Next Article in Journal
Sinus Augmentation for Implant Placement Utilizing a Novel Synthetic Graft Material with Delayed Immediate Socket Grafting: A 2-Year Case Study
Previous Article in Journal
Compression Stockings Improve Lower Legs Symptom in Patients with Pulmonary Artery Hypertension Treated by Pulmonary Vasodilators—A Pilot Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Mediators of Amylin Action in Metabolic Control
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Glycemic Challenge Is Associated with the Rapid Cellular Activation of the Locus Ceruleus and Nucleus of Solitary Tract: Circumscribed Spatial Analysis of Phosphorylated MAP Kinase Immunoreactivity

by
Geronimo P. Tapia
1,2,†,
Lindsay J. Agostinelli
3,†,
Sarah D. Chenausky
1,4,
Jessica V. Salcido Padilla
1,4,
Vanessa I. Navarro
1,2,
Amy Alagh
3,
Gabriel Si
3,
Richard H. Thompson
3,5,
Sivasai Balivada
1 and
Arshad M. Khan
1,3,6,*
1
UTEP Systems Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Biological Sciences, The University of Texas at El Paso, El Paso, TX 79968, USA
2
Ph.D. Program in Bioscience, Department of Biological Sciences, The University of Texas at El Paso, El Paso, TX 79968, USA
3
Department of Biological Sciences, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089, USA
4
M.S. Program in Biology, Department of Biological Sciences, The University of Texas at El Paso, El Paso, TX 79968, USA
5
School of Information, The University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78701, USA
6
Border Biomedical Research Center, The University of Texas at El Paso, El Paso, TX 79968, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
These authors contributed equally to this work.
J. Clin. Med. 2023, 12(7), 2483; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12072483
Submission received: 9 January 2023 / Revised: 24 February 2023 / Accepted: 2 March 2023 / Published: 24 March 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Molecular Mechanisms of Appetite Regulation)

Abstract

:
Rodent studies indicate that impaired glucose utilization or hypoglycemia is associated with the cellular activation of neurons in the medulla (Winslow, 1733) (MY), believed to control feeding behavior and glucose counterregulation. However, such activation has been tracked primarily within hours of the challenge, rather than sooner, and has been poorly mapped within standardized brain atlases. Here, we report that, within 15 min of receiving 2-deoxy-d-glucose (2-DG; 250 mg/kg, i.v.), which can trigger glucoprivic feeding behavior, marked elevations were observed in the numbers of rhombic brain (His, 1893) (RB) neuronal cell profiles immunoreactive for the cellular activation marker(s), phosphorylated p44/42 MAP kinases (phospho-ERK1/2), and that some of these profiles were also catecholaminergic. We mapped their distributions within an open-access rat brain atlas and found that 2-DG-treated rats (compared to their saline-treated controls) displayed greater numbers of phospho-ERK1/2+ neurons in the locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1812) (LC) and the nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS). Thus, the 2-DG-activation of certain RB neurons is more rapid than perhaps previously realized, engaging neurons that serve multiple functional systems and which are of varying cellular phenotypes. Mapping these populations within standardized brain atlas maps streamlines their targeting and/or comparable mapping in preclinical rodent models of disease.

1. Introduction

Glucose is the essential metabolic fuel source for the brain, with a constant and adequate supply needed for proper function and survival. Iatrogenic hypoglycemia is a formidable clinical problem for patients suffering from diabetes mellitus [1], and despite steady improvements in long-term diabetes management [2], remains a major limiting factor in such management for both Type I and Type II diabetics [1,3,4,5]. Glucose counterregulatory mechanisms defend the body against precipitous drops in blood sugar by increasing glucagon and reducing insulin secretion from pancreatic α and β cells, respectively, and by increasing epinephrine release from the adrenal medulla [6]. In Type I and advanced (end-stage) Type II diabetes, these defenses are impaired, thereby compromising the body’s ability to maintain glucose homeostasis. Such impairment becomes exacerbated during hypoglycemia-associated autonomic failure (HAAF), in which antecedent iatrogenic hypoglycemia causes defective glucose counterregulation by blunting plasma epinephrine responses to a given level of subsequent hypoglycemia—all in the face of impaired glucagon release and insulin reductions [7]. In addition to defective glucose counterregulation, HAAF is often associated with hypoglycemia unawareness, in which “cognitive function is so disturbed that the patient can become drowsy, uncoordinated, confused or even comatose” [8].
Human and animal models for HAAF exist but need refinement [9], partly because not all of the fundamental mechanisms underlying counterregulation have been elucidated [10]. Although sensors in the periphery (e.g., portal vein [11]) contribute to counterregulatory responses to glycemic challenges, there is a growing understanding that brain mechanisms also contribute to these responses [12,13,14,15]. Therefore, there have been intensive efforts to understand how the central nervous system (Carus, 1814) [16] (CNS) (see Section 2.1 for the formal (italicized) atlas nomenclature used in this study) senses circulating levels of glucose and initiates counterregulatory responses, and how CNS impairments contribute to defective glucose counterregulation [17,18,19,20]. A compelling body of evidence has established a critical role for various rhombic brain (His, 1893) [21] (RB) regions in glucosensing, feeding and/or counterregulatory responses to glycemic challenges [22,23,24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41], with much of this work focused on the role of catecholaminergic neurons in the medulla (Winslow, 1733) [42] (MY) in these functions [17,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51]. Certain neuronal populations in the hypothalamus (Kuhlenbeck, 1927) [52] (HY) also appear to be glucosensing and/or critical for counterregulation [53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66], and neural circuits linking the RB to the HY help trigger feeding and hypothalamo–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis responses to glycemic challenges [15,67,68,69,70,71,72,73,74]. Moreover, peripheral glucosensing mechanisms can be linked by neural circuits to RB regions controlling counterregulatory responses [18,19,75,76].
Despite these advances in our understanding of RB glucosensing and counterregulation, two points regarding the spatiotemporal dynamics of these responses remain unclear, both of which relate to the aims of the present study. First, in terms of temporal dynamics, it is not fully understood how quickly RB neuronal networks track changes in glycemic status in vivo. Immunodetection of the transcription factor, Fos, has been used to track RB neuronal activation associated with glycemic challenge [30,38,41,50,75], but its protein expression typically peaks within 1–3 h [77]. Thus, it is difficult to attribute its delayed expression to changes in glycemic status per se rather than to secondary effects set in motion by such changes. Ideally, an anatomically nonlabile biomarker of an “activated” neuron should be induced at a time more proximate to the stimulus event (discussed in [78]). In this study, the phosphorylated forms of p44/42 MAP kinases were used to rapidly track glycemic challenge, a strategy first employed by the Watts laboratory [67,68,69,70].
Second, in terms of spatial dynamics, the locations of RB neuronal populations that are activated following glycemic challenge have not been charted precisely within standardized atlases of the brain, rendering their reproducible experimental targeting and documentation more difficult (discussed in [79]). Nor is it clear if RB neuronal populations more traditionally studied in the context of arousal rather than hormonal counterregulation are also activated rapidly by glycemic challenge. This knowledge could help us understand if such regions might play a role in producing the cognitive perceptions of hypoglycemic events, the absence of which could contribute to hypoglycemia unawareness in diabetic individuals [8,80]. Here, we build upon previous work [69] to map RB neuronal activation associated with glycemic challenge to an open-access rat brain atlas [81] and identify the locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1812) [82], a key arousal-promoting RB structure, as a region displaying rapid activation to glycemic challenge, alongside other RB structures more traditionally understood to serve in networks for glucosensing and counterregulation. Taken together, the results of this work, portions of which have been presented in preliminary form [83,84,85,86,87,88,89], provide evidence that RB neuronal populations implicated in glucosensing, counterregulation, and arousal can track the onset of glycemic challenge within 15 min. Importantly, our work provides two sets of novel results addressing the aims of our study: (1) the demonstration of rapid temporal activation in the RB following glycemic challenge and (2) the precise spatial mapping of these activation patterns using an open-access brain atlas. The cellular activation patterns mapped in this study are a useful starting point towards the development of global CNS cellular activation maps for glycemic challenges.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Neuroanatomical Naming Conventions Used in this Study

In this study, as part of our ongoing effort to encourage the use of standardized nomenclature to enable the seamless registration of neuroanatomical datasets across laboratories (and such datasets within our own laboratory), we use standard terms for brain structures as proposed by Swanson [81,90]. These terms are listed in italics together with the associated citation that first uses the term as defined. Note that the author–date citation is part of the standard term, and these nomenclature-embedded citations are also included in the references list. If a definitive assignment of priority for the term was not possible, it was assigned by Swanson the citation “(>1840)”; that is, “defined sometime after the year 1840”. Refer to Swanson [81,90] for further details regarding this standardized nomenclature system.

2.2. Subjects and Surgical Procedures

All procedures were approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the University of Southern California (protocol code #11239). Adult male Sprague–Dawley rats (Harlan Labs, Indianapolis, IN, USA; n = 12; body weight (BW) between 258–270 g on the day of surgery) were single-housed and kept on a 12h:12h light:dark cycle (lights on: 06:00) with ad libitum access to standard laboratory animal chow and water. On the day of surgery, each rat was anesthetized with a cocktail of 50% ketamine (100 mg/mL), 25% xylazine (100 mg/mL), and 10% acepromazine (10 mg/mL) dissolved in 0.9% sterile saline (delivered i.m. at 0.1 mL of cocktail/100 g BW). The animal was judged to be at the appropriate surgical plane of anesthesia by tracking expected reductions in the respiratory rate following anesthesia, as well as the absence of a reflex response to a brief tail or paw pinch.
Intra-atrial catheterizations by way of the jugular vein, under aseptic surgical conditions, were performed as previously reported [67,91], with additional details noted in Section S1 of the Supplemental Methods. Postoperative care consisted of the administration of flunixin meglumine analgesic (0.25 mg/100 g BW, i.m.) and gentamicin antibiotic (0.2–0.4 mg/100 g BW, i.m.), and the careful daily monitoring and gentle handling of each animal throughout the course of the 3–6-day recovery period, during which time each animal was weighed to determine whether they reached presurgical body weight. During this recovery period, their newly-installed catheters were also inspected and flushed daily with heparinized saline to ensure their patency.

2.3. In Vivo Experimental Procedures

On the day of the experiment, during the early portion of the light phase, the animals received an injection, through the catheter, of 0.9% sterile saline as a vehicle control (n = 7) or 250 mg/kg of 2-deoxy-d-glucose (2-DG; catalog #D6134-5G; MilliporeSigma, Inc., Burlington, MA, USA) dissolved in the vehicle (n = 5). This intravenous procedure was intended to eliminate pain-related neuronal activation that would otherwise occur by administering the treatment through intraperitoneal injection. Approximately fifteen minutes after receiving saline or 2-DG, subjects received an intravenous infusion of ~1.0 mL sodium pentobarbital anesthesia (50 mg/kg) over a 30 s period. Deeply sedated, the subjects were relocated to a fume hood, under which they were perfused transcardially, first with 0.01 M sodium phosphate-buffered saline and then with freshly depolymerized 4% (w/v) p-formaldehyde, buffered to a pH of 9.5 using sodium borate. The fixed brain was removed from the cranium, placed overnight on a shaker at 4 °C in 4% (w/v) p-formaldehyde containing 12% (w/v) sucrose, blocked into forebrain and hindbrain segments, and then frozen using hexane supercooled on a bed of powdered dry ice. The frozen blocks were removed from the hexane bath and stored in vials at −80 °C until sectioning.

2.4. Tissue Processing and Immunocytochemistry

2.4.1. Histological Processing of Brain Tissue

Rhombic brain (His, 1893) [21] (RB) blocks were removed from −80 °C and mounted onto the freezing stage of a Reichert sliding microtome and cut into 20 µm-thick coronal sections. Sections kept for histochemical staining were collected as eight 1-in-8 series in tissue culture wells filled with cryoprotectant (50% phosphate buffer, 30% ethylene glycol, 20% glycerol; pH 7.4) and stored at −20 °C until further processing. In most cases, the fourth tissue series collected was reserved for preparing a cytoarchitectural Reference (R) series using Nissl staining (see Section 2.5), while the remaining tissue series were reserved for immunohistochemical analyses. The R series for any one subject is defined as the tissue series from which we used cytoarchitectural information to identify the boundary conditions for mapping the atlas gray matter regions (Swanson and Bota, 2010) [92] of our immunoreactive neuronal populations, which were visualized in adjacent tissue series from the same subject.

2.4.2. Immunohistochemistry

Main Procedures

Using Tris-buffered saline (TBS) (0.05 M; pH of 7.4 at room temperature), sections were rinsed of cryoprotectant (5 rinses for 5 min each; 5 × 5) and were then incubated in blocking solution consisting of TBS containing 2% (v/v) normal donkey serum (catalog # S30–100 ML; EMD-Millipore, Burlington, MA, USA) and 0.1% (v/v) Triton X-100 for ~1.5–2.0 h at room temperature. The tissue was reacted with a cocktail consisting of blocking solution together with primary antibodies targeting dopamine-β-hydroxylase (EC 1.14.17.1, https://iubmb.qmul.ac.uk/enzyme/EC1/14/17/1.html), choline acetyltransferase (EC 2.3.1.6, https://iubmb.qmul.ac.uk/enzyme/EC2/3/1/6.html), or the phosphorylated forms of ERKs 1 and 2 (EC 2.7.11.24, https://iubmb.qmul.ac.uk/enzyme/EC2/7/11/24.html) (see Table 1 for details). After another 5 × 5 TBS rinse, the sections were reacted with the appropriate secondary cocktail (again with blocking solution), followed by a conjugate reagent (Table 1). The tissue underwent a final 5 × 5 wash series before being mounted on Superfrost™ Plus slides, which were coverslipped with sodium bicarbonate-buffered glycerol (pH of 8.6 at room temperature), sealed with clear nail polish, stored at 4 °C, and protected from ambient light until further analysis.

Antibody Information and Immunohistochemical Controls

Table 1 provides information about the antibodies used in this study. Preadsorption experiments performed by ourselves or other laboratories have demonstrated the specificity of the primary antibodies we used in this study for binding to their intended antigen targets. First, as noted previously [68], preadsorption of the rabbit phospho-ERK1/2 antibody we used in this study with the immunizing peptide used to produce it abolished all immunostaining in brain tissue (AMK; unpublished data). Second, the mouse monoclonal anti-DBH antibody we used reportedly produces immunoreactivity that is completely abolished in rat brain tissue sections when the antibody is preadsorbed with a 10-fold excess of bovine adrenal DBH [93]. Similarly, the primary anti-ChAT antibody we used, which was raised against human placental ChAT, reportedly did not produce immunoreactive signal when preadsorbed with a rat recombinant ChAT protein [94]. Additionally, Sutton and colleagues [95,96] have demonstrated that rat nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS) tissue displays specific bands for phospho-ERK1/2 using the same antibody we used in our study, bands which can be co-labeled using an antibody against the unphosphorylated forms of the enzyme. Please refer to Section S2 of the Supplemental Methods for details about our immunohistochemistry control experiments.

2.5. Nissl Staining of the Reference (R) Tissue Series

The R series of tissue sections (adjacent to those that underwent immunohistochemical staining) was washed 5 × 5 in TBS and mounted onto gelatin-coated slides, dehydrated in ascending concentrations of ethanol (50%, 70%, 95%, 100%), and defatted with xylene. Sections were then rehydrated and stained with a 0.25% w/v thionine solution (sold as “thionin acetate” by Sigma-Aldrich, St. Louis, MO, USA; MW 287.34 g/mol; cat #T7029; ([97], p. 151); [98]). A 0.4% glacial acetic acid solution was used to remove excess thionine and the tissue slides were dehydrated in ascending concentrations of ethanol solutions (50%, 70%, 95%, 100% × 3), and then cleared in mixed-isomer xylenes before being coverslipped using DPX mountant for bright-field microscopic observation (Section 2.6.1).

2.6. Microscopy and Photomicrography

2.6.1. Bright-Field Microscopy

A total of 93 R series (Nissl-stained) tissue sections, for a subset of saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects (n = 2 and 3, respectively), were photographed under bright-field illumination using an Olympus BX63 light microscope with an X–Y–Z motorized stage and an attached DP74 color CMOS camera (cooled, 20.8 MP pixel-shift, 60 fps) (Olympus America, Inc., Waltham, MA, USA). Image acquisition was carried out using the cellSense™ Dimension software (Version 2.3; Olympus) installed on a Hewlett Packard PC workstation. The software was used to drive the motorized stage and generate tiled and stitched wide-field mosaic photomicrographs of the entire tissue section using a ×10 magnification objective lens (Olympus UPlanSApo, N.A. 0.40, FN 26.5 mm). Stitching was performed with a 15% overlap of individual images.

