Age, Experience and Language and Literacy Skills in English-Arabic Speaking Syrian Refugees
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Bilingualism
1.2. Role of Experience and Age of Acquisition
Effects of Age Versus Experience
1.3. Relations among L1 and L2 Skills
1.4. Linguistic Features of the Languages: Similarities and Differences between Arabic and English
1.5. Special Issues Related to Refugee Status
1.6. The Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Materials
2.2.1. Word and Pseudoword Reading
In English
In Arabic
2.2.2. Phonological Awareness
In English
In Arabic
2.2.3. Morphological Awareness
In English
In Arabic
2.2.4. Vocabulary
In English
In Arabic
2.2.5. Nonverbal Reasoning
2.3. Procedures
3. Results
3.1. Comparison of Groups on Measures
3.2. Relationships among Measures by Age Group
3.3. English and Arabic Variables Related to English Word Reading
3.3.1. English Variables Related to English Word Reading
English Variables Related to Word Reading in Younger Children
English Variables Related to Word Reading in Older Children
3.3.2. English Variables Related to English Pseudoword Reading
Variables Related to Pseudoword Reading in Younger Children
Variables Related to Pseudoword Reading in Older Children
3.4. Arabic Variables Uniquely Related to English Word Reading
3.4.1. Variables Related to Word Reading in Younger Children
3.4.2. Variables Related to Word Reading in Older Children
4. Discussion
4.1. Group Similarities and Differences on Language and Literacy Variables
4.2. Variables Related to English Word Level Reading
4.2.1. Similarities in English Variables Related to English Word and Pseudoword Reading
4.2.2. Differences in English Variables Related to English Word and Pseudoword Reading
4.2.3. Arabic Measures Related to English Word Reading
4.2.4. The Role of Morphological Awareness
4.3. The Role of Age of Acquisition
4.4. Implications
4.5. Limitations
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
1 | In contrast: research conducted with young children compares children who learn two languages at an early age. For example, simultaneous bilinguals who learn both languages before age 2 are compared to early bilinguals who acquire both languages by age 4 (DeHouwer 2012, 2021; Schulz and Grimm 2019). |
2 | Age was highly related to word reading when entered as the first step, β = 0.560, t (95) = 6.56, p < 0.001, when the groups were combined. This value is consistent for all the regression analyses with word reading. However, the goal of the study was to compare the two age groups matched on length of exposure to English in Canada and linguistic experiences in the home as per the analyses reported in this paper. |
3 | Age was highly related to pseudoword reading when entered as the first step, β = 0.337, t (95) = 3.47, p < 0.001, when the groups were combined. However, the goal of the study was to compare the two age groups. |
4 | The same pattern of results was found for the younger group when Arabic word reading was and was not included in the analyses in Table 7. The lack of a significant relationship between Arabic and English word reading is likely due to the very low scores on Arabic word reading for the younger group (See Table 1). |
5 | Similar findings using morphological awareness to compensate for weaker reading skills have been documented in monolingual English-speaking adults with dyslexia (Law et al. 2017). |
References
- Abu-Rabia, Salim. 2007. The Role of Morphology and Short Vowelization in Reading Arabic among Normal and Dyslexic Readers in Grades 3, 6, 9, and 12. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 36: 89–106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Abu-Rabia, Salim, and Haitham Taha. 2006. Phonological errors predominate in Arabic spelling across grades 1–9. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 35: 167–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Anthony, Jason L., and Christopher J. Lonigan. 2004. The Nature of Phonological Awareness: Converging Evidence from Four Studies of Preschool and Early Grade School Children. Journal of Educational Psychology 96: 43–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Archila-Suerte, Pilar, Jason Zevin, Ferenc Bunta, and Arturo E. Hernandez. 2012. Age of acquisition and proficiency in a second language independently influence the perception of non-native speech. Bilingualism 15: 190–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baker, Wendy, Pavel Trofimovich, Moly Mack, and James E. Flege. 2002. The Effect of Perceived Phonetic Similarity on Non-native Sound Learning by Children and Adults. In Proceedings of the Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, vol. 26, pp. 36–47. [Google Scholar]
