Do Domain Knowledge and Retrieval Practice Predict Students’ Study Order Decisions?
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants
2.2. Geology Course Description
2.3. Materials
2.4. Procedure
2.4.1. Phase 1
2.4.2. Phase 2
3. Results
3.1. Changes in Prior Knowledge
3.1.1. Students’ Subjective Reports of Prior Knowledge
3.1.2. Objective Assessments of Students’ Prior Knowledge
3.2. Students’ Study Order Decisions
3.3. Practice Classification Performance during the Self-Regulated Learning Phases
3.4. Performance on the Studied Exemplar Test
3.5. Performance on the Novel Exemplar Test
3.6. Follow-Up Prompts and Questions Regarding Students’ Rationale for Their Study Decisions
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Abel, Roman, Matthias Brunmair, and Sophia Christin Weissgerber. 2021. Change one category at a time: Sequence effects beyond interleaving and blocking. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 47: 1083–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Agarwal, Pooja K., Jeffrey D. Karpicke, Sean H. Kang, Henry L. Roediger III, and Kathleen. B. McDermott. 2008. Examining the testing effect with open-and closed-book tests. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 22: 861–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ariel, Robert, Jeffery D. Karpicke, Amber E. Witherby, and Sarah K. Tauber. 2021. Do judgments of learning directly enhance learning of educational materials? Educational Psychology Review 33: 693–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Babineau, Addison L., and Sarah K. Tauber. Forthcoming. Students’ decisions to switch between categories or stay within them are related to practice classification performance. Memory & Cognition.
- Braithwaite, David W., and Robert L. Goldstone. 2012. Inducing mathematical concepts from specific examples: The role of schema-level variation. Paper presented at the Thirty-Fourth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Sapporo, Japan, August 1–4; Sapporo: Cognitive Science Society, pp. 138–43. [Google Scholar]
- Braithwaite, David W., and Robert L. Goldstone. 2015. Effects of variation and prior knowledge on abstract concept learning. Cognition and Instruction 33: 226–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brunmair, Matthias, and Tobias Richter. 2019. Similarity matters: A meta-analysis of interleaved learning and its moderators. Psychological Bulletin 145: 1029–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Carpenter, Shana K., Harold Pashler, and Nicholas J. Cepeda. 2009. Using tests to enhance 8th grade students’ retention of U.S. history facts. Applied Cognitive Psychology 23: 760–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carpenter, Shana K., Steven C. Pan, and Andrew C. Butler. 2022. The science of effective learning with spacing and retrieval practice. Nature Reviews Psychology 1: 496–511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Carvalho, Paulo F., and Robert L. Goldstone. 2014. Putting category learning in order: Category structure and temporal arrangement affect the benefit of interleaved over blocked study. Memory & Cognition 42: 481–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Darabi, Aubteen A., David W. Nelson, and Srinivas Palanki. 2007. Acquisition of troubleshooting skills in a computer simulation: Worked example vs. conventional problem solving instructional strategies. Computers in Human Behavior 23: 1809–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dunlosky, John, and Katherine A. Rawson. 2015. Do students use testing and feedback while learning? A focus on key concept definitions and learning to criterion. Learning and Instruction 39: 32–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eglington, Luke G., and Sean H. Kang. 2017. Interleaved presentation benefits science category learning. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 6: 475–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Elio, Renée, and John R. Anderson. 1981. The effects of category generalizations and instance similarity on schema abstraction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 7: 397–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Firth, Jonathan, Ian Rivers, and James Boyle. 2021. A systematic review of interleaving as a concept learning strategy. Review of Education 9: 642–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foss, Donald J., and Joseph W. Pirozzolo. 2017. Four semesters investigating frequency of testing, the testing effect, and transfer of training. Journal of Educational Psychology 109: 1067–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Große, Cornelia S., and Alexander Renkl. 