrői 2017; i.o. but see Kabak and Revithiadou (2009) for a recursive phonological phrase analysis of a lexically specified group in Turkish). | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
7 | There are different notions of word-hood (e.g., compound words, roots plus their affixes, phonologically defined words etc.—see (Haspelmath 2023) for a discussion on different types of words). In Turkish, at least two distinct types of words are shown to exist, first is the morphosyntactically defined word, i.e., root plus its affixes (in most cases this is also the domain of the vowel harmonic word). Second is the prosodic word in the way it is defined in Section 2.1 above (see Kornfilt 1996, Section 6 for a discussion of how the domain of root plus affixes (i.e., the domain of vowel harmony), differs from the domain of prominence). Both types of words show certain domain-related phenomena in the phonology of Turkish. It seems that most cases of segmental phonological events in Turkish are sensitive to the words that are the root plus affixes, e.g., vowel harmony, voicing alternation (Inkelas and Orgun 1995), or hiatus resolution (Kabak 2007). The prosodic word that is defined in this paper (but also in Güneş 2013, 2015, 2020a, 2021) to my knowledge does not trigger any segmental phonological phenomena within or across its boundaries. Similarly, the abovementioned segmental phenomena that are observed to apply within and/or across root plus affixes are not sensitive to whether or not that root plus its affixes overlaps with a prosodic word or not. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
8 | I assume that nominals are topped by a DP layer in Turkish (Kornfilt 2018a, 2018b). | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
9 | As far as I can tell, most of the phase-based phonology literature uses the terms phase and Spell-Out domain interchangeably, even though these terms refer to syntactic phrases with different sizes in Chomsky’s (2000, 2001) theory. For the purposes of this paper, I assume that phase-based approaches to prosody view the entire phase (e.g., vP and CP) as the syntactic chunk that has privileged status at PF, rather than the Spell-Out domains (e.g., TP and VP). Based on this assumption, I henceforth follow terminological convention and use the term phase rather than Spell-Out domain. Note that my arguments against phase-based approaches to phonology that assume the PIC-at-PF still holds when one switches from the idea that phases (e.g., vPs and CPs) are mapped to the idea that Spell-Out domains (e.g., VPs and TPs) are mapped. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
10 | Whenever v is not a phase head, i.e., in unaccusative and passive clauses, final φs in Turkish are viewed by Üntak-Tarhan (2006) as created as an automatic byproduct of CP phasal Spell-Out. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
11 | Neither Newell (2005, 2008, 2015) nor Üntak-Tarhan (2006) discussed the presence of PIC at the PF of Turkish, nor did they attribute any PF visible PIC effects to prosodic constituency formation in Turkish. However, these studies are essential in understanding what phasal domains have been claimed to have a PF imprint and what phonological operations are suggested to diagnose the presence of phasal domains at the PF of Turkish, especially to be able to discuss if the modular version of PFPIC is present in Turkish. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
12 | Both Üntak-Tarhan (2006) and Newell (2005, 2008, 2015) discuss sentence-level prominence, which concerns vP and CP phases, not discussing other phasal domains of Turkish (i.e., DP/NP and AP). Consequent studies that investigate the prosodic properties of branching syntactic phrases (especially DPs/NPs and APs) in the area that precedes the final phonological phrases (see, e.g., Kamali 2011; Güneş 2013, 2015, 2020a; Féry 2017), observe that final and non-final phonological phrases are formed in the same way. Additionally, citing Kornfilt (1996, p. 113) on how the prominence properties of complex verbs are sorted in the same way as phrases, Newell (2005, p. 46) derives the prominence properties of complex verbs via the standard derivational mechanisms of the interface system which is also responsible in parsing phrases (also see Güneş and Göksel 2013). If prosodically prominent domain generation (sentence-level or phrasal-level) is taken as a single phonological operation, then one can extend the analyses and claims made about sentence-level prominence domains to phrase-level prominence domains. In this uniformed account of prominence formation, the only difference between sentence-level prominence and phrase-level prominence would then be attributed to the linear position of the phonological phrase (see Güneş 2015 for such a uniformed analysis of prosodic prominence in Turkish). As for the PFPIC view, for the sake of illustration of a phase-based account, I then conclude that both APs and DPs/NPs are phonologically relevant phases in Turkish. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
13 | Note that one cannot simply associate the distinction between ω-, φ- or ɩ-boundaries with increasing boundary strength in Turkish, as other phonological properties distinguish between prosodic categories, e.g., φs are head-initial whereas ɩs are head-final. Nor can one stipulate that the instruction to Spell-Out a syntactic phase XP as a particular prosodic category is contained in the lexical entry for X, as ωs in Turkish correspond to a structurally defined notion in Turkish (an M-word, see Section 2.2). Thus, how to ensure that a phrase is associated with the correct prosodic category type without incurring a “look ahead” problem is not a simple matter. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