- Bialystok, Ellen. 2010. Bilingualism. Cognitive Science 1: 559–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Birdsong, David. 2005. Interpreting age effects in second language acquisition. Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches 109: 127. [Google Scholar]
- Birdsong, David. 2006. Age and Second Language Acquisition and Processing: A Selective Overview. Language Learning 56: 9–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Birdsong, David, and Michelle Molis. 2001. On the Evidence for Maturational Constraints in Second-Language Acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language 44: 235–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bosch, Sina, Joao Versissimo, and Harald Clahsen. 2019. Inflectional Morphology in Bilingual Language Processing: An age-of-acquisition Study. Language Acquisition 26: 339–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Branum-Martin, Lee, Sha Tao, Sarah Garnaat, Ferenc Bunta, and David J. Francis. 2012. Meta-analysis of Bilingual Phonological Awareness: Language, Age, and Psycholinguistic Grain Size. Journal of Educational Psychology 104: 932–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burnham, Denis. 2003. Language specific speech perception and the onset of reading. Reading and Writing 16: 573–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carlisle, Joanne F. 2000. Awareness of the structure and meaning or morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 12: 169–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Castles, Anne, and Max Coltheart. 2004. Is There a Causal Link from Phonological Awareness to Success in Learning to Read? Cognition 91: 77–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chall, S. Jeanne. 1983. Stages of Reading Development. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Christian, Kate, Frederick J. Morrison, Julie A. Frazier, and Greta Massetti. 2000. Specificity in the nature and timing of cognitive growth in kindergarten and first grade. Journal of Cognition and Development 1: 429–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Connor, Carol M., Seung-Hee Son, Annemarie H. Hindman, and Frederick J. Morrison. 2005. Teacher Qualifications, Classroom Practices, Family Characteristics, and Preschool Experience: Complex Effects on First Graders’ Vocabulary and Early reading Outcomes. Journal of School Psychology 43: 343–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cummins, Jim. 1981. Empirical and Theoretical Underpinnings of Bilingual Education. Journal of Education 163: 16–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cummins, Jim. 1991. Interdependence of first-and second-language proficiency in bilingual children. In Language Processing in Bilingual Children. Edited by E. Bialystok. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 70–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cummins, Jim. 2008. BICS and CALP: Empirical and Theoretical Status of the Distinction. In Encyclopedia of Language and Education. Boston: Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daniels, Peter T. 2017. Writing systems. In The Handbook of Linguistics. Hoboken: John Wiley and Sons, pp. 75–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Daniels, Peter T., and William Bright. 1996. The World’s Writing Systems. New York: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- DeHouwer, Annick. 2012. Early Bilingualism. The Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DeHouwer, Annick. 2021. Bilingual Development in Children. In Elements of Child Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- DeKeyser, Robert. 2012. Individual Differences in Native Language Attainment and their Implications for research on Second Language Acquisition. Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 2: 260–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DeKeyser, Robert, and Jennifer Larson-Hall. 2005. What Does the Critical Period Really Mean? In Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 88–108. [Google Scholar]
- Díaz, Begoña, Kepa Erdocia, Robert F. De Menezes, Jutta L. Mueller, Núria Sebastián-Gallés, and Itziar Laka. 2016. Electrophysiological correlates of second-language syntactic processes are related to native and second language distance regardless of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology 7: 133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Dunn, Lloyd M., and Douglas M. Dunn. 2007. Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th ed. (PPVT-IV). Toronto: Pearson. [Google Scholar]
- Durgunoğlu, Aydin Y. 2002. Cross-linguistic transfer in literacy development and implications for language learners. Annals of Dyslexia 52: 189–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ehri, C. Linnea. 2017. Orthographic Mapping and Literacy Development Revisited. Theories of Reading Development 15: 127–46. [Google Scholar]
