4.1. Distribution of Future Forms
The results of our analysis reveal patterns of usage for morphological future and periphrastic constructions with
haber and
ir.
Table 3 provides a percentage of use of these three forms in our data.
The form overwhelmingly chosen for use to express future time is the periphrasis with ir (61% of the tokens). Both morphological future and the haber periphrasis occur approximately in similar proportions.
The periphrastic construction with
ir is not only the most frequent one in the corpus as a whole, but also the most prevalent one throughout recent decades.
Table 4 represents the frequency per million of the three future forms across the time span represented in the corpus. In the most recent decades, the frequency per million for
ir is nearly twice what it was in the 1960s, whereas for
haber, the frequency per million is a quarter of what it was in the sixties. We note that these corpora differ in ways other than date. For example, the percentage of urban vs. rural data is much higher in the 2000s than in the 1960s. The speakers interviewed likely differed in important ways with regard to extralinguistic factors not currently available to the authors from the corpus, and variation is likely also constrained by stylistic factors such as degree of formality. The point is that the differences in frequency per million may reflect differences across the corpora independent of time.
4.2. Linguistic Conditioning
Since the
haber periphrastic form has persisted to present day as a future marker in Galician, we are able to explore these data as a way to understand future expression currently, and additionally, these data could potentially serve as a metric with which to consider the diachronic change evidenced in late Latin and several other Romance languages. As is evident in
Table 4,
haber use in the corpus declines. Resultantly, some speakers do not use this periphrasis in the speech sample. In order to understand which factors incite a speaker to choose one periphrasis over another, for the statistical modeling we limited the data to all future tokens spoken just by speakers who minimally used the
haber future once. This reduced the number of target tokens (N = 956). The following tables present the results of three multiple linear regression analyses of the data predicting
haber usage.
In order to determine the ways in which the innovative periphrastic forms (
haber,
ir) differ from the more conservative morphological future, we conducted separate linear regression models using the lme4 function to predict the periphrastic form(s) [see
Tagliamonte (
2012) for a description of the R analysis]. A summary of these two independent analyses [
haber vs. morphological,
ir vs. morphological] is provided in
Table 5, with the
haber results shaded on the left and
ir on the right.
Importantly, the results of the two multiple linear regression analyses suggest that the
haber periphrasis and the
ir periphrasis differ from the morphological form in similar ways. In both models, the morphological future is favored (compared to a periphrastic form) with second and third persons and when it is in the apodosis of a conditional clause expressing contingency. In neither analysis is century
6 or adverb use a significant predictor of future expression. Unsurprisingly, the morphological future is more grammaticalized than the periphrastic form. This is evident from the finding that the periphrastic
haber and
ir forms are favored in first person, which is associated with intention and, as has been previously shown, is a former step to the focal meaning of future (prediction). Further, the morphological form is also favored in the apodosis with contingent meaning, which we interpret as evidence of increased grammaticalization (cf.
Bybee et al. 1994, p. 274).
The results of the two analyses summarized in
Table 5 show marked similarities between the periphrastic forms in relation to the morphological future. Although morphological future presents characteristics of greater degree of grammaticalization, the results do not allow a determination of the degree of grammaticalization of the periphrastic forms in relation to one another. To make this determination, we compared factors constraining
haber periphrasis compared to
ir. This informed our research goal of determining a cline of grammaticalization for these future forms.
Table 6 summarizes the results of this multiple linear regression analysis.
This analysis, predicting the
haber compared to the
ir periphrastic form, demonstrates that with the exception of grammatical person, no significant differences are found between the two forms. As is evident in
Table 6, third person subjects do not differ significantly from first person subjects. This implies that both periphrases can be used to express prediction in similar ways. Further evidence in favor of this interpretation is that both occur in combination with inanimate subjects to the same extent in our dataset [
haber 8% (N = 24),
ir 9% (N = 42)]. When compared to
ir, the
haber periphrasis is more likely in the second person. With regard to the expression of obligation,
Coates (
1983, p. 37) notes, “it is generally true that examples with second person subjects are stronger” than those with first and third person subjects. This result may suggest evidence of the retention of the original meaning of obligation for the
haber in a context (second person subject) that is particularly prone to expressing deontic modality. Given the potential for competition between
ir and
haber forms throughout the history of the language, future studies should determine whether
haber may disappear as a future marker or might specialize as expressing deontic modality meanings.
