When Memory and Metamemory Align: How Processes at Encoding Influence Delayed Judgment-of-Learning Accuracy
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. The Delayed JOL Effect
1.2. Encoding and Delayed JOL Accuracy
1.3. The Current Study
2. Experiment 1
2.1. Materials and Methods
2.1.1. Participants
2.1.2. Materials
2.1.3. Procedure
2.2. Results
2.2.1. Final-Test Performance
2.2.2. JOL Accuracy
2.2.3. Cue Accessibility, Utilization, and Diagnosticity
2.2.4. Cue Accessibility and JOL Accuracy
2.2.5. Mediation Analysis of JOL Accuracy
2.3. Discussion
3. Experiment 2
3.1. Materials and Methods
3.1.1. Participants
3.1.2. Materials
3.1.3. Procedure
3.2. Results
3.2.1. Retention Interval
3.2.2. Final-Test Performance
3.2.3. JOL Accuracy
3.2.4. Cue Accessibility, Utilization, and Diagnosticity
3.2.5. Cue Accessibility and JOL Accuracy
3.2.6. Mediation Analysis of JOL Accuracy
3.3. Discussion
4. Meta-Analysis of Experiments 1 and 2
4.1. JOL Accuracy
4.2. Mediation Analysis of JOL Accuracy
5. General Discussion
5.1. Target Retrieval and JOL Accuracy
5.2. Cued-Recall Final Tests
5.3. Noncriterial Recollection and JOL Accuracy
5.4. Limitations
5.5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Item Type | Cue | Target | Foil 1 | Foil 2 | Foil 3 |
Unrelated | ALLERGY | DIVORCE | BIRDS | BRAIN | DAMAGE |
Unrelated | BACON | MAYOR | LINT | ELEVATOR | FRECKLE |
Unrelated | BALLOON | ACRE | SPOUSE | LIBRARY | TAPE |
Unrelated | BELLY | STOVE | OATS | CELLAR | EXAM |
Unrelated | BEVERAGE | PHYSICS | ARTERY | TEAM | LIQUID |
Unrelated | BOOZE | ICING | EMPLOYER | READER | MEETING |
Unrelated | BREEZE | TOASTER | PUPPY | FEVER | POND |
Unrelated | BUTLER | SEASHORE | PLAQUE | CUSTARD | DUNE |
Unrelated | COTTON | SAILING | SANDALS | TEXT | PECAN |
Unrelated | CRAYON | HALO | CHORE | PEDAL | CARROTS |
Unrelated | DYNASTY | GRIP | SCENE | RAISIN | SLEEVE |
Unrelated | EXIT | BROOM | MULE | PILL | ANTIDOTE |
Unrelated | GHOST | PORCH | SHOE | SKELETON | BEAVER |
Unrelated | LAMB | PARADE | LOBSTER | COMPASS | PAINT |
Unrelated | LUNG | SHADOW | SHIELD | CABOOSE | MUMMY |
Unrelated | NICOTINE | PACKAGE | TURTLE | SOCCER | SPATULA |
Unrelated | PLUM | HELMET | EAGLE | ORCHID | SIGNAL |
Unrelated | POSSUM | GLACIER | FLOOD | CARBON | FLAVOR |
Unrelated | THIEF | SNOW | ACROBAT | PARROT | LAWN |
Unrelated | TROUSERS | CHEF | TRAITOR | PADDLE | PRIZE |
Weakly Related | BREAD | JELLY | ROLL | SANDWICH | WHEAT |
Weakly Related | CAVERN | MOUNTAIN | CABIN | HOLE | TUNNEL |
Weakly Related | CHAPEL | TEMPLE | STEEPLE | PRIEST | CROSS |
Weakly Related | COCOON | WORM | MOTH | NEST | SHELL |
Weakly Related | COFFIN | TOMB | BURIAL | GRAVE | VAMPIRE |
Weakly Related | DAGGER | BLADE | SWORD | BLOOD | MURDER |
Weakly Related | DENTIST | CAVITY | OFFICE | DOCTOR | DRILL |
Weakly Related | DIAMOND | EMERALD | GOLD | PEARL | RUBY |
Weakly Related | DOCK | SHIP | PIER | LAKE | PORT |
Weakly Related | DRESSER | CLOSET | CLOTHES | DESK | TABLE |
Weakly Related | GLOBE | CIRCLE | SPHERE | EARTH | ATLAS |
Weakly Related | HARP | SONG | VIOLIN | PIANO | FLUTE |
Weakly Related | INFERNO | FLAME | HEAT | VOLCANO | BLAZE |
Weakly Related | MARSH | WEED | JUNGLE | LAND | GRASS |
Weakly Related | MUSTACHE | RAZOR | FACE | MOUTH | HAIR |
Weakly Related | OREGANO | HERB | PIZZA | GARLIC | SAUCE |
Weakly Related | REPTILE | FROG | MAMMAL | SCALES | LIZARD |
Weakly Related | SUNRISE | MOON | MORNING | DAWN | BEACH |
Weakly Related | THRONE | CASTLE | SEAT | QUEEN | CROWN |
Weakly Related | TOOL | MACHINE | WRENCH | KITCHEN | SHOVEL |
1 | The gamma statistic with a 2 × 2 contingency table is equivalent to Yule’s Q. With 2 × m contingency tables, gamma is not equivalent to Yule’s Q and is no longer consistent with signal-detection measures. |
References
- Anwyl-Irvine, Alexander L., Jessica Massonnié, Adam Flitton, Natasha Kirkham, and Jo K. Evershed. 2020. Gorilla in our midst: An online behavioral experiment builder. Behavior Research Methods 52: 388–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Balduzzi, Sara, Gerta Rücker, and Guido Schwarzer. 2019. How to perform a meta-analysis with R: A practical tutorial. Evidence-Based Mental Health 22: 153–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Baron, Reuben M., and David A. Kenny. 1986. The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 51: 1173–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barrett, Adam B., Zoltan Dienes, and Anil K. Seth. 2013. Measures of metacognition on signal-detection theoretic models. Psychological Methods 18: 535–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Benjamin, Aaron S., and Michael Diaz. 2008. Measurement of relative metamnemonic accuracy. In Handbook of Metamemory and Memory. Edited by John Dunlosky and Robert A. Bjork. New York: Psychology Press, Chp. xiv. pp. 73–94. 487p. [Google Scholar]