2.6.2. Epifluorescence and Dark-Field Microscopy

Selected tissue sections from subjects receiving saline or 2-DG were photographed using epifluorescence illumination. Identifying information was first obscured on all tissue slides by a team member (AMK), and the sections were then photographed by another team member (GPT) who was thus blind to the treatment groups. Photography was carried out using an Axio Imager M.2 upright epifluorescence microscope (Carl Zeiss, Inc., Thornwood, NY, USA) equipped with an X–Y–Z motorized stage and a 100 W halogen light source. The appropriate filter cubes were used to detect the immunofluorescence of the sections in separate noninterfering channels, which was captured by an EXi Blue monochrome camera (Teledyne QImaging, Inc., Surrey, BC, Canada) driven by Volocity software (versions 6.0.0–6.1.1; Quorum Technologies, Puslinch, ON, Canada) installed on a Mac Pro computer (Apple Corp., Cupertino, CA, USA). Single fields of view and ×20-magnification (Plan-Apochromat objective lens; N.A., 0.8) wide-field mosaics of individual fields of view (stitched with 20–40% overlap) were acquired within Volocity and exported as uncompressed TIFF-formatted files. These photomicrographs were also exported as compressed JPEG-formatted files for conference presentation of preliminary results [85,86]. Regions of interest (ROIs) were photographed for each subject from the left and right sides of the rhombic brain (His, 1893) [21] (RB), and included the locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1912) [82], area postrema (>1840), nucleus of solitary tract, medial part (>1840), dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840), and hypoglossal nucleus (>1840). Care was taken to ensure that all photomicrographs were captured under similar illumination conditions. For certain sections where the companion R series tissue was damaged or otherwise unavailable for reference, a photomicrograph was taken—under dark-field illumination—of the fluorescence tissue series in order to obtain fiducial landmarks to help delineate cytoarchitectural boundaries and to determine the atlas level assignment of the tissue section.

2.6.3. Confocal Microscopy

For selected rhombic brain (His, 1893) [21] (RB) tissue sections, additional imaging was performed using a Zeiss LSM 700 confocal laser scanning system connected to a Zeiss Observer.Z1 inverted fluorescence microscope (Carl Zeiss, Inc., Thornwood, NY, USA), located in the Cellular Characterization and Biorepository Core Facility of the Border Biomedical Research Center at UT El Paso. The system is equipped with four solid-state lasers, including those at 488 nm, 555 nm, and 639 nm wavelengths, which were used to detect Alexa 488, Cy3, and Alexa 647 fluorophore signals, respectively. The microscope was driven by Zen 2009 software (release version 6.0 SP2; version 6.0.0.303; Carl Zeiss Microimaging GmbH, Dublin, CA, USA).
Once it was determined that the signal was within the detector’s dynamic range (see Section S3.1 of the Supplemental Methods for further details), sequential unidirectional scans of the tissue were taken using Plan-Neofluar objective lenses of ×10 (N.A., 0.3; Ph1), ×20 (N.A., 0.5), or ×40 (N.A., 1.3; oil) magnifications. Pinhole settings were kept at or close to 1 Airy unit [99] for each channel, defined as the width of the point-spread function for that channel; however, for images used for colocalization analysis, the pinholes were adjusted above or below the 1 Airy unit setting so that the widths of the optical sections for each channel were matched in the axial (z) dimension to ensure accurate comparison of signals [100].
With a few exceptions, frame sizes were set to 2048 × 2048 pixels. Image sizes were 618.9 μm × 618.9 μm (pixel size = 0.30 μm). Scanning was performed at a slow scan speed (setting: 4) with 16 frames averaged over a scan time of ~36 min (~12 min/channel). Scans were exported as full-resolution uncompressed TIFF-formatted files and imported onto artboards in Adobe Illustrator CC (version 25.0; Adobe Systems, Inc., San Jose, CA, USA) to prepare figures for this study.

2.6.4. Checks for Fluorescence Crosstalk

A number of quality-control checks, which are also recommended as community standards (e.g., [100,101]), were performed to evaluate crosstalk (bleed-through) of the fluorescence signals we detected in this study. These included evaluations of crosstalk at the level of fluorescence labeling, epifluorescence imaging, and confocal imaging (see Section S4 of the Supplemental Methods for details).

2.7. Spatial Analyses and Tabulation

2.7.1. Sampled Regions

For this initial characterization of immunofluorescent labeling patterns in saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects, we sampled selected regions in the rhombic brain (His, 1893) [21] (RB) at three selected rostrocaudal levels (Figure 1). Specifically, the tissue sections we examined were sampled at inferred anteroposterior stereotaxic coordinates of −9.80 mm from bregma (β) (corresponding to level 51 of BM4.0), −13.44 mm from β (level 67), and −13.76 from β (level 69). Our analyses included observations of immunofluorescent patterns in portions of the locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1912) [82] (LC), area postrema (>1840), nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS), dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX), and hypoglossal nucleus (>1840) (XII).

2.7.2. Plane-of-Section Analysis, Atlas Level Assignments, and Regional Parcellations

To determine whether the tissue sections were at the rostrocaudal locations that corresponded to the atlas levels we aimed to sample, we performed a plane-of-section analysis for each tissue section collected. We have briefly reported on this type of analysis for Fos-immunoreactive tissue under conditions of fasting and refeeding [102] and note here additional details germane to the present study (for a more thorough review of this topic, please consult Simmons and Swanson [103]).
First, all R series tissue was evaluated for its suitability to help determine cytoarchitectonic boundaries by documenting the condition of each tissue section on a spreadsheet. Sections deemed suitable for use were photographed as described in Section 2.6.1. Team members (GPT, JVSP) provisionally assigned an atlas level to the section by closely examining and documenting the nearest-neighbor spatial relations of identifiable cell groups in the R series photomicrographs and compared these with regions within the Brain Maps 4.0 atlas. In situations where two closely spaced sections corresponded to the same atlas level, only one of the sections was deemed the best to represent the distribution patterns for that atlas level (i.e., only one section and its labeling patterns were used to map patterns to that particular level).
Once initial assignments were made to all R series photomicrographs, they were migrated into Adobe Illustrator (AI) 2022 and annotated manually. A transparent data layer overlay was created for each photomicrograph and boundaries were drawn around relevant cell groupings identifiable in the Nissl-stained patterns to produce regional parcellations. The left and right sides of the sections were also carefully noted to allow for accurate registration of Nissl-defined boundaries with tissue series processed for immunofluorescence. This was achieved by recording asymmetry in the shapes of the tissue hemispheres, especially in certain key areas (e.g., area between the pyramidal tracts; cross-sectional profiles of ventrally located blood vessels investing the tissue). For instances where a Nissl-stained section was missing or damaged and could not be used to gather information to help with the plane-of-section analysis and atlas level assignments, the chemoarchitecture of the fluorescently labeled tissue series and its associated fiducials were used instead. In some cases, visualization of the fluorescently labeled tissue section under dark-field illumination was sufficient to provide team members with important fiducials to make the determinations needed. The specific criteria and considerations used to determine the correct spatial context for each ROI is detailed in Section S5 of the Supplemental Methods.

2.7.3. Standardized Mapping

Images of immunolabeled tissue sections were imported into Adobe Illustrator (AI) and aligned precisely with the data layer containing the photomicrograph of the corresponding adjacent R series Nissl-stained tissue section that served as a cytoarchitectural reference (described in Section 2.4.1 and Section 2.5). Region boundaries that had been drawn over the Nissl-photograph data layer were then superimposed on the underlying fluorescence image, enabling each image set to be registered to the proper level of the Brain Maps 4.0 (BM4.0) open-access atlas [81].
To plot immunolabeled elements onto BM4.0 atlas templates in AI, fluorescently labeled fibers and perikaryon profiles were traced and marked with line segments and circles, respectively, on a separate data layer. If these representations shared a common region boundary (i.e., if they were all within the same region), they were grouped together in their layer. This layer was then copied and pasted as a collective set of drawn elements into an underlying data layer containing the corresponding atlas level template with the corresponding brain region. This import of drawn elements was achieved by aligning the grouped elements to be in as close of a registration as possible with the bounded regions on the map. Alignments generally consisted of rotating the drawn patterns to orient correctly with the map’s regional boundaries. In this manner, maps of the DBH-, ChAT-, and phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive signals were created for each subject.

2.7.4. Region-Delimited Quantitation and Assessment of Signal Colocalization

Counting Procedures

The rationale for our counting procedures is provided for interested readers in Section S6 of the Supplemental Methods and is based on published recommendations [104,105]. Immunolabeled perikaryon profiles were manually counted and annotated in a manner similar to that reported previously by one of us [67], but digital transparent overlays were utilized rather than acetate sheets affixed to printed hard copies of the images. Specifically, images of sections with ROIs corresponding to BM4.0 atlas levels 51, 67, and 69 were opened in Adobe Illustrator (AI) 2022 and arranged into files containing the superimposed merged-channel and single-channel images, each in a separate layer. Two additional layers were created for the manual annotation of DBH- and pERK1/2-labeled perikaryon profiles.
To mark the locations of DBH-labeled perikaryon profiles, the reviewer made visible the layer containing the single-channel image corresponding to DBH immunofluorescence and used the Ellipse Tool to place white-colored ellipses over labeled perikaryon profiles in the DBH layer. Keeping the layer containing DBH+ perikaryon profile markings visible, the team member toggled the single-channel pERK1/2 image on and off, determining if each DBH+ perikaryon profile was single- or double-labeled based on the pERK1/2 profile’s visibility in one or both channels, respectively.
To record the double-labeled perikaryon profiles, the Smart Guides “snap-to-center” function in AI was used to place a black ellipse on exactly the centroid coordinate of the DBH+ profile marking on a ‘pERK1/2 + DBH’ sublayer of the ‘pERK1/2’ layer. When all co-labeled perikaryon profiles were identified in this manner, the pERK1/2 single-channel image was used to record profiles displaying only the signal for pERK1/2 in a sublayer called ‘pERK1/2 only’. For annotations of images corresponding to levels 67 and 69 of BM4.0, the ChAT immunolabel readily identified the cholinergic motor neurons of the DMX; any perikaryon profiles that were DBH+ or pERK1/2+ in the vicinity of the DMX but not ChAT+ were not considered DMX neurons.
To obtain the perikaryon profile counts, the layer or sublayer was selected and the number of objects within that layer was recorded. The number of pERK1/2+ + DBH+ perikaryon profiles and pERK1/2-only profiles were added to determine the total number of pERK1/2-labeled profiles, and the number of pERK1/2+ + DBH+ perikaryon profiles was subtracted from the total number of DBH+ profiles to determine the number of DBH-only profiles.

Correction of Oversampling Error

We adapted the method of Abercrombie [106] to correct for overestimation of our perikaryon profile counts, as detailed in Section S6.2 of the Supplemental Methods.

2.7.5. Statistical Analysis

The pERK1/2+, DBH+, and ChAT+ bilateral perikaryon profiles within the LC, NTSm, and DMX were tallied on the sections corresponding to BM4.0 atlas levels 51, 67, and 69. The average of the bilateral perikaryon profile counts [(left side counts + right side counts)/2] for each region, from each of the saline- and 2-DG-treated rats, was considered as a representative number of immunoreactive cells. The number of profiles from each brain region was used as a dependent variable, with saline or 2-DG treatment as the independent variable for statistical and exploratory data analyses. A Shapiro–Wilk test [107] was performed to determine the Gaussian distribution of the dependent variable in each treatment group. Phospho-ERK1/2+ perikaryon profile counts in the NTSm at levels 67 and 69 in 2-DG-treated rats did not meet the Gaussian distribution assumption at a p-value of 0.05. Since the distributions were not normal, and to be consistent throughout the study, nonparametric statistical tests were used to analyze all differences in profile counts across the sampled brain regions in this study.
Significance of the difference in the distributions of immunoreactive perikaryon profile counts between saline- and 2-DG-treated rats was analyzed for each region using the Wilcoxon rank sum test, with two-sided significance [108,109]. A p-value less than or equal to 0.05 was considered statistically significant. False discovery rate was controlled by calculating Benjamini–Hochberg [110] adjusted p-values, with an FDR of 5% or below considered as statistically significant. Effect sizes were calculated and used to interpret the magnitude of the difference between saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects. Effect sizes from 0.1 to less than 0.3, 0.3 to less than 0.5, and greater than or equal to 0.5 were categorized as small, moderate, and large magnitudes, respectively. RStudio (R, version 3.6.2) [111] with rstatix [112] and ggplot2 [113] packages were used for statistical analysis and to construct box plots and pie charts.

3. Results

The results of this study are divided into two main parts. First, rostral rhombic brain (His, 1893) [21] (RB) data for saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects are presented, specifically for the locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1912) [82] (LC). Second, caudal RB data are considered, similarly comparing labeling patterns for the two groups for the nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS) and dorsal motor nucleus of vagus (>1840) (DMX). For each part, individual subjects are first showcased to focus on paired comparisons of the histological results, the individual patterns of immunoreactivities in atlas maps, and the morphological considerations of the signals. This is followed by group-level statistical comparisons of immunoreactive perikaryon profile counts between saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects and counts of activated perikaryon profiles sorted by cellular phenotype.

3.1. Cellular Activation Profiles in the LC for Saline- and 2-DG-Treated Subjects

3.1.1. Representative Results in Single Pairs of Subjects

Phospho-ERK1/2-Immunoreactive Patterns Mapped to Level 51

Figure 2 shows the mapped distributions of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive neurons in a portion of the LC that we sampled from subjects receiving intravenous saline or 2-DG injection. To identify the appropriate rostrocaudal locations of the tissue sections in both groups, we were guided by fiducial landmarks within the tissue, either visualized in the same section under dark-field illumination (Figure 2A(i)) or within a thionine-stained section from an adjacent series of tissue collected from the same subject (i.e., R series; see Materials and Methods) (Figure 2B(i)). The reference tissue allowed us to map the locations of the profiles observed in the fluorescence images to level 51 of the BM4.0 atlas [81]. Saline treatment was not associated with an appreciable number of phospho-ERK1/2+ perikaryon profiles in the LC (Figure 2A(iii)). In contrast, a robust elevation in the numbers of phospho-ERK1/2+ perikaryon profiles was observed following 2-DG treatment (Figure 2B(iii)). Virtually all of the phospho-ERK1/2 immunoreactivity appeared to be within DBH-immunoreactive neurons (Figure 2B(iv)), a cellular phenotype well-documented to be abundant in this structure (e.g., [114,115,116,117,118,119]); these double-labeled perikaryon profiles were mapped as gold-colored circle glyphs on the corresponding atlas map (Figure 2B(v)). At this level for this subject, all phospho-ERK1/2+ profiles appeared to be double-labeled for DBH-immunoreactivity, since none appeared to contain only the single label for phospho-ERK1/2 alone.

Phospho-ERK1/2+ and DBH+ Perikaryon Profiles at Other Levels of the LC

Although we did not formally chart patterns of immunoreactivity to BM4.0 atlas templates for tissue sections containing the LC at levels other than level 51, we did observe phospho-ERK1/2+ profiles throughout the LC at all rostrocaudal levels containing this structure, primarily within 2-DG-treated subjects. For example, for another 2-DG-treated subject (#K10-027), perikaryon profiles in the LC again showed robust immunoreactivity for the activation marker in tissue sections just caudal to that mapped at level 51 (Figure 3). Specifically, at a rostrocaudal location that maps ~480 μm further caudally to a tissue plane that falls between BM4.0 atlas levels 52 and 53, phospho-ERK1/2+ perikaryon profiles were again evident, especially in those situated dorsally within the structure (Figure 3B).
Viewed under the higher power of a ×20 magnification objective lens and captured as a single-plane confocal scan, the DBH-immunoreactive signal could be observed within perikarya and neurite extensions, as has been reported by others [114,115,116,117,118,119]. Some of these neurites were oriented in parallel to the lateral margin of the fourth ventricle principal part (Swanson, 2018) [81] (V4) (Figure 3A). Although double-labeled perikaryon profiles are evident at this level (Figure 3D), the relative abundance, brightness, and saturation of the Alexa-488 fluorescence swamped the dimmer Cy3 signal of the phospho-ERK1/2 immunoreactivity in these profiles. These patterns were more readily observable if the opacity of the Alexa-488 signal was set to 35% without altering the Cy3 signal (Figure 3C; compare profiles marked by arrows in panels C and D). In the double-labeled neurons, the presence of phospho-ERK1/2 immunoreactivity in the nuclear compartments of some perikarya was clearly evident, with a notable absence of a fluorescent signal marking the presence of the nucleolus in many of these profiles (Figure 3D).