- Eilers, Rebecca E., Barbara Z. Pearson, and Alan B. Cobo-Lewis. 2006. Social Factors in Bilingual Development: The Miami Experience. In Childhood Bilingualism. Berlin: De Gruyter, pp. 68–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eruyar, Seyda, John Maltby, and Panos Vostanis. 2018. Mental health problems of Syrian refugee children: The role of parental factors. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 27: 401–9. [Google Scholar]
- Farnia, Fataneh, and Esther Geva. 2011. Cognitive correlates of vocabulary growth in English language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics 32: 711–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fazel, Mina, Ruth V. Reed, Catherine Panter-Brick, and Alan Stein. 2012. Mental health of displaced and refugee children resettled in high-income countries: Risk and protective factors. The Lancet 379: 266–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ferguson, Charles. A. 1959. Diglossia. Word 15: 325–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flege, James. 1992. Speech Learning in a Second Language. In Phonological Development: Models, Research, and Implications. Timonium: York Press, pp. 565–604. [Google Scholar]
- Flege, James E., Grace H. Yeni-Komshian, and Serena Liu. 1999. Age Constraints on Second-Language Acquisition. Journal of Memory and Language 41: 8–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Flege, James E., Murray J. Munro, and Ian R. A. Mackay. 1995. Effects of age of second-language learning on the production of English consonants. Speech Communication 16: 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gersten, Russell, and Scott Baker. 2000. What We Know About Effective Instructional Practices for English-Language Learners. Exceptional Children 66: 454–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Geva, Esther, and Min Wang. 2001. The Development of Basic Reading Skills in Children: A Cross-Language Perspective. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 21: 182–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gottardo, Alexandra. 2002. The Relationship between Language and Reading Skills in Bilingual Spanish-English Speakers. Topics of Language Disorders 22: 46–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gottardo, Alexandra, and Amy Grant. 2008. Defining Bilingualism. In Encyclopedia of Language and Literacy Development. London: CLLRNet. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/publication/267152186_Defining_Bilingualism (accessed on 8 November 2022).
- Gottardo, Alexandra, Keith E. Stanovich, and Linda S. Siegel. 1996. The Relationships Between Phonological Sensitivity, Syntactic Processing, and Verbal Working Memory in the Reading Performance of Third grade Children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 63: 563–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gottardo, Alexandra, Xi Chen, and Michelle Ru Yun Huo. 2021. Understanding Within-and Cross-Language Relations Among Language, Preliteracy Skills, and Word Reading in Bilingual Learners: Evidence from the Science of Reading. International Literacy Association: Reading Research Quarterly 56: S371–S390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hernandez, Arturo E., and Ping Li. 2007. Age of acquisition: Its neural and computational mechanisms. Psychological Bulletin 133: 638–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hopp, Holger, and Monika S. Schmid. 2013. Perceived Foreign Accent in First Language Attrition and Second Language Acquisition: The Impact of Age of Acquisition and Bilingualism. Applied Psycholinguistics 34: 361–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ibrahim, Asadi, Michal Shany, Anat Ben-Semon, and Raphiq Ibrahim. 2014. Individual Diagnostic Tests in the Assessment of Learning Disabilities in Arabic: Tests and Manual. Haifa: University of Haifa. [Google Scholar]
- Ioup, Georgette. 2008. Exploring the role of age in the acquisition of a second language phonology. Phonology and Second Language Acquisition 36: 41–62. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, Jacqueline S., and Elissa L. Newport. 1989. Critical Period Effects in Second Language Learning: The Influence of Maturational State on the Acquisition of English as a Second Language. Cognitive Psychology 21: 60–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kieffer, Michael J., and Nonie K. Lesaux. 2012. Direct and Indirect Roles of Morphological Awareness in the English Reading Comprehension of Native English, Spanish, Filipino, and Vietnamese Speakers. Language Learning 62: 117–204. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koda, Keiko. 2008. Impacts of prior literacy experience on learning to read in a second language. In Learning to Read Across Languages: Cross-Linguistic Relationships in First- and Second-Language Literacy Development. Edited by K. Koda and A. M. Zehler. New York: Routledge, pp. 68–96. [Google Scholar]