2007. Finding and fixing errors in worked examples: Can this foster learning outcomes? Learning and Instruction 17: 612–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guo, Jian-Peng, and Ming F. Pang. 2011. Learning a mathematical concept from comparing examples: The importance of variation and prior knowledge. European Journal of Psychology of Education 26: 495–525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guo, Jian-Peng, Ling-Yan Yang, and Yi Ding. 2014. Effects of example variability and prior knowledge in how students learn to solve equations. European Journal of Psychology of Education 29: 21–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacoby, Larry L., Christopher N. Wahlheim, and Jennifer H. Coane. 2010. Test-enhanced learning of natural concepts: Effects on recognition memory, classification, and metacognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 36: 1441–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kang, Sean H., and Harold Pashler. 2012. Learning painting styles: Spacing is advantageous when it promotes discriminative contrast. Applied Cognitive Psychology 26: 97–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kang, Sean H., Mark A. McDaniel, and Harold Pashler. 2011. Effects of testing on learning of functions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 18: 998–1005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kang, Sean H., Tamar H. Gollan, and Harold Pashler. 2013. Don’t just repeat after me: Retrieval practice is better than imitation for foreign vocabulary learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20: 1259–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Karpicke, Jeffery D. 2009. Metacognitive control and strategy selection: Deciding to practice retrieval during learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 138: 469–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Kornell, Nate, and Lisa K. Son. 2009. Learners’ choices and beliefs about self-testing. Memory 17: 493–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kornell, Nate, and Robert A. Bjork. 2008. Learning concepts and categories: Is spacing the “enemy of induction”? Psychological Science 19: 585–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Little, Jeri L., and Mark A. McDaniel. 2014. Individual differences in category learning: Memorization versus rule abstraction. Memory & Cognition 43: 283–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lu, Xinyi, Trevor B. Penney, and Sean H. Kang. 2020. Category similarity affects study choices in self-regulated learning. Memory & Cognition 49: 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McCabe, Jennifer. 2012. Metacognitive awareness of learning strategies in undergraduates. Memory & Cognition 39: 462–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Metcalfe, Janet, Nate Kornell, and Lisa K. Son. 2007. A cognitive-science based programme to enhance study efficacy in a high and low risk setting. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 19: 743–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Middlebrooks, Catherine D., and Alan D. Castel. 2018. Self-regulated learning of important information under sequential and simultaneous encoding conditions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 44: 779–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Middlebrooks, Catherine D., Kou Murayama, and Alan D. Castel. 2016. The value in rushing: Memory and selectivity when short on time. Acta Psychologica 170: 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morehead, Kayla, Matthew G. Rhodes, and Sarah DeLozier. 2016. Instructor and student knowledge of study strategies. Memory 24: 257–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nosofsky, Robert M., Craig A. Sanders, Brian J. Meagher, and Bruce J. Douglas. 2018. Toward the development of a feature-space representation for a complex natural category domain. Behavior Research Methods 50: 530–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Osana, Helena P., Emmanuelle Adrien, and Nathalie Duponsel. 2017. Effects of instructional guidance and sequencing of manipulatives and written symbols on second graders’ numeration knowledge. Education Sciences 7: 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Pan, Steven C., Arpita Gopal, and Timothy C. Rickard. 2016. Testing with feedback yields potent, but piecewise, learning of history and biology facts. Journal of Educational Psychology 108: 563–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pan, Steven C., Sarah Hutter, Dominic D’Andrea, Daanish Unwalla, and Timothy C. Rickard. 2018. In search of transfer following cued recall practice: The case of biology concepts. Applied Cognitive Psychology 33: 629–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quilici, Jill L., and Richard E. Mayer. 1996. Role of examples in how students learn to categorize statistics word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology 88: 144–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rau, Martina A., Vincent Aleven, and Nikol Rummel. 2010. Blocked versus Interleaved Practice with Multiple Representations in an Intelligent Tutoring System for Fractions. In Intelligent Tutoring Systems. ITS 2010. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. Edited by V. Aleven, J. Kay and J. Mostow. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer, vol. 6094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rittle-Johnson, Bethany, Jon R. Star, and Kelley Durkin. 2009. The importance of prior knowledge when comparing examples: Influences on conceptual and procedural knowledge of equation solving. Journal of Educational Psychology 101: 836–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roediger, Henry L., III, and Jeffery D. Karpicke. 2006. Test-enhanced learning: Taking memory tests improves long-term retention. Psychological Science 17: 249–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rohrer, Doug, and Kelli Taylor. 2007. The shuffling of mathematics problems improves learning. Instructional Science 35: 481–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Samani, Joshua, and Steven C. Pan. 2021. Interleaved practice enhances memory and problem-solving ability in undergraduate physics. NPJ Science of Learning 6: 32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shah, Rupal, Matthew Sibbald, Nasir Jaffer, Linda Probyn, and Rodrigo B. Cavalcanti. 2016. Online self-study of chest X-rays shows no difference between blocked and mixed practice. Medical Education 50: 540–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tauber, Sarah K., John Dunlosky, Katherine A. Rawson, Christopher N. Wahlheim, and Larry L. Jacoby. 2013. Self-regulated learning of a natural category: Do people interleave or block exemplars during study? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 20: 356–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Tullis, Jonathan G., Jason R. Finley, and Aaron S. Benjamin. 2013. Metacognition of the testing effect: Guiding learners to predict the benefits of retrieval. Memory & Cognition 41: 429–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Witherby, Amber E., and Shana K. Carpenter. 2022. The rich-get-richer effect: Prior knowledge predicts new learning of domain-relevant information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 48: 483–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yan, Veronica X., and Faria Sana. 2021. The Robustness of the interleaving benefit. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 10: 589–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yan, Veronica X., Brendan A. Schuetze, and Luke G. Eglington. 2020. A review of the interleaving effect: Theories and lessons for future research. PsyArXiv. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yan, Veronica X., Elizabeth L. Bjork, and Robert A. Bjork. 2016. On the difficulty of mending metacognitive illusions: A priori theories, fluency effects, and misattributions of the interleaving benefit. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 145: 918–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Yan, Veronica X., Nicholas C. Soderstrom, Gayan S. Seneviratna, Elizabeth L. Bjork, and Robert A. Bjork. 2017. How should exemplars be sequenced in inductive learning? Empirical evidence versus learners’ opinions. Journal of Experimental Psychology Applied 23: 403–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zulkiply, Norehan, John McLean, Jennifer S. Burt, and Debra Bath. 2012. Spacing and induction: Application to exemplars presented as auditory and visual text. Learning and Instruction 22: 215–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Phase 1 Questions | Study Group | Retrieval Practice Group | |
---|---|---|---|
1. | Are you currently or have you ever been a Geology Major or Minor? | n = 1 | n = 1 |
2. | Are you currently or have you ever been an Applied Geoscience Major? | 0 | 0 |
3. | Do you collect rocks? | n = 3 | n = 3 |
4. | Do you own a rock and mineral field guide? | 0 | 0 |
Phase 2 Questions | |||
1. | Do you collect rocks? | n = 2 | n = 4 |
2. | Do you own a rock and mineral field guide? | n = 1 | n = 1 |
Course Assignment | Study Group | Retrieval Practice Group | |
---|---|---|---|
Between Phase 1 and Phase 2 | |||
1. | Igneous and Metamorphic Rocks Quiz | .94 (.04) | .99 (.01) |
2. | Igneous and Metamorphic Rocks Laboratory Assignment | .93 (.01) | .96 (.01) * |
3. | Sedimentary Rocks Quiz | .91 (.04) | .98 (.01) |
4. | Sedimentary Rocks Laboratory Assignment | .93 (.01) | .93 (.01) |
5. | Mid-term Exam | .80 (.02) | .83 (.02) |
After Phase 2 | |||
6. | Final Exam | .71 (.02) | .74 (.02) |
Overall | Study Group | Retrieval Practice Group | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Switch study decisions | ||||
1. | I wanted to compare the features of the previous rock type with the feature of a different rock type. | 63.64% | 66.67% | 60.61% |
2. | I thought it was best to see examples from different rock types back-to-back. | 51.52% | 48.49% | 54.55% |
3. | I felt like I knew the rock type well enough, and I wanted to move on. | 50.00% | 39.39% | 60.61% |
4. | I had seen several of the same rock type in a row and I was tired of looking at that rock type. | 31.82% | 33.33% | 30.30% |
5. | I thought the previous rock type was too challenging and I wanted to practice something else. | 9.10% | 15.15% | 3.03% |
6. | I never selected to study a different type of rock when I correctly answered a practice trial. | 0% | 0% | 0% |
Stay study decisions | ||||
7. | I wanted to see which features the rock type had in common by seeing multiple examples from the category. | 75.76% | 69.70% | 81.82% |
8. | I thought it was best to see several of the same rock type in a row. | 68.18% | 63.63% | 72.73% |
9. | I didn’t think I knew the rock type well enough and wanted to practice it again. | 57.58% | 48.49% | 66.67% |
10. | I wanted to verify that I really knew the rock type before I moved on. | 50.00% | 54.55% | 45.45% |
11. | I wanted a reminder of the correct rock type that I just studied. | 37.88% | 42.42% | 33.33% |
12. | I had not seen this rock type before, and I wanted to see more examples. | 19.70% | 21.21% | 18.18% |
13. | I never selected to study the same type of rock when I incorrectly answered a practice trial. | 3.03% | 6.06% | 0% |
Overall | Study Group | Retrieval Practice Group | |
---|---|---|---|
Beliefs about the study choices of a novice | |||
Always select to study the same type of rock. | 17.31% | 17.86% | 16.67% |
Often select to study the same type of rock but also select to study a different type of rock a few times. | 36.54% | 28.57% | 45.83% |
Select to study the same type of rock half of the time and a different type of rock for the other half of the time. | 30.77% | 28.57% | 33.33% |
Often select to study a different type of rock but also select to study the same type of rock a few times. | 9.62% | 17.86% | 0% * |
Always select to study a different type of rock. | 5.77% | 7.14% | 4.17% |
Beliefs about the study choices of an expert | |||
Always select to study the same type of rock. | 3.85% | 7.14% | 0% |
Often select to study the same type of rock but also select to study a different type of rock a few times. | 11.54% | 10.71% | 12.50% |
Select to study the same type of rock half of the time and a different type of rock for the other half of the time. | 7.69% | 14.29% | 0% |
Often select to study a different type of rock but also select to study the same type of rock a few times. | 32.69% | 35.71% | 29.17% |
Always select to study a different type of rock. | 44.23% | 32.14% | 58.33% |
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations. |
© 2022 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Babineau, A.L.; Witherby, A.E.; Ariel, R.; Pelch, M.A.; Tauber, S.K. Do Domain Knowledge and Retrieval Practice Predict Students’ Study Order Decisions? J. Intell. 2022, 10, 122. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040122
Babineau AL, Witherby AE, Ariel R, Pelch MA, Tauber SK. Do Domain Knowledge and Retrieval Practice Predict Students’ Study Order Decisions? Journal of Intelligence. 2022; 10(4):122. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040122
Chicago/Turabian StyleBabineau, Addison L., Amber E. Witherby, Robert Ariel, Michael A. Pelch, and Sarah K. Tauber. 2022. "Do Domain Knowledge and Retrieval Practice Predict Students’ Study Order Decisions?" Journal of Intelligence 10, no. 4: 122. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040122
APA StyleBabineau, A. L., Witherby, A. E., Ariel, R., Pelch, M. A., & Tauber, S. K. (2022). Do Domain Knowledge and Retrieval Practice Predict Students’ Study Order Decisions? Journal of Intelligence, 10(4), 122. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence10040122