3.1.2. Group-Level Effects of Saline Versus 2-Deoxy-d-Glucose Administration on LC Activation

Quantitative Analysis of LC Activation

To determine LC activation in association with saline and 2-DG treatments, pERK1/2- and DBH-immunoreactive perikaryon profiles were counted in the LC at BM4.0 atlas level 51 for all subjects. Figure 4 shows box plots of Abercrombie-corrected phospho-ERK1/2+ and DBH+ perikaryon profile counts and Supplemental Table S1 presents the descriptive statistics of our quantitative analysis. Median numbers of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive profiles in saline and 2-DG groups were 15 and 33, respectively (Table S1), and the distributions in the two groups differed significantly (Wilcoxon statistic = 25, n1 = n2 = 5, FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; Table S2). Specifically, the number of pERK1/2-immunoreactive profiles displayed in the LC was significantly elevated for 2-DG- compared to saline-treated rats (Figure 4, ‘total’ column (pERK1/2: +); FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; effect size: large; Table S2). Similarly, median numbers of double-labeled profiles in saline and 2-DG groups were 13 and 33, respectively (Table S1), with distributions in the two groups that again were significantly different (Wilcoxon statistic = 25, n1 = n2 = 5, FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; Table S2). Thus, the double-labeled counts of the LC profiles of 2-DG rats were elevated significantly compared to those of saline-treated rats (Figure 4, ‘double-labeled’ column (pERK1/2, DBH: both +); effect size: large; Table S2). In contrast, there were no statistically significant differences in DBH perikaryon profile counts between saline- and 2-DG-treated rats (Figure 4, ‘total’ column (DBH: +) and ‘single-labeled’ column (DBH: +, pERK1/2: −); see also Table S2).

Relative Differences in the Numbers of DBH-Immunoreactive Perikaryon Profiles in the LC of Saline- and 2-DG-Treated Subjects

To determine whether 2-DG treatment was associated with changes in the numbers of DBH-immunoreactive neurons in the LC, the relative percentages of Abercrombie-corrected single (pERK1/2+ only or DBH+ only) and double-labeled (pERK1/2+ and DBH+) perikaryon profiles were calculated for saline and 2-DG groups and visualized as pie charts in (Supplemental) Figure S1. Consistent with our microscopic observations, a majority of the pERK1/2+ perikaryon profiles in the LC were identified to be DBH+ in both saline- (94.9%) and 2-DG-treated (98.3%) rats (Figure S1, pERK1/2+ row, pERK1/2+ & DBH+; yellow-colored fraction). For the total numbers of DBH+ perikaryon profiles, a higher percentage of pERK1/2+ profiles were observed in the LC of 2-DG- (36.2%) than saline-treated (17.7%) rats (Figure S1, DBH+ row, pERK1/2+ and DBH+, yellow-colored fraction). In contrast, the average total numbers of DBH+ perikaryon profiles were not significantly different between the two groups (FDR-adjusted p = 0.22; Table S2).

3.2. Cellular activation profiles in the NTS and DMX for saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects

3.2.1. Representative results in single pairs of subjects at atlas level 67

Phospho-ERK1/2-Immunoreactive Patterns Mapped to Level 67

Figure 5 shows the mapped distributions of the phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive signal in activated NTS and DMX neurons following saline or 2-DG administration. Saline treatment was not associated with a substantial number of phospho-ERK1/2-immunopositive perikaryon profiles in the NTS at level 67 (Figure 5A(iii)). In comparison, 2-DG treatment was associated with a modest elevation of above-background fluorescent signal marking phospho-ERK1/2 immunoreactive perikaryon profiles within the NTSm (see faintly-fluorescing profiles in Figure 5B(iii), marked by arrows; also see Figure S2A,B for enlarged views). This elevation, which reached statistical significance (see Section 3.2.2 for group-level effects), corresponded to both single-labeled phospho-ERK1/2 profiles and those double-labeled for DBH immunoreactivity (Figure 5B(vi)) that were represented as red- and yellow-colored circle glyphs, respectively, on the corresponding atlas map (Figure 5B(v)).
ChAT-immunopositive neurons in the DMX, depicted in blue, displayed low levels of basal activation in saline-treated subjects (Figure 5A(vii)) and appear as magenta circles on the atlas maps when immunoreactive for phospho-ERK1/2. In some subjects, the numbers of phospho-ERK1/2+ perikaryon profiles in the DMX were elevated following 2-DG but not saline treatment (Figure 5B(vii)), but, as noted in Section 3.2.2, this difference did not reach statistical significance at a group level. In some instances, ChAT-labeled perikaryon profiles were observed to also be DBH-immunopositive, shown as neon green circles, and represented as black circles when immunoreactive for all three markers. In contrast, no phospho-ERK1/2 immunoreactivity was observed in the hypoglossal nucleus (>1840) (XII), and few or no DBH-immunoreactive fibers were observed within the nucleus of solitary tract, lateral part (>1840).

3.2.2. Group-Level Effects of Saline Versus 2-Deoxy-d-Glucose Administration on NTS and DMX Activation at BM4.0 Atlas Level 67

Quantitative Analysis of NTS Activation and DBH-immunoreactive Perikarya at Level 67

To determine NTS activation in association with saline and 2-DG treatments, pERK1/2- and DBH-immunoreactive perikaryon profiles were counted in the NTS at BM4.0 atlas level 67 for all subjects (n = 6 for the saline-treated group; n = 5 for the 2-DG-treated group). Figure 6 shows box plots of Abercrombie-corrected phospho-ERK1/2+ and DBH+ perikaryon profile counts. Tables S3 and S4 present the descriptive and test statistics, respectively, of our quantitative analysis.
Median numbers of pERK1/2-immunoreactive profiles in saline and 2-DG groups were 1.7 and 6.1, respectively (Table S3), and the distributions in the two groups differed significantly (Wilcoxon statistic = 30, FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; Table S4). The number of pERK1/2-immunoreactive profiles within the NTS significantly increased following 2-DG treatment as compared to saline-treated controls (Figure 6, ‘total’ column (pERK1/2: +); FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; effect size: large; Table S4). The median numbers of double-labeled profiles in the saline and 2-DG groups were 0.2 and 1.9, respectively (Table S3), that maintained significantly different distributions between groups (Wilcoxon statistic = 29, FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; Table S4). The elevated counts observed in 2-DG-treated rats translated across single- (Figure 6, ‘single-labeled’ column (pERK1/2: +, DBH: −); FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; effect size: large; Table S4) and dual-labeled perikaryon profile counts (Figure 6, ‘double-labeled’ column (pERK1/2: +, DBH: +); FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; effect size: large; Table S4). Saline- and 2-DG-treated rats did not significantly differ in the total DBH-immunoreactive profile counts (Figure 6, ‘total’ column (DBH: +); see also Table S4).

Quantitative Analysis of NTS Activation and DBH-Immunoreactive Perikarya at Level 67

To determine the numbers of DBH-immunoreactive neurons in the NTS at level 67 in saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects, the relative percentages of Abercrombie-corrected single- (pERK1/2+ only or DBH+ only) and double-labeled (pERK1/2+ and DBH+) perikaryon profiles were calculated for saline and 2-DG groups and visualized as pie charts in Figure S2C. In contrast to the relative percentages observed in the LC at atlas level 51, the majority of the pERK1/2+ perikaryon profiles in the NTS at level 67 were identified to be DBH in both saline- (86.9%) and 2-DG-treated (74.0%) rats (Figure S2C, pERK1/2+ row, pERK1/2+ only; maroon-colored fraction). For total numbers of DBH+ perikaryon profiles, a higher percentage of colocalized pERK1/2+ profiles was observed in the NTS of 2-DG- (55.0%) than saline-treated (11.9%) rats (Figure S2C, DBH+ row, pERK1/2+ and DBH+; yellow-colored fraction).

Quantitative Analysis of DMX Activation and DBH- and ChAT-Immunoreactive Perikarya at Level 67

To determine DMX activation in association with saline and 2-DG treatments, pERK1/2-, ChAT-, and DBH-immunoreactive perikaryon profiles were counted in the DMX at BM4.0 atlas level 67 for all but one subject (n = 6 for the saline-treated group, n = 5 for the 2-DG-treated group; tissue for the 2-DG-treated subject, K10-008, was not available at this level). Figure 7 shows box plots of Abercrombie-corrected phospho-ERK1/2+, ChAT+ and DBH+ perikaryon profile counts for these eleven subjects. Tables S3 and S4 present the descriptive statistics and test statistics, respectively, of all comparisons at this level for these subjects. Although we observed treatment-associated differences in Abercrombie-corrected profiles displaying pERK1/2 immunoreactivity in the DMX at BM4.0 atlas level 67 in some subjects (Figure 5), at a group level, the numbers of profiles (single-, double-, or triple-labeled) did not differ significantly between saline- and 2-DG-treated rats (see Figure 7 and Table S4). Similarly, no differences were observed in the numbers of ChAT+ and DBH+ perikaryon profiles between treatment groups.
In the absence of significant changes in the number of DMX perikaryon profiles, the relative percentages of Abercrombie-corrected single-, double-, and triple-labeled profiles were calculated for saline and 2-DG groups and visualized as pie charts in Figure S3. The majority of the pERK1/2+ perikaryon profiles in the DMX at level 67 were identified to be ChAT+ but negative for DBH in both saline- (97.7%) and 2-DG-treated (97.8%) rats (Figure S3, pERK1/2+ row, pERK1/2+ and ChAT+; magenta-colored fraction). Interestingly, for the total number of ChAT+ perikaryon profiles, the percentage of pERK1/2+ and ChAT+ double-labeled perikaryon profiles appears to be greater in 2-DG- (45.0%) than saline-treated (33.9%) rats.

3.2.3. Representative Results in Single Pairs of Subjects at Atlas Level 69

Phospho-ERK1/2-Immunoreactive Patterns Mapped to Level 69

Figure 8 shows the mapped distributions of phospho-ERK1/2 immunoreactivity in NTS and DMX perikaryon profiles at level 69 following saline or 2-DG administration. Saline treatment was associated with a relatively low number of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive perikaryon profiles in the NTS at level 69 (Figure 8A(iii); see profiles marked by arrows; also see Figure S4A,B for enlarged views). In contrast, 2-DG treatment corresponded to greater numbers of immunoreactive profiles in comparable tissue sections (Figure 8B(iii); see profiles marked by arrows; also see Figure S4A,B for enlarged views). Saline treatment was also associated with a low amount of basally-activated ChAT-immunopositive perikarya that dimly emitted a phospho-ERK1/2 signal in the DMX at level 69 (Figure 8A(vii)). Similar to level 67, the total numbers of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive perikaryon profiles, and the relative brightness of the fluorescent signal, increased to a slight extent following 2-DG administration (Figure 8B(vii)). In both groups, fewer ChAT-immunoreactive profiles that were also DBH-immunopositive or triple-labeled were observed at this level. A few phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive profiles were found in the NTSco but not in the NTSl or XII.
Figure 9 presents confocal microscopic images of tissue sections that better showcase the elevated numbers of perikaryon profiles displaying phospho-ERK1/2 immunoreactivity within the NTS and DMX of 2-DG-treated subjects as compared to those receiving the saline vehicle. Increased numbers of perikaryon profiles co-labeled with DBH immunoreactivity, and those expressing only phospho-ERK1/2 immunoreactivity, were observed in the NTS in association with 2-DG treatment (Figure 9B”(ii)). Within the DMX, saline-treated subjects displayed few phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive profiles that mostly populated the medial portion of the nucleus (Figure 9A”(ii)). The 2-DG treatment was associated with minor elevations in the numbers of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive profiles within the DMX that did not appear to show a preferential distribution through the nucleus (Figure 9B”(ii)).

3.2.4. Group-Level Effects of Saline Versus 2-Deoxy-d-Glucose Administration on NTS and DMX Activation at BM4.0 Atlas Level 69

Quantitative Analysis of NTS Activation and DBH-immunoreactive Perikarya at Level 69

To determine NTS activation in association with saline and 2-DG treatments, phospho-ERK1/2- and DBH-immunoreactive perikaryon profiles were counted in the NTS at BM4.0 atlas level 69 for saline- (n = 6) and 2-DG-treated (n = 4) subjects. Figure 10 shows box plots of Abercrombie-corrected phospho-ERK1/2+ and DBH+ perikaryon profile counts. Tables S5 and S6 present the descriptive and test statistics, respectively, of our quantitative analysis. Median numbers of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive profiles in saline and 2-DG groups were 3.6 and 8.3, respectively (Table S5), and the distributions in the two groups differed significantly (Wilcoxon statistic = 24, FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; effect size: large; Table S6). The number of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive profiles within the NTS significantly increased following 2-DG treatment as compared to saline-treated controls (Figure 10; ‘total’ column (pERK1/2: +); FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; effect size: large; Table S6). The median numbers of double-labeled profiles in saline and 2-DG groups were 0.8 and 2.9, respectively (Table S5), that again were significantly different (Wilcoxon statistic = 24, FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; Table S6). Total numbers of DBH-labeled profiles (Figure 10, ‘total’ column (DBH: +); see also Table S6) did not significantly vary between treatment groups.

Quantitative Analysis of NTS Activation and DBH-Immunoreactive Perikarya at Level 69

To identify 2-DG-treatment-associated changes in the perikaryon phenotype composition in the NTS at level 69, the relative percentages of Abercrombie-corrected single (pERK1/2+ only or DBH+ only) and double-labeled (pERK1/2+ and DBH+) perikaryon profiles were calculated for saline and 2-DG groups and visualized as pie charts in Figure S4C. Similar to the relative percentages observed in the NTS region at atlas level 67, a majority of the pERK1/2+ perikaryon profiles were identified to be DBH in both saline- (74.8%) and 2-DG-treated (62.7%) rats (Figure S4C, pERK1/2+ row, pERK1/2+ only; maroon-colored fraction). Consistent with level 67 NTS perikaryon profile relative percentages, in DBH+ perikaryon profiles, a higher percentage of pERK1/2+ profiles was observed in the NTS of 2-DG- (27.7%) than saline-treated (9.2%) rats (Figure S4C, DBH+ row, pERK1/2+ and DBH+; yellow-colored fraction).

Quantitative Analysis of DMX Activation and DBH- and ChAT-Immunoreactive Perikarya at Level 69

To determine DMX activation in association with saline and 2-DG treatments, phospho-ERK1/2-, ChAT-, and DBH-immunoreactive perikaryon profiles were counted in the DMX at BM4.0 atlas level 69 for saline- (n = 6) and 2-DG-treated (n = 4) subjects. Figure 11 shows box plots of Abercrombie-corrected phospho-ERK1/2+, ChAT+, and DBH+ perikaryon profile counts. Tables S5 and S6 present the descriptive and test statistics, respectively, of our quantitative analysis. Median numbers of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive profiles in saline and 2-DG groups were 8.8 and 8.5, respectively (Table S5), and the distributions in the two groups were not statistically different (Wilcoxon statistic = 11.5, FDR-adjusted p = 1; Table S6). The total numbers of ChAT-labeled profiles (Figure 11, ‘total’ column (ChAT: +); see also Table S6) or ChAT-profiles also immunopositive for DBH (Figure 11; see also Table S6) did not vary significantly between treatment groups. Thus, triple-labeled perikaryon profile counts (Figure 11, ‘triple-labeled’ column; see also Table S6) were not significantly different between groups.
In the absence of significant differences in the numbers of DMX perikaryon profiles, the relative percentages of Abercrombie-corrected single-, double-, and triple-labeled profiles were calculated for saline and 2-DG groups and visualized as pie charts in Figure S5. Similar to the level 67 results, the majority of the phospho-ERK1/2+ perikaryon profiles in the DMX at level 69 were identified to be ChAT+ and negative for DBH in both saline- (99.7%) and 2-DG-treated (100%) subjects (Figure S5, pERK1/2+ row, pERK1/2+ and ChAT+; magenta-colored fraction). However, among total ChAT+ perikaryon profiles, the percentage of pERK1/2+ and ChAT+ double-positive perikaryon profiles appears to be greater in 2-DG- (53.5%) than saline-treated (43.6%) subjects, but the difference is not as prominent as for the level 67 relative percentages.

4. Discussion

In this study, we found that an intravenous glycemic challenge, 2-deoxy-d-glucose (2-DG), is associated with the rapid activation of rhombic brain (His, 1893) (RB) [21] neurons, specifically those in the locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1812) (LC) [82] and nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS). These were sampled and mapped to discrete rostrocaudal levels of the Brain Maps 4.0 open-access rat brain atlas [81]. The activation was encoded by elevations in the numbers of perikaryon profiles displaying immunoreactivity for the phosphorylated forms of ERK1/2 in these regions within 15 min of the challenge. These activated neurons included catecholaminergic and non-catecholaminergic neurons, some of which also displayed basal levels of activation in saline-treated subjects. Below, we discuss these findings in relation to the relevant physiology and anatomy of neural circuits engaged during feeding and glycemic counterregulation.