14 | The prosodic realizations discussed in this paper represent Standard Turkish and the PRAAT visualizations come from audio recordings of native speakers. Audio recordings of each of the possible prosodic realizations discussed in Section 3 of this paper were presented to 16 native speakers of Standard Turkish. Each speaker confirmed the naturalness of each realization in their given contexts, respectively. The observation that Turkish permits optional prosodic realizations in certain structural environments (e.g., see Section 3.2) is not new: most of the data presented in this paper comes from previous work (references are provided for each example). I thank Sun Ah Jun (2013) for making me aware of the possibility of variable prosodic realizations of relative clauses (see Figure 6). I also thank Aslı Göksel for confirming the judgments attributed to the data presented in Figure 6 and for bringing to my attention the tunes discussed in Figure 7 and Figure 8. Beyond adopting the view that the relevant variable prosodic realizations exist (which is incontestable, given the observations presented in this paper), I make no claims about the data. Particularly, I make no claims about the frequency with which a particular prosodic realization can be found in corpora, or about whether certain demographic groups use a particular realization more frequently than others. The methodology that I employed to confirm that variable prosodic realizations exist involved creating stimuli artificially (in other words, my stimuli were not naturally occurring data taken from corpora) and then collecting acceptability judgments. This is the standard practice in generative linguistics—(almost) all generative linguistic research on morphology and syntax adopts this methodology. This being the case, I reject as unreasonable an anonymous reviewer’s view that my claim that variable prosodic realizations exist is undermined by not reporting naturally occurring examples of each prosodic variant and/or by not using naturally occurring examples as stimuli for obtaining acceptability judgments. This view holds phonology oriented generative linguistic research to higher empirical standards than any other form of generative linguistic research, and without providing a valid reason for doing so. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
15 | In Figure 3, the visible interruption of the F0 contour on the area where the final consonant of the participle verb and the initial consonant of the copular domain is due to the fact that both of these consonants are voiceless and do not lead to consistent repetition of the sound waves to be interpreted as pitch points. In short, this interruption is not a linguistic pause or a sign of a boundary, but a phonetic result of the fact that two consecutive voiceless sounds occur at this juncture. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
16 | Note that the embedded CP, i.e., that of the relative clause, does not Match with an intonational phrase as this clause is not employed as a speech act and there is no ForceILLP projection. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
17 | Note that ω-combination is only limited by adjacency, i.e., only adjacent ωs can be combined (Güneş 2015). Additionally, the optional prosodic realization exemplified in (30) is not restricted to relative clauses. This is demonstrated by the example in (i), which contains a complex attributive adjective phrase (Güneş 2020b, ex. (29)). The PFNOPIC analysis sketched in (32) extends without modification to this example, as (ii) demonstrates. PFPIC approaches are equally as ineffective here as with the relative clause case described in the main text, and for the same reasons. A structure with a similar meaning but without a relative clause is observed to exhibit a similar optional variable prosodic realization, yet without the clausal phase-based boundaries of the relative clause, i.e., uzun pelerinli kadın ‘the woman with a long cape’. The optionality is not predicted by PFPIC accounts.
| |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
18 | Here I use an example from English so that it is easier to understand the notion of tunes for the cross-linguistic reader. English and Turkish belong to different prosodic typological groups. Turkish is a phrase language without lexical stress (for most lexemes), whereas English is an intonation language with lexical stress. Therefore, no specific parallels between English and Turkish prosodic properties can be made. This paper specifically discusses the prosodic grammar of Turkish and no cross-linguistic generalizations should be made, especially involving languages that belong to different typological groups. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
19 | Although this tune resembles the well-described Rise–Fall–Rise tune of English, it is by no means related in content. There is no indication of correction, insinuation, or contradiction in the Turkish one. The difference, above other things, may be sourced from the fact that the rise (or sometimes the fall) of the English contour is borne by a pitch accent, however in the Turkish contour, only boundary tones are used to create this seemingly similar rising falling rising pattern in the contour. | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