4.3. Haber as a Grammaticalizing Form
As grammaticalizing forms move from lexical forms toward grammatical markers, they reduce in phonetic form (e.g.,
Phillips 2006;
Bybee 2010). Consider, for example, the grammaticalization of Latin
cantare habeo as Galician
cantarei in which the auxiliary resulted in an inflectional ending (-
ei). Phonetic reduction is also evident in periphrastic forms in present-day languages. In Spanish, for example, the construction [
ir ‘go’ (
present)
a +
infinitive] undergoes phonological reduction, both in terms of segment lenition and deletion (
Lipski 2008, p. 113;
Silva-Corvalán and Enrique-Arias 2017, p. 237) and durational shortening (
Brown and Rivas 2022). We ask, then, whether there is evidence of phonetic reduction in the
haber periphrasis in Galician.
Based upon the orthographic transcription of the CORILGA conversations, we note that the Galician
haber periphrastic form is variably realized both with the preposition (
haber de +
infinitive) and without (
haber +
infinitive), as is shown in (15) and (16) respectively:
(15) | OIED-URB-CDUB-SANTIAGODECOMPOSTELA-36-1995 |
| Sea tarde ou sea cedo, eu hei de vir, usté tranquilo |
| ‘Sooner or later I will come back, do not worry’ |
(16) | OIED-URB-CDUB-SANTIAGODECOMPOSTELA-36-1995 |
| Despois han-lle vir os da comisión |
| ‘After that, the committee members will come’ |
The forms lacking the preposition are more phonetically reduced (exhibit less phonetic substance) compared to instances of haber accompanied by de. The subset of the tokens of use of haber provide insights, therefore, into the gradual phonetic erosion of a form having gone through grammaticalization. During the process of grammaticalization, do phonetic conditioning factors conspire to propel the reduction further? A positive result might provide impetus to reconsider historical data through this lens.
To our knowledge, an analysis of this type has not been conducted on Galician data. Based on the extent to which comparisons can be drawn to Spanish and Portuguese, these data also provide synchronic evidence with which to consider the processes that took place historically. Such analyses could help identify more specific pathways and mechanisms of change. We examined the target tokens with phonetic conditioning factors in mind. We coded the accompanying infinitive for whether it has a consonant onset or not, hypothesizing that the reduction in the preposition
de would be favored by a following consonant due to coarticulatory effects of production. Additionally, abundant evidence suggests that a high token frequency accelerates reductive change (
Bybee 1999). We estimated the frequency of the infinitive based upon the future tokens in our corpus. The mean raw frequency for infinitives in our study was 5. Tokens with a count above 5 were coded as ‘high’, and those 5 and below, as ‘low’. For the same reasons as described previously, we suspect corpus differences (across the decades).
Table 7 summarizes the rate of preposition use (as opposed to omission) with the
haber tokens in our dataset. The preposition
de is omitted more commonly before consonant-initial and high-frequency infinitives and its omission is highest in the data of the most recent recordings.
In order to determine which factors condition preposition use (maintenance vs. reduction), we submitted the
haber tokens to a multiple linear regression model using R, with the speaker and infinitive as random intercepts. We included frequency as a continuous factor (log of raw frequency).
Table 8 summarizes the results of this model. If the infinitive following the periphrastic future begins with a vowel, preposition use is highly favored. In contrast, a following pre-consonantal phonetic context favors omission of the
de. The corpora reflecting speech samples from earlier decades (1960s, 1990s) favor de use compared to the interviews conducted in the twenty-first century. As is evident in
Table 7, in just 4 of the 13 cases of
haber used in the later corpora is a preposition employed between
haber and the infinitive. Word frequency does not significantly predict preposition omission.
The findings summarized above suggest that as forms grammaticalize, in addition to the contributions of the commonly cited effects of frequency of use (token frequency), phonetic factors of the production context are, of course, operative. Effects of context of use, therefore, can accumulate in memory (
Bybee 2002) and shape the pattern of change. These results support the notion that the phonetic reduction in the grammaticalizing forms is enhanced in certain production contexts, while phonetic reduction in other words is inhibited.