4.1. Physiological Considerations

The activation of neural circuits during glycemic challenge, as with other systems-level processes, can be observed and measured at multiple scales, with careful considerations across these scales providing a convergence of evidence in support of the conclusions one draws for the whole system. Here, we consider multiscale questions for our measured readout (phospho-ERK1/2 immunoreactivity), our chosen stimulus (2-DG injection), and their coupling in the brain, with an eye towards an accounting of how these components, when considered together, provide evidence that the brain is mounting a physiological response to a glycemic challenge, a response that has spatiotemporal components that can be reliably tracked.

4.1.1. Phospho-ERK1/2 as Rapid Trackers of Neuronal Activation

As with most phenomenological concepts in science ([120] (pp. 3–5)), the term ‘activation’ means different things under different contexts. In the brain, activation could refer to any of the myriad changes in structural or functional conditions, including, to name just a few, neuronal firing rates, the intracellular Ca2+ metabolism, neurotransmission, gene transcription, and protein translation or post-translational modifications. Whether such measures of activation always reflect relevant physiological changes underlying a given function or set of functions has been the subject of much discussion [77,78,121,122,123,124,125,126].
Here, we refer to ‘activation’ as a change in the phosphorylation status of mitogen-activated protein (MAP) kinases in RB neurons in association with 2-DG treatment. MAP kinases (EC 2.7.11.24, https://iubmb.qmul.ac.uk/enzyme/EC2/7/11/24.html) comprise protein kinases catalyzing a hierarchically-organized regulatory cascade of phosphorylation events [127]. Components of the ERK subfamily of MAP kinases have been localized within the RB structures we examined here, including the LC, NTS, and DMX [128]. Phosphorylation of ERK1/2 kinases at residues within a Thr–Xaa–Tyr dual-phosphorylation motif in the enzyme activation loop (in human: Thr202 and Tyr204; in rat: Thr183 and Tyr185) has been demonstrated to be essential for catalytic activity [129]. The use of phospho-specific antibodies against MAP kinase is an established method of indirectly identifying a catalytically active enzyme [130], although the phosphorylated residues bound and detected by these antibodies are technically products of phosphorylation reactions catalyzed by the MAP kinases hierarchically upstream of the immunolabeled enzyme [130].
Importantly, although we define activation here in terms of phosphorylation state changes, we do not measure phosphorylation per se, but the numbers of neurons displaying changes in immunoreactive signal for phosphorylated MAP kinases. The phospho-specific antibodies are selective for labeling only the phosphorylated and not the unphosphorylated forms of the enzyme. Thus, this labeling reflects activation of a pool of MAP kinase molecules, as observed from accumulated immunoreactive signals in the perikaryon, and at times the nucleus, of the imaged cell profile. The resulting immunoreactive signal, which varied in intensity between saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects and across regions, was not quantitatively compared here. Rather, the numbers of perikaryon profiles displaying immunoreactive signal above background levels were counted as a means to assess population-level recruitment of cells.
We observed greater numbers of phospho-ERK1/2+ neuronal profiles in the LC at level 51 and NTSm at levels 67 and 69 in 2-DG-treated subjects relative to saline-treated control subjects. The physiological significance of this alteration in the numbers of neurons immunoreactive for phospho-ERK1/2 is not known at this time. However, we infer from these changes that greater numbers of neurons are being ‘recruited’ to perform physiologically relevant activities in the face of this glycemic challenge. In support of this view, three lines of evidence suggest that the increases reflect a change in the overall signaling status of LC and NTSm neurons to initiate de novo changes in gene expression and/or changes in neuronal firing rates in response to 2-DG administration. These sets of evidence will be considered in order of their decreasing relevance to the context of glycemic challenge.
First, we have previously shown that, in association with glycemic challenges such as intravenous delivery of insulin or 2-DG, phospho-ERK1/2 reliably tracks cellular activation within the paraventricular hypothalamic nucleus (>1840) (PVH) and arcuate hypothalamic nucleus (>1840) [67,68,69,70,72]. Furthermore, under these conditions, at least where we have established it for the parvicellular division (>1840) of the PVH, phospho-ERK1/2 not only tracks cellular activation during these challenges, but likely plays a causal role in downstream processes engaged by these challenges, such as the recruitment of transcription factors, induction of certain gene expression patterns, and increases in neuronal firing rates [70,73]. Thus, it seems reasonable to consider the possibility that, under the same glycemic challenges, the phospho-ERK1/2 observed in RB neurons may also serve similar functions. However, the links in the chain of evidence required to establish a causal role for phospho-ERK1/2 in RB neuronal responses to glycemic challenge, as it has been established for PVH responses to these challenges [71], have yet to be forged.
Second, outside of our studies of phospho-ERK1/2 in association with glycemic challenge in the RB, these enzymes have been studied under other contexts in this subdivision of the brain. For example, in the LC, ERK1/2 tracks cellular activation associated with pain-related anxiety in male arthritic rats [131] and appears to contribute to tonic inhibition of LC neurons under certain experimental conditions [132]. Optogenetic stimulation of hypocretin/orexin-expressing neurons in the hypothalamus triggers elevations in biochemically detectable phospho-ERK1 and 2 in LC tissue homogenates [133]. In the NTS, phospho-ERK1/2 mediates the suppressive effects of cholecystokinin or a melanocortin receptor agonist on food intake [95,96,134]. In other RB regions, including those that correspond to the functionally defined rostral ventrolateral medulla, the ERK pathway is involved in urotensin-induced increases in sympathetic vasomotor tone [135]. Thus, throughout portions of the RB, phosphorylated ERK1/2 appear to participate within causal chains of signal transduction that link stimuli to appropriate physiological responses. This line of evidence, therefore, supports a similar role for ERK1/2 under conditions of glycemic challenge.
Finally, the more general question of whether phospho-ERK1/2 elevations correlate with other increases in ‘activation’, such as elevated firing rates for neurons or increased signaling cascades leading to gene expression, has been systematically addressed by Fields and colleagues in a number of incisive studies using dorsal root ganglion neurons as a model system. From these studies, they have established that ERK1/2 is tuned to track specific patterns of action potential firing with a fairly high fidelity and coordinates transcriptional networks [136,137,138,139].
Taken together, these three lines of evidence offer us with independent but conceptually integrable sets of experimental results suggesting that the phosphoryl modifications of ERK1/2 kinases, under these experimental conditions, constitute an intracellular biochemical process that rapidly and reliably tracks our intervention. Moreover, they support the notion that not only are they tracking our intervention, but they could be involved in mediating the physiologically meaningful responses to it. Having established this framework for our measured response, it is the nature of the intervention we used which concerns us next.

4.1.2. Intravenous 2-Deoxy-d-Glucose as a Physiologically Relevant Glycemic Challenge

While phospho-ERK1/2 is well-positioned as a reliable readout of glycemic challenge, is 2-DG a physiologically relevant stimulus for producing such a challenge? Historically, Warburg’s discovery of aerobic glycolysis in tumors [140,141] contributed to the great interest in utilizing glycolysis inhibitors, such as 2-deoxy-d-glucose (2-DG), as a means to impair tumor glucose utilization (e.g., [142,143]), an interest that led to its use as a treatment for cancer patients [144]. This strategy of impairing glucose utilization also found currency in other clinical domains, most notably in using 2-DG to assess physiological counterregulatory responses in humans [145] and other animals [146,147,148]. At the scale of the intact living organism, 2-DG has also been used as a means to experimentally induce food intake [74,148,149,150,151,152,153], a behavioral response that is influenced by time-of-day [151], diet [152], and nutritional status [153]. Notwithstanding that feeding and glucoregulatory control circuits are likely components of overlapping but nonredundant neural systems, the effects of 2-DG on both counterregulatory processes and food intake have been established well enough to provide justification of its continued utility for exploring these systems at this scale.
This assertion is further supported by studies of 2-DG that have been performed at the cellular or cell-free (isolated biochemical) scales. In particular, enzyme kinetics studies, each conducted under distinct experimental starting conditions, provide a striking convergence of circumstantial or direct evidence that 2-DG is an inhibitor of glycolysis [154,155,156,157]. The main nodal points for direct 2-DG binding—and the inhibition of glycolysis it causes—appear to be at reaction steps catalyzed by hexokinase (EC 2.7.1.1, https://iubmb.qmul.ac.uk/enzyme/EC2/7/1/1.html) [154,156,157] and glucose-6-phosphate isomerase (GPI; EC 5.3.1.9, https://iubmb.qmul.ac.uk/enzyme/EC5/0301p.html#0109) [155]. Although 2-DG has been empirically determined to bind with a lower affinity to brain hexokinase than glucose does (in rat: Km = 1.1 × 10−4 mol/L for 2-DG vs. 4.5 × 10−5 mol/L for glucose [158]), Lineweaver–Burk [159] analyses applied to assay measurements of hexokinase kinetics revealed that 2-DG serves as a competitive inhibitor of the enzyme in the presence of varying concentrations of glucose, but not Mg-ATP [156]. Kidney-derived GPI, in contrast, has been shown in a cell-free system to be a principal target of 2-DG-6-PO4 [155], which is produced by hexokinase in the presence of 2-DG. Taken together, the data indicate that there are multiple primary modes to block glycolysis by 2-DG. Moreover, allosteric feedback inhibition of hexokinase by 2-DG-6-PO4 could serve as a secondary block of the pathway [160]. Targeted inhibition at these steps appears to be reflected in the disposition of glucose utilization in vivo [160], and hexokinase isoforms of the glucokinase family (EC 2.7.1.2, https://iubmb.qmul.ac.uk/enzyme/EC2/0701a.html#2) are expressed in the brain and have recently been found to be critical for glucoprivic feeding triggered by 2-DG [161]. Thus, biochemical and physiological evaluations of 2-DG actions support its role in impairing glucose utilization.
Yet, despite the evidence for its direct role as a glycolysis inhibitor, evidence has also been gradually accumulating for more complex actions of 2-DG that occur alongside and perhaps as a consequence of its actions on intermediary metabolism. Early work using isolated peripheral nerve preparations demonstrated an inhibitory effect of 2-DG on electrogenic activity of the sodium–potassium pump in the firing nerve [162,163], and 2-DG appears to have observable effects on neural electrical activity more centrally by triggering extrasynaptic GABA release, which results in tonic GABA currents in central neurons at the network level and the inhibition of KATP channels in single neurons [164]. These observations have important clinical implications for individuals who are placed on a ketogenic diet to treat seizures [165]. Thus, aside from and/or perhaps as a result of 2-DG actions on glycolysis, neuronal and circuit-level electrophysiology is altered in the brain. Considered together with recently reported observations that brain endothelial cells undergo aerobic glycolysis (as described above for tumors) and that 2-DG can alter blood–brain-barrier transcellular transport mechanisms and permeability in these cells [166], it would appear that the effects of systemic 2-DG injection are diverse and complex. While here we have briefly just enumerated those effects that still fall within the contexts of glycemic challenge and inhibition of metabolism, we now consider below effects that appear somewhat distinct from these contexts.

4.1.3. Stimulus-Response Coupling: 2-DG Coupling with Phospho-ERK1/2 and Alternate Coupling Relationships and Associations

In this study, we provide evidence of an association between 2-DG administration and increases in the numbers of observable cells in our sample that display immunoreactive phospho-ERK1/2. There are compelling reasons, discussed above, supporting the physiological relevance of using 2-DG as a glycemic challenge and the reliability of the ensuing phospho-ERK1/2 responses to track this challenge. Nevertheless, invoking cause–effect relationships between external stimuli and intracellular signaling cascades is challenging (e.g., see Ref. [167]), and the linkage between the two in our study awaits further strengthening and should be considered here as an initial characterization of a stimulus-response relationship. Many questions remain. For example, 2-DG is a mannose, and as such can profoundly affect cells by altering their complement of glycosylated proteins [168,169], which could alter the sensitivity or responses of cells being studied here. Thus, 2-DG could be coupled to glycosylation signaling processes independent of its glycolytic targets or downstream effects on electrical signaling or blood–brain-barrier permeability. Moreover, 2-DG can trigger a mild hypothermia and hypotension when given systemically [170,171], which are responses we did not measure in our study. It is not clear if these physiological changes engage the same rhombic brain (His, 1893) [21] (RB) neuronal populations that glycemic challenge does, although one laboratory has insightfully examined the populations engaged by hypotension and glucoprivation ([171]; see Section 4.2 for more on this).
Along similar lines, the argument that 2-DG can trigger multiple physiological conditions finds a counterpoint from our observations, thus far reported only in preliminary form [83,85], that phospho-ERK1/2 can be triggered in the same RB regions by multiple glycemic challenges. Specifically, we have reported that intravenous injection of insulin can trigger phospho-ERK1/2 in the LC [83] much as 2-DG does in the present study. These findings provide compelling support for the idea that the coupling of 2-DG to phospho-ERK1/2 is encoding a physiological response to changes in glycemic status. Our work also builds upon earlier work conducted by the Watts laboratory demonstrating that numbers of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive neurons are elevated in the NTS and DMX in association with low concentrations of blood glucose and also exhibit a time-of-day difference in peak expression [69]. Thus, phospho-ERK1/2 does appear to track closely with glycemic challenges in portions of the RB, although it is now clear that there are other key molecules that may contribute to glucosensing or mark the presence of glucosensing neurons (e.g., AMP kinase [172,173]; GLUT2 transporter [28]; phospho-TH [174]). The activation of these molecules is likely only salient in specific places in the RB and under varying conditions, encoding glycemic and other processes in a manner germane to the subsystems the molecules serve. One could imagine that they essentially represent context-specific combinatorial codes—an emergent “biodigital jazz” [175] that manifests as a dynamic encoding of ever-changing events (also see Refs. [78,139,176]). We now discuss the physiological importance of such encoding in the RB in relation to our results.

4.1.4. Physiological Significance

Keeping in mind the previous discussion concerning the reliability of our measured readout, the relevance of 2-DG as a glycemic challenge, and how well the two might be coupled, we turn now to consider the functional importance of our results. First, we consider the rapid recruitment of LC neurons observed after 2-DG challenge. Given the role the LC plays in controlling wakefulness, arousal, and cognition (e.g., Refs. [177,178]), this recruitment can be integrated conceptually within the broader framework of producing or resetting the arousal state during glycemic challenge, perhaps as a means to assist the organism in the awareness of nutrient and/or energy deficit. If true, this may reflect a recruitment of a set of LC-connected circuits that allows the organism to mount a larger-scale resetting of the behavioral state, in parallel to the circuits transmitting signals encoding glucosensing and counterregulatory responses being handled also or exclusively by other RB structures, such as the NTS and DMX. Such functional dichotomy may help explain observations that hypoglycemia unawareness can be reversed in insulin-dependent diabetics without effectively reversing impaired glucose counterregulation [179]. Perhaps the neural systems producing these distinct clinical signs of energy deficit are also distinct, or at least partly so. It is clear, however, from recent evidence for a modular organization of neural elements in the LC [180,181] that this structure is well-suited to handling both sets of information to effect both outcomes. That the LC can receive neurally-encoded information about glucose deficit is supported by electrophysiological evidence in the mouse that neurons in the rostral ventrolateral medulla, known to be activated by hypoglycemia and many other physiological conditions, can monosynaptically activate LC neurons by releasing glutamate [182]. Moreover, cellular activation in the LC has been tracked using Fos expression following systemic or fourth ventricular injection of insulin or 2-DG [36,38,183,184,185]; but see Ref. [186], and glycemic challenges are also associated with altered LC single-unit activity [187], tyrosine hydroxylase expression [188] and phosphorylation [189] (but see Ref. [174]), and norepinephrine transporter expression [190]. Insulin administration is also associated with decreased glucose utilization in the LC, but evidence indicates that this may be a direct effect of insulin itself and not the hypoglycemia produced by insulin [185,191,192].
However, the LC also figures prominently in a bevy of studies demonstrating that its neurons are highly sensitive to perturbations caused by hypoxia or hypercapnia, adjusting their excitability to conserve energy under such conditions (e.g., [193,194,195,196,197]). The literature therefore indicates that LC neurons may be multimodally responsive to a variety of physiological perturbations, not just those that affect glycemic status (see also Ref. [185]). The elevated numbers of neurons immunolabeled for phospho-ERK1/2 in the LC of 2-DG-treated subjects may reflect a generalized mechanism of activation for the neurons of this structure, one that is not necessarily encoding signals exclusive to changes in glycemic status.
This may also be true for the NTSm. As with cells in the LC, cells within this structure appear to be responsive to hypoxia and hypercapnia [194]. On the other hand, much evidence also indicates that NTS neurons actively respond to changes in glycemic status [23,24,25,26,27,28,29,31,198,199]. They possess ATP-sensitive potassium channels which appear to confer glucosensing capabilities to them [198]. These neurons depolarize in response to low extracellular glucose, but not in the presence of high glucose [199]. Thus, the phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactivity we observed in NTS perikaryon profiles may reflect activation of glucosensing elements. Some of these profiles did not display immunoreactivities for either ChAT or DBH in the NTS and are therefore members of one or more additional phenotypes that are known to populate this region [200]. It is also unclear if these are neuronal profiles; indeed, the NTS has astrocytes critical for glucosensing, counterregulatory responses and other functions [26,27,46] (reviewed in Refs. [201,202]).
Finally, as we close out our discussion of functional considerations concerning our findings, several open questions remain about the roles of these activated regions for larger processes of metabolic regulation and feeding control. For example, since the exact organization of the extended networks served by these activated regions remains unknown at the present time, it remains unclear whether there are subsystems that allow for the body to adequately switch its anaerobic versus oxidative capacity [140]. Nor is it clear whether this switch in metabolism in the face of glycemic challenge allows the organism to switch macronutrient diets accordingly [45] and to tailor its ability to counterregulate and/or ingest additional food to gain energy.