20 | Due to its specific event-related intonational meaning, the temporal proximity contour can only occur minimally on predicates (verbal, nominal or adjectival). | |||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
21 | ||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||||
22 | In fact, Newell (2017, p. 21) states that there is no PIC even in syntax. |
References
- Athanasopoulou, Angeliki A., Irene Vogel, and Hossep Dolatian. 2017. Acoustic Properties of Canonical and Non-Canonical Stress in French, Turkish, Armenian and Brazilian Portuguese. Paper presented at the Interspeech 2017, Stockholm, Sweden, August 20–24. [Google Scholar]
- Athanasopoulou, Angeliki, Irene Vogel, and Hossep Dolatian. 2021. Acoustic properties of word and phrasal prominence in Uzbek. Proceedings of the Workshop on Turkic and Languages in Contact with Turkic 5: 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bennett, Ryan, and Emily Elfner. 2019. The syntax-prosody interface. Annual Review of Linguistics 5: 151–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bennett, Ryan, Emily Elfner, and James McCloskey. 2016. Lightest to the right: An apparently anomalous displacement in Irish. Linguistic Inquiry 47: 169–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bonet, Eulália, Lisa Lai-Shen Cheng, Laura J. Downing, and Joan Mascaró. 2019. (In)direct Reference in the Phonology-Syntax Interface under Phase Theory: A Response to “Modular PIC” (D’Alessandro and Scheer 2015). Linguistic Inquiry 50: 751–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bošković, Željko. 2014. Now I’m a phase, now I’m not a phase: On the variability of phases with extraction and ellipsis. Linguistic Inquiry 45: 27–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bošković, Željko, and Serkan Şener. 2014. The Turkish NP. In Crosslinguistic Studies on Noun Phrase Structure and Reference. Edited by Patricia Cabredo Hofherr and Anne Zribi-Hertzs. Leiden: Brill, pp. 102–40. [Google Scholar]
- Bresnan, Joan. 1978. A realistic transformational grammar. In Linguistic Theory and Psychological Reality. Edited by Morris Halle, Joan Bresnan and George A. Miller. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 1–59. [Google Scholar]
- Cheng, Lisa Lai-Shen, and Laura Downing. 2007. The phonology and syntax of relative clauses in Zulu. SOAS Working Papers in Linguistics 15: 51–63. [Google Scholar]
- Cheng, Lisa Lai-Shen, and Laura Downing. 2012. Prosodic domains do not match Spell-Out domains. McGill Working Papers in Linguistics 22: 1–14. [Google Scholar]
- Cheng, Lisa Lai-Shen, and Laura Downing. 2016. Phasal syntax = Cyclic phonology? Syntax 19: 156–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, Noam. 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist Inquiries: The Framework. In Step by Step: Essays on Minimalist Syntax in Honor of Howard Lasnik. Edited by Roger Martin, David Michaels, Juan Uriagereka and Samuel Jay Keyser. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 89–155. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by Phase. In Ken Hale: A Life in Language. Michael Kenstowicz. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 1–52. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2004. Beyond explanatory adequacy. In Structures and Beyond. The Cartography of Syntactic Structures, Volume 3. Edited by Adriana Belletti. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 104–31. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2005. Three factors in language design. Linguistic Inquiry 36: 1–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chomsky, Noam, and Morris Halle. 1968. The Sound Pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row. [Google Scholar]
- D’Alessandro, Roberta, and Tobias Scheer. 2015. Modular PIC. Linguistic Inquiry 46: 593–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dobashi, Yoshihito. 2003. Phonological Phrasing and Syntactic Derivation. Ph.D. dissertation, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Downing, Bruce T. 1970. Syntactic Structure and Phonological Phrasing in English. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Texas, Austin, TX, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Elfner, Emily. 2012. Syntax-Prosody Interaction in Irish. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Elordieta, Gorka. 2008. An overview of theories of the syntax-phonology interface. International Journal of Basque Linguistics and Philology (ASJU) 42: 209–86. [Google Scholar]
- Embick, David, and Rolf Noyer. 2001. Movement operations after syntax. Linguistic Inquiry 32: 555–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Enç, Mürvet. 2004. Functional categories in Turkish. In Proceedings of the first Workshop on Altaic Formal Linguistics. Edited by Aniko Csirmaz, Youngjoo Lee and Mary Ann Walter. Cambridge, MA: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, pp. 208–26. [Google Scholar]
