Prosodic Word Recursion in a Polysynthetic Language (Blackfoot; Algonquian)
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Recursion in Prosodic Phonology
2.1. Prosodic Phonology
- (1)
- Prosodic categories
2.2. Prosodic Word Recursion
- (2)
3. Language Background
3.1. Syntactic Structure of the Verbal Complex
- (3)
- Morphological template of the verbal complexperson–(prefix*)–[stem]–suffixes
3.1.1. The Stem Is a vP/VP Phrase
(4) | Intransitive verbs | |||
a. | áakomahksimma | b. | áakomahkowa | |
áak–[omahk–i]–mm–a | áak–[omahk–o]–Ø–wa | |||
fut–[large–ai]–ind–3 | fut–[large–ii]–ind–3 | |||
‘it (e.g., the pot) will be large’ | ‘it will be big’ (BB) | |||
[FR 190] |
(5) | Transitive verbs | ||
a. | isspínnii | ámo pookáá | |
[ssp–inn–ii]–Ø–wa | amo pookaa | ||
[high–by.hand.v–3sub]–ind–3 | dem child.an | ||
‘he lifted that child’ (BB) | |||
b. | isspínnima | ámo sináákia’tsis | |
[ssp–inn–i]–m–a | amo [sin–a–aki]–a’tsis | ||
[high–by.hand.v–ti]–ind–3 | dem [mark–v–ai]–ins.in | ||
‘he lifted that book’ (BB) | |||
c. | isspínnaki | (pookáíks / sináákia’tsiists) | |
[ssp–inn–aki]–Ø–wa | pookaa–iksi / [[sin–a–aki]–a’tsis]–istsi | ||
[high–by.hand.v–ai]–ind–3 | child–an.pl / [[mark–v–ai]–ins]–in.pl | ||
‘he lifted (s.t./s.o.)’ (BB) |
- (6)
(7) | Root syntacticization as phrasal adjunct | ||||
a. | Intransitive: | root | VP[ V DP | ||
b. | Transitive: | root | DP v | V DP |
(8) | Adjunct to verb stem | (9) | Adjunct to noun stem |
ómahksiníkkssapiwa | ómahkomitaa | ||
omahk–inikk–[ss–api]–Ø–wa | omahk–omitaa–wa | ||
big–sulking–[thus–look.ai]–ind–3 | big–dog–prx | ||
‘she gave a sulking glance’ [FR 73] | ‘big dog’ [FR 188] |
- (10)
- Phrasal adjunct to verb stemáakso’kaanópiiwaaak–[[yo’k–aa]–n]–opii]–Ø–wafut–[[sleep–ai]–nmlz]–sit.ai]–ind–3‘he will doze (off)’ [FR 320]
3.1.2. The Verbal Complex Is a CP Phrase
(11) | Independent | (12) | Imperative |
CP[ Kitsówatoo’poaa ]CP | CP[ Oowátook ]CP | ||
kit–[io–wat–oo]–’p–oaa–Ø | [oo–wat–oo]–k–Ø | ||
2–[eat–v–ti]–ind–2pl–3 | [eat–v–ti]–2pl.imp–cmd | ||
‘You all ate it.’ (BB) | ‘(you all) eat it!’ (BB) |
(13) | Conjunctive | |
CP[ Nítssksinii’pa | CP[ kitsówatoohsoaayi ]CP ]CP | |
Nit–[ssk–in–i]–’p–a | kit–[io–wat–oo]–hs–oaa–yi | |
1–ti]–ind–3 | 2–[eat–v–ti]–cnj–2pl–dep | |
‘I know you ate it.’ | (Frantz 2017, [123, (f); re-glossed) |
3.1.3. Neither the Stem Nor the Verbal Complex Are a Complex Head
3.2. Blackfoot Phonology and Syllable Structure
3.2.1. Phonological Inventory
(14) | Contrast between [k] and [ks] | |||
a. | 〚ʔɪskɪ́t〛 | b. | 〚ʔɪsksɪ́t〛 | |
isskít | issksít | |||
[issk–Ø–i]–t–Ø | [issk–i]–t–Ø | |||
[by.body–v–ti]–2sg.imp–cmd | [urinate–ai]–2sg.imp–cmd | |||
‘break it!’ [FR 266] | ‘urinate!’ [FR 268] |
3.2.2. Syllable Structure
(15) | UR | IPA | Gloss | |
/apíːt/ | [ʔa.píːt] | ‘sit!’ | (BB) | |
/imitâː/ | [ʔi.mi.tâː] | ‘dog’ | (BB) | |
/otán/ | [ʔo.tʌ́n] | ‘his/her daughter’ | (BB) |
(16) | Shape | IPA | Gloss | ||
a. | CV | [ʔâː.ko.kaː] | ‘he will rope’ | (BB) | |
CVV | [ʔâː.koː.kaː] | ‘she will hold a Sundance’ | (BB) | ||
b. | CVC | [só.kaʔ.sim] | ‘shirt, dress’ | (BB) | |
[ʔɪm.mo.jáːn] | ‘fur coat’ | (BB) | |||
[pʌs.káːn] | ‘dance’ | (BB) | |||
CVVC | — | — |
(17) | IPA | Orthography | Gloss | |||
a. | [ʔɪs.sa.pɪ́t] | issapít! | ‘look!’ | (NC) | ||
[ʔa.píːt] | apíít! | ‘sit!” | (NC) | |||
b. | [pɪs.kʌ́.ni̥] | pisskáni | ‘buffalo jump’ | (NC) | ||
[pʌs.káː.ni̥] | passkááni | ‘dance” | (NC) |
(18) | IPA | Orthography | Gloss | ||
a. | [ʔo.nɪ.ˢtáxs] | onistááhsa | ‘calf’ | (BB) | |
b. | [sí.kxʷ.koʔs] | síkohko’sa | ‘cast iron pan’ | (BB) | |
c. | [míʔ.ksː.kɪmm] | mí’ksskimma | ‘metal’ | (BB) |
(19) | Minimal verbal and nominal complexes | ||||
IPA | Orthography | Gloss | |||
CVVC | [píːt] | píít | ‘enter!’ | (BB) | |
[sóːt] ∼ [so.wóːt] | sóót ∼ sowóót | ‘go to war!” | (BB) | ||
[kóːn] | kóón | ‘ice” | (BB) | ||
CVCC | [pónn] | pónn | ‘bracelet’ | (BB) | |
[kóʔs] | kó’s | ‘dish, bowl’ | (BB) |
3.2.3. Alternations
(20) | a. | After C | b. | After V |
〚nitâːksoxʷksipiˢtaː〛 | 〚ʔamopíˢta:ni〛 | |||
nitáaksoohksipistaa | amopístaani | |||
nit–aak–[yoohk–pist–aa]–(hp) | [[amo–pist–aa]–n]–i | |||
1–fut–[lid–tie.v–ai]–(ind) | [[gather–tie.v–ai]–nmlz]–in.sg | |||
‘I will close the tipi flap’ [FR 319] | ‘ceremonial bundle’ [FR 13] |
(21) | a. | After C | b. | After V |
〚ʔomatsípiːs〛 | 〚ʔamóípiːsaːwḁ〛 | |||
omatsípiisa | amóípiisaawa | |||
[omat–ipi–:s]–Ø | [amo–ipi–:s]–Ø=aawa | |||
[start–bring.v–2sg:3.imp]–cmd | [gather–bring.v–2sg:3.imp]–cmd=prx.pl | |||
‘transport him!’ [FR 193] | ‘gather them!’ [FR 195] |
- (22)
- After V〚sɛːpíːs〛saipíís[sa–ipi–:s]–Ø[out–bring.v–2sg:3.imp]–cmd‘bring her out!’ [FR 236]
(23) | After C | After V | UR | Gloss | ||
a. | [-ipiˢt] | ∼ | [-piˢt] | /-piˢt/ | ‘tie.v’ | |
b. | [-ipi] | ∼ | [-ipi] | /-ipi/ | ‘bring.v’ |
(24) | a. | 〚ʔákaːjóxʷkanɪnːimḁ〛 | b. | 〚ʔâksoxʷkójijiːwáji̥〛 |
ákaayóóhkaninnima | áaksoohkóyiyiiwáyi | |||
akaa–[yoohk–an–inn–i]–m–a | aak–[yoohk–oyi–i–yii]–Ø–w=ayi | |||
ref–[lid–sheet–by.hand.v–ti]–ind–3 | fut–[lid–mouth–v–3sub]–ind–3=obv.sg | |||
‘he has shut it (as a window)’ [FR 318] | ‘she will cover it with a lid’ [FR 319] |
4. Prosodic Structure of the Verbal Complex
4.1. Distinct Left Edge Constraints
(25) | Left edge | (26) | After V |
〚ʔiːtsːkáːt〛 | 〚ʔɛ́ːsoːkɛ́ːitsːkaːwḁ〛 | ||
iitsskáát | áísookáíitsskaawa | ||
[iitssk–aa]–t–Ø | a–isooka–[iitssk–aa]–Ø–wa | ||
[scuffle–ai]–2sg.imp–cmd | ipfv–used.to–[scuffle–ai]–ind–3 | ||
‘fight!’ [FR 38] | ‘he used to fight’ [FR 319] |
(27) | Left edge | (28) | After V |
〚ʔiːpiˢtótsit〛 | 〚nitájiːpiˢtoʦiʔpḁ〛 | ||
iipístotsit | nitáyiipistotsii’pa | ||