- Landerl, Karin, Anne Castles, and Rauno Parrila. 2021. Cognitive Precursors of Reading: A Cross-Linguistics Perspective. Scientific Studies of Reading 6: 111–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Law, Jeremy M., Jan Wouters, and Pol Ghesquière. 2017. The influences and outcomes of phonological awareness: A study of MA, PA and auditory processing in pre-readers with a family risk of dyslexia. Developmental Science 20: e12453. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lesaux, Nonie K., Amy C. Crosson, Michael J. Kieffer, and Margaret Pierce. 2010. Uneven Profiles: Language Minority Learners’ Word Reading, Vocabulary, and Reading Comprehension Skills. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology 31: 475–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lesaux, Nonie K., and Linda S. Siegel. 2003. The Development of Reading in Children Who Speak English as a Second Language. Developmental Psychology 39: 1005–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Lesaux, Nonie K., Andre A. Rupp, and Linda S. Siegel. 2007. Growth in Reading Skills of Children from Diverse Linguistic Backgrounds: Findings from a 5-year Longitudinal Study. Journal of Educational Psychology 99: 821–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDonald, Janet L. 2000. Grammaticality judgments in a second language: Influences of age of acquisition and native language. Applied Psycholinguistics 21: 395–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melby-Lervag, Monica, and Arne Lervag. 2011. Cross-linguistic transfer of oral language, decoding, phonological awareness and reading comprehension: A meta-analysis of the correlational evidence. Journal of Research in Reading 34: 114–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Montrul, Silvina, and Rebecca Foote. 2014. Age of acquisition interactions in bilingual lexical access: A study of the weaker language of L2 learners and heritage speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism 18: 274–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morrison, Frederick J., Denise M. Alberts, and Elizabeth M. Griffith. 1997. Nature–nurture in the classroom: Entrance age, school readiness, and learning in children. Developmental Psychology 33: 254–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morrison, Frederick J., Lisa Smith, and Maureen Dow-Ehrensberger. 1995. Education and cognitive development: A natural experiment. Developmental Psychology 31: 789–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Naglieri, Jack A. 1985. Matrix Analogies Test: Expanded form. New York: Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Nagy, William. 2007. Metalinguistic awareness and the vocabulary-comprehension connection. In Vocabulary Acquisition: Implications for Reading Comprehension. New York: Guilford Press, pp. 52–77. [Google Scholar]
- Nagy, William E., and Judith A. Scott. 2000. Vocabulary processes. Handbook of Reading Research 3: 269–84. [Google Scholar]
- Nagy, William E., Garcia Earnest García, Aydin Y. Durgunoğlu, and Barbara Hancin-Bhatt. 1993. Spanish-English bilingual students’ use of cognates in English reading. Journal of Reading Behavior 25: 241–59. [Google Scholar]
- Oller, Kimbrough D., Barabara Z. Pearson, and Alan B. Cobo-Lewis. 2007. Profile Effects in Early Bilingual Language and Literacy. Applied Psycholinguistics 28: 191–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paradis, Johanne. 2019. English Second Language Acquisition from Early Childhood to Adulthood: The Role of Age, First Language, Cognitive, and Input Factors. In Proceedings of the 43rd Boston University Conference on Language Development. Edited by Megan M. Brown and Brady Dailey. Somerville: Cascadilla Press, pp. 11–26. [Google Scholar]
- Paradis, Johanne, Adriana Soto-Corominas, Evangelia Daskalaki, Xi Chen, and Alexandra Gottardo. 2021. Morphosyntactic Development in First Generation Arabic—English Children: The Effect of Cognitive, Age, and Input Factors over Time and across Languages. Languages 6: 51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pérez, Anita Méndez, Elizabeth D. Peña, and Lisa M. Bedore. 2010. Cognates facilitate word recognition in young Spanish-English bilinguals’ test performance. Early Childhood Services 4: 55. [Google Scholar]