- Faust, Noam. 2014. Where it’s [at]: A phonological effect on phasal boundaries in the construct state Modern Hebrew. Lingua 150: 315–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fenger, Paula. 2020. Words Within Words: The Internal Syntax of Verbs. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Féry, Caroline. 2010. The intonation of Indian languages: An areal phenomenon. In Problematizing Language Studies: Cultural, Theoretical and Applied Perspectives—Essays In Honor of Rama Kant Agnihotri. Edited by S. Imtiaz Hasnain and Shreesh Chaudhury. New Delhi: Aakar Books, pp. 288–312. [Google Scholar]
- Féry, Caroline. 2017. Intonation and Prosodic Structure. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ghini, Marco. 1993. Phi-formation in Italian: A new proposal. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics 12: 41–78. [Google Scholar]
- Göksel, Aslı. 2010. Focus in words with truth values. Iberia 2: 89–112. [Google Scholar]
- Good, Jeff, and Alan C. L. Yu. 2000. Affix-placement variation in Turkish. In Proceedings of the 25th Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society: Special Session on Caucasian, Dravidian, and Turkic Linguistics. Edited by Jeff Good and Alan C. L. Yu. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society, pp. 63–74. [Google Scholar]
- Good, Jeff, and Alan C. L. Yu. 2005. Morphosyntax of two Turkish subject pronominal paradigms. In Clitic and Affix Combinations: Theoretical Perspectives. Edited by Lorie Heggie and Fransisco Ordóñez. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 315–41. [Google Scholar]
- Griffiths, James, and Güliz Güneş. 2014. Ki issues in Turkish. Parenthetical coordination and adjunction. In Parenthesis and Ellipsis. Cross-Linguistic and Theoretical Perspectives. Edited by Marlies Kluck, Dennis Ott and Mark de Vries. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 173–217. [Google Scholar]
- Guekguezian, Peter Ara. 2017. Prosodic Recursion and Syntactic Cyclicity inside the Word. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Güneş, Güliz. 2013. Limits of prosody in Turkish. “Updates in Turkish Phonology” Dilbilim Araştırmaları Dergisi [Journal of Linguistic Research] 2013: 133–69. [Google Scholar]
- Güneş, Güliz. 2014. Constraints on syntax-prosody correspondence: The case of clausal and subclausal parentheticals in Turkish. Lingua 150: 278–314. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Güneş, Güliz. 2015. Deriving Prosodic Structures. Utrecht: LOT Dissertation Series, no. 397. [Google Scholar]
- Güneş, Güliz. 2020a. Türkçede bürün ve sözdizim arakesiti [Syntax and Prosody Interface in Turkish]. In Kuramsal ve uygulamalı sesbilim [Theoretical and Applied Phonology]. Edited by İpek Pınar Uzun. Ankara: Seçkin Yayıncılık, pp. 157–95. [Google Scholar]
- Güneş, Güliz. 2020b. Variability in the realization of agreement in Turkish: A morphotactic account. In Morphological Complexity within and across Boundaries: Essays in Honour of Aslı Göksel. Edited by Aslı Gürer, Dilek Uygun Gökmen and Balkız Öztürk Başaran. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 236–61. [Google Scholar]
- Güneş, Güliz. 2021. Morphosyntax and Phonology of Agreement in Turkish. Syntax 2: 143–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Güneş, Güliz, and Aslı Göksel. 2013. Morphoprosodic delimitation of the focus domain in the WORD: In the footsteps of Sebüktekin 1984. Paper presented at the LINGDAY 2013: 45 Years of Linguistics at Boğaziçi University in Honour of Prof. Dr. Hikmet Sebüktekin, Istanbul, Turkey, June 5. [Google Scholar]
- Güneş, Güliz, and Aslı Göksel. 2017. Variable prosodic domains and violations of PIC—Evidence for non-cyclic (phrase-based) phonology. Paper presented at the 40th Generative Linguistics in the Old World (GLOW 40), Workshop I: Syntax-Phonology Interface, Leiden, The Netherlands, March 14–18. [Google Scholar]
- Gussenhoven, Carlos. 2004. The Phonology of Tone and Intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Haegeman, Liliane, and Virginia Hill. 2014. The syntactization of discourse. In Syntax and Its Limits. Edited by Raffaella Folli, Christina Sevdali and Robert Truswell. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 370–90. [Google Scholar]
- Hamlaoui, Fatima, and Kriszta Szendrői. 2017. The syntax-phonology mapping of intonational phrases in complex sentences: A flexible approach. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 2: 55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haspelmath, Martin. 2023. Defining the word. WORD 69: 283–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heim, Johannes, Hermann Keupdjio, Zoe Wai-Man Lam, Adriana Osa-Gómez, Sonja Thoma, and Martina Wiltschko. 2016. Intonation and particles as speech act modifiers: A syntactic analysis. Studies in Chinese Linguistics 37: 109–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hewitt, Mark S. 1994. Deconstructing Foot Binarity in Koniag Alutiiq. Vancouver: University of British Columbia. [Google Scholar]
- Hirschberg, Julia. 2004. Pragmatics and intonation. In The Handbook of Pragmatics. Edited by Laurence R. Horn and Gregory Ward. London: Blackwell, pp. 515–37. [Google Scholar]
- Inkelas, Sharon, and Cemil Orhan Orgun. 1995. Level Ordering and Economy in the Lexical Phonology of Turkish. Language 71: 763–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- İpek, Canan. 2011. Phonetic realization of focus with no on-focus pitch range expansion in Turkish. Paper presented at the Annual 17th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Hong Kong, China, August 17–21; pp. 140–43. Available online: http://internationalphoneticassociation.org/icphs-proceedings/ICPhS2011/index.htm (accessed on 22 April 2024).