[yiip–istot–i]–t–Ø | nit–a–[yiip–istot–i]–hp–a | ||
[decrease–.v–ti]–2sg.imp–cmd | 1–ipfv–[decrease–caus.v–ti]–ind–3 | ||
‘decrease the volume of it!’ [FR 35] | ‘I am decreasing the amount’ [FR 313] |
(29) | Left edge | After V | UR | Gloss | ||
a. | [iːtsːk] | ∼ | [iːtsːk] | /iːtsːk/ | ‘scuffle’ | |
b. | [iːp] | ∼ | [jiːp] | /jiːp/ | ‘decrease’ | |
c. | *[jiːp] | ∼ | *[jiːp] |
(30) | Left edge | (31) | a. | After C |
〚pʊm:óːs〛 | 〚ʔâːksipʊ́mːojiːwáji̥〛 | |||
pommóós | áaksipómmoyiiwáyi | |||
[pomm–o–:s]–Ø | aak–[pomm–o–yii]–Ø–w=ayi | |||
[transfer–v–2sg:3.imp]–cmd | fut–[transfer–v–3sub]–ind–3=obv.sg | |||
‘transfer (e.g., the medicine bundle) to him!’ [FR 91] | ‘he will transfer it to her’ [FR 91] | |||
b. | After V | |||
〚ʔɛ́ːpʊmːakiwḁ〛 | ||||
áípommakiwa | ||||
a–[pomm–Ø–aki]–Ø–wa | ||||
ipfv–[transfer–v–ai]–ind–prx | ||||
‘the one transferring’ [FR 249] |
(32) | Left edge | (33) | a. | After C |
〚pʊmːáːt〛 | 〚#x294;âːkxʷpʊmːaːwḁ〛 | |||
pommáát | áakohpommaawa | |||
[pomm–aa]–t–Ø | aak–[ohpomm–aa]–Ø–wa | |||
[buy–ai]–2sg.imp–cmd | fut–[buy–ai]–ind–3 | |||
‘buy!’ [FR 175] | ‘she will buy’ [FR 175] | |||
b. | After V | |||
[ʔɔ́xʷpʊmːa] | ||||
áóhpommaawa | ||||
a–[ohpomm–aa]–Ø–wa | ||||
ipfv–[buy–ai]–ind–3 | ||||
‘s/he is shopping’ (BB) |
- (34)
- Roots with invariant [i]
- a.
- Left edge〚ʔipótsimatsís〛ipótsimatsísa[ipotsim–at–:s]–Ø[poison–v–2sg:3.imp]–cmd‘poison him!’ [FR 92]
- b.
- After C〚ʔâːksipótsimatsiːwḁ〛áaksipótsimatsiiwaaak–[ipotsim–at–ii]–Ø–wafut–[poison–v–3sub]–ind–3‘she will poison him’ [FR 92]
- (35)
- Roots with invariant [ox]
- a.
- Left edge〚ʔoxʷpóískinis〛ohpóísskinisa[ohpo–isski–n–:s]–Ø[grease–face–by.hand.v–2sg:3.imp]–cmd‘paint his face!’ [FR 174]
- b.
- After C〚ʔâːkxʷpoiskiniːwáji̥〛áakohpoisskiniiwáyiaak–[ohpo–isski–n–ii]–Ø–w=ayifut–[grease–face–by.hand.v–3sub]–ind–3=obv.sg‘she will paint his face’ [FR 174]
(36) | Left edge | After prefix | UR | Gloss | ||
a. | [pʊmː] | ∼ | [ipʊmː] | /pomː/ | ‘transfer’ | |
b. | [pʊmː] | ∼ | [oxʷpʊmː] | {/pomː, oxʷpʊmː/} | ‘buy’ | |
c. | [ipotsim] | ∼ | [ipotsim] | /ipotsim/ | ‘poison’ | |
d. | [oxʷpo] | ∼ | [oxʷpo] | /oxpo/ | ‘grease’ | |
e. | *[pʊmː] | ∼ | [pʊmː] |
(37) | a. | 〚ʔɛ́ː.pʊm.ma.ki.wḁ〛 | b. | *〚ʔá.pʊm.ma.ki.wḁ〛 |
áípommakiwa | ápommakiwa | |||
a–[pomm–Ø–aki]–Ø–wa | a–[pomm–Ø–aki]–Ø–wa | |||
ipfv–[transfer–v–ai]–ind–prx | ipfv–[transfer–v–ai]–ind–prx | |||
‘the one transferring’ (=31b) | ‘the one transferring’ |
(38) | Left edge | After C | = | After V | Gloss | |
a. | [pomː] | [ipomː] | = | [ipomː] | ‘transfer’ | |
[kipita] | [ipːita] | = | [ipːita] | ‘aged’ | ||
b. | [pomː] | [oxpomː] | = | [oxpomː] | ‘buy’ | |
c. | [iːp] | [jiːp] | = | [jiːp] | ‘decrease’ | |
d. | [maːn] | [an] | = | [an] | ‘recent’ | |
[niːpo] | [ipo] | = | [ipo] | ‘upright’ | ||
e. | [maːk] | [jaːk] | = | [jaːk] | ‘arrange’ | |
[naːm] | [jaːm] | = | [jaːm] | ‘alone’ |
(39) | Distinct left edges | ||
No prefixes: | PPh{ | PWd(stem–suffixes | |
With prefixes: | PPh{ person–prefix*– | PWd(stem–suffixes |
(40) | Left edge | (41) | After V |
〚{(ʔiːpiˢtótsit〛 | 〚{nitá(jiːpiˢtoʦiʔpḁ〛 | ||
iipístotsit | nitáyiipistotsii’pa | ||
[yiip–istot–i]–t–Ø | nit–a–[yiip–istot–i]–hp–a | ||
[decrease–.v–ti]–2sg.imp–cmd | 1–ipfv–[decrease–caus.v–ti]–ind–3 | ||
‘decrease the volume of it!’ (=27) | ‘I am decreasing the amount’ (=28) |
(42) | a. | 〚{ʔɛ́(ɛ́pʊmːakiwḁ〛 | b. | * 〚{ʔá(pʊmːakiwḁ〛 |
áípommakiwa | ápommakiwa | |||
a–[pomm–Ø–aki]–Ø–wa | a–[pomm–Ø–aki]–Ø–wa | |||
ipfv–[transfer–v–ai]–ind–prx | ipfv–[transfer–v–ai]–ind–prx | |||
‘the one transferring’ (=37a) | ‘the one transferring’ (=37b) |
4.2. Distinct Right Edge Constraints
(43) | [ʔa.píːt] | |
apíít | ||
[ap–ii]–t–Ø | ||
[sit–ai]–2sg.imp–cmd | ||
‘sit!’ (NC) | (=15) |
(44) | 〚ni.tsíː.piç.pɪn.naːn〛 | ||
nitsíípiihpinnaan | |||
nit–[ii\op–ii]–hp–nnaan | |||
1–[ic\sit–ai]–ind–1pl | |||
‘we sat/stayed’ | (Frantz 2017, p. 6) |
(45) | After C | (46) | After V | ||
〚nitsikákomɪmːokɪnːaːni̥〛 | 〚nitsikákomɪmːʌnːaːni̥〛 | ||||
nitsikákomimmokinnaani | nitsikákomimmannaani | ||||
nit–ik–[akom–imm–ok]–Ø–nnaan–i | nit–ik–[akom–imm–aa]–Ø–nnaan–i | ||||
1–deg–[favor–by.mind.v–inv]–ind–1pl–3pl | 1–deg–[favor–by.mind.v–3obj]–ind–1pl–3pl | ||||
‘They love us (excl.).’ | (Frantz 2017, p. 61, (i)) | ‘We (excl.) love them.’ | (Frantz 2017, p. 57, (g)) |
(47) | 〚nitsikákomɪmːokḁ〛 | |
nitsikákomimmoka nitána | ||
nit–ik–[akom–imm–ok]–Ø–a | ||
1–deg–[favor–by.mind.ta–inv]–ind–3 | ||
‘My daughter loves me.’ | (Frantz 2017, p. 61, (c)) |
(48) | After C | After V | UR | Gloss | ||
a. | [-inːaːn] | ∼ | [-nːaːn] | /-nːaːn/ | ‘1pl’ | |
b. | *[-C] | ∼ | *[-C] | ‘pl’ |
(49) | Distinct right edges | ||
No prefixes: | stem)PWd –suffixes }PPh | ||
With prefixes: | person–prefix*– | stem)PWd –suffixes }PPh |
4.3. Minimal Size Constraints and Obligatory Stress
(50) | Minimal noun stems | ||||
Size | IPA | Orthography | Gloss | ||
CVVC | [kóːn] | kóón | ‘ice’ | (BB) | |
CVCC | [pónn] | pónn | ‘bracelet’ | (BB) | |
[kóʔs] | kó’s | ‘dish, bowl’ | (BB) |
(51) | Minimal verb stems | ||||
Size | IPA | Orthography | Gloss | ||
CVV | [píː-] | píí- | ‘enter!’ | (BB) | |
[sóː-] ∼ [sowóː-] | sóó- ∼ sowóó- | ‘go to war!’ | (BB) |
(52) | a. | Stress within PWd | b. | Stress outside PWd |
〚ʔɪstáːwḁ〛 | 〚ʔiksímsːtaːwḁ〛 | |||
isstááwa | iksímsstaawa | |||
[isst–aa]–Ø–wa | iksim–[sst–aa]–Ø–wa | |||
[wish–ai]–ind–3 | secret–[wish–ai]–ind–3 | |||
‘she wants’ [FR 272] | ‘he thought’ [FR 61] |
(53) | Prosody of the verbal complex |
{person–prefix*– (stem)PWd –suffixes}PPh |
5. Evidence for Recursive Prosodic Words
5.1. Prefixes and the PPh: No Shared Phonological Properties
- (55)
- a.