- Boker, Steven, Michael Neale, Hermine Maes, Michael Wilde, Michael Spiegel, Timothy Brick, Jeffrey Spies, Ryne Estabrook, Sarah Kenny, Timothy Bates, and et al. 2011. OpenMx: An Open Source Extended Structural Equation Modeling Framework. Psychometrika 76: 306–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Brewer, Gene A., Richard L. Marsh, Arlo Clark-Foos, and Joseph T. Meeks. 2010. Noncriterial recollection influences metacognitive monitoring and control processes. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 63: 1936–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bröder, Arndt, and Monika Undorf. 2019. Metamemory viewed through the judgment lens. Acta Psychologica 197: 153–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bulevich, John B., Ayanna K. Thomas, and Charles Parsow. 2016. Filling in the gaps: Using testing and restudy to promote associative learning. Memory 24: 1267–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carroll, Marie, and Thomas O. Nelson. 1993. Effect of overlearning on the feeling of knowing is more detectable in within-subject than in between-subject designs. The American Journal of Psychology 106: 227–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Castel, Alan D., David P. McCabe, and Henry L. Roediger III. 2007. Illusions of competence and overestimation of associative memory for identical items: Evidence from judgments of learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 14: 107–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheung, Mike. W.-L. 2015. metaSEM: An R Package for Meta-Analysis using Structural Equation Modeling. Frontiers in Psychology 5: 1521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cheung, Mike W.-L., and Wai Chan. 2005. Meta-analytic structural equation modeling: A two-stage approach. Psychological Methods 10: 40–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cosentino, Stephanie, Carolyn Zhu, Elodie Bertrand, Janet Metcalfe, Sarah Janicki, and Sarah Cines. 2016. Examination of the metacognitive errors that contribute to anosognosia in alzheimer’s disease. Cortex: A Journal Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and Behavior 84: 101–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dickersin, Kay, and Jesse A. Berlin. 1992. Meta-analysis: State-of-the-science. Epidemiologic Reviews 14: 154–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunlosky, John, and Christopher Hertzog. 1998. Training programs to improve learning in later adulthood: Helping older adults educate themselves. In Metacognition in Educational Theory and Practice. Edited by Douglas J. Hacker, John Dunlosky and Arthur C. Graesser. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc., pp. 249–76. [Google Scholar]
- Dunlosky, John, Christopher Hertzog, and Amy Powell-Moman. 2005. The Contribution of Mediator-Based Deficiencies to Age Differences in Associative Learning. Developmental Psychology 41: 389–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunlosky, John, Michael L. Mueller, and Keith W. Thiede. 2016. Methodology for investigating human metamemory: Problems and pitfalls. In The Oxford Handbook of Metamemory. Edited by John Dunlosky and Sarah K. Tauber. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 23–37. [Google Scholar]
- Eakin, Deborah K., and Christopher Hertzog. 2012. Age invariance in feeling of knowing during implicit interference effects. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences 67: 555–62. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Finn, Brigid, and Janet Metcalfe. 2008. Judgments of learning are influenced by memory for past test. Journal of Memory and Language 58: 19–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Geraci, Lisa, David P. McCabe, and Jimmeka J. Guillory. 2009. On interpreting the relationship between remember–know judgments and confidence: The role of instructions. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal 18: 701–09. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grainger, Catherine, David M. Williams, and Sophie E. Lind. 2016. Judgment of learning accuracy in high-functioning adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders 46: 3570–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gonzalez, Richard, and Thomas O. Nelson. 