- İpek, Canan. 2015. The Phonology and Phonetics of Turkish Intonation. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- İpek, Canan, and Sun-Ah Jun. 2014. Distinguishing phrase-final and phrase-medial high tone on finally stressed words in Turkish. In Proceedings of the 7th International Conference on Speech Prosody. Edited by Nick Campbell, Dafydd Gibbon and Daniel Hirst. Dublin: International Speech Communication Association. [Google Scholar]
- Ishihara, Shinichiro. 2007. Major phrase, focus intonation and multiple spell-out (MaP, FI, MSO). The Linguistic Review 24: 137–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ishihara, Shinichiro. 2014. Match theory and the recursivity problem. In Proceedings of FAJL 7: Formal Approaches to Japanese Linguistics. Edited by Shigeto Kawahara and Mika Igarashi. Cambridge, MA: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, pp. 69–88. [Google Scholar]
- Ishihara, Shinichiro. 2022. On the (lack of) correspondence between syntactic clauses and intonational phrases. In Prosody and Prosodic Interfaces. Edited by Haruo Kubozono, Junko Ito and Armin Mester. Oxford: Oxford Academic, August 18. [Google Scholar]
- Itō, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2003. Weak layering and word binarity. In A New Century of Phonology and Phonological Theory. A Festschrift for Professor Shosuke Haraguchi on the Occasion of His Sixtieth Birthday. Edited by Takeru Honma, Masao Okazaki, Toshiyuki Tabata and Shin-ichi Tanaka. Tokyo: Kaitakusha, pp. 26–65. First published 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Itō, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2013. Prosodic subcategories in Japanese. Lingua 124: 20–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Itō, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2022. Match theory and prosodic wellformedness constraints. In Prosodic Studies: Challenges and Prospects. Edited by Hongming Zhang and Youyong Qian. London: Routledge, pp. 252–74. [Google Scholar]
- Jun, Sun-Ah. 2013. University of California, Los Angeles, Los Angeles, CA, USA. Personal communication. [Google Scholar]
- Jun, Sun-Ah, ed. 2005. Prosodic Typology. The Phonology of Intonation and Phrasing. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jun, Sun-Ah, and Gorka Elordieta. 1997. Intonational structure of Lekeitio Basque. In Intonation: Theory, Models and Applications. Edited by Antonis Botinis, Georgios Kouroupetroglou and George Carayiannis. Athens: European Speech Communication Association, pp. 193–96. [Google Scholar]
- Kabak, Barış. 2007. Hiatus resolution in Turkish: An underspecification account. Lingua 117: 1378–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kabak, Barıș. 2016. Refin(d)ing Turkish stress as a multifaceted phenomenon. Paper presented at the Second Conference on Central Asian Languages and Linguistics (ConCALL-2), Indiana University, Bloomington, IN, USA, October 7–9. [Google Scholar]
- Kabak, Barıș, and Anthi Revithiadou. 2009. An interface approach to prosodic word recursion. In Phonological Domains: Universals and Deviations. Edited by Janet Grijzenhout and Barış Kabak. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 105–34. [Google Scholar]
- Kabak, Barıș, and Irene Vogel. 2001. The phonological word and stress assignment in Turkish. Phonology 18: 315–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahnemuyipour, Arsalan, and Jaklin Kornfilt. 2010. The syntax and prosody of Turkish “pre-stressing” affixes. In Interfaces in Linguistics: New Research Perspectives. Edited by Raffaella Folli and Christiane Ulbrich. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 205–21. [Google Scholar]
- Kaisse, Ellen M. 1985. Connected Speech: The Interaction of Syntax and Phonology. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kamali, Beste. 2011. Topics at the PF Interface of Turkish. Ph.D. dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Kamali, Beste, and Bridget Samuels. 2008. The syntax of Turkish pre-stressing suffixes. Paper presented at the 3rd Conference on Tone and Intonation in Europe, Lisbon, Portugal, September 15–17. [Google Scholar]
- Kan, Seda. 2009. Prosodic Domains and the Syntax-Prosody Mapping in Turkish. Master’s dissertation, Boğaziçi University, Boğaziçi, Turkey. [Google Scholar]
- Karlsson, Anastasia, Güneş Güliz, Hamed Rahmani, and Sun-Ah Jun. 2020. 14 South West and Central Asia. In The Oxford Handbook of Language Prosody, Oxford Handbooks. Edited by Carlos Gussenhhoven and Aoju Chen. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 207–24. [Google Scholar]
- Kelepir, Meltem. 2001. Topics in Turkish Syntax: Clausal Structure and Syntax. Ph.D. dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Kelepir, Meltem. 2003. Olmak, değil, var ve yok. In 16. Dilbilim Kurultayı bildirileri [Proceedings of the 16th Congress of Linguistics]. Edited by Güray König, S. Nalan Büyükkantarcıoğlu and Firdevs Karahan. Ankara: Hacettepe University, pp. 70–81. [Google Scholar]