- Prefix at left edge〚ʔiːˢt͡sipʊ́mːatoːt〛iistsipómmatootyiist–[pomm–at–oo]–t–Øon.back–[transfer–v–ti]–2sg.imp–cmd‘unload it from your back!’ [FR 315]
- b.
- Prefix after V〚niːtáʔpajiːˢt͡sipʊmːatoːmḁ〛niitá’payiistsipommatoomaniita’p–a–yiist–[pomm–at–oo]–m–areally–ipfv–on.back–[transfer–v–ti]–ind–3‘he started to take it off his back/body’ [FR 315]
- c.
- *〚niːtáʔpɛːiˢt͡sipʊmːatoːmḁ〛niitá’paiistsipommatoomaniita’p–a–yiist–[pomm–at–oo]–m–areally–ipfv–on.back–[transfer–v–ti]–ind–3‘he started to take it off his back/body’
(56) | 〚ʔsːkéjʔpapʊmːḁ〛 | (57) | cf. | 〚ʔipapʊmːḁ〛 |
sskáí’papomma | ipapómma | |||
sska’–[ipap–o]–mm–a | [ipap–o]–mm–a | |||
shock–[emit.burst–ii]–ind–3 | [emit.burst–ii]–ind–3 | |||
‘the lightning really flashed’ [FR 64] | ‘there was lightning’ [FR 83] |
(58) | (ʔitɔ́xʷtoji | amí | (ʔiːmaxki̥naj |
iitáóhtoyii | amí | iimahkihkinay | |
ii\it–a–[yooht–o–ii]–Ø–wa | am–i | [ii\omahk–ihkin–aa]–yi | |
ic\then–ipfv–[hear–ta–3sub]–ind–3 | dem–obv | [ic\big–hair–ai]–obv | |
ʔɔ́xʷkomiːnɛː | |||
áóhkomiinai. | |||
a–[ohkom–i]–Ø–yini=ai | |||
ipfv–[bellow–ai]–ind–3obv=obv.sg | |||
‘[He was still picking and] he heard this goat.’ | |||
(Pear Story, told by Totsinámm (BB)) |
- (59)
- 〚ʔiːksíjçpijiwḁ〛iiksíyihpiyiwaii\ksiw–[ihpi–yi]–Ø–aic\ground.level–[dance–ai]–ind–3‘he danced low’ [FR 66]
- (60)
- a.
- 〚ʔiˢtsiksiwɛ́ːnakaʔsit〛istsiksiwáínaka’sitist–ksiw–a–[inak–a’si]–t–Øthere–ground.level–ipfv–[roll–ai]–2sg.imp–cmd‘roll there!’ [FR 66]
- b.
- 〚ksiwawakaːsiwḁ〛ksiwawakaasiwaksiw–[awa–ka–a’si]–waground.level–[meander–leg–ai]–prx‘spider’ (lit. ‘ground-level deer’) [FR 142]
- c.
- 〚ksowóxʷtsi〛ksowóóhtsiksiw–ooht–iground.level–ward–in.sg‘down low’ [FR 142]
(61) | Minimal prefixes | ||
V | a- | ‘ipfv’ | |
CV | sa- | ‘out’ | |
VC | on- | ‘hurry’ |
(62) | Stress on one of multiple prefixes | |
[ʔitanɪ́ˢtsɪksɪmʔsːtaja] | ||
itanístsiksimsstaya | ||
it–anist–iksim–[sst–aa]–Ø–yi=aawa | ||
then–manner–secret–[wish–ai]–ind–3pl=prx.pl | ||
‘they decided thus’ | (BB, 2013-02-13, ‘Old Woman in the Cold’) |
5.2. Prefixes and the PWd: Only Left-Edge Restrictions Are Shared
- (63)
- a.
- Root at left edge〚kanáʔpsːik〛kaná’pssik[kan–a’pssi]–k–Ø[all–be.ai]–2pl.imp–cmd‘you (pl.) gather for an event!’ [FR 164]
- b.
- Root after a prefix〚ʔâːkxʷkanáʔpsːija:wḁ〛áakohkaná’pssiyaawaaak–[ohkan–a’pssi]–k–Øfut–[all–be.ai]–2pl.imp–cmd‘they will gather for a sporting event’ [FR 164]
- (64)
- Prefix after C
- a.
- 〚ʔâːkxʷkánsːʌmːawḁ〛áakohkánssammawaaak–ohkan–[ss–amm–a]–Ø–wafut–all–[thus–watch.ta–3obj]–ind–3‘she will be watched by all’ [FR 163]
- b.
- *〚ʔâːksikánsːʌmːawḁ〛áaksikánssammawaaak–kan–[ss–amm–a]–Ø–wafut–all–[thus–watch.ta–3obj]–ind–3‘she will be watched by all’
- (65)
- cf. 〚ʔɪsːʌ́mːiːwáji̥〛issámmiiwáyi[iss–amm–ii]–Ø–w=ayi[thus–watch.ta–3sub]–ind–3=obv.sg‘he looked at him’ [FR 263]
- (66)
- Prefix after V
- a.
- 〚ʔɔ́xʷkanokɪmːawḁ〛áóhkanokimmawaa–ohkan–[ok–imm–a]–Ø–waipfv–all–[bad–by.mind.ta–3obj]–ind–3‘he is scolded by all’ [FR 163]
- b.