- Proctor, C. Patrick, and Elaine Mo. 2009. The relationship between cognate awareness and English comprehension among Spanish–English bilingual fourth grade students. TESOL Quarterly 43: 126–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramirez, Gloria, Xi Chen, Esther Geva, and Heidi Kiefer. 2010. Morphological awareness in Spanish-speaking English language learners: Within and cross-language effects on word reading. Reading and Writing 23: 337–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Reed, Ruth V., Mina Fazel, Lynne Jones, Catherine Panter-Brick, and Alan Stein. 2012. Mental health of displaced and refugee children resettled in low-income and middle-income countries: Risk and protective factors. The Lancet 379: 250–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Roehr-Brackin, Karen. 2018. Metalinguistic Awareness and Second Language Acquisition, 1st ed. London: Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saiegh-Haddad, Elinor. 2013. A Tale of One Letter: Morphological Processing in Early Arabic Spelling. Writing Systems Research 5: 169–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saiegh-Haddad, Elinor, and Esther Geva. 2008. Morphological awareness, phonological awareness, and reading in English–Arabic bilingual children. Reading and Writing 21: 481–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saiegh-Haddad, Elinor, and Rachel Schiff. 2016. The Impact of Diglossia on Voweled and Unvoweled Word Reading in Arabic: A Developmental Study from Childhood to Adolescence. Scientific Studies of Reading 20: 311–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Saiegh-Haddad, Elinor, and Roni Henkin-Roitfarb. 2014. The Structure of Arabic Language and Orthography. In Handbook of Arabi Literacy. Berlin: Springer, pp. 3–28. [Google Scholar]
- Saito, Kazuya. 2015. The Role of Age of Acquisition in Late Second Language Oral Proficiency Attainment. Studies in Second Language Acquisition 4: 713–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Scarborough, Hollis S. 2005. Developmental relationships between language and reading: Reconciling a beautiful hypothesis with some ugly facts. In The Connections Between Language and Reading Disabilities. Edited by H. W. Catts and A. G. Kamhi. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers, pp. 3–24. [Google Scholar]
- Scharpf, Florian, Elisa Kaltenbach, Angela Nickerson, and Tobias Hecker. 2021. A systematic review of socio-ecological factors contributing to risk and protection of the mental health of refugee children and adolescents. Clinical Psychology Review 83: 101930. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schiff, Rachel, and Elinor Saiegh-Haddad. 2018. Development and Relationships Between Phonological Awareness, Morphological Awareness and Word Reading in Spoken and Standard Arabic. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schulz, Petra, and Angela Grimm. 2019. The age factor revisited: Timing in acquisition interacts with age of onset in bilingual acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology 9: 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Share, David L. 1995. Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition 55: 151–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Skibbe, Lori E., Carol M. Connor, Frederick J. Morrison, and Abigail M. Jewkes. 2011. Schooling effects on preschoolers’ self-regulation, early literacy, and language growth. Early Childhood Research Quarterly 26: 42–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Smart, Jack, and Frances Altorfer. 2005. Teach Yourself Gulf Arabic. New York: McGraw-Hill. [Google Scholar]
- Tibi, Sana, and John R. Kirby. 2017. Investigating Phonological Awareness and Naming Speed as Predictors of Reading in Arabic. Scientific Study of Reading 22: 70–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tibi, Sana, Ashley A. Edwards, Christopher Schatschneider, and John R. Kirby. 2020. Predicting Arabic word reading: A cross-classified generalized random-effects analysis showing the critical role of morphology: Correction. Annals of Dyslexia 70: 379. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Treiman, Rebecca, and Brett Kessler. 2006. Spelling as statistical learning: Using consonantal context to spell vowels. Journal of Educational Psychology 98: 642–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tunmer, Williams E., and James W. Chapman. 2012. The Simple View of Reading Redux: Vocabulary Knowledge and the Independent Components Hypothesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities 45: 453–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tunmer, William E., and Wesley A. Hoover. 2017. Cognitive and Linguistic Factors in Learning to Read. In Reading Acquisition. London: Routledge, pp. 175–214. [Google Scholar]
- Turan, Figen, and Gozde Gul. 2008. Early Precursor of Reading: Acquisition of Phonological Awareness Skills. Educational Sciences: Theory and Practice 8: 279–84. [Google Scholar]
- UNESCO. 2022. The Right to Education of Venezuelan Migrant and Refugee Children and Adolescents: Multidimensional Risks and Exacerbation of Vulnerabilities during the Pandemic. Available online: https://www.unesco.org/en/articles/right-education-venezuelan-migrant-and-refugee-children-and-adolescents-multidimensional-risks (accessed on 9 November 2022).