- Kelepir, Meltem. 2007. Copular forms in Turkish, Turkmen, and Nogay. In Proceedings of the 2nd Workshop on Altaic Formal Linguistics. Edited by Meltem Kelepir and Balkız Öztürk. Cambridge, MA: MIT Working Papers in Linguistics, pp. 83–100. [Google Scholar]
- Konrot, Ahmet. 1981. Physical correlates of linguistic stress in Turkish. University of Essex Language Center Occasional Papers 24: 26–53. [Google Scholar]
- Kornfilt, Jaklin. 1996. On some copular clitics in Turkish. In Papers on the Conference ‘The Word as a Phonetic Unit’ (ZAS Papers in Linguistics 11). Edited by Artemis Alexiadou, Nanna Fuhrop, Ursula Kleinhenz and Paul Law. Berlin: Zentrum für Allgemeine Sprachwissenschaft, pp. 96–114. [Google Scholar]
- Kornfilt, Jaklin. 2001. On the syntax and morphology of clausal complements and adjuncts in the Turkic languages. In Aspects of Typology and Universals. Edited by Walter Bisang. Berlin: Akademie Publishers, pp. 63–82. [Google Scholar]
- Kornfilt, Jaklin. 2003. Subject case in Turkish nominalized clauses. In Syntactic Structures and Morphological Information. Edited by U. Junghanns and L. Szucsich. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 129–215. [Google Scholar]
- Kornfilt, Jaklin. 2018a. DP versus NP: A cross-linguistic typology? In Studies in Historical and Synchronic Altaic. Edited by Alexander Vovin and William McClure. Leiden and Boston: Brill, pp. 138–58. [Google Scholar]
- Kornfilt, Jaklin. 2018b. NP versus DP: Which one fits Turkish nominal phrases better? Turkic Languages 22: 155–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kouneli, Maria, Paula Fenger, and Jonathan David Bobaljik. 2022. Syntactic Limits on Phonological Dominance. In Proceedings of the 40th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics (WCCFL 40). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press. [Google Scholar]
- Kratzer, Angelika, and Elisabeth Selkirk. 2007. Phase theory and prosodic spellout: The case of verbs. The Linguistic Review 24: 93–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krifka, Manfrid. 2023. Layers of assertive clauses: Propositions, judgements, commitments, acts. In Propositionale Argumente im Sprachvergleich: Theorie und Empirie/Propositional Arguments in Cross-Linguistic Research: Theoretical and Empirical Issues. Edited by Jutta Hartmann and Angelika Wöllstein. Tübingen: Narr. [Google Scholar]
- Ladd, D. Robert. 1986. Intonational phrasing: The case for recursive prosodic structure. Phonology 3: 311–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ladd, D. Robert. 2008. Intonational Phonology, 2nd ed. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Lee, Seunghun J., and Elisabeth Selkirk. 2022. Xitsonga Tone: The Syntax-Phonology Interface. In Prosody and Prosodic Interfaces. Edited by Haruo Kubozono, Junko Itō and Armin Mester. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 337–73. [Google Scholar]
- Levi, Susannah V. 2002. Intonation in Turkish: The Realization of Noun Compounds and Genitive Possessive NPs. Seattle: University of Washington. [Google Scholar]
- Levi, Susannah V. 2005. Acoustic correlates of lexical accent in Turkish. Journal of the International Phonetic Association 35: 73–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewis, Geoffrey. L. 1967. Turkish Language. Oxford: OUP. [Google Scholar]
- Marvin, Tatjana. 2002. Topics in Stress and the Syntax of Words. Ph.D. dissertation, MIT, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Marvin, Tatjana. 2011. Phases at the Word Level. Linguistica (Ljubljana) 51: 181–99. [Google Scholar]
- Marvin, Tatjana. 2013. Is word structure relevant for stress assignment. In Distributed Morphology Today: Morphemes for Morris Halle. Edited by Ora Matushansky and Alec Marantz. London: The MIT Press, pp. 79–93. [Google Scholar]
- McCawley, James D. 1968. The Phonological Component of a Grammar of Japanese. The Hague: Mouton. [Google Scholar]
- Mester, Armin. 1994. The quantitative trochee in Latin. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 12: 1–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyagawa, Shigeru. 2022. Syntax in the Treetops. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Miyagawa, Shigeru, and Virginia Hill. 2023. Commitment phrase: Linking proposition to illocutionary force. Linguistic Inquiry, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nespor, Marina, and Irene Vogel. 1986. Prosodic Phonology. Dordrecht: Foris. [Google Scholar]
- Newell, Heather. 2005. The Phonological Phase. In McGill Working Papers in Linguistics 19(2). Edited by Raphael Mercado and Yukio Furukawa. Montreal: McGill Working Papers in Linguistics, pp. 21–64. [Google Scholar]
- Newell, Heather. 2008. Aspects of the Morphology and Phonology of Phases. Ph.D. dissertation, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Newell, Heather. 2015. Phonology without strata. Paper presented at the 2015 Annual Conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association, Ottawa, ON, Canada, May 30–June 1; Available online: https://cla-acl.artsci.utoronto.ca/actes-2015-proceedings/ (accessed on 22 April 2024).