- * 〚ʔákanokɪmːawḁ〛ákanokimmawaa–kan–[ok–imm–a]–Ø–waipfv–all–[bad–by.mind.ta–3obj]–ind–3‘he is scolded by all’
- (67)
- cf. 〚ʔâːkokɪmːiːwaji̥〛áakokimmiiwáyiaak–[ok–imm–ii]–Ø–w=ayifut–[bad–by.mind.ta–3sub]–ind–3=obv.sg‘she will scold him’ [FR 184]
(68) | 〚ʔɪˢtːx̩ʷksísokaʔsimi〛 | (69) | cf. 〚sokáʔsimi〛 |
isttohksisoka’simi | soká’simi | ||
isttohk–[soka’sim]–i | [soka’sim]–i | ||
thin–[shirt.n]–in.sg | [shirt.n]–in.sg | ||
‘shirt’ [FR 109] | ‘shirt, dress, outer garment’ (BB) |
(70) | Minimal prefixes | ||
V | a- | ‘ipfv’ | |
CV | sa- | ‘out’ | |
VC | on- | ‘hurry’ |
5.3. Prefixes and Prosody: Prefixes Are Prosodified as a PWd Adjunct
(71) | prefix | root–v–V | –I–C | ||
a. | *prefix– | root–v–V | –I–C | (free clitic) | |
b. | *prefix– | root–v–V | –I–C | (prosodic word) | |
c. | prefix– | root–v–V | –I–C | (PWd adjunct) | |
d. | prefix– | root–v–V | –I–C | (internal clitic) |
- (72)
- 〚ʔâːksikotskiɛ́stojiwḁ〛áaksikotskiáísstoyiwaaak–kotski–a–[ssto–yi]–Ø–wafut–extreme–ipfv–[cold–ii]–ind–3‘it will be extremely cold’ [FR 139]
- (73)
- cf. 〚koːtskiɛ́ːsajâːkiːwḁ〛kootskiáísayáakiiwakootski–a–[[say–i]–[aakii]]–Ø–waextreme–ipfv–[[lie–ai]–[woman]]–ind–3‘she is a terrible liar’ [FR 139]
- (74)
- cf. 〚ʔ(ɪ)stojíːwḁ〛(i)sstoyííwa[ssto–yii]–Ø–wa[cold–ii]–ind–3‘it is/was cold’ [FR 274]
- (75)
- 〚stámxʷkánɛːsapaʔkotsːtoːtaːwḁ〛stámohkánaisapa’kotsstootaawastam–ohkan–a–[sapa’kot–sstoo]–t–Ø=aawajust–all–ipfv–[stack–put.ti]–2sg.imp–cmd=prx.pl‘stack all of them (e.g., chairs)! [FR 241]
- (76)
- cf. 〚kanáʔpsːik〛kaná’pssik[kan–a’pssi]–k–Ø[all–be.ai]–2pl.imp–cmd‘you (pl.) gather for an event!’ (=63a)
- (77)
- cf. 〚sapáʔkotxʷtoːt〛sapá’kotohtoot[sapa’kot–ohtoo]–t–Ø[stack–put.ti]–2sg.imp–cmd‘layer it (e.g., the material that you are sewing)!’ [FR 241]
- (78)
- ((ʔâːks( ikotski(ɛ́(stoji.wḁáaksikotskiáísstoyiwaaak–kotski–a–[ssto–yi]–Ø–wafut–extreme–ipfv–[cold–ii]–ind–3‘it will be extremely cold’ (=72)
- (79)
- ((stám( xʷkán(ɛ(ɛsapaʔkotsːtoːtaːwḁstámohkánaisapa’kotsstootaawastam–ohkan–a–[sapa’kot–sstoo]–t–Ø=aawajust–all–ipfv–[stack–put.ti]–2sg.imp–cmd=prx.pl‘stack all of them (e.g., chairs)! (=75)
5.4. Syntactic and Lexical Properties Do Not Motivate Prosodic Word Recursion
(80) | Aspect and modal operators | ||
a- | -ya- | ‘ipfv’ | |
ohkott- | -ohkott- | ‘able’ |
(81) | Negative and focus operators | ||
saw- | -isaw-, -ssaw- | ‘neg’ | |
kaak- | -ikak- | ‘foc’ |
(82) | Numerals and quantifiers | ||
naat-, niist- | -ist- | ‘two’ | |
kan- | -ohkan- | ‘all’ |
(83) | Control verbs | ||
omat- | -omat- | ‘start to’ | |
mato- | -oto- | ‘go to’ | |
ssaak- | -issaak-, -sssaak- | ‘try to’ |
(84) | Relative roots | ||
ist-, st- | -it- | ‘then, there’ | |
oht-, t- | -oht- | ‘from, source, purpose’ | |
ohp-, p- | -ohp- | ‘with’ |
(85) | Aspectual, degree, event modifiers | ||
ksist- | -iksist- | ‘finish’ | |
sam- | -isam- | ‘long’ (in time) | |
kam- | -ikkam- | ‘fast’ | |
maohk- | -omaohk- | ‘red’ | |
simi- | -isimi- | ‘secretly, on the sly’ | |
mak- | -ok- | ‘bad’ |
(86) | Locative, directional, and positional modifiers | ||
sipi- | -isipi- | ‘at night’ | |
isttss- | -isttss- | ‘forest’ | |
pisst- | -ipsst- | ‘inside’ | |
poohsap- | -ipoohsap- | ‘towards speaker’ | |
niipo- | -ipo- | ‘upright’ |
6. Analysis of Correspondence
6.1. Overview of Theories and Constraints
- Wrap Theory (Kabak and Revithiadou 2009; Truckenbrodt 1999)
- Match Theory (Selkirk 2011)
6.1.1. Alignment Theory
- (87)
- Align(Lex, L/R, PWd, L/R)For every Lex, there is a prosodic word, PWd, such that the left/right edge of Lex and the left/right edge of the PWd align.
- (88)
- Exhaustivity(p,q)Assign a violation for every prosodic constituent q, , which is dominated by a prosodic constituent p. (After Werle 2009, p. 23)
- (89)
- *Recursivity(p)Assign a violation for every prosodic constituent p which dominates a prosodic constituent p. (After Werle 2009, p. 23)
6.1.2. Wrap Theory
- (90)
- Wrap-XPEach XP is contained in a phonological phrase. (Truckenbrodt 1999, p. 228)
- (91)
- Recursion derived with Wrap-XP (Truckenbrodt 1999)
(92) | Wrap |
Each X/XP is contained in its own PW/PPh, respectively. | |
(Kabak and Revithiadou 2009, p. 116) |
- (93)
- Recursion derived with Wrap-XP (Kabak and Revithiadou 2009)
6.1.3. Match Theory
(94) | Syntax-prosody correspondences in Match Theory | ||||
‘syntactic clause’ | ([Comp, C] or [Comp, Force]) | ⟷ | ɩ | (intonational phrase) | |
‘syntactic phrase’ | (XP) | ⟷ | φ | (phonological phrase) | |
‘syntactic word’ | (Lex) | ⟷ | ω | (prosodic word) |
(95) | MatchPhase |
Suppose there is a syntactic phrase (XP) in the syntactic representation that exhaustively dominates a set of one or more terminal nodes . Assign one violation mark if there is no phonological phrase (φ) in the phonological representation that exhaustively dominates all and only the phonological exponents of the terminal nodes in . | |
(Elfner 2012, p. 28) |
(96) | MatchWord | |
a. | MatchWord(All) | |
Suppose there is an X in the input syntactic representation that exhaustively dominates a set of morphemes . Assign a violation mark for every segment that (1) is an exponent of a morpheme in and (2) is not dominated by a PWd in the output phonological representation corresponding to the X. | ||
b. | MatchWord(Only) | |
Suppose there is an X in the input syntactic representation that exhaustively dominates a set of morphemes . Assign a violation mark for every segment that (1) is an exponent of a morpheme that is not in and (2) is dominated by a PWd in the output phonological representation corresponding to the X. | ||
(Guekguezian 2017, p. 22, (14)) |
(97) | BinMin (Bin) | |
A PPh must consist of at least two prosodic words. | (Inkelas and Zec 1995) |
6.2. The Phrasal vP (Not a Lex Head) Corresponds to a PWd
6.3. Each Constituent (Not Just the vP) Corresponds to a PWd
(98) | Input | To test whether the OT system allows… |
pre–stem | full clitic typology | |
pre–pre–stem | multiple recursion |
(99) | Exhaustivity(PPh,σ) (Abbrev: Exh) |
Assign a violation for every prosodic constituent which is dominated by a prosodic constituent PPh. (After Werle 2009, p. 23) |
(100) | *Recursivity(PWd) (Abbrev: *Rec) |
Assign a violation for every prosodic constituent PWd which dominates a prosodic constituent PWd. (After Werle 2009, p. 23) |
(101) | BinMin (Abbrev: Bin) | |
A PPh must consist of at least two sister PWds. | (Modified from Inkelas and Zec 1995) |
6.3.1. Alignment Theory
(102) | Align(vP,L,PWd,L) (Abbrev: AlL(vP)) |
For every vP phase, there is a prosodic word, PWd, such that the left edge of the vP and the left edge of the PWd align. |
(103) | Align(vP,R,PWd,R) (Abbrev: AlR(vP)) |
For every vP phase, there is a prosodic word, PWd, such that the right edge of the vP and the right edge of the PWd align. |
(104) | Align(PWd,L,vP,L) (Abbrev: AlL(PWd)) |
For every prosodic word, PWd, there is a vP phase, such that the left edge of the PWd and the left edge of the vP align. |
(105) | Align(PWd,R,vP,R) (Abbrev: AlR(PWd)) |
For every prosodic word, PWd, there is a vP phase, such that the right edge of the PWd and the right edge of the vP align. |
6.3.2. Wrap Theory
(108) | Wrap |
Each X/XP is contained in its own PW/PPh, respectively. | |
(Kabak and Revithiadou 2009, p. 116) |
(109) | SP-Correspondence Constraints (Blackfoot) | |
Let S be an input syntactic representation and P its corresponding output phonological representation. | ||
a. | SP:Max (SP:M): A constituent of type vP phase with phonological content in S corresponds to some constituent of type PWd in P. | |
b. | PS:Dep (PS:D): A constituent of type PWd in P corresponds to some constituent of type vP phase in S. |
6.3.3. Match Word Theory
- (112)
- Match(vP,PWd) (Abbrev: M(vP))Suppose there is a syntactic vP phase in the syntactic representation that exhaustively dominates a set of one or more terminal nodes . Assign one violation mark if there is no prosodic word (PWd) in the phonological representation that exhaustively dominates all and only the phonological exponents of the terminal nodes in .
- (113)
- Match(PWd,vP) (Abbrev: M(PWd))Suppose there is a prosodic word (PWd) in the phonological representation that exhaustively dominates a set of phonological exponents. Assign one violation mark if there is no syntactic vP phase in the syntactic representation that exhaustively dominates one or more terminal nodes which correspond to all and only the phonological exponents of the PWd.
6.3.4. Match Phrase Theory
- (116)
- Match(XP,PWd) (Abbrev: M(XP))Suppose there is a syntactic phrase (XP) in the syntactic representation, where XP = vP or XP dominates vP, which exhaustively dominates a set of one or more terminal nodes . Assign one violation mark if there is no prosodic word (PWd) in the phonological representation that exhaustively dominates all and only the phonological exponents of the terminal nodes in .
- (117)
- Match(PWd,XP) (Abbrev: M(PWd))Suppose there is a prosodic word (PWd) in the phonological representation that exhaustively dominates a set of phonological exponents. Assign one violation mark if there is no syntactic constituent (XP), where XP = vP or XP dominates vP, in the syntactic representation that exhaustively dominates one or more terminal nodes which correspond to all and only the phonological exponents of the PWd.
6.4. A *Recursivity Constraint Is Necessary
6.5. Summary of Models
7. Discussion
- (122)
- 〚ʔɛ́ː.pʊm.ma.ki.wḁ〛áípommakiwaa–[pomm–Ø–aki]–Ø–waipfv–[transfer–v–ai]–ind–prx‘the one transferring’ (=37a)
8. Conclusions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
1 | First person; |
2 | Second person; |
3 | Third person; |
AI | Animate intransitive; |
AN | Animate; |
CAUS | Causative; |
CMD | Command clause; |
CNJ | Conjunctive order; |
CONJ | Conjunction; |
DEG | Degree marker; |
DEM | Demonstrative; |
DEP | Dependent clause; |
FOC | Focus; |
FUT | Future; |
IC | Initial change; |
II | Inanimate intransitive; |
IMP | Imperative; |
IN | Inanimate; |
IND | Independent order; |
INS | Instrumental; |
INV | Inverse; |
IPFV | Imperfective; |
NEG | Negative; |
NMLZ | Nominalizer; |
OBJ | Object; |
OBV | Obviative; |
PL | Plural; |
PRF | Perfect; |
PRX | Proximate; |
SG | Singular; |
SHEET | Two-dimensional flexible material; |
SUB | Subject; |
TA | Transitive animate; |
TI | Transitive inanimate; |
WARD | Direction/locative nominal; |
WH | Content question marker. |
1 | Any examples marked with ‘(BB)’ or ‘(NC)’ are from the author’s fieldwork with Totsinámm (Beatrice Bullshields) and Ááhsaikamo’sáákii (Natalie Creighton), and the IPA transcriptions are based on the speakers’ pronunciations and surrounded by single brackets, [ ]. Data from Frantz and Russell (2017) is cited with ‘[FR #]’, where # is the page number. For examples taken from reference materials, the IPA transcriptions are converted from the orthography and are surrounded by double brackets, 〚 〛. (See Weber 2020 for a description of how IPA transcriptions are derived from the orthography.) For the morphemic analysis of examples, I use the orthography in Frantz (2017) and Frantz and Russell (2017), which maps closely to phonemic or broad phonetic transcription (except that /ʔ/ = <’>, /j/ = <y>, /x/ = <h>, /ɛː/ = <ai>, /ɔː/ = <ao>, and other long sounds are doubled.) The stem is given in square brackets, [ ]. |
2 | This suffix takes different forms depending on the person features of both arguments. For certain configurations of subject and object, the suffix agrees with one of the two arguments, while for others, the suffix is simply -ok ‘inv’. This type of direct/inverse agreement system occurs across the Algonquian family (Oxford 2014). |
3 | This feature is similar to [H(igh animacy)] in Wiltschko and Ritter (2015). |
4 | This syntactic analysis follows Déchaine and Weber (2018) and Weber (2020). Other syntactic analyses of Algonquian verb stems agree that the first suffix after the root is a light verbal head, but differ in how the root and the second suffix in transitive verbs are syntacticized (Bliss 2013; Branigan et al. 2005; Brittain 2003; Hirose 2000; Quinn 2006). |
5 | Déchaine and Weber (2018) argues that a root in Blackfoot and Plains Cree may syntacticize in one of three ways: XP-adjuncts, X-adjuncts computed online, or precompiled X-adjuncts. Here I focus only on XP-adjoined roots. |
6 | A semantic argument for why verbal heads are bound is also pursued in Déchaine and Weber (2015) for Blackfoot and Plains Cree and in Slavin (2012) for Oji-Cree. In contrast, Déchaine and Weber (2018) takes the view that this is a morphosyntactic requirement. |
7 | |
8 | |
9 | This pattern of vowel coalescence holds for roots which begin in long [iː] or long [oː] at the left edge. A [w] is epenthesized between a vowel and a root which begins in long [ɛː], [aː], or [ɔː], which Weber (2020) analyzes as mora preservation on [-high] vowels. For the sake of space, I abstract away from the various vowel hiatus resolution strategies here. |
10 | A small subset of this type also involve an irregular pattern of root-internal gemination which arose from a historical process of short vowel deletion with subsequent full assimilation of the resulting consonant cluster (Berman 2006; Elfner 2006; Thomson 1978). For example, the root kipita ‘aged’ surfaces as [kipita] at the left edge of the CP verbal complex, but [ippita] after a prefix. |
11 | |
12 | The PPh constituent is also well established as the domain of several phonological processes, such as vowel coalescence and /t/-assibilation (Bliss 2013; Weber 2020). I do not discuss these processes further in this paper. |
13 | There are some roots which can apparently be smaller, such as p ‘release’ and ss ‘break’. These always occur immediately adjacent to a bound verbalizing head, and cannot occur left of another root. In Algonquianist terminology, they are initials, but never prefixes. |
14 | Although this example involves a noun and not a verb, the root isttohk ‘thin’ can also occur before verb stems, where I expect the same process to occur. I could find no examples of a /k/-final prefix before a consonant-initial verb stem in Frantz and Russell (2017). |
15 | For some speakers, this noun is [asoka’simi] ‘shirt’ with an initial vowel [a]. It is possible that for these speakers, the initial vowel alternates between [a] at the left edge of the PPh and [i] elsewhere. |
16 | The parentheses around the initial vowel in this form probably indicate intraspeaker or interspeaker variation. |
17 | A similar typology occurs in Peperkamp (1997) without Alignment constraints. In her model, lexical structure is built cyclically, so includes a Faithfulness constraint which requires previously built prosodic structure to be respected Peperkamp (1997, p. 189). This constraint dominates a *Rec constraint to derive recursive PWd structures. |
18 | If the rhythmic categories of feet, syllables, and moras are separated from the prosodic hierarchy, as in Inkelas (1990), then Exhaustivity could be redefined as a family of Parse-into-X constraints (Ito and Mester 2009a). |
19 | For this kind of interpretation of ‘lexical category word’ for other morphologically complex languages, see Guekguezian (2017) and Newell (2008). |
20 | |
21 | These constraints on their own do not ensure that each vP phase corresponds to ‘its own’ PWd. Presumably this comes from the interaction of other constraints, such as high-ranked constraints which prohibit a vP phase in the input from corresponding to multiple PWds in the output, and which prohibit a PWd in the output from corresponding to multiple vP phases in the input. |
22 | Note that in the second tableau, candidates (e) and (f) have the same violation profile. These two must differ in some other constraint not shown here. |
References
- Alber, Birgit, Natalie DelBusso, and Alan Prince. 2016. From intensional properties to universal support. Language 92: e88–e116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Armoskaite, Solveiga. 2006. Heteromorphemic assibilation of k in Blackfoot. Qualifying Paper, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Baker, Mark C. 1988. Incorporation: A Theory of Grammatical Function Changing. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baker, Mark C. 1996. The Polysynthesis Parameter. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baker, Mark C. 2003. Lexical Categories: Verbs, Nouns and Adjectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baker, Mark C. 2009. Is head movement still needed for noun incorporation? Lingua 119: 148–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barrie, Michael, and Éric Mathieu. 2016. Noun incorporation and phrasal movement. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 34: 1–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beckman, Mary E., and Janet B. Pierrehumbert. 1986. Intonational structure in Japanese and English. Phonology 3: 255–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bellik, Jennifer, Junko Itô, Nick Kalivoda, and Armin Mester. 2022. Matching and Alignment. In Prosody and Prosodic Interfaces. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Bennett, Ryan. 2018. Recursive prosodic words in Kaqchikel (Mayan). Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 3: 67. 1–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bennett, Ryan, and Emily Elfner. 2019. The syntax–prosody interface. Annual Review of Linguistics 5: 151–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berman, Howard. 2006. Studies in Blackfoot prehistory. International Journal of American Linguistics 72: 264–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bliss, Heather. 2013. The Blackfoot Configurationality Conspiracy. Ph.D. thesis, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Bonet, Eulàlia, Maria-Rosa Lloret, and Joan Mascaró. 2007. Allomorph selection and lexical preferences: Two case studies. Lingua 117: 903–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bloomfield, Leonard. 1946. Algonquian. In Linguistic structures of Native America, ed. by Harry Hoijer et al., pp. 85–129. New York: Viking Fund Publications in Anthropology, Volume 6. [Google Scholar]
- Booij, Geert. 1995. The phonology of Dutch. Oxford: Clarendon Press. [Google Scholar]
- Booij, Geert. 1996. Cliticization as prosodic integration: The case of Dutch. The Linguistic Review 13: 219–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Borer, Hagit. 2013. Structuring Sense III: Taking Form. Oxford: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Branigan, Phil, Julie Brittain, and Carrie Dyck. 2005. Balancing syntax and prosody in the Algonquian verb complex. In Papers of the 36th Algonquian Conference. Winnipeg: University of Manitoba, pp. 75–93. [Google Scholar]
- Brittain, Julie. 2003. A distributed morphology account of the syntax of the Algonquian verb. In Proceedings of the 2003 Annual Conference of the Canadian Linguistic Association, Halifax, NS, Canada, June 1–4; pp. 25–39. [Google Scholar]
- Bye, Patrick, and Paul de Lacy. 2000. Edge asymmetries in phonology and morphology. In Proceedings of NELS 30. Amherst: Graduate Linguistic Student Association, Department of Linguistics, University of Massachusetts Amherst, pp. 121–35. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 1995. Bare phrase structure. In Evolution and Revolution in Linguistic Theory: Essays in honor of Carlos Otero. Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press, pp. 51–109. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2000. Minimalist inquiries: The framework. In Step by Step: Essays in Minimalist Syntax in Honor of Howard Lasnik. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 89–155. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam. 2001. Derivation by phase. In Ken Hale: A Life in Language. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 1–52. [Google Scholar]
- Chomsky, Noam, and Morris Halle. 1968. The Sound Pattern of English. New York: Harper and Row. [Google Scholar]
- Déchaine, Rose-Marie, and Natalie Weber. 2015. Head-merge, adjunct-merge, and the syntax of root categorisation. In Proceedings of the Poster Session of the 33rd West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, Burnaby, BC, Canada, March 27–29; Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 38–47. [Google Scholar]
- Déchaine, Rose-Marie, and Natalie Weber. 2018. Root syntax: Evidence from Algonquian. In Papers of the Forty-Seventh Algonquian Conference. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Déchaine, Rose-Marie, and Martina Wiltschko. 2010. Micro-variation in agreement, clause-typing and finiteness: Comparative evidence from Plains Cree and Blackfoot. In Papers of the Forty-second Algonquian Conference. Albany: SUNY Press. [Google Scholar]
- Derrick, Donald. 2007. Syllabification and Blackfoot /s/. In Proceedings of the Northwest Linguistics Conference (NWLC), Burnaby, BC, Canada, February 18–19; Vancouver: Simon Fraser University Working Papers in Linguistics, vol. 1, pp. 62–76. [Google Scholar]
- Dobashi, Yoshihito. 2003. Phonological Phrasing and Syntactic Derivation. Ph.D. thesis, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Downing, Laura J. 1999. Prosodic stem ≠ prosodic word in Bantu. In Studies on the Phonological Word. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing, pp. 73–98. [Google Scholar]
- Elfner, Emily. 2006. The Mora in Blackfoot. Master’s thesis, University of Calgary, Calgary, AB, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Elfner, Emily. 2012. Syntax-Prosody Interactions in Irish. Ph.D. thesis, University of Massachusetts Amherst, Amherst, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Elfner, Emily. 2015. Recursion in prosodic phrasing: Evidence from Connemara Irish. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 33: 1169–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elfner, Emily. 2018. The syntax-prosody interface: Current theoretical approaches and outstanding questions. Linguistics Vanguard 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Féry, Caroline. 2011. German sentence accents and embedded prosodic phrases. Lingua 121: 1906–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Féry, Caroline, and Hubert Truckenbrodt. 2005. Sisterhood and tonal scaling. Studia Linguistica 59: 223–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frantz, Donald G. 2017. Blackfoot Grammar, 3rd ed. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. [Google Scholar]
- Frantz, Donald G., and Norma Jean Russell. 2017. Blackfoot Dictionary of Stems, Roots, and Affixes, 3rd ed. Toronto: University of Toronto Press. [Google Scholar]
- Frota, Sónia. 2000. Prosody and Focus in European Portuguese: Phonological Phrasing and Intonation. New York: Garland. [Google Scholar]
- Goad, Heather, and Akiko Shimada. 2014. In some languages, /s/ is a vowel. In Proceedings of the 2013 Annual Meeting on Phonology. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, Antony Dubach. 1997. The Prosodic Structure of Irish, Scots Gaelic, and Manx. Ph.D. thesis, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Guekguezian, Peter Ara. 2017. Prosodic Recursion and Syntactic Cyclicity inside the Word. Ph.D. thesis, University of California, Santa Cruz, CA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Gussenhoven, Carlos. 2004. The Phonology of Tone and Intonation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hale, Kenneth Locke, and Samuel Jay Keyser. 1993. On argument structure and the lexical expression of syntactic relations. In The View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger. Cambridge: MIT Press, chp. 2. pp. 53–109. [Google Scholar]
- Hall, T. Alan. 1999. The phonological word: A review. In Studies on the Phonological Word. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing, pp. 1–22. [Google Scholar]
- Halle, Morris, and Alec Marantz. 1993. Distributed Morphology and the pieces of inflection. In The View from Building 20: Essays in Linguistics in Honor of Sylvain Bromberger. Cambridge: MIT Press, pp. 111–77. [Google Scholar]
- Harley, Heidi, and Rolf Noyer. 1999. Distributed Morphology. Glot International 4: 3–9. [Google Scholar]
- Harley, Heidi, and Rolf Noyer. 2014. Distributed Morphology. In The Second Glot International State-of-the-Article Book: The Latest in Linguistics. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 463–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hayes, Bruce. 1989. The prosodic hierarchy in meter. In Rhythm and Meter. Orlando: Academic Press, pp. 201–60. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, Bruce. 1995. Metrical Stress Theory: Principles and Case Studies. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hirose, Tomio. 2000. Origins of Predicates: Evidence from Plains Cree. Ph.D. thesis, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Hyman, Larry. 1985. A Theory of Phonological Weight. Dordrecht: Foris. [Google Scholar]
- Inkelas, Sharon. 1990. Prosodic Constituency in the Lexicon. New York: Garland. [Google Scholar]
- Inkelas, Sharon, and Draga Zec. 1995. The phonology-syntax interface. In The Handbook of Phonological Theory. Oxford: Blackwell. [Google Scholar]
- Itô, Junko. 1986. Syllable Theory in Prosodic Phonology. Ph.D. thesis, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, USA. Published 1988, New York: Garland. [Google Scholar]
- Itô, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2003. Weak layering and word binarity. In A New Century of Phonology and Phonological Theory: A Festschrift for Professor Shosuke Haraguchi on the Occasion of His Sixtieth Birthday. Slightly Revised Version of 1992 LRC Working Paper. Tokyo: Kaitakusha, pp. 26–65. [Google Scholar]
- Itô, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2007. Prosodic adjunction in Japanese compounds. In Proceedings of Formal Approaches to Japanese Linguistics. [FAJL] 4, in MIT Working Papers in Linguistics 55. Cambridge: Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, pp. 97–111. [Google Scholar]
- Itô, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2009a. The extended prosodic word. In Phonological Domains: Universals and Deviations. Number 16 in Interface Explorations. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 135–94. [Google Scholar]
- Itô, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2009b. The onset of the prosodic word. In Phonological Argumentation: Essays on Evidence and Motivation, Advances in Optimality Theory. London: Equinox. [Google Scholar]
- Itô, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2012. Recursive prosodic phrasing in Japanese. In Prosody Matters: Essays in Honor of Elisabeth Selkirk. Advances in Optimality Theory. Sheffield: Equinox Press, pp. 280–303. [Google Scholar]
- Itô, Junko, and Armin Mester. 2019. Match as syntax-prosody Max/Dep: Prosodic enclisis in English. English Linguistics 36: 1–28. [Google Scholar]
- Jackendoff, Ray. 1977. X Syntax: A Study of Phrase Structure. Number 2 in Linguistic Inquiry Monographs. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jun, Sun-Ah. 2005. Prosodic typology. In Prosodic Typology: The Phonology of Intonation and Phrasing. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 430–58. [Google Scholar]
- Kabak, Baris, and Anthi Revithiadou. 2009. An interface approach to prosodic word recursion. In Phonological Domains: Universals and Deviations. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 105–33. [Google Scholar]
- Kalivoda, Nicholas. 2018. Syntax-Prosody Mismatches in Optimality Theory. Ph.D. thesis, University of Southern California, California, CA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Kawahara, Shigeto, and Takahito Shinya. 2008. The intonation of gapping and coordination in Japanese: Evidence for intonational phrase and utterance. Phonetica 65: 62–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Kyumin. 2015. Spatial PPs and structure of motion verbs in Blackfoot. In Proceedings of the Eighteenth and Nineteenth Workshop on Structure and Constituency in the Languages of the Americas. [Papers for WSCLA 18/19], UBCWPL 39. Vancouver: University of British Columbia, pp. 125–39. [Google Scholar]
- Kim, Kyumin. 2017. Animacy and transitivity alternations in Blackfoot. In Papers of the 47th Algonquian Conference. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press, pp. 123–40. [Google Scholar]
- Kratzer, Angelika. 1996. Severing the external argument from its verb. In Phrase Structure and the Lexicon. Volume 33 of Studies in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory. Berlin: Springer, pp. 109–37. [Google Scholar]
- Kubozono, Haruo. 1987. The Organisation of Japanese Prosody. Ph.D. thesis, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, UK. [Google Scholar]
- Kubozono, Haruo. 1989. Syntactic and rhythmic effects on downstep in Japanese. Phonology 6: 39–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Labrune, Laurence. 2012. Questioning the universality of the syllable: Evidence from Japanese. Phonology 29: 113–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ladd, D. Robert. 1986. Intonational phrasing. Phonology Yearbook 3: 311–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ladd, D. Robert. 2008. Intonational Phonology, 2nd ed. Volume 119 of Cambridge Studies in Linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Marantz, Alec. 1997. No escape from syntax. In Linguistics 4. Philadelphia: Department of Linguistics, University of Pennsylvania, Penn Graduate Linguistics Society, pp. 201–25. [Google Scholar]
- Martínez-Paricio, Violeta. 2013. An Exploration of Minimal and Maximal Metrical Feet. Ph.D. thesis, UiT The Arctic University of Norway, Tromsø, Norway. [Google Scholar]
- Mascaró, Joan. 2007. External allomorphy and lexical representation. Linguistic Inquiry 38: 715–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McCarthy, John J., and Alan Prince. 1994a. Generalized alignment. In Yearbook of Morphology 1993. Dordrect: Kluwer Academic Publishers, pp. 79–153. [Google Scholar]
- McCarthy, John J., and Alan S. Prince. 1994b. The emergence of the unmarked: Optimality in prosodic morphology. In Papers from the Annual Meeting of the North East Linguistic Society 24. Amherst: Department of Linguistics, University of Massachusetts, Graduate Linguistic Student Association, pp. 333–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCarthy, John J., and Alan Prince. 1995. Faithfulness and reduplicative identity. In Papers in Optimality Theory. Volume 18 of University of Massachusetts Occasional Papers in Linguistics. Amherst: Graduate Linguistic Student Association, Department of Linguistics, University of Massachusetts Amherst, pp. 249–384. [Google Scholar]
- McCawley, James D. 1968. The Phonological Component of a Grammar of Japanese. Number 2 in Monographs on Linguistic Analysis. The Hague: Mouton. [Google Scholar]
- Miller, Taylor Lampton. 2018. The Phonology-Syntax Interface and Polysynthesis: A Study of Kiowa and Saulteaux Ojibwe. Ph.D. thesis, University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware. [Google Scholar]
- Miller, Taylor Lampton. 2020. Navigating the phonology-syntax interface and Tri-P mapping. In Proceedings of the 2019 Annual Meeting on Phonology. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miller, Taylor L., and Hannah Sande. 2021. Is word-level recursion actually recursion? Languages 6: 100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miyashita, Mizuki. 2018. Vowel-consonant coalescence in Blackfoot. In Papers of the Forty-Seventh Algonquian Conference. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press, pp. 217–35. [Google Scholar]
- Nespor, Marina, and Irene Vogel. 1982. Prosodic domains of external sandhi rules. In The Structure of Phonological Representations, Part I. Dordrecht: Foris Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Nespor, Marina, and Irene Vogel. 1983. Prosodic structure above the word. In Prosody: Models and Measurements. Berlin: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Nespor, Marina, and Irene Vogel. 2007. Prosodic Phonology: With a new foreword, 2nd ed. Berlin: Walter de Gruyters. [Google Scholar]
- Newell, Heather. 2008. Aspects of the Morphology and Phonology of Phases. Ph.D. thesis, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Oxford, Will R. 2014. Microparameters of Agreement: A diachronic Perspective on Algonquian Verb Inflection. Ph.D. thesis, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Peperkamp, Sharon Andrea. 1997. Prosodic Words. Number 34 in HIL Dissertations. The Hague: Holland Academic Graphics. [Google Scholar]
- Peterson, Tyler. 2004. Theoretical issues in the representation of the glottal stop in Blackfoot. In Proceedings from the 7th Workshop on American Indigenous Languages [WAIL 7]. Volume 15 of Santa Barbara Papers in Linguistics. Santa Barbara: Department of Linguistics, University of California Santa Barbara, pp. 106–21. [Google Scholar]
- Pierrehumbert, Janet, and Mary Beckman. 1988. Japanese Tone Structure. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Poser, William J. 1984. The Phonetics and Phonology of Tone and Intonation in Japanese. Ph.D. thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Prince, Alan, and Paul Smolensky. 1993. Optimality Theory: Constraint Interaction in Generative Grammar. Rutgers Technical Reports TR-2. Piscataway: Rutgers University Center for Cognitive Science. [Google Scholar]
- Quinn, Conor. 2006. Referential Access Dependency in Penobscot. Ph.D. dissertation, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Reis, Silvaa, and Maria Amélia. 2008. Laryngeal Specification in Blackfoot Obstruents. Qualifying paper, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Ritter, Elizabeth, and Sara Thomas Rosen. 2010. Animacy in Blackfoot: Implications for event structure and clause structure. In Syntax, Lexical Semantics and Event Structure. Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 124–52. [Google Scholar]
- Ritter, Elizabeth, and Martina Wiltschko. 2014. The composition of INFL. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 32: 1331–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schiering, René, Balthasar Bickel, and Kristin A. Hildebrandt. 2010. The prosodic word is not universal, but emergent. Journal of Linguistics 46: 657–709. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1986. On derived domains in sentence phonology. Phonology Yearbook 3: 371–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 2011. The syntax-phonology interface. In The Handbook of Phonological Theory, 2nd ed. Hoboken: John Wiley & Sons, Ltd., chp. 14. pp. 435–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1974. French liaison and the X notation. Linguistic Inquiry 5: 573–90. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1978. On prosodic structure and its relation to syntactic structures. In Nordic Prosody II: Papers from a Symposium. Trondheim: TAPIR, pp. 111–40. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1980. The role of prosodic categories in English word stress. Linguistic Inquiry 11: 563–605. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1981. On the nature of phonological representation. In The Cognitive Representation of Speech. Amsterdam: North Holland. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1984. Phonology and Syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1995. Sentence prosody. In The Handbook of Phonological Theory. Oxford: Blackwell, chp. 16. pp. 550–69. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 1996. The prosodic structure of function words. In Signal to Syntax: Bootstrapping from Speech to Grammar in Early Acquisition. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 187–213. [Google Scholar]
- Selkirk, Elisabeth. 2009. On clause and intonational phrase in Japanese. Gengo Kenkyu 136: 35–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slavin, Tanya. 2012. The Syntax and Semantics of Stem Composition in Oji-Cree. Ph.D. thesis, University of Toronto, Toronto, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Starke, Michal. 2009. Nanosyntax: A short primer to a new approach to language. Nordlyd 36: 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomson, Gregory E. 1978. The origin of Blackfoot geminate stops and nasals. In Linguistic Studies of Native Canada. Vancouver: University of British Columbia. [Google Scholar]
- Truckenbrodt, Hubert. 1995. Phonological Phrases: Their Relation to Syntax, Focus, and Prominence. Ph.D. thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Truckenbrodt, Hubert. 1999. On the relation between syntactic phrases and phonological phrases. Linguistic Inquiry 30: 219–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tyler, Matthew. 2019. Simplifying Match Word: Evidence from English functional categories. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 4: 15. 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Uriagereka, Juan. 1999. Multiple spell-out. In Working Minimalism. Cambridge: MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- van der Hulst, Harry. 2010. A note on recursion in phonology. In Recursion and Human Language. Number 104 in Studies in Generative Grammar. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 301–41. [Google Scholar]
- Vigário, Marina. 2003. The Prosodic Word in European Portuguese. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. [Google Scholar]
- Vigário, Marina. 2010. Prosodic structure between the prosodic word and the phonological phrase: Recursive nodes or an independent domain? The Linguistic Review 27: 485–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Vogel, Irene. 2009. The status of the Clitic Group. In Phonological Domains: Universals and Deviations. Number 16 in Interface Explorations. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 15–46. [Google Scholar]
- Vogel, Irene. 2019. Life after the Strict Layer Hypothesis. In Prosodic Studies: Challenges and Prospects. Routledge Studies in Chinese Linguistics. Abingdon: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- Wagner, Michael. 2005. Prosody and Recursion. Ph.D. thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Wagner, Michael. 2010. Prosody and recursion in coordinate structures and beyond. Natural Language & Linguistic Theory 28: 183–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Weber, Natalie. 2016. Accent and prosody in Blackfoot verbs. In Papers of the Forty-Fourth Algonquian Conference. Albany: SUNY Press, pp. 348–69. [Google Scholar]
- Weber, Natalie. 2020. Syntax, Prosody, and Metrical Structure in Blackfoot. Ph.D. thesis, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Weber, Natalie. 2021. Phase-based constraints within Match Theory. In Supplemental Proceedings of the 2020 Annual Meeting on Phonology. Washington, DC: Linguistic Society of America. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Weber, Natalie. Forthcoming. Phonological domains in Blackfoot: Structures shared with Algonquian and the misbehavior of preverbs. In Papers of the 52nd Algonquian Conference. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press.
- Weber, Natalie, and Lisa Matthewson. 2017. The semantics of Blackfoot arguments. In Papers of the Forty-Fifth Algonquian Conference. East Lansing: Michigan State University Press, pp. 213–32. [Google Scholar]
- Werle, Adam. 2009. Word, Phrase and Clitic Prosody in Bosnian, Serbian and Croatian. Ph.D. dissertation, University of Massachusetts, Amherst, MA, USA. [Google Scholar]
- Wiltschko, Martina, and Elizabeth Ritter. 2015. Animating the narrow syntax. The Linguistic Review 32: 869–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Windsor, Joseph W. 2017. Predicting prosodic structure by morphosyntactic category: A case study of Blackfoot. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics 2: 10. 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Zec, Draga, and Sharon Inkelas. 1991. The place of clitics in the prosodic hierarchy. In Proceedings of the Tenth West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics. Stanford: CSLI Publications, pp. 505–19. [Google Scholar]
C | Features of the IP | Clause Types |
---|---|---|
-wa ‘3’, -yini ‘3sg.obv’, -yi ‘3pl’ | [+realis] | independent |
-wa ‘3’ | [+realis] | independent (non-assertive) |
-Ø ‘3’, -yini 3sg.obv’, -yi ‘3pl’ | [+realis] | unreal |
-i ‘dep’ | neutral | conjunctive, subjunctive |
-Ø ‘imp’ | [-realis] | imperative |
C | Matrix/Embedded? | Clause Types |
---|---|---|
-wa ‘3’, -yini ‘3sg.obv’, -yi ‘3pl’ | matrix | independent |
-wa ‘3’ | matrix | independent (non-assertive) |
-Ø ‘3’, -yini ‘3sg.obv’, -yi ‘3pl’ | neutral | unreal |
-i ‘dep’ | embedded | conjunctive, subjunctive |
-Ø ‘imp’ | matrix | imperative |
Labial | Coronal | Dorsal | Glottal | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Stops | p pː | t tː | k kː | ʔ |
Assibilants | ˢt ˢtː | ks | ||
Fricatives | s sː | x | ||
Nasals | m mː | n nː | ||
Glides | w | j | (w) |
Front | Central | Back | |
---|---|---|---|
High | i iː | o oː | |
Mid | ɛː | ɔː | |
Low | a aː |
p | k | m | n | j | w | iː | oː | ɛː | ɔː | aː | i | o | a | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Left edge | ||||||||||||||
After prefix |
a. Free Clitic | b. Prosodic Word | c. PWd Adjunct | d. Internal Clitic | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Prefix left edge = PPh? | ✓ | ✘ | ✘ | ✘ |
Prefix right edge = PWd? | ✘ | ✓ | ✘ | ✘ |
Prefix minimal size = PWd? | ✘ | ✓ | ✘ | ✘ |
Stem left edge = PWd? | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✘ |
Stem minimal size = PWd? | ✓ | ✓ | ✓ | ✘ |
Diagnostic | Prefix | PPh |
---|---|---|
Glides prohibited at left edge? | ✘ | ✓ |
Glottal stop prohibited at right edge? | ✘ | ✓ |
Glides contrast at right edge? | ✘ | ✓ |
Minimal size? | V, CV, VC | CVVC, CVCC |
Obligatory stress? | ✘ | ✓ |
Diagnostic | Prefix | PWd |
---|---|---|
Stops prohibited at left edge? | ✓ | ✓ |
Right edge [k] occurs before epenthetic [i]? | ✘ | ✓ |
Minimal size? | V, CV, VC | CVV |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Weber, N. Prosodic Word Recursion in a Polysynthetic Language (Blackfoot; Algonquian). Languages 2022, 7, 159. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030159
Weber N. Prosodic Word Recursion in a Polysynthetic Language (Blackfoot; Algonquian). Languages. 2022; 7(3):159. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030159
Chicago/Turabian StyleWeber, Natalie. 2022. "Prosodic Word Recursion in a Polysynthetic Language (Blackfoot; Algonquian)" Languages 7, no. 3: 159. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030159
APA StyleWeber, N. (2022). Prosodic Word Recursion in a Polysynthetic Language (Blackfoot; Algonquian). Languages, 7(3), 159. https://doi.org/10.3390/languages7030159