1996. Measuring ordinal association in situations that contain tied scores. Psychological Bulletin 119: 159–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goh, Jin, Judith A. Hall, and Robert Rosenthal. 2016. Mini Meta-Analysis of Your Own Studies: Some Arguments on Why and a Primer on How. Social and Personality Psychology Compass 10: 535–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hanczakowski, Maciej, Tomasz Pasek, Katarzyna Zawadzka, and Giuliana Mazzoni. 2013. Cue familiarity and ‘don’t know’ responding in episodic memory tasks. Journal of Memory and Language 69: 368–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hardy, Rebecca J., and Simon G. Thompson. 1998. Detecting and describing heterogeneity in meta-analysis. Statistics in Medicine 17: 841–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayes, Andrew F. 2013. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach. New York: The Guilford Press. [Google Scholar]
- Hedges, Larry V. 1981. Distribution Theory for Glass’s Estimator of Effect size and Related Estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics 6: 107–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hébert, Sylvie, and Isabelle Peretz. 1997. Recognition of music in long-term memory: Are melodic and temporal patterns equal partners? Memory & Cognition 25: 518–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hertzog, Christopher, Erika K. Fulton, Starlette M. Sinclair, and John Dunlosky. 2014. Recalled aspects of original encoding strategies influence episodic feelings of knowing. Memory & Cognition 42: 126–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hertzog, Christopher, Jerrod C. Hines, and Dayna R. Touron. 2013. Judgments of learning are influenced by multiple cues in addition to memory for past test accuracy. Archives of Scientific Psychology 1: 23–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hertzog, Christopher, John Dunlosky, and Starlette M. Sinclair. 2010. Episodic feeling-of-knowing resolution derives from the quality of original encoding. Memory & cognition 38: 771–784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Higgins, Julian P. T., James Thomas, Jacqueline Chandler, Miranda Cumpston, Tianjing Li, Matthew J. Page, and Vivian A. Welch, eds. 2019. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions. Chichister: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Higham, Phillip A., Timothy J. Perfect, and Davide Bruno. 2009. Investigating strength and frequency effects in recognition memory using type-2 signal detection theory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 35: 57–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hughes, Gregory I., Holly A. Taylor, and Ayanna K. Thomas. 2018. Study techniques differentially influence the delayed judgment-of-learning accuracy of adolescent children and college-aged adults. Metacognition and Learning 13: 109–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Isingrini, Michel, Mathilde Sacher, Audrey Perrotin, Laurence Taconnat, Céline Souchay, Hélène Stoehr, and Badiâa Bouazzaoui. 2016. Episodic feeling-of-knowing relies on noncriterial recollection and familiarity: Evidence using an online remember-know procedure. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal 41: 31–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jameson, Kimberly A., Louis Narens, Keith Goldfarb, and Thomas O. Nelson. 1990. The influence of near-threshold priming on metamemory and recall. Acta Psychologica 73: 55–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kane, Janet H., and Richard C. Anderson. 1978. Depth of processing and interference effects in the learning and remembering of sentences. Journal of Educational Psychology 70: 626–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kanters, Steve. 2022. Fixed- and Random-Effects Models. Methods in Molecular Biology 2345: 41–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koriat, Asher. 1993. How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. Psychological Review 100: 609–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koriat, Asher. 1997. Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: A cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 126: 349–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koriat, Asher, and Ravit Levy-Sadot. 2001. The combined contributions of the cue-familiarity and accessibility heuristics to feelings of knowing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27: 34–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krinsky, Richard, and Thomas O. Nelson. 