4.2. Anatomical Considerations

In this study, we identified the LC at level 51, and the NTSm and DMX at levels 67 and 69, as harboring elevated numbers of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive neurons, which were identified as being either catecholaminergic or non-catecholaminergic; these neurons have been implicated in maintaining the arousal state, autonomic control, and/or nutrient sensing [203,204]. Below, we discuss these principal findings in relation to our methodologies and also in relation to the known anatomy of the RB.

4.2.1. Standardized Mapping of Activation Patterns in the Brain

A key goal of this study was to build upon and expand the prevailing literature by explicitly mapping patterns of neuronal activation to glycemic challenge using a standardized rat brain atlas. As we have argued previously [79,102,205,206], the use of a standardized atlas to formally map cellular distributions affords investigators with the ability to examine multiple spatial datasets in a form that facilitates their direct comparison. We opted, as with all our spatial mapping efforts for the rat (e.g., Refs. [102,205,207]), to utilize the open-access rat brain atlas of Swanson [81], which is accompanied by a defined nomenclature that is traceable to the primary literature.
By mapping the patterns of immunoreactivity that we observed to this reference space, we can align and interrelate our datasets with those of our own and of other laboratories that have mapped their datasets to the same atlas. For example, the Watts laboratory has shown [69] that at plasma glucose levels < 3.15 mM, increased numbers of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive cells are present within the NTS and the DMX at rostrocaudal locations that fall within levels 65–66, 68–69, and 69–70 of the Brain Maps 4.0 atlas [81]. Importantly, at plasma glucose levels > 3.15 mM, the numbers of cells that display such immunoreactivity were much fewer by comparison. In the present study, 2-DG was utilized as the glycemic challenge, which increases plasma glucose levels while decreasing glucose utilization. Consistent with the findings of the Watts laboratory, then, the activation we observed at these atlas levels may be attributable to decreases in glucose utilization rather than hyperglycemia per se.
Moreover, other activation has been noted and mapped at or near to the atlas levels we report here. For example, peripheral injection of the GLP-1 receptor agonist, exendin-4, is associated with Fos expression in the rat LC and NTSm in neuronal populations mapped to atlas levels 52, 66, 70, and 71 [208]. On those same levels, GLP-1 immunoreactive cells and fibers were also mapped and localized to NTS subregions and surrounding areas that were largely nonoverlapping with the Fos-immunoreactive neurons. Similarly, we observed increases in Fos-immunoreactive NTSm neurons (and notably, virtually no DMX neurons) at atlas level 69 in animals allowed to feed for 2 h after an imposed 40 hr fast, as compared to subjects that were fasted for 40 h but not allowed to feed again [102]. This level of the NTSm is the same level in the present study where we observed neurons to be recruited by our glycemic challenge. Thus, data gathered by our laboratory from two separate studies indicate that the NTSm, at atlas level 69, contains neurons responsive to refeeding or glycemic challenge. It remains to be determined whether the same neurons can be activated in this region by both perturbations.

4.2.2. Alignment of Activation Patterns with Connectivity Data for the Brain

The patterns of activation that we document here can also be placed in register with known neural connection patterns established by neuroanatomical tract-tracing experiments that have been mapped using the Brain Maps 4.0 [81] framework or its earlier iterations. For example, the rat LC at level 51 reportedly receives input from a brain region, designated as MnPO by the authors [209], which corresponds essentially identically with the median preoptic nucleus (Loo, 1931) [210] (MEPO) in Brain Maps 4.0 [81]. Using the BM4.0 framework, these investigators carefully charted the axonal boutons and terminal fields of anterogradely labeled fibers from injections of 10-kDa biotinylated dextran amine (BDA) [211] into this structure. Clearly identifiable axons and boutons at the light-microscope level were observed in the LC (see Figure 7a–c in Ref. [209]), which were found to closely appose tyrosine hydroxylase-immunoreactive cells of the LC (see their Figure 7a,b). These projections were displayed on digital templates accompanying the second edition of Brain Maps [212], and their existence was confirmed by injection of a retrograde tracer into the LC. The retrograde pattern of labeling, interestingly, revealed labeled cells not only in the MEPO, but also in extrahypothalamic regions, including the bed nuclei of terminal stria, anterior division, oval nucleus (Ju and Swanson, 1989) [213] (BSTov; see their Figure 8f’), a region which has been previously reported in preliminary communications [78,214,215] as also displaying elevations of phospho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive neurons following 2-DG administration. Collectively, these nascent observations would indicate that the atlas-based mapping of MEPO → LC and BSTov → LC connections, together with our atlas-based mapping of BSTov [78,214,215] and LC activation (Refs. [83,84,85,86,87,88,89]; the present study) in association with 2-DG treatment, reveals a larger set of neural connections that may participate in relaying descending information to the LC that could be salient during glycemic challenge. This is also supported by evidence for a set of connections to the LC, albeit sparse, that have been reported from the lateral hypothalamic area, juxtaventromedial region, dorsal zone (Swanson, 2004) [216] LHAjvd [217]. These connections, however, invest the NTS more heavily, as shown in maps of regions of the NTS slightly more caudal to the level we mapped here (see their Figure 4, panels AAA and BBB).
Although the connections between peripheral organs and neuronal populations in the RB have been intensively studied, few have traced and mapped these connections in the rat using the Brain Maps atlas framework in its various editions. However, Garcia-Luna et al. [76] have provided an elegant demonstration of the power of atlas-based mapping using this spatial model to help register peripheral nervous system connections with neuronal populations in the RB. Specifically, these colleagues have shown that vagal sensory endings innervating the wall of the hepatic portal and superior mesenteric veins make multi-order connections with neuronal populations in the dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX). Furthermore, they showed that injections of viral tracer into the left nodose ganglion (>1840) of rats produced transsynaptic labeling that, as a function of survival time, progressively reached NTS glial cells (24 h) and AP, NTS, and DMX neurons (48 and 72 h).

4.3. Limitations of this Study

In this study, we provided new mapping and atlas-based localization of neuronal populations activated within 15 min of an intravenous delivery of a glycemic challenge. This activation is defined in this study as the elevations in the numbers of perikaryon (cell body) profiles immunoreactive for phospho-ERK1/2. A few caveats must be kept in mind regarding the use of this marker to track activation. First, unlike in other systems [70,71,73,95,96,135,218], phospho-ERK1/2 has not yet been established to be within the causal chain of events that link systemic glycemic challenge to hypoglycemic counterregulation at the level of RB. Second, it is not clear whether the activation observed is a result of neural inputs triggering network activity and/or results from a direct action of 2-DG on the cells themselves. The latter possibility is certainly plausible, considering that peak accumulation of [14C]2-DG in rat brain tissues is observed in as little as five minutes following its intravenous administration (e.g., see Figure 5 in Ref. [219]). Finally, only a single time point was examined in this study, and this only in very circumscribed portions of the LC, NTS, and DMX. High-throughput cell counting methods involving deep learning algorithms and computer vision, such as those that we are developing [220], could streamline analytical workflow procedures to allow for a broader spatiotemporal survey of brain-wide activation patterns following glycemic challenge.

5. Conclusions

The results of this study provide new evidence that there are rapidly activating neurons, and perhaps glial elements, that are engaged within 15 min of glycemic challenge in neurons of the LC and NTSm. Some, but not all, of the neurons are catecholaminergic. Importantly, we mapped these neurons to standardized atlas templates from Brain Maps 4.0, an open-access rat brain atlas that allows for data from multiple labs to be compared with one another. We discuss how our datasets match up with those showing neural connections atlas-mapped to these regions from the hypothalamus or periphery. The new maps furnished in this study constitute an important step towards a standardized global mapping and brain-wide assessment of cellular responses to glycemic challenges, which may allow for more refined clinical or therapeutic interventions for those individuals experiencing complications as a result of iatrogenic hypoglycemia and/or impaired glucose utilization.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/jcm12072483/s1, Figure S1: Relative percent differences of perikaryon phenotypes in locus ceruleus (Wenzel & Wenzel, 1912) [82] (LC) region in saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects; Figure S2: Activation of nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS) and relative percent differences of NTS perikaryon phenotypes in saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects; Figure S3. Relative percent differences of perikaryon phenotypes in BM4.0 atlas level 67-registered dorsal motor nucleus of vagus (>1840) (DMX) in saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects; Figure S4. Activation of the nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS) and relative percent changes of perikaryon profile phenotypes in BM4.0 atlas level 69-registered NTS 15 min after 2-DG administration; Figure S5. Relative percent changes of perikaryon phenotypes in the BM4.0 atlas level 69-registered dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX) 15 min after 2-DG administration; Table S1: Summary of descriptive statistics for LC perikaryon profiles at Atlas Level 51; Table S2. Summary of non-parametric test statistics and effect sizes for LC perikaryon profiles at Atlas Level 51; Table S3. Summary of descriptive statistics for NTS and DMX perikaryon profiles at Atlas Level 67; Table S4. Summary of non-parametric test statistics and effect sizes for NTS and DMX perikaryon profiles at Atlas Level 67; Table S5. Summary of descriptive statistics for NTS and DMX perikaryon profiles at atlas level 69; Table S6. Summary of non-parametric test statistics and effect sizes for NTSm/DMX perikaryon profiles at Atlas Level 69; File S1. Supplemental Methods.

Author Contributions

Experimental conception and design: A.M.K. Project management: A.M.K., R.H.T., G.P.T. and S.B. Surgeries and in vivo experiments: L.J.A. and A.M.K. Perfusions and histology: L.J.A., A.A., S.D.C. and G.S. Tissue inventory: A.M.K., G.P.T., A.A. and S.D.C. Immunohistochemistry: S.D.C. and V.I.N. Microscopy and imaging: G.P.T., A.M.K. and S.D.C. Cytoarchitectonic analysis and atlas-based mapping: G.P.T. and J.V.S.P. Quantitative and exploratory data analyses: S.B., G.P.T., J.V.S.P. and A.M.K. Artboard construction: A.M.K., G.P.T., V.I.N. and S.B. Research funding procurement: A.M.K. and R.H.T. Research supervision and training: A.M.K., G.P.T., R.H.T., S.B., L.J.A. and V.I.N. Conference presenters: G.P.T., J.V.S.P., S.D.C., and A.A./G.S. Manuscript preparation: A.M.K., G.P.T., S.B. and V.I.N., with final checks from all co-authors. Manuscript revisions: A.M.K. and G.P.T. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by funds awarded to A.M.K. from the National Institutes of Health (NIH) (DK081937, GM109817, GM127251). L.J.A. and A.A. were supported by awards from the USC Undergraduate Research Assistance Program (awarded to A.M.K., and jointly to A.M.K. and R.H.T.) and the Rose Hills Foundation (to A.M.K.). S.D.C. was supported, in part, by the NIH-funded UTEP SMART MIND program (R25DA033613; PI: L. E. O’Dell). G.P.T. and J.V.S.P. are 2022 Fellows of the UTEP ASPIRE Program funded by the National Science Foundation (PI: B. Flores; Co-PIs: M. O. Montes, J. I. Villalobos). This project was also supported by the NIH-funded Border Biomedical Research Center at UTEP (U54MD007592).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The animal study protocol was approved by the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of The University of Southern California (protocol code 11249, approved on 26 August 2009).

Data Availability Statement

All relevant protocols and data are contained within the article, along with its Supplemental Files.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Alan G. Watts and Graciela Sanchez-Watts (USC) for allowing the generous use of their surgical and laboratory spaces during the early stages of this project, Larry W. Swanson (USC) and Cathleen Crayton for facilitating weekly discussions of our group in Swanson’s research space and for the use of his laboratory for tissue processing, and Ted Berger (USC) for use of his laboratory. Tiffanie Nham, Michael Zobel, and Jordan Michaels participated in valuable discussions. Jeannie Y. Zhang, Danielle Goodrich, Michael Zobel, Ellen M. Walker, Evangelina Espinoza, Kenichiro Negishi, and Marina Peveto provided valuable technical assistance or datasets, and Anne Jokiaho (USC) graciously facilitated access to legacy records at USC. AMK and RHT thank David Glasgow in the USC Undergraduate Education Office for his support of our students. We also thank Armando Varela-Ramirez for his assistance in the Cellular Characterization and Biorepository Core Facility of the UTEP Border Biomedical Research Center and Larry W. Swanson for clarifications concerning his atlas nomenclature. All BM4.0 atlas maps were modified and/or reproduced with permission under the conditions outlined by a Creative Commons BY-NC 4.0 license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/4.0/). The authors express their appreciation for the valuable digital resources provided by the Internet Archive and The University and State Library of Saxony-Anhalt in Halle, Germany. AMK and GPT dedicate this study to the memory of the late Sue Ritter (Washington State University) for her pioneering and systematic investigations of hindbrain mechanisms of glucoregulatory control.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

Abbreviations

APArea postrema (>1840)
BSTovBed nuclei of terminal stria, anterior division, oval nucleus (Ju and Swanson, 1989) [211]
CCentral canal (Bellingeri, 1823) [221]
CmdCentral canal of medulla (>1840)
DMXDorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840)
htHypoglossal triangle (>1840)
LCLocus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1812) [82]
LDTLaterodorsal tegmental nucleus (>1840)
MEPOMedian preoptic nucleus (Loo, 1931) [210]
MEVMidbrain nucleus of trigeminal (>1840)
MYMedulla (Winslow, 1733) [42]
NTSNucleus of solitary tract (>1840)
NTSceNucleus of solitary tract, central subzone (>1840)
NTScoNucleus of solitary tract, commissural part (>1840)
NTSgeNucleus of solitary tract, gelatinous subzone (>1840)
NTSlNucleus of solitary tract, Lateral part (>1840)
NTSmNucleus of solitary tract, Medial part (>1840)
NTSmcNucleus of solitary tract, Medial part, Caudal zone general, caudal subzone (>1840)
NTSmcNucleus of solitary tract, Medial part, Caudal zone general (>1840)
PBParabrachial nucleus (>1840)
PCGPontine central gray (>1840)
PCGgPontine central gray general (Swanson, 2004) [216]
RBRhombic brain (His, 1893) [21]
scpSuperior cerebellar peduncle (Procháska, 1800) [222]
slmLimiting sulcus of fourth ventricle (>1840)
tsSolitary tract(>1840)
V4fFloor of fourth ventricle (Reil, 1807–1808) [223]
vtVagal triangle (>1840)
XIIHypoglossal nucleus (>1840)
zNucleus z (>1840)