- UNHCR. 2022. UNHCR Education Report 2022–All Inclusive the Campaign for Refugee Education. Available online: https://www.unhcr.org/publications/education/631ef5a84/unhcr-education-report-2022-inclusive-campaign-refugee-education.html (accessed on 8 November 2022).
- Valdés, Guadalupe, and Richard A. Figueroa. 1994. Bilingual and Testing: A Special Case of Bias. Norwood: Ablex Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Wagner, Richard, Joseph Torgesen, and Carol Rashotte. 2013. Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing, 2nd ed. (CTOPP-2). Toronto: Pearson Assessments. [Google Scholar]
- Wei, Miao, Anand A. Joshi, Mingxia Zhang, Leilei Mei, Franklin R. Manis, Qinghua He, Rachel L. Beattie, Gui Xue, David W. Shattuck, Richard M. Leahy, and et al. 2015. How age of acquisition influences brain architecture in bilinguals. Journals of Neurolinguistics 36: 35–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Westernoff, Fern. 2014. The home oral language activities (HOLA) program: Building parent partnerships for academic success. Journal of Interactional Research in Communication Disorders 5: 97–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Woodcock, Richard W., Kevin S. McGrew, and Nancy Mather. 2001. Woodcock-Johnson III NU Complete. Toronto: Pearson Assessment. [Google Scholar]
Construct/Measure | Younger Group | Older Group | F-Value | p-Value | η2 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age in months | 91.51 (10.9) | 131.85 (13.4) | 250.90 | p < 0.001 | 0.727 |
Age of arrival | 69.16 (13.4) | 109.43 (15.0) | 188.44 | p < 0.001 | 0.667 |
Length of exposure to English (months) | 22.35 (8.3) | 22.42 (8.6) | 0.001 | p = 0.970 | 0.000 |
English vocabulary | 67.02 (21.8) | 75.06 (25.0) | 2.75 | p = 0.101 | 0.028 |
English vocabulary (PPVT-IV SS) | 65.98 (14.4) | 48.30 (15.6) | 32.98 | p < 0.001 | 0.260 |
English word reading | 25.65 (11.2) | 36.23 (11.0) | 21.61 | p < 0.001 | 0.187 |
English pseudoword reading | 10.09 (9.7) | 14.68 (10.3) | 4.99 | p = 0.028 | 0.050 |
English phonological awareness | 14.91 (9.2) | 19.96 (8.6) | 7.70 | p = 0.007 | 0.076 |
English morphological awareness | 3.12 (2.8) | 4.55 (3.5) | 4.66 | p = 0.033 | 0.047 |
Arabic vocabulary | 37.95 (12.0) | 49.23 (10.2) | 24.57 | p < 0.001 | 0.207 |
Arabic word reading | 9.79 (19.8) | 40.38 (30.3) | 32.48 | p < 0.001 | 0.257 |
Arabic phonological awareness | 9.07 (6.7) | 13.28 (6.1) | 10.42 | p = 0.002 | 0.100 |
Arabic morphological production | 18.02 (10.9) | 35.32 (15.8) | 37.18 | p < 0.001 | 0.283 |
Measures | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1.English vocabulary | 1 | 0.524 ** | 0.321 * | 0.289 * | 0.624 ** | 0.320 * | 0.278 * | 0.305 * | 0.157 | 0.202 |