1985. The feeling of knowing for different types of retrieval failure. Acta Psychologica 58: 141–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leonesio, R. Jacob, and Thomas O. Nelson. 1990. Do different metamemory judgments tap the same underlying aspects of memory? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 16: 464–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lupker, Stephen J., Joanne L. Harbluk, and Andrew S. Patrick. 1991. Memory for things forgotten. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 17: 897–907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mazzoni, Giuliana, and Thomas O. Nelson. 1995. Judgments of learning are affected by the kind of encoding in ways that cannot be attributed to the level of recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 21: 1263–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McCabe, David P., and Lisa D. Geraci. 2009. The influence of instructions and terminology on the accuracy of remember-know judgments. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal 18: 401–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McCabe, David P., and Nicholas C. Soderstrom. 2011. Recollection-based prospective metamemory judgments are more accurate than those based on confidence: Judgments of remembering and knowing (JORKS). Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 140: 605–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Metcalfe, Janet. 2002. Is study time allocated selectively to a region of proximal learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 131: 349–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Metcalfe, Janet, Bennett L. Schwartz, and Scott G. Joaquim. 1993. The cue-familiarity heuristic in metacognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 19: 851–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mueller, Michael L., John Dunlosky, and Sarah K. Tauber. 2016. The effect of identical word pairs on people’s metamemory judgments: What are the contributions of processing fluency and beliefs about memory? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 69: 781–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Myers, Sarah J., Matthew G. Rhodes, and Hannah E. Hausman. 2020. Judgments of learning (JOLs) selectively improve memory depending on the type of test. Memory & Cognition 48: 745–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, Douglas L., Cathy L. McEvoy, and Thomas A. Schreiber. 1998. The University of South Florida Word Association, Rhyme, and Word Fragment Norms. Available online: http://www.usf.edu/FreeAssociation/ (accessed on 23 September 2022).
- Nelson, Thomas O. 1984. A comparison of current measures of the accuracy of feeling-of-knowing predictions. Psychological Bulletin 95: 109–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, Thomas O. 1996. Gamma is a measure of the accuracy of predicting performance on one item relative to another item, not of the absolute performance on an individual item. Applied Cognitive Psychology 10: 257–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, Thomas O., and John Dunlosky. 1991. When people’s judgments of learning (JOLs) are extremely accurate at predicting subsequent recall: The “delayed-JOL effect”. Psychological Science 2: 267–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, Thomas O., and Loius Narens. 1990. Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation 26: 125–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nelson, Thomas O., Louis Narens, and John Dunlosky. 2004. A revised methodology for research on metamemory: Pre-judgment recall and monitoring (PRAM). Psychological Methods 9: 53–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Paivio, Allan, James M. Clark, and Mustaq Khan. 1988. Effects of concreteness and semantic relatedness on composite imagery ratings and cued recall. Memory & Cognition 16: 422–30. [Google Scholar]
- Parks, Colleen M. 2007. The role of noncriterial recollection in estimating recollection and familiarity. Journal of Memory and Language 57: 81–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pyc, Mary A., and Katherine Rawson. 2009. Testing the retrieval effort hypothesis: Does greater difficulty correctly recalling information lead to higher levels of memory? Journal of Memory and Language 60: 437–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. 2022. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 23 September 2022).