- Newell, Heather. 2017. Nested phase interpretation and the PIC. In The Structure of Words at the Interfaces. Edited by Heather Newell, Máire Noonan, Glyne Piggott and Lisa deMena Travis. Oxford: Oxford Studies in Theoretical Linguistics, pp. 20–40. [Google Scholar]
- Odden, David. 1987. Kimatuumbi phrasal phonology. Phonology Yearbook 4: 13–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Özçelik, Öner. 2012. Representation and Acquisition of Stress: The Case of Turkish. Ph.D. dissertation, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Özçelik, Öner. 2014. Prosodic faithfulness to foot edges: The case of Turkish stress. Phonology 31: 229–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Özge, Umut, and Cem Bozşahin. 2010. Intonation in the grammar of Turkish. Lingua 120: 132–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pierrehumbert, Janet B., and Mary E. Beckman. 1988. Japanese Tone Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Pycha, Anne. 2006. A duration-based solution to the problem of stress realization in Turkish. In UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report. Berkeley: UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report, pp. 141–51. [Google Scholar]
- Richards, Marc. 2006. Weak pronouns, object shift and multiple spell-out: Evidence for phases at the PF-interface. In Minimalist Essays. Edited by Cedric Boeckx. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 160–81. [Google Scholar]
- Samuels, Bridget. 2009. The Structure of Phonological Theory. Ph.D. dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Samuels, Bridget. 2010. Phonological derivation by phase: Evidence from Basque. In Proceedings of the 33rd Annual Penn Linguistics Colloquium. Edited by Jon Scott Stevens. Philadelphia, PA: Penn Graduate Linguistics Society, pp. 166–75. [Google Scholar]
- Samuels, Bridget. 2011. Phonological Architecture: A Biolinguistic Perspective, Volume 2. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Samuels, Bridget. 2015. Biolinguistics in Phonology: A Prospectus. Phonological Studies 18: 161–71. [Google Scholar]
- Scheer, Tobias. 2011. A Guide to Morphosyntax-Phonology Interface Theories: How Extra-Phonological Information Is Treated in Phonology since Trubetzkoy’s Grenzsignale. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Scheer, Tobias. 2012. Direct Interface and One-Channel Translation: A Non-Diacritic Theory of Morphosyntax-Phonology Interface. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Sebüktekin, Hikmet. 1984. Turkish word stress: Some observations. In Proceedings of the Turkish Linguistics Conference. Edited by Eser Erguvanlı-Taylan and Ayhan Aksu-Koç. Istanbul: Boğaziçi University Publications, pp. 295–307. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 1972. The phrasal phonology of English and French. Ph.D. dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 1984. Phonology and Syntax: The Relation between Sound and Structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 1986. On derived domains in sentence phonology. Phonology Yearbook 3: 371–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 1995a. Sentence prosody: Intonation, stress, and phrasing. In The Handbook of Phonological Theory. Edited by John Goldsmith. Cambridge, MA: Blackwell, pp. 550–69. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 1995b. The prosodic structure of function words. In Papers in Optimality Theory. Amherst, MA: GLSA, pp. 439–70. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 2000. The interaction of constraints on prosodic phrasing. In Prosody: Theory and Experiment. Edited by Merle Horne. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 231–61. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 2009. On clause and intonational phrase in Japanese: The syntactic grounding of prosodic constituent structure. Gengo Kenkyu 136: 35–74. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O. 2011. The syntax-phonology interface. In The Handbook of Phonological Theory, 2nd ed. Edited by John Goldsmith, Jason Riggle and Alan C. L. Yu. Hoboken: Wiley-Blackwell, pp. 435–84. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O., and Koichi Tateishi. 1988. Constraints on minor phrase formation in Japanese. In Proceedings of the 24th Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Edited by M. Gary Larson and Diane Brentari. Chicago: Chicago Linguistics Society, pp. 316–36. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O., and Koichi Tateishi. 1991. Syntax and downstep in Japanese. In Interdisciplinary Approaches to Language: Essays in Honor of S.-Y. Kuroda. Edited by Carol Georgopoulos and Roberta Ishihara. Dordrecht: Kluwer, pp. 519–43. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth O., and Seunghun J. Lee, eds. 2015. Constituency in sentence phonology. Special Issue of Phonology 32: 1–18. [Google Scholar]
- Şener, Serkan. 2010. (Non-)Peripheral Matters in Turkish Syntax. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Şener, Serkan, and Daiko Takahashi. 2010. Ellipsis of Arguments in Japanese and Turkish. Nanzan Linguistics 6: 79–99. [Google Scholar]