References

  1. Cryer, P.E. Banting Lecture: Hypoglycemia: The limiting factor in the management of IDDM. Diabetes 1994, 43, 1378–1389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Hirsch, I.B. Insulin analogues. N. Engl. J. Med. 2005, 352, 174–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Leese, G.P.; Wang, J.; Broomhall, J.; Kelly, P.; Marsden, A.; Morrison, W.; Brian, M.; Frier, M.D.; Andrew, D.; Morris, M.D.; et al. Frequency of severe hypoglycemia requiring emergency treatment in type 1 and type 2 diabetes: A population-based study of health service resource use. Diabetes Care 2003, 26, 1176–1180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Cryer, P.E. Mechanisms of sympathoadrenal failure and hypoglycemia in diabetes. J. Clin. Investig. 2006, 116, 1470–1473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Cryer, P.E. Hypoglycemia: Still the limiting factor in the glycemic management of diabetes. Endocr. Pract. 2008, 14, 750–756. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Cryer, P.E. Hierarchy of physiological responses to hypoglycemia: Relevance to clinical hypoglycemia in Type I (Insulin Dependent) Diabetes Mellitus. Horm. Metab. Res. 1997, 29, 92–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Cryer, P.E. Mechanisms of hypoglycemia-associated autonomic failure and its component syndromes in diabetes. Diabetes 2005, 54, 3592–3601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Smith, D.; Amiel, S.A. Hypoglycaemia unawareness and the brain. Diabetologia 2002, 45, 949–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Senthilkumaran, M.; Zhou, X.-F.; Bobrovskaya, L. Challenges in modelling hypoglycaemia-associated autonomic failure: A review of human and animal studies. Int. J. Endocrinol. 2016, 9801640. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. McCrimmon, R. Consequences of recurrent hypoglycaemia on brain function in diabetes. Diabetologia 2021, 64, 971–977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Hevener, A.L.; Bergman, R.N.; Donovan, C.M. Portal vein afferents are critical for the sympathoadrenal response to hypoglycemia. Diabetes 2000, 49, 8–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Frizzell, R.T.; Jones, E.M.; Davis, S.N.; Biggers, D.W.; Myers, S.R.; Connolly, S.C.; Neal, D.; Jaspan, J.; Cherrington, A. Counterregulation during hypoglycemia is directed by widespread brain regions. Diabetes 1993, 42, 1253–1261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Routh, V.H.; Donovan, C.M.; Ritter, S. Hypoglycemia detection. Transl. Endocrinol. Metab. 2012, 3, 47–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Tesfaye, N.; Seaquist, E.R. Neuroendocrine responses to hypoglycemia. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2010, 1212, 12–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Watts, A.G.; Kanoski, S.E.; Sanchez-Watts, G.; Langhans, W. The physiological control of eating: Signals, neurons, and networks. Physiol. Rev. 2022, 102, 689–813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Carus, C.G. Versuch einer Darstellung des Nervensystems und Insbesondere des Gehirns nach ihrer Bedeutung, Entwickelung und Vollendung im thierischen Organismus; Breitkopf und Härtel: Leipzig, Germany, 1814; Available online: https://archive.org/details/b24918131 (accessed on 1 March 2023).
  17. Ritter, S.; Li, A.J.; Wang, Q.; Dinh, T.T. The value of looking backward: The essential role of the hindbrain in counterregulatory responses to glucose deficit. Endocrinology 2011, 152, 4019–4032. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Donovan, C.M.; Watts, A.G. Peripheral and central glucose sensing in hypoglycemic detection. Physiology 2014, 29, 314–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Chan, O.; Sherwin, R.S. Is there cross talk between portal and hypothalamic glucose-sensing circuits? Diabetes 2014, 63, 2617–2619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Verberne, A.J.M.; Sabetghadam, A.; Korim, W.S. Neural pathways that control the glucose counterregulatory response. Front. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  21. His, W. Vorschläge zur Eintheilung des Gehirns. In Archiv für Anatomie und Entwickelungsgeschichte; Veit et Comp.: Leipzig, Germany, 1893; pp. 172–179. Available online: https://play.google.com/books/reader?id=ff7fqzGSX7QC&pg=GBS.PA172&hl=en (accessed on 2 July 2022).
  22. DiRocco, R.J.; Grill, H.J. The forebrain is not essential for sympathoadrenal hyperglycemic response to glucoprivation. Science 1979, 204, 1112–1114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Mizuno, Y.; Oomura, Y. Glucose responding neurons in the nucleus tractus solitarius of the rat: In vitro study. Brain Res. 1984, 307, 109–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Dallaporta, M.; Himmi, T.; Perrin, J.; Orsini, J.-C. Solitary tract nucleus sensitivity to moderate changes in glucose level. NeuroReport 1999, 10, 2657–2660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Yettefti, K.; Orsini, J.-C.; Perrin, J. Characteristics of glycemia-sensitive neurons in the nucleus tractus solitarii: Possible involvement in nutritional regulation. Physiol. Behav. 1997, 61, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Marty, N.; Dallaporta, M.; Foretz, M.; Emery, M.; Tarussio, D.; Bady, I.; Binnert, C.; Beermann, F.; Thorens, B. Regulation of glucagon secretion by glucose transporter type 2 (glut2) and astrocyte-dependent glucose sensors. J. Clin. Investig. 2005, 115, 3545–3553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Klip, A.; Hawkins, M. Desperately seeking sugar: Glial cells as hypoglycemia sensors. J. Clin. Investig. 2005, 115, 3403–3405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Lamy, C.M.; Sanno, H.; Labouèbe, G.; Picard, A.; Magnan, C.; Chatton, J.-Y.; Thorens, B. Hypoglycemia-activated GLUT2 neurons of the nucleus tractus solitarius stimulate vagal activity and glucagon secretion. Cell Metab. 2014, 19, 527–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Boychuk, C.R.; Gyarmati, P.; Xu, H.; Smith, B.N. Glucose sensing by GABAergic neurons in the mouse nucleus tractus solitarii. J. Neurophysiol. 2015, 114, 999–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Parker, L.M.; Kumar, N.N.; Lonergan, T.; McMullan, S.; Goodchild, A.K. Distribution and neurochemical characterization of neurons in the rat ventrolateral medulla activated by glucoprivation. Brain Struct. Funct. 2015, 220, 117–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Roberts, B.L.; Zhu, M.; Zhao, H.; Dillon, C.; Appleyard, S.M. High glucose increases action potential firing of catecholamine neurons in the nucleus of the solitary tract by increasing spontaneous glutamate inputs. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2017, 313, R229–R239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  32. Pitra, S.; Smith, B.N. Musings on the wanderer: What’s new in our understanding of vago-vagal reflexes? VI. Central vagal circuits that control glucose metabolism. Am. J. Physiol. Gastrointest. Liver Physiol. 2021, 320, G175–G182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Garfield, A.S.; Shah, B.P.; Madara, J.C.; Burke, L.K.; Patterson, C.M.; Flak, J.; Neve, R.; Evans, M.; Lowell, B.; Myers, M.G., Jr.; et al. A parabrachial-hypothalamic cholecystokinin neurocircuit controls counterregulatory responses to hypoglycemia. Cell Metab. 2014, 20, 1030–1037. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. de Araujo, I.E. Expanding the brain glucosensing territory. Cell Metab. 2014, 20, 933–935. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Kakall, Z.M.; Nedoboy, P.; Farnham, M.M.J.; Pilowsky, P.M. Activation of μ-opioid receptors in the rostral ventrolateral medulla blocks the sympathetic counterregulatory response to glucoprivation. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2018, 315, R1115–R1122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Sato, M.; Minabe, S.; Sakano, T.; Magata, F.; Nakamura, S.; Watanabe, Y.; Inoue, N.; Uenoyama, Y.; Tsukamura, H.; Matsuda, F. Morphological analysis of the hindbrain glucose sensor-hypothalamic neural pathway activated by hindbrain glucoprivation. Endocrinology 2021, 162, bqab125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Ritter, S.; Taylor, J.S. Vagal sensory neurons are required for lipoprivic but not glucoprivic feeding in rats. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 1990, 258, R1395–R1401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Ritter, S.; Dinh, T.T. 2-Mercaptoacetate and 2-deoxy-d-glucose induce Fos-like immunoreactivity in rat brain. Brain Res. 1994, 641, 111–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Ritter, S.; Ritter, J.B.; Cromer, L. 2-deoxy-d-glucose and mercaptoacetate induce different patterns of macronutrient ingestion. Physiol. Behav. 1999, 66, 709–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Ritter, S.; Dinh, T.T.; Zhang, Y. Localization of hindbrain glucoreceptive sites controlling food intake and blood glucose. Brain Res. 2000, 856, 37–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Sanders, N.M.; Ritter, S. Repeated 2-deoxy-d-glucose-induced glucoprivation attenuates Fos expression and glucoregulatory responses during subsequent glucoprivation. Diabetes 2000, 49, 1865–1874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Winslow, J.B. An Anatomical Exposition of the Structure of the Human Body; Translated from the French Original [1732]; Douglas, G., Translator; Prevost: London, UK, 1733; Available online: https://archive.org/details/anatomicalexposi00wins/page/n9/mode/2up (accessed on 4 March 2023).
  43. Ritter, S.; Bugarith, K.; Dinh, T.T. Immunotoxic destruction of distinct catecholamine subgroups produces selective impairment of glucoregulatory responses and neuronal activation. J. Comp. Neurol. 2001, 432, 197–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Ritter, S.; Watts, A.G.; Dinh, T.T.; Sanchez-Watts, G.; Pedrow, C. Immunotoxin lesion of hypothalamically projecting norepinephrine and epinephrine neurons differentially affects circadian and stressor-stimulated corticosterone secretion. Endocrinology 2003, 144, 1357–1367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Li, A.-J.; Wang, Q.; Dinh, T.T.; Wiater, M.F.; Eskelsen, A.K.; Ritter, S. Hindbrain catecholamine neurons control rapid switching of metabolic substrate use during glucoprivation in male rats. Endocrinology 2013, 154, 4570–4579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Rogers, R.C.; McDougal, D.H.; Ritter, S.; Qualls-Creekmore, E.; Hermann, G.E. Response of catecholaminergic neurons in the mouse hindbrain to glucoprivic stimuli is astrocyte dependent. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2018, 315, R153–R164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  47. Li, A.-I.; Wang, Q.; Ritter, S. Activation of catecholamine neurons in the ventral medulla reduces CCK-induced hypophagia and c-Fos activation in dorsal medullary catecholamine neurons. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2018, 315, R442–R452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Ritter, S.; Li, A.-I.; Wang, Q. Hindbrain glucoregulatory mechanisms: Critical role of catecholamine neurons in the ventrolateral medulla. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 208, 112568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Li, A.-I.; Wang, Q.; Ritter, S. Repeated pharmacogenetic catecholamine neuron activation in the ventrolateral medulla attenuates subsequent glucoregulatory responses. Diabetes 2020, 69, 2747–2755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Senthilkumaran, M.; Bobrovskaya, L.; Verberne, A.J.M.; Llewellyn-Smith, I.J. Insulin-responsive autonomic neurons in rat medulla oblongata. J. Comp. Neurol. 2018, 526, 2665–2682. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Jokiaho, A.J.; Donovan, C.M.; Watts, A.G. The rate of fall of blood glucose determines the necessity of forebrain-projecting catecholaminergic neurons for male rat sympathoadrenal responses. Diabetes 2014, 63, 2854–2865. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Kuhlenbeck, H. Vorlesungen über das Zentralnervensystem der Wirbeltiere; Fischer: Jena, Germany, 1927. [Google Scholar]
  53. Oomura, Y.; Ono, T.; Ooyama, H.; Wayner, M.J. Glucose and osmosensitive neurones of the rat hypothalamus. Nature 1969, 222, 282–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Oomura, Y.; Ooyama, H.; Sugimori, H.; Nakamura, T.; Yamada, Y. Glucose inhibition of the glucose-sensitive neurone in the rat lateral hypothalamus. Nature 1974, 247, 284–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Oomura, Y.; Yoshimatsu, H. Neural network of glucose monitoring system. J. Auton. Nerv. Syst. 1984, 10, 359–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Routh, V.H.; Hao, L.; Santiago, A.M.; Sheng, Z.; Zhou, C. Hypothalamic glucose sensing: Making ends meet. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2014, 8, 236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  57. Tu, L.; Fukuda, M.; Tong, Q.; Xu, Y. The ventromedial hypothalamic nucleus: Watchdog of whole-body glucose homeostasis. Cell Biosci. 2022, 12, 71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Zhou, C.; Teegala, S.B.; Khan, B.A.; Gonzalez, C.; Routh, V.H. Hypoglycemia: Role of hypothalamic glucose-inhibited (GI) neurons in detection and correction. Front. Physiol. 2018, 9, 192. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Williams, R.H.; Alexopoulos, H.; Jensen, L.T.; Fugger, L.; Burdakov, D. Adaptive sugar sensors in hypothalamic feeding circuits. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2008, 105, 11975–11980. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Venner, A.; Karnani, M.M.; Gonzalez, J.A.; Jensen, L.T.; Fugger, L.; Burdakov, D. Orexin neurons as conditional glucosensors: Paradoxical regulation of sugar sensing by intracellular fuels. J. Physiol. 2011, 589, 5701–5708. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Melnick, I.V.; Price, C.J.; Colmers, W.F. Glucosensing in parvocellular neurons of the rat paraventricular hypothalamus. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2011, 34, 272–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Foster, N.N.; Azam, S.; Watts, A.G. Rapid-onset hypoglycemia suppresses Fos expression in discrete parts of the ventromedial nucleus of the hypothalamus. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2016, 310, R1177–R1185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Korim, W.S.; Farah, L.B.; McMullan, S.; Verberne, A.J.M. Orexinergic activation of medullary premotor neurons modulates the adrenal sympathoexcitation to hypothalamic glucoprivation. Diabetes 2014, 63, 1895–1906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Sabetghadam, A.; Korim, W.S.; Verberne, A.J. Perifornical hypothalamic pathway to the adrenal gland: Role for glutamatergic transmission in the glucose counterregulatory response. Auton. Neurosci. 2017, 203, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Korim, W.S.; Llewellyn-Smith, I.J.; Verberne, A.J.M. Activation of medulla-projecting perifornical neurons modulates the adrenal sympathetic response to hypoglycemia: Involvement of orexin type 2 (OX2-R) receptors. Endocrinology 2016, 157, 810–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Papazoglou, I.; Lee, J.-H.; Cui, Z.; Li, C.; Fulgenzi, G.; Bahn, Y.J.; Staniszewska-Goraczniak, H.; Piñol, R.; Hogue, I.; Enquist, L.; et al. A distinct hypothalamus-to-β cell circuit modulates insulin secretion. Cell Metab. 2022, 34, 285–298.e7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Khan, A.M.; Watts, A.G. Intravenous 2-deoxy-d-glucose injection rapidly elevates levels of the phosphorylated forms of p44/42 Mitogen-Activated Protein kinases (Extracellularly Regulated Kinases 1/2) in rat hypothalamic parvicellular paraventricular neurons. Endocrinology 2004, 145, 351–359. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Khan, A.M.; Ponzio, T.A.; Sanchez-Watts, G.; Stanley, B.G.; Hatton, G.I.; Watts, A.G. Catecholaminergic control of mitogen-activated protein kinase signaling in paraventricular neuroendocrine neurons in vivo and in vitro: A proposed role during glycemic challenges. J. Neurosci. 2007, 27, 7344–7360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Gorton, L.M.; Khan, A.M.; Bohland, M.; Sanchez-Watts, G.; Donovan, C.M.; Watts, A.G. A role for the forebrain in mediating time-of-day differences in glucocorticoid counterregulatory responses to hypoglycemia. Endocrinology 2007, 148, 6026–6039. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Khan, A.M.; Kaminski, K.L.; Sanchez-Watts, G.; Ponzio, T.A.; Kuzmiski, J.B.; Bains, J.S.; Watts, A.G. MAP kinases couple hindbrain-derived catecholamine signals to hypothalamic adrenocortical control mechanisms during glycemia-related challenges. J. Neurosci. 2011, 31, 18479–18491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  71. Watts, A.G.; Khan, A.M. Identifying links in the chain: The dynamic coupling of catecholamines, peptide synthesis, and peptide release in hypothalamic neuroendocrine neurons. Adv. Pharmacol. 2013, 68, 421–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Khan, A.M.; Walker, E.M.; Dominguez, N.; Watts, A.G. Neural input is critical for arcuate hypothalamic neurons to mount intracellular signaling responses to systemic insulin and deoxyglucose challenges in male rats: Implications for communication within feeding and metabolic control networks. Endocrinology 2014, 155, 405–416. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Khan, A.M.; Walker, E.M.; Watts, A.G. Tracking the coupling of external signals to intracellular programs controlling peptide synthesis and release in hypothalamic neuroendocrine neurons. In Stress: Neuroendocrinology and Neurobiology (Handbook of Stress, Volume 2); Fink, G., Ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2017; pp. 67–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Aklan, I.; Atasoy, N.S.; Yavuz, Y.; Ates, T.; Coban, I.; Koksalar, F.; Filiz, G.; Topcu, I.C.; Oncul, M.; Dilsiz, P.; et al. NTS catecholamine neurons mediate hypoglycemic hunger via medial hypothalamic feeding pathways. Cell Metab. 2020, 31, 313–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Bohland, M.; Matveyenko, A.V.; Saberi, M.; Khan, A.M.; Watts, A.G. Activation of hindbrain neurons is mediated by portal-mesenteric vein glucosensors during slow-onset hypoglycemia. Diabetes 2014, 63, 2866–2875. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  76. Garcia-Luna, C.; Sanchez-Watts, G.; Arnold, M.; de Lartigue, G.; DeWalt, N.; Langhans, W.; Watts, A.G. The medullary targets of neurally conveyed sensory information from the rat hepatic portal and superior mesenteric veins. eNeuro 2021, 8, ENEURO.0419-20.2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Kovács, K.J. c-Fos as a transcription factor: A stressful (re)view from a functional map. Neurochem. Int. 1998, 33, 287–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Watts, A.G.; Khan, A.M.; Sanchez-Watts, G.; Salter, D.; Neuner, C.M. Activation in neural networks controlling ingestive behaviors: What does it mean, and how do we map and measure it? Physiol. Behav. 2006, 89, 501–510. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Khan, A.M. Controlling feeding behavior by chemical and gene-directed targeting in the brain: What’s so spatial about our methods? Front. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  80. Towler, D.A.; Havlin, C.E.; Craft, S.; Cryer, P. Mechanism of awareness of hypoglycemia. Perception of neurogenic (predominantly cholinergic) rather than neuroglycopenic symptoms. Diabetes 1993, 42, 1791–1798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Swanson, L.W. Brain maps 4.0—Structure of the rat brain: An open access atlas with global nervous system nomenclature ontology and flatmaps. J. Comp. Neurol. 2018, 526, 935–943. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  82. Wenzel, J.; Wenzel, K. De Penitiori Structura Cerebri Hominis et Brutorum; Cotta: Tübingen, Germany, 1812; Available online: https://play.google.com/books/reader?id=ff7fqzGSX7QC&pg=GBS.PA172&hl=en (accessed on 5 May 2022).