2.English word reading | 0.571 ** | 1 | 0.704 ** | 0.661 ** | 0.728 ** | 0.489 ** | 0.720 ** | 0.670 ** | 0.641 ** | 0.452 ** |
3.English pseudoword reading | 0.263 | 0.782 ** | 1 | 0.613 ** | 0.520 ** | 0.442 ** | 0.584 ** | 0.596 ** | 0.544 ** | 0.273 * |
4. English PA | 0.261 | 0.793 ** | 0.763 ** | 1 | 0.410 ** | 0.476 ** | 0.604 ** | 0.836 ** | 0.462 ** | 0.414 ** |
5. English morphological awareness | 0.754 ** | 0.679 ** | 0.477 ** | 0.507 ** | 1 | 0.524 ** | 0.462 ** | 0.310 * | 0.428 ** | 0.360 ** |
6. Arabic vocabulary | 0.101 | 0.477 ** | 0.331 * | 0.572 ** | 0.232 | 1 | 0.649 ** | 0.466 ** | 0.631 ** | 0.443 ** |
7. Arabic word reading | −0.125 | 0.413 ** | 0.500 ** | 0.582 ** | −0.054 | 0.498 ** | 1 | 0.702 ** | 0.747 ** | 0.455 ** |
8. Arabic PA | 0.034 | 0.632 ** | 0.661 ** | 0.739 ** | 0.227 | 0.506 ** | 0.642 ** | 1 | 0.592 ** | 0.483 ** |
9. Arabic morphological production | 0.120 | 0.735 ** | 0.780 ** | 0.821 ** | 0.290 | 0.553 ** | 0.712 ** | 0.774 ** | 1 | 0.320 * |
10. MAT | 0.140 | 0.310 * | 0.201 | 0.412 ** | 0.019 | 0.308 * | 0.226 | 0.356 * | 0.364 * | 1 |
Step & Variables | rs | Std. Error | β | t-Value | p-Value | Unique | Common |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. MAT | 1.000 | 0.405 | 0.310 | 2.09 * | 0.043 | - | - |
2. MAT | 0.353 | 0.252 | −0.010 | −0.10 | 0.918 | 0.000 | 0.096 |
English PA | 0.901 | 0.132 | 0.662 | 6.13 *** | <0.001 | 0.223 | 0.405 |
Eng MA | 0.772 | 0.594 | 0.100 | 0.68 | 0.503 | 0.003 | 0.459 |
Eng vocab | 0.647 | 0.066 | 0.324 | 2.54 * | 0.015 | 0.038 | 0.287 |
Step & Variables | rs | Std. Error | β | t-Value | p-Value | Unique | Common |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. MAT | 1.000 | 0.223 | 0.452 | 3.62 *** | <0.001 | - | - |
2. MAT | 0.540 | 0.159 | 0.098 | 1.10 | 0.277 | 0.008 | 0.197 |
English PA | 0.790 | 0.116 | 0.400 | 4.38 *** | <0.001 | 0.120 | 0.317 |
Eng MA | 0.870 | 0.337 | 0.469 | 4.31 *** | <0.001 | 0.116 | 0.414 |
Eng vocab | 0.626 | 0.044 | 0.095 | 0.94 | 0.353 | 0.006 | 0.269 |
Step & Variables | rs | Std. Error | β | t-Value | p-Value | Unique | Common |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. MAT | 1.00 | 0.360 | 0.201 | 1.31 | 0.196 | - | - |
2. MAT | 0.259 | 0.288 | −0.123 | −1.01 | 0.321 | 0.011 | 0.030 |
English PA | 0.982 | 0.151 | 0.773 | 5.42 *** | <0.001 | 0.305 | 0.278 |
Eng MA | 0.614 | 0.679 | 0.066 | 0.34 | 0.737 | 0.001 | 0.227 |
Eng vocab | 0.339 | 0.075 | 0.029 | 0.17 | 0.863 | 0.000 | 0.069 |
Step & Variables | rs | Std. Error | β | t-Value | p-Value | Unique | Common |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. MAT | 1.00 | 0.224 | 0.273 | 2.02 * | 0.048 | - | - |
2. MAT | 0.399 | 0.198 | −0.056 | 1.10 | 0.642 | 0.002 | 0.072 |
English PA | 0.899 | 0.145 | 0.500 | 4.10 *** | <0.001 | 0.187 | 0.189 |
Eng MA | 0.762 | 0.419 | 0.357 | 2.46 * | 0.018 | 0.067 | 0.203 |
Eng vocab | 0.470 | 0.055 | −0.036 | −0.27 | 0.792 | 0.001 | 0.102 |
Step & Variables | rs | Std. Error | β | t-Value | p-Value | Unique | Common |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Arabic vocab | 0.639 | 0.095 | 0.072 | 0.55 | 0.588 | 0.005 | 0.222 |
Arabic MA | 0.987 | 0.143 | 0.633 | 3.55 *** | 0.001 | 0.124 | 0.417 |
Arabic PA | 0.847 | 0.224 | 0.079 | 0.46 | 0.648 | 0.008 | 0.391 |
2. Arabic vocab | 0.619 | 0.095 | 0.097 | 0.74 | 0.466 | 0.010 | 0.217 |
Arabic MA | 0.955 | 0.154 | 0.729 | 3.77 *** | <0.001 | 0.160 | 0.381 |
Arabic PA | 0.820 | 0.226 | 0.119 | 0.68 | 0.499 | 0.015 | 0.384 |
Arabic Word Reading | 0.536 | 0.069 | −0.197 | −1.25 | 0.219 | 0.038 | 0.132 |
Step & Variables | rs | Std. Error | β | t-Value | p-Value | Unique | Common |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1. Arabic vocab | 0.663 | 0.129 | 0.097 | 0.77 | 0.446 | 0.003 | 0.236 |
Arabic MA | 0.869 | 0.092 | 0.333 | 2.40 * | 0.020 | 0.055 | 0.355 |
Arabic PA | 0.909 | 0.210 | 0.420 | 3.46 *** | 0.001 | 0.121 | 0.327 |
2. Arabic vocab | 0.0638 | 0.130 | 0.001 | 0.01 | 0.996 | 0.000 | 0.239 |
Arabic MA | 0.836 | 0.097 | 0.180 | 1.23 | 0.226 | 0.015 | 0.396 |
Arabic PA | 0.874 | 0.227 | 0.273 | 2.08 * | 0.043 | 0.044 | 0.404 |
Arabic Word Reading | 0.940 | 0.058 | 0.403 | 2.41 * | 0.020 | 0.044 | 0.475 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Gottardo, A.; Al-Janaideh, R.; Paradis, J.; Soto-Corominas, A.; Chen, X.; Amin, N. Age, Experience and Language and Literacy Skills in English-Arabic Speaking Syrian Refugees. Languages 2023, 8, 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8020101
Gottardo A, Al-Janaideh R, Paradis J, Soto-Corominas A, Chen X, Amin N. Age, Experience and Language and Literacy Skills in English-Arabic Speaking Syrian Refugees. Languages. 2023; 8(2):101. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8020101
Chicago/Turabian StyleGottardo, Alexandra, Redab Al-Janaideh, Johanne Paradis, Adriana Soto-Corominas, Xi Chen, and Norah Amin. 2023. "Age, Experience and Language and Literacy Skills in English-Arabic Speaking Syrian Refugees" Languages 8, no. 2: 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8020101
APA StyleGottardo, A., Al-Janaideh, R., Paradis, J., Soto-Corominas, A., Chen, X., & Amin, N. (2023). Age, Experience and Language and Literacy Skills in English-Arabic Speaking Syrian Refugees. Languages, 8(2), 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages8020101