- Speas, Peggy, and Carol Tenny. 2003. Configurational properties of point of view roles. In Asymmetry in Grammar. Volume 1, Syntax and Semantics. Edited by Anne-Marie Di Sciullo. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 315–43. [Google Scholar]
- Starke, Michael. 2009. Nanosyntax: A short primer to a new approach to language. In Tromsø Working Papers on Language and Linguistics: Nordlyd 36.1. Edited by Peter Svenonius, Gillian Ramchand, Michal Starke and Knut Tarald Taraldsen. Tromsø: Septentrio Academic Publishing, pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
- Tang, Sze-Wing. 2015. Cartographic syntax of pragmatic projections. In Chinese Syntax in a Cross-Linguistic Perspective. Edited by Audrey Li, Andrew Simpson and Wei-Tien Dylan Tsai. Oxford and New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 429–41. [Google Scholar]
- Truckenbrodt, Hubert. 1995. Phonological Phrases: Their Relation to Syntax, Focus, and Prominence. Ph.D. dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Truckenbrodt, Hubert. 1999. On the relation between syntactic phrases and phonological phrases. Linguistic Inquiry 30: 219–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Truckenbrodt, Hubert. 2012. The interface of semantics with phonology and morphology. In Semantics: An International Handbook of Natural Language Meaning, Volume 3. Edited by Claudia Maienborn, Klaus von Heusinger and Paul Portner. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 2039–69. [Google Scholar]
- Truckenbrodt, Hubert. 2015. Intonation phrases and speech acts. In Parenthesis and Ellipsis: Cross-Linguistic and Theoretical Perspective. Edited by Marlies Kluck, Dennis Ott and Mark de Vries. Berlin and Boston: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 301–49. [Google Scholar]
- Türk, Olcay. 2020. Gesture, Prosody and Information Structure Synchronisation in Turkish. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Victoria University of Wellington, Wellington, New Zealand. [Google Scholar]
- Üntak-Tarhan, Fatma Aslı. 2006. Topics in Syntax-Phonology Interface: Sentential Stress and Phases. Master’s thesis, Boğaziçi University, Boğaziçi, Istanbul. Available online: http://acikbilim.yok.gov.tr/handle/20.500.12812/324767 (accessed on 22 April 2024).
- Uriagereka, Juan. 1999. Multiple Spell Out. In Working Minimalism. Edited by Samuel D. Epstein and Norbert Hornstein. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, pp. 109–35. [Google Scholar]
- van der Hulst, Harry, and Jergen van de Weijer. 1991. Topics in Turkish Phonology. In Turkish Linguistics Today. Edited by Hendrik E. Boeschoten and Ludo T. Verhoeven. Leiden: E.J. Brill, pp. 11–59. [Google Scholar]
- Vogel, Irene. 2020. Fixed stress as phonological redundancy: Effects on production and perception in Hungarian and other languages. In Approaches to Hungarian. Papers from the 2017 Budapest Conference. Edited by Veronika Hegedűs and Irene Vogel. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 188–206. [Google Scholar]
- Vogel, Irene, Angeliki Athanasopoulou, and Nadya Pincus. 2016. Prominence, contrast and the functional load hypothesis: An acoustic investigation. In Dimensions of Phonological Stress. Edited by Jeffrey Heinz, Rob Goedemans and Harry van der Hulst. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 123–67. [Google Scholar]
- Wagner, Michael. 2005. Prosody and Recursion. Ph.D. dissertation, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Wagner, Michael. 2010. Prosody and recursion in coordinate structures and beyond. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory 28: 183–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wagner, Michael. 2015. Phonological evidence in syntax? In Syntax—Theory and Analysis: An International Handbook. Edited by Tibor Kiss and Artemis Alexiadou. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 1154–97. [Google Scholar]
- Wiltschko, Martina, and Johannes Heim. 2016. The Syntax of Confirmationals: A Neo-performative Analysis. In Outside the Clause: Form and Function of Extra-Clausal Constituents. Edited by Gunther Kaltenböck, Evelien Keizer and Arne Lohmann. Amsterdam: John Benjamins, pp. 305–40. [Google Scholar]
- Zora, Hatice, Matthias Heldner, and Iris-Corinna Schwarz. 2016. Perceptual correlates of Turkish word stress and their contribution to automatic lexical access: Evidence from early ERP components. Frontiers in Neuroscience 10: 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Güneş, G. Prosodic Rephrasing and Violations of the Phase Impenetrability Condition. Languages 2024, 9, 162. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050162
Güneş G. Prosodic Rephrasing and Violations of the Phase Impenetrability Condition. Languages. 2024; 9(5):162. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050162
Chicago/Turabian StyleGüneş, Güliz. 2024. "Prosodic Rephrasing and Violations of the Phase Impenetrability Condition" Languages 9, no. 5: 162. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050162
APA StyleGüneş, G. (2024). Prosodic Rephrasing and Violations of the Phase Impenetrability Condition. Languages, 9(5), 162. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages9050162