  83. Khan, A.M. Insulin-induced hypoglycemia is associated with ERK phosphorylation in locus coeruleus neurons: Implications for arousal triggered by metabolic stress. Presented at the Neurobiology of Stress Workshop 2010, Boulder, CO, USA, 15–19 June 2010.
  84. Jayewickreme, C.; Ramchandani, A.; Si, G.; Khan, A.M. Insulin-induced hypoglycemia is associated with ERK phosphorylation in locus coeruleus neurons: Implications for arousal triggered by metabolic stress. In Proceedings of the 2010 Neuroscience Meeting Planner, Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, San Diego, CA, USA, 13–17 November 2010; Society for Neuroscience: Washington, DC, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  85. Chenausky, S.D.; Carreon, T.A.; Khan, A.M. Rapid activation of hindbrain neurons following glycemic challenge: Regions and phenotypes. In COURI Symposium Abstracts, Summer 2012 [Paper 15]; The University of Texas at El Paso: El Paso, TX, USA, 2012; Available online: http://digitalcommons.utep.edu/couri_abstracts_sum12/15 (accessed on 1 March 2023).
  86. Chenausky, S.D.; Ramchandani, A.; Si, G.; Thompson, R.H.; Khan, A.M. Hindbrain autonomic nuclei: High resolution imaging of chemoarchitecture and cellular responses to glycemic challenges. In Proceedings of the 2012 Neuroscience Meeting Planner, Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience New Orleans, LA, USA, 13–17 October 2012; Society for Neuroscience: Washington, DC, USA, 2012; Program No. 93.14. [Google Scholar]
  87. Barnes, J.N.; Molina, D.D.; Tapia, G.P.; Khan, A.M. Atlas-based spatial analysis of rat hindbrain regions that display rapid activation in association with glycemic challenge. In Fall 2019 UTEP COURI Symposium; The University of Texas at El Paso: El Paso, TX, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  88. Tapia, G.P.; Agostinelli, L.J.; Chenausky, S.D.; Barnes, J.; Molina, D.D.; Khan, A.M. Atlas-based spatial analysis of rat hindbrain regions that display rapid activation in association with glycemic challenge. In Proceedings of the 2019 Neuroscience Meeting Planner, Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, Chicago, IL, USA, 19–23 October 2019; Society for Neuroscience: Washington, DC, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  89. Tapia, G.P.; Agostinelli, L.J.; Salcido, J.V.; Esquivel, J.; Khan, A.M. Atlas-based spatial analysis and quantitative assessment of the rat hindbrain regional response to glycemic challenge. Program No. 254.02. Presented at Global Connectome Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, Online, 11–13 January 2021.
  90. Swanson, L.W. Neuroanatomical Terminology: A Lexicon of Classical Origins and Historical Foundations; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  91. Salter, D.; Watts, A.G. Differential suppression of hyperglycemic, feeding, and neuroendocrine responses in anorexia. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2003, 284, R174–R182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  92. Swanson, L.W.; Bota, M. Foundational model of nervous system structural connectivity with a schema for wiring diagrams, connectome, and basic plan architecture. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2010, 107, 20610–20617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  93. Rinaman, L. Postnatal development of catecholamine inputs to the paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus in rats. J. Comp. Neurol. 2001, 438, 411–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Márquez-Ruiz, J.; Morcuende, S.; Navarro-López, J.D.D.; Escudero, M. Anatomical and pharmacological relationship between acetylcholine and nitric oxide in the prepositus hypoglossi nucleus of the cat: Functional implications for eye-movement control. J. Comp. Neurol. 2007, 503, 407–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Sutton, G.M.; Patterson, L.M.; Berthoud, H.-R. Extracellular Signal-Regulated Kinase 1/2 signaling pathway in solitary nucleus mediates cholecystokinin-induced suppression of food intake in rats. J. Neurosci. 2004, 24, 10240–10247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  96. Sutton, G.M.; Duos, B.; Patterson, L.M.; Berthoud, H.-R. Melanocortinergic modulation of cholecystokinin- induced suppression of feeding through Extracellular Signal-Regulated Kinase signaling in rat solitary nucleus. Endocrinology 2005, 146, 3739–3747. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  97. Lenhossék, M. Der Feinere Bau Des Nervensystems im Lichte Neuester Forsuchungen, 2nd ed.; Kornfeld, H: Berlin, Germany, 1895; Available online: https://archive.org/details/derfeinerebaude00lenh/page/n3/mode/2up (accessed on 21 May 2022).
  98. Kiernan, J.A. Classification and naming of dyes, stains and fluorochromes. Biotech. Histochem. 2001, 76, 261–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Airy, G.B. On the diffraction of an object-glass with circular aperture. Trans. Camb. Phil. Soc. 1835, 5, 283–291. Available online: https://archive.org/details/transactionsofca05camb/page/n305/mode/2up (accessed on 2 June 2022).
  100. North, A.J. Seeing is believing? A beginner’s guide to practical pitfalls in image acquisition. J. Cell. Biol. 2006, 172, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  101. Jost, A.P.-T.; Waters, J.C. Designing a rigorous microscopy experiment: Validating methods and avoiding bias. J. Cell Biol. 2019, 218, 1452–1466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  102. Zséli, G.; Vida, B.; Martinez, A.; Lechan, R.M.; Khan, A.M.; Fekete, C. Elucidation of the anatomy of a satiety network: Focus on connectivity of the parabrachial nucleus in the adult rat. J. Comp. Neurol. 2016, 524, 2803–2827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  103. Simmons, D.M.; Swanson, L.W. Comparing histological data from different brains: Sources of error and strategies for minimizing them. Brain Res. Rev. 2009, 60, 349–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Saper, C. Any way you cut it: A new journal policy for the use of unbiased counting methods. J. Comp. Neurol. 1996, 364, 5. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Coggeshall, R.E.; Lekan, H.A. Methods for determining numbers of cells and synapses: A case for more uniform standards of review. J. Comp. Neurol. 1996, 364, 6–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Abercrombie, M. Estimation of nuclear population from microtome sections. Anat. Rec. 1946, 94, 239–247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  107. Shapiro, S.S.; Wilk, M.B. An analysis of variance test for normality (complete samples). Biometrika 1965, 52, 591–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Wilcoxon, F. Individual comparisons by ranking methods. Biometr. Bull. 1945, 1, 80–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Kruskal, W.H. Historical notes on the Wilcoxon unpaired two-sample test. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1957, 52, 356–360. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Benjamini, Y.; Hochberg, Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J. R. Stat. Soc. B 1995, 57, 289–300. Available online: https://www.jstor.org/stable/2346101 (accessed on 1 March 2023). [CrossRef]
  111. R Core Team. R: A language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2019; Available online: https://www.R-project.org/ (accessed on 1 March 2023).
  112. Kassambara, A. rstatix: Pipe-Friendly Framework for Basic Statistical Tests. R Package, Version 0.7.0. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=rstatix (accessed on 1 March 2023).
  113. Wickham, H. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis; Springer-Verlag: New York, NY, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  114. Swanson, L.W. The locus coeruleus: A cytoarchitectonic, Golgi and immunohistochemical study in the albino rat. Brain Res. 1976, 110, 39–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Grzanna, R.; Morrison, J.H.; Coyle, J.T.; Molliver, M.E. The immunohistochemical demonstration of noradrenergic neurons in the rat brain: The use of homologous anti-serum to dopamine-β-hydroxylase. Neurosci. Lett. 1977, 4, 127–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Grzanna, R.; Molliver, M.E.; Coyle, J.T. Visualization of central noradrenergic neurons in thick sections by the unlabeled antibody method: A transmitter-specific Golgi image. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1978, 75, 2502–2506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  117. Cimarusti, D.L.; Saito, K.; Vaughn, J.E.; Barber, R.; Roberts, E.; Thomas, P.E. Immunocytochemical localization of dopamine-β-hydroxylase in rat locus coeruleus and hypothalamus. Brain Res. 1979, 162, 55–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Grzanna, R.; Molliver, M.E. The locus coeruleus in the rat: An immunohistochemical delineation. Neuroscience 1980, 5, 21–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Hökfelt, T.; Johansson, O.; Goldstein, M. Central catecholamine neurons with special reference to adrenaline neurons. In Handbook of Chemical Neuroanatomy, Volume 2. Part 1: Classical Transmitters in the CNS; Björklund, A., Hökfelt, T., Eds.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1984; pp. 157–276. [Google Scholar]
  120. Feynman, R.P. The Pleasure of Finding Things Out: The Best Short Works of Richard P. Feynman; Robbins, J., Ed.; Basic Books: New York, NY, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  121. Hoffman, G.E.; Smith, M.S.; Verbalis, J.G. c-Fos and related immediate early gene products as markers of activity in neuroendocrine systems. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 1993, 14, 173–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  122. Luckman, S.M.; Dyball, R.E.; Leng, G. Induction of c-fos expression in hypothalamic magnocellular neurons requires synaptic activation and not simply increased spike activity. J. Neurosci. 1994, 14, 4825–4830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  123. Bullock, T.H. Signals and signs in the nervous system: The dynamic anatomy of electrical activity is probably information-rich. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1997, 94, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  124. Hoffman, G.E.; Lyo, D. Anatomical markers of activity in neuroendocrine systems: Are we all ‘Fos-ed out’? J. Neuroendocrinol. 2002, 14, 259–268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  125. Guzowski, J.F.; Timlin, J.A.; Roysam, B.; McNaughton, B.L.; Worley, P.F.; Barnes, C.A. Mapping behaviorally relevant neural circuits with immediate early gene expression. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2005, 15, 599–606. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Hudson, A.E. Genetic reporters of neuronal activity: C-Fos and G-CaMP6. Methods Enzymol. 2018, 603, 197–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  127. Chen, Z.; Gibson, T.B.; Robinson, F.; Silvestro, L.; Pearson, G.; Xu, B.; Wright, A.; Vanderbilt, C.; Cobb, M.H. MAP kinases. Chem. Rev. 2001, 101, 2449–2476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  128. Flood, D.G.; Finn, J.P.; Walton, K.M.; Dionne, C.A.; Contreras, P.C.; Miller, M.S.; Bhat, R.V. Immunolocalization of the mitogen-activated protein kinases p42MAPK and JNK1, and their regulatory kinases MEK1 and MEK4, in adult rat central nervous system. J. Comp. Neurol. 1998, 398, 373–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  129. Anderson, N.G.; Maller, J.L.; Tonks, N.K.; Sturgill, T.W. Requirement for integration of signals from two distinct phosphorylation pathways for activation of MAP kinase. Nature 1990, 343, 651–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  130. Willmann, R.; Haischer, D.J.; Gust, A.A. Analysis of MAPK activities using MAPK-specific antibodies. Methods Mol. Biol. 2014, 1171, 27–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  131. Borges, G.; Miguelez, C.; Neto, F.; Mico, J.A.; Ugedo, L.; Berrocoso, E. Activation of extracellular signal-regulated kinase (ERK1/2) in the locus coeruleus contributes to pain-related anxiety in arthritic male rats. Int. J. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2017, 20, 463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  132. Wu, R.-N.; Kuo, C.-C.; Min, M.-Y.; Chen, R.-F.; Yang, H.-W. Extracellular signal-regulated kinases mediate an autoregulation of GABAB-receptor-activated whole-cell current in locus coeruleus neurons. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 7869. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Heydendael, W.; Sengupta, A.; Beck, S.; Bhatnagar, S. Optogenetic examination identifies a context-specific role for orexins/hypocretins in anxiety-related behavior. Physiol. Behav. 2014, 130, 182–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  134. Campos, C.A.; Ritter, R.C. NMDA-type glutamate receptors participate in reduction of food intake following hindbrain melanocortin receptor activation. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2015, 308, R1–R9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  135. Cao, Y.-K.; Guo, Q.; Ma, H.-J.; Wang, R.; Teng, X.; Wu, Y. Microinjection of urotensin II into the rostral ventrolateral medulla increases sympathetic vasomotor tone via the GPR14/ERK pathway in rats. Hypertens. Res. 2020, 43, 765–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  136. Fields, R.D.; Eshete, F.; Stevens, B.; Itoh, K. Action potential-dependent regulation of gene expression: Temporal specificity in Ca2+, cAMP-Responsive Element Binding Proteins, and Mitogen-Activated Protein Kinase signaling. J. Neurosci. 1997, 17, 7252–7266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  137. Iacobas, D.A.; Iacobas, S.; Lee, P.R.; Cohen, J.E.; Fields, R.D. Coordinated activity of transcriptional networks responding to the pattern of action potential firing in neurons. Genes 2019, 10, 754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  138. Lee, P.R.; Cohen, J.E.; Iacobas, D.A.; Iacobas, S.; Fields, R.D. Gene networks activated by specific patterns of action potentials in dorsal root ganglion neurons. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 43765. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  139. Lee, P.R.; Fields, R.D. Activity-dependent gene expression in neurons. Neuroscientist 2021, 27, 355–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  140. Warburg, O.; Posener, K.; Negelein, E. Über den Stoffwechsel der Carcinomzelle. Biochem. Zeitschr. 1924, 152, 309–344. Available online: https://archive.org/details/sim_biochemische-zeitschrift-beitraege-zur-chemischen_1924_152_0/page/308/mode/2up (accessed on 12 July 2022). [CrossRef]