- Reder, Lynne M., and Frank E. Ritter. 1992. What determines initial feeling of knowing? Familiarity with question terms, not with the answer. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 18: 435–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rhodes, Matthew G. 2016. Judgments of learning: Methods, data, and theory. In The Oxford Handbook of Metamemory. Edited by John Dunlosky and Sarah K. Tauber. New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 65–80. [Google Scholar]
- Rhodes, Matthew G., and Alan D. Castel. 2008. Memory predictions are influenced by perceptual information: Evidence for metacognitive illusions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 137: 615–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rhodes, Matthew G., and Sarah K. Tauber. 2011a. The influence of delaying judgments of learning on metacognitive accuracy: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 137: 131–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rhodes, Matthew G., and Sarah K. Tauber. 2011b. The influence of the veracity of retrieved information on the accuracy of judgements of learning. Memory 19: 853–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schwartz, Bennett L., and Janet Metcalfe. 1994. Methodological problems and pitfalls in the study of human metacognition. In Metacognition: Knowing about Knowing. Edited by Janet Metcalfe and Arthur P. Shimamura. Cambridge: The MIT Press, Chp. xiii. pp. 93–113. 334p. [Google Scholar]
- Schneider, Walter, Amy Eschman, and Anthony Zuccolotto. 2002. E-Prime: User’s Guide. Pittsburgh: Psychology Software Tools. [Google Scholar]
- Schoemann, Alexander M., Aaron J. Boulton, and Stephen D. Short. 2017. Determining power and sample size for simple and complex mediation models. Social Psychological and Personality Science 8: 379–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spellman, Barbara A., Aaron Bloomfield, and Robert A. Bjork. 2008. Measuring memory and metamemory: Theoretical and statistical problems with assessing learning (in general) and using gamma (in particular) to do so. In Handbook of Metamemory and Memory. Edited by John Dunlosky and Robert A. Bjork. New York: Psychology Press, Chp. xiv. pp. 95–114. 487p. [Google Scholar]
- Spellman, Barbara A., and Robert A. Bjork. 1992. When predictions create reality: Judgments of learning may alter what they are intended to assess. Psychological Science 3: 315–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snodgrass, Joan G., and June Corwin. 1988. Pragmatics of measuring recognition memory: Applications to dementia and amnesia. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 117: 34–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Son, Lisa K., and Janet Metcalfe. 2005. Judgments of learning: Evidence for a two-stage process. Memory & Cognition 33: 1116–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Souchay, Céline, Chris J. A. Moulin, David Clarys, Laurence Taconnat, and Michel Isingrini. 2007. Diminished episodic memory awareness in older adults: Evidence from feeling-of-knowing and recollection. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal 16: 769–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Souchay, Céline, Michel Isingrini, and Laurence Espagnet. 2000. Aging, episodic memory feeling-of-knowing, and frontal functioning. Neuropsychology 14: 299–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swets, John A. 1986. Indices of discrimination or diagnostic accuracy: Their ROCs and implied models. Psychological Bulletin 99: 100–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thiede, Keith W., and John Dunlosky. 1994. Delaying students’ metacognitive monitoring improves their accuracy in predicting their recognition performance. Journal of Educational Psychology 86: 290–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomas, Ayanna K., John B. Bulevich, and Stacey Dubois. 2011. Context affects feeling-of-knowing accuracy in younger and older adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 37: 96–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomas, Ayanna K., John Bulevich, and Stacey J. Dubois. 2012. An analysis of the determinants of the feeling of knowing. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal 21: 1681–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomas, Ayanna K., Meeyeon Lee, and David A. Balota. 2013. Metacognitive monitoring and dementia: How intrinsic and extrinsic cues influence judgments of learning in people with early-stage Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychology 27: 452–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tulving, Endel. 1985. Memory and consciousness. Canadian Psychology/Psychologie Canadienne 26: 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Viswesvaran, Chockalingham, and Deniz S. Ones. 1995. Theory testing: Combining psychometric meta-analysis and structural equations. Personnel Psychology 48: 865–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yonelinas, Andrew P., Mariam Aly, Wei-Chung Wang, and Joshua D. Koen. 2010. Recollection and familiarity: Examining controversial assumptions and new directions. Hippocampus 20: 1178–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Item Type | Variable | Study-Technique Group | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
SP | EE | RP | ||
Unrelated | Remember | .40 (.27) | .45 (.23) | .40 (.21) |
Know | .35 (.19) | .31 (.17) | .44 (.18) | |
No Memory | .25 (.22) | .23 (.20) | .15 (.14) | |
Related | Remember | .47 (.27) | .66 (.17) | .65 (.19) |
Know | .32 (.24) | .21 (.17) | .29 (.12) | |
No Memory | .21 (.20) | .13 (.16) | .06 (.08) |
Item Type | Variable | Study-Technique Group | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
SP | EE | RP | ||
Unrelated | Targets | .17 (.24) | .22 (.17) | .22 (.19) |
Noncriterial Cues | .27 (.24) | .33 (.21) | .24 (.18) | |
JOL | 46.58 (22.80) | 53.57 (19.10) | 49.31 (19.24) | |
Final Test | .69 (.23) | .86 (.17) | .83 (.16) | |
Related | Targets | .28 (.26) | .44 (.19) | .55 (.24) |
Noncriterial Cues | .29 (.22) | .49 (.29) | .30 (.24) | |
JOL | 50.45 (23.47) | 65.72 (16.10) | 66.10 (17.51) | |
Final Test | .70 (.19) | .83 (.13) | .86 (.15) |
Item Type | Variable | Study-Technique Group | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
SP | EE | RP | ||
Unrelated | JOL Acc. | .31 (.35) | .35 (.41) | .28 (.41) |
JOL Acc. (NG) | .32 (.42) | .38 (.38) | .42 (.34) | |
JOL Acc. (NG, L) | .37 (.41) | .43 (.35) | .47 (.31) | |
Related | JOL Acc. | .31 (.27) | .45 (.32) | .58 (.30) |
JOL Acc. (NG) | .35 (.25) | .39 (.35) | .57 (.26) | |
JOL Acc. (NG, L) | .40 (.27) | .49 (.28) | .60 (.26) |
Item Type | Variable | Study-Technique Group | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
SP | EE | RP | ||
Unrelated | Target Utilization | .99 (.02) | .95 (.09) | .99 (.04) |
Noncrit. Utilization | .91 (.20) | .82 (.36) | .86 (.20) | |
Target Diagnosticity | .22 (.34) | .07 (.40) | 18 (.36) | |
Noncrit. Diagnosticity | .06 (.42) | −.07 (.50) | −.11 (.50) | |
Related | Target Utilization | .81 (.23) | .77 (.19) | .92 (.13) |
Noncrit. Utilization | .86 (.19) | .81 (.41) | .83 (.20) | |
Target Diagnosticity | .58 (.26) | .49 (.36) | .58 (.29) | |
Noncrit. Diagnosticity | .02 (.49) | .08 (.48) | −.17 (.52) |
Predictor | Outcome Variable | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mediator 1: Target Accessibility | Mediator 2: Noncriterial Cue Accessibility | JOL Accuracy (Related) | |||||||
Coeff | SE | p | Coeff | SE | p | Coeff | SE | p | |
Intercept | −.23 | .04 | <.001 | .01 | .04 | .899 | .45 | .07 | <.001 |
Retrieval Practice | .29 | .05 | <.001 | .03 | .06 | .684 | .10 | .09 | .295 |
Target Accessibility | - | - | - | - | - | - | .58 | .19 | .004 |
Noncriterial Cue Accessibility | - | - | - | - | - | - | −.02 | .17 | .917 |
R2 = .36 F(1, 52) = 29.16, p < .001 | R2 < .01 F(1, 52) = 0.17, p = .684 | R2 = .32 F(3, 50) = 7.65, p = .0003 |
Item Type | Variable | Study-Technique Group | |
---|---|---|---|
Study Practice | Retrieval Practice | ||
Unrelated | Targets | .07 (.13) | .14 (.21) |
Noncriterial Cues | .13 (.20) | .12 (.20) | |
JOL | 32.46 (21.38) | 35.71 (21.42) | |
Final Test | .65 (.24) | .74 (.21) | |
Related | Targets | .32 (.26) | .56 (.26) |
Noncriterial Cues | .19 (.21) | .27 (.28) | |
JOL | 47.77 (23.82) | 63.16 (21.38) | |
Final Test | .73 (.21) | .88 (.17) |
Item Type | Variable | Study-Technique Group | |
---|---|---|---|
Study Practice | Retrieval Practice | ||
Unrelated | JOL Acc. | .26 (.52) | .09 (.51) |
JOL Acc. (NG) | .25 (.53) | .06 (.44) | |
JOL Acc. (NG, L) | .22 (.53) | .12 (.53) | |
Related | JOL Acc. | .44 (.33) | .62 (.37) |
JOL Acc. (NG) | .49 (.35) | .64 (.36) | |
JOL Acc. (NG, L) | .57 (.30) | .65 (.30) |
Item Type | Variable | Study Practice | Retrieval Practice |
---|---|---|---|
Unrelated | Target Utilization | .95 (.15) | .97 (.10) |
Noncrit. Utilization | .79 (.31) | .71 (.48) | |
Target Diagnosticity | .04 (.44) | .00 (.38) | |
Noncrit. Diagnosticity | −.04 (.48) | −.33 (.42) | |
Related | Target Utilization | .83 (.23) | .85 (.32) |
Noncrit. Utilization | .89 (.34) | .78 (.39) | |
Target Diagnosticity | .48 (.38) | .52 (.36) | |
Noncrit. Diagnosticity | −.05 (.47) | −.22 (.42) |
Predictor | Outcome Variable | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Mediator 1: Target Accessibility | Mediator 2: Noncriterial Cue Accessibility | JOL Accuracy (Related) | |||||||
Coeff | SE | p | Coeff | SE | p | Coeff | SE | p | |
Intercept | −.16 | .04 | <.001 | −.03 | .04 | .476 | .48 | .06 | <.001 |
Retrieval Practice | .18 | .06 | .004 | .10 | .06 | .107 | .14 | .09 | .146 |
Target Accessibility | - | - | - | - | - | - | .28 | .19 | .156 |
Noncriterial Cue Accessibility | - | - | - | - | - | - | −.04 | .18 | .825 |
R2 = .123 F(1, 63) = 8.82, p = .004 | R2 = .041 F(1, 63) = 2.67, p = .107 | R2 = .096 F(3, 61) = 2.16, p = .102 |
Variable | Retrieval Practice | Target Retrieval | Noncriterial Cues |
---|---|---|---|
Retrieval Practice | – | ||
Target Retrieval | .46 *** | – | |
Noncriterial Cues | .09 | .12 | – |
JOL Acc (REL) | .34 *** | .40 *** | .08 |
Path | Estimate | LL 95% CI | UL 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|
1. RP → Target Accessibility | .46 | .32 | .60 |
2. Target Accessibility → JOL Accuracy | .30 | .12 | .49 |
3. Indirect Effect: RP → TA → JOL Acc. | .14 | .06 | .26 |
4. RP → NCR Accessibility | .09 | −.09 | .27 |
5. NCR → JOL Accuracy | .03 | −.14 | .19 |
6. Indirect Effect: RP → NCR → JOL Acc. | .002 | −.02 | .03 |
7. RP → JOLacc (Direct Effect) | .20 | .01 | .39 |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Hughes, G.I.; Thomas, A.K. When Memory and Metamemory Align: How Processes at Encoding Influence Delayed Judgment-of-Learning Accuracy. J. Intell. 2022, 10, 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040101
Hughes GI, Thomas AK. When Memory and Metamemory Align: How Processes at Encoding Influence Delayed Judgment-of-Learning Accuracy. Journal of Intelligence. 2022; 10(4):101. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040101
Chicago/Turabian StyleHughes, Gregory Isaac, and Ayanna Kim Thomas. 2022. "When Memory and Metamemory Align: How Processes at Encoding Influence Delayed Judgment-of-Learning Accuracy" Journal of Intelligence 10, no. 4: 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040101
APA StyleHughes, G. I., & Thomas, A. K. (2022). When Memory and Metamemory Align: How Processes at Encoding Influence Delayed Judgment-of-Learning Accuracy. Journal of Intelligence, 10(4), 101. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040101