  141. Koppenol, W.H.; Bounds, P.L.; Dang, C.V. Otto Warburg’s contributions to current concepts of cancer metabolism. Nat. Rev. Cancer 2011, 11, 325–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  142. Woodward, G.E.; Hudson, M.T. The effect of 2-desoxy-d-glucose on glycolysis and respiration of tumor and normal tissues. Cancer Res. 1954, 14, 599–605. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  143. Burk, D. Über die Begründung einer Chemotherapie des Krebses auf der grundlage einer primären hemmung der glucosephosphorylierung (Hexokinasereaktion) durch beeinflussung von substrat, coenzym und enzym. Wien. Klin. Wochenschr. 1957, 35, 1102–1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  144. Landau, B.R.; Laszlo, J.; Stengle, J.; Burk, D. Certain metabolic and pharmacologic effects in cancer patients given infusions of 2-deoxy-d-glucose. J. Natl. Cancer Inst. 1958, 21, 485–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Laszlo, J.; Harlan, W.R.; Klein, R.F.; Kirshner, N.; Estes, E.H., Jr.; Bogdonoff, M.D. The effect of 2-deoxy-d-glucose infusions on lipid and carbohydrate metabolism in man. J. Clin. Investig. 1961, 40, 171–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  146. Landau, B.R.; Lubs, H.A. Animal responses to 2-deoxy-d-glucose administration. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med. 1958, 99, 124–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. Smith, G.P.; Epstein, A.N. Increased feeding in response to decreased glucose utilization in the rat and monkey. Am. J. Physiol. 1969, 217, 1083–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  148. Pénicaud, L.; Thompson, D.A.; Le Magnen, J. Effects of 2-deoxy-d-glucose on food and water intake and body temperature in rats. Physiol. Behav. 1986, 36, 431–435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  149. Tsujii, S.; Bray, G.A. Effects of glucose, 2-deoxyglucose, phlorizin, and insulin on food intake of lean and fatty rats. Am. J. Physiol. 1990, 258, E476–E481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  150. Edmonds, B.K.; Edwards, G.L. Dorsomedial hindbrain participation in glucoprivic feeding response to 2DG but not 2DG-induced hyperglycemia or activation of the HPA axis. Brain Res. 1998, 801, 21–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Thompson, C.I.; Fleming, R.; Franken, K.A.; Hornback, D.A.; Boha, S.P. Dual 24-hour feeding response to 2DG in rats: Daytime increase and nighttime decrease. Physiol. Behav. 1989, 45, 155–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  152. Delprete, E.; Scharrer, E. Effects of 2-deoxy-d-glucose on food intake of rats are affected by diet composition. Physiol. Behav. 1992, 51, 951–956. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  153. Singer, L.K.; Ritter, S. Differential effects of infused nutrients on 2-deoxy-d-glucose- and 2-mercaptoacetate-induced feeding. Physiol. Behav. 1994, 56, 193–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  154. Sols, A.; Crane, R.K. Substrate specificity of brain hexokinase. J. Biol. Chem. 1954, 210, 581–595. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  155. Wick, A.N.; Drury, D.R.; Nakada, H.I.; Wolfe, J.B. Localization of the primary metabolic block produced by 2-deoxyglucose. J. Biol. Chem. 1957, 224, 963–969. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  156. Bachelard, H.S.; Clark, A.G.; Thompson, M.F. Cerebral-cortex hexokinases. Elucidation of reaction mechanisms by substrate and dead-end inhibitor kinetic analysis. Biochem. J. 1971, 123, 707–715. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  157. Bachelard, H.S. Deoxyglucose and brain glycolysis. Biochem. J. 1972, 127, 83P. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  158. Grossbard, L.; Schimke, R.T. Multiple hexokinases of rat tissues. Purification and comparison of soluble forms. J. Biol. Chem. 1966, 241, 3546–3560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  159. Lineweaver, H.; Burk, D. The determination of enzyme dissociation constants. J. Am. Chem. Soc. 1934, 56, 658–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  160. Horton, R.W.; Meldrum, B.S.; Bachelard, H.S. Enzymic and cerebral metabolic effects of 2-deoxy-d-glucose. J. Neurochem. 1973, 21, 507–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  161. Kessler, S.; Labouèbe, G.; Croizier, S.; Gaspari, S.; Tarussio, D.; Thorens, B. Glucokinase neurons of the paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus sense glucose and decrease food consumption. iScience 2021, 24, 103122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  162. Hertog, A.d.; Greengard, P.; Ritchie, J.M. On the metabolic basis of nerve activity. J. Physiol. 1969, 204, 511–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  163. Hertog, A.d.; Ritchie, J.M. A comparison of the effect of temperature, metabolic inhibitors and of ouabain on the electrogenic component of the sodium pump in mammalian non-myelinated nerve fibers. J. Physiol. 1969, 204, 523–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  164. Forte, N.; Medrihan, L.; Cappetti, B.; Baldelli, P.; Benfenati, F. 2-deoxy-d-glucose enhances tonic inhibition through the neurosteroid-mediated activation of extrasynaptic GABAA receptors. Epilepsia 2016, 57, 1987–2000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  165. Hartman, A.L. Glycolysis inhibition: The gift of GAB(A). Epilepsy Curr. 2017, 17, 118–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  166. Kim, E.S.; Kim, K.-S.; Lee, C.H.; Jeon, M.-T.; Lee, S.B.; Lee, J.H.; Kim, D.-G. Brain endothelial cells utilize glycolysis for the maintenance of the transcellular permeability. Mol. Neurobiol. 2022, 59, 4315–4333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  167. Dumont, J.E.; Dremier, S.; Pirson, I.; Maenhaut, C. Cross signaling, cell specificity and physiology. Am. J. Physiol. Cell Physiol. 2002, 282, C2–C28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  168. Datema, R.; Schwarz, R.T. Interference with glycosylation of glycoproteins. Inhibition of formation of lipid-linked oligosaccharides in vivo. Biochem. J. 1979, 184, 113–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  169. Kutoglu, M.; Maher, J.C.; Lampidis, T.J. Differential toxic mechanisms of 2-deoxy-d-glucose versus 2-fluorodeoxy-d-glucose in hypoxic and normoxic tumor cells. Antioxid. Redox Signal. 2007, 9, 1383–1390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  170. Osaka, T. 2-deoxy-d-glucose-induced hypothermia in anesthetized rats: Lack of forebrain contribution and critical involvement of the rostral raphe/parapyramidal regions of the medulla oblongata. Brain Res. Bull. 2015, 116, 73–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  171. Parker, L.M.; Le, S.; Wearne, T.; Hardwick, K.; Kumar, N.N.; Robinson, K.J.; McMullan, S.; Goodchild, A. Neurochemistry of neurons in the ventrolateral medulla activated by hypotension: Are the same neurons activated by glucoprivation? J. Comp. Neurol. 2017, 525, 2249–2264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  172. Gujar, A.D.; Ibrahim, B.A.; Tamraker, P.; Cherian, A.K.; Briski, K.P. Hindbrain lactostasis regulates hypothalamic AMPK activity and metabolic neurotransmitter mRNA and protein responses to hypoglycemia. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2014, 306, R457–R469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  173. Grill, H.J. Hindbrain energy status controls hypothalamic metabolic and neuropeptide signals. Focus on "hindbrain lactostasis regulates hypothalamic AMPK activity and hypothalamic metabolic neurotransmitter mRNA and protein responses to hypoglycemia". Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2014, 306, R439–R440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  174. Damanhuri, H.A.; Burke, P.G.R.; Ong, L.K.; Bobrovskaya, L.; Dickson, P.W.; Dunkley, P.R.; Goodchild, A.K. Tyrosine hydroxylase phosphorylation in catecholaminergic brain regions: A marker of activation following acute hypotension and glucoprivation. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e50535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  175. Tron Legacy, directed by J. Kosinski; written by E. Kitsis, A. Horowitz, B. Klugman, L. Sternthal (2010; Walt Disney Pictures); 125 min (see 55:40–57:00 for reference to “biodigital jazz”). Available online: https://archive.org/details/tron-legacy-2010 (accessed on 1 March 2023).
  176. Swanson, L.W. Biochemical switching in hypothalamic circuits mediating responses to stress. Prog. Brain Res. 1991, 87, 181–200. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  177. Sara, S.J. The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 211–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  178. McCormick, D.A.; Nestvogel, D.B.; He, B.J. Neuromodulation of brain state and behavior. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2020, 43, 391–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  179. Dagogo-Jack, S.; Rattarasarn, C.; Cryer, P.E. Reversal of hypoglycemia unawareness, but not defective glucose counterregulation, in IDDM. Diabetes 1994, 43, 1426–1434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  180. Uematsu, A.; Tan, B.Z.; Ycu, E.A.; Cuevas, J.S.; Koivumaa, J.; Junyent, F.; Kremer, E.; Witten, I.; Deisseroth, K.; Johansen, J. Modular organization of the brainstem noradrenaline system coordinates opposing learning states. Nat. Neurosci. 2017, 20, 1602–1611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  181. Seo, D.-O.; Bruchas, M.R. Polymorphic computation in locus coeruleus networks. Nat. Neurosci. 2017, 20, 1517–1519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  182. Holloway, B.B.; Stornetta, R.L.; Bochorishvili, G.; Erisir, A.; Viar, K.E.; Guyenet, P.G. Monosynaptic glutamatergic activation of locus coeruleus and other lower brainstem noradrenergic neurons by the C1 cells in mice. J. Neurosci. 2013, 33, 18792–18805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  183. Briski, K.P.; Marshall, E.S. Caudal brainstem Fos expression is restricted to periventricular catecholamine neuron-containing loci following intraventricular administration of 2-deoxy-d-glucose. Exp. Brain Res. 2000, 133, 547–551. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  184. Yuan, P.-Q.; Yang, H. Neuronal activation of brain vagal-regulatory pathways and upper gut enteric plexuses by insulin hypoglycemia. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 2002, 283, E436–E448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  185. Ao, Y.; Wu, S.; Go, V.L.W.; Toy, N.; Yang, H. Maintaining euglycemia prevents insulin-induced Fos expression in brain autonomic regulatory circuits. Pancreas 2005, 31, 142–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  186. Tkacs, N.C.; Pan, Y.; Sawhney, G.; Mann, G.L.; Morrison, A.R. Hypoglycemia activates arousal-related neurons and increases wake time in adult rats. Physiol. Behav. 2007, 91, 240–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  187. Morilak, D.A.; Fornal, C.A.; Jacobs, B.L. Effects of physiological manipulations on locus coeruleus neuronal activity in freely moving cats. III. Glucoregulatory challenge. Brain Res. 1987, 422, 32–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  188. Rusnák, M.; Jeloková, J.; Vietor, I.; Sabban, E.L.; Kvetňanský, R. Different effects of insulin and 2-deoxy-d-glucose administration on tyrosine hydroxylase gene expression in the locus coeruleus and the adrenal medulla in rats. Brain Res. Bull. 1998, 46, 447–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  189. Senthilkumaran, M.; Johnson, M.E.; Bobrovskaya, L. The effects of insulin-induced hypoglycaemia on tyrosine hydroxylase phosphorylation in rat brain and adrenal gland. Neurochem. Res. 2016, 41, 1612–1624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  190. Figlewicz, D.P.; Szot, P.; Israel, P.A.; Payne, C.; Dorsa, D.M. Insulin reduces norepinephrine transporter mRNA in vivo in rat locus coeruleus. Brain Res. 1993, 602, 161–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  191. Grunstein, H.S.; James, D.E.; Storlien, L.H.; Smythe, G.A.; Kraegen, E.W. Hyperinsulinemia suppresses glucose utilization in specific brain regions: In vivo studies using the euglycemic clamp in the rat. Endocrinology 1985, 116, 604–610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  192. Doyle, P.; Cusin, I.; Rohrer-Jeanrenaud, F.; Jeanrenaud, B. Four-day hyperinsulinemia in euglycemic conditions alters local cerebral glucose utilization in specific brain nuclei of freely moving rats. Brain Res. 1995, 684, 47–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  193. Nieber, K.; Sevcik, J.; Illes, P. Hypoxic changes in rat locus coeruleus neurons in vitro. J. Physiol. 1995, 486 Pt 1, 33–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  194. Bodineau, L.; Larnicol, N. Brainstem and hypothalamic areas activated by tissue hypoxia: Fos-like immunoreactivity induced by carbon monoxide inhalation in the rat. Neuroscience 2001, 108, 643–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  195. Guyenet, P.G.; Abbott, S.B.G. Chemoreception and asphyxia-induced arousal. Respir. Physiol. Neurobiol. 2013, 188, 333–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  196. Magalhães, K.S.; Spiller, P.F.; da Silva, M.P.; Kuntze, L.B.; Paton, J.F.R.; Machado, B.H.; Moraes, D.J.A. Locus Coeruleus as a vigilance centre for active inspiration and expiration in rats. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 15654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  197. Illes, P.; Sevcik, J.; Finta, E.P.; Frölich, R.; Nieber, K.; Nörenberg, W. Modulation of locus coeruleus neurons by extra- and intracellular adenosine 5'-triphosphate. Brain Res. Bull. 1994, 35, 513–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  198. Dallaporta, M.; Perrin, J.; Orsini, J.-C. Involvement of adenosine triphosphate-sensitive K+ channels in glucose-sensing in the rat solitary tract nucleus. Neurosci. Lett. 2000, 278, 77–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  199. De Bernardis Murat, C.; Leão, R.M. A voltage-dependent depolarization induced by low external glucose in neurons of the nucleus of the tractus solitarius: Interaction with KATP channels. J. Physiol. 2019, 597, 2515–2532. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  200. Holt, M.K. The ins and outs of the caudal nucleus of the solitary tract: An overview of cellular populations and anatomical connections. J. Neuroendocrinol. 2022, 34, e13132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  201. Martinez, D.; Kline, D.D. The role of astrocytes in the nucleus tractus solitarii in maintaining central control of autonomic function. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2021, 320, R418–R424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  202. Troadec, J.-D.; Gaigé, S.; Barbot, M.; Lebrun, B.; Barbouche, R.; Abysique, A. Glial modulation of energy balance: The dorsal vagal complex is no exception. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2022, 23, 960. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  203. Loewy, A.D.; Spyer, K.M. (Eds.) Central Regulation of Autonomic Functions; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  204. Blessing, W.W. The Lower Brainstem and Bodily Homeostasis; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1997; Available online: https://archive.org/details/lowerbrainstembo0000bles/page/n5/mode/2up (accessed on 21 May 2022).
  205. Khan, A.M.; Grant, A.H.; Martinez, A.; Burns, G.A.P.C.; Thatcher, B.S.; Anekonda, V.T.; Thompson, B.W.; Roberts, Z.S.; Moralejo, D.H.; Blevins, J.E. Mapping molecular datasets back to the brain regions they are extracted from: Remembering the native countries of hypothalamic expatriates and refugees. Adv. Neurobiol. 2018, 21, 101–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  206. Khan, A.M.; Perez, J.; Wells, C.E.; Fuentes, O. Computer vision evidence supporting craniometric alignment of rat brain atlases to streamline expert-guided, first-order migration of hypothalamic spatial datasets related to behavioral control. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2018, 12, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  207. Santarelli, A.J.; Khan, A.M.; Poulos, A.M. Contextual fear retrieval-induced Fos expression across early development in the rat: An analysis using established nervous system nomenclature ontology. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2018, 155, 42–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  208. Gu, G.; Roland, B.; Tomaselli, K.; Dolman, C.S.; Lowe, C.; Heilig, J.S. Glucagon-like peptide-1 in the rat brain: Distribution of expression and functional implication. J. Comp. Neurol. 2013, 521, 2235–2261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  209. Uschakov, A.; Gong, H.; McGinty, D.; Szymusiak, R. Efferent projections from the median preoptic nucleus to sleep- and arousal-regulatory nuclei in the rat brain. Neuroscience 2007, 150, 104–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  210. Loo, Y.T. The forebrain of the opossum, Didelphis virginiana. Part II. Histology. J. Comp. Neurol. 1931, 52, 1–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  211. Reiner, A.; Honig, M.G. Dextran amines: Versatile tools for anterograde and retrograde studies of nervous system connectivity. In Neuroanatomical Tract-Tracing Methods 3: Molecules, Neurons, and Systems; Zaborszky, L., Wouterlood, F.G., Lanciego, J.L., Eds.; Springer Science + Business Media, Inc.: New York, NY, USA, 2006; pp. 304–335. [Google Scholar]
  212. Swanson, L.W. Brain Maps: Structure of the Rat Brain; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  213. Ju, G.; Swanson, L.W. Studies on the cellular architecture of the bed nuclei of the stria terminalis in the rat: I. Cytoarchitecture. J. Comp. Neurol. 1989, 280, 587–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  214. Khan, A.M.; Watts, A.G. Tracking intracellular events associated with glucoprivic challenge in three forebrain regions that contain CRH neurons receiving hindbrain noradrenergic input. Presented at the 2005 Workshop on the Neuroendocrinology of Stress, Fort Collins, CO, USA, 1–3 June 2005, American Neuroendocrine Society: Fort Collins, CO, USA, 2005; Poster 29. [Google Scholar]
  215. Khan, A.M.; Watts, A.G. Glucoprivic challenge is associated with elevated levels of phosphorylated signaling intermediates in discrete forebrain regions: Immunofluorescence studies of ERK1/2 and CREB [Program #634.15]. In 2005 Abstract Viewer/Itinerary Planner; Society for Neuroscience: Washington, DC, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  216. Swanson, L.W. Brain Maps: Structure of the Rat Brain. A Laboratory Guide with Printed and Electronic Templates for Data, Models and Schematics, 3rd ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2004; 215p, with CD-ROM; Brain Maps: Computer Graphics Files 3.0. [Google Scholar]
  217. Hahn, J.D.; Swanson, L.W. Connections of the juxtaventromedial region of the lateral hypothalamic area in the male rat. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2015, 9, 66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  218. Gao, Y.-J.; Ji, R.-R. c-Fos and pERK, which is a better marker for neuronal activation and central sensitization after noxious stimulation and tissue injury? Open Pain J. 2009, 2, 11–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  219. Sokoloff, L.; Reivich, M.; Kennedy, C.; Des Rosiers, M.H.; Patlak, C.S.; Pettigrew, K.D.; Sakurada, O.; Shinohara, M. The [14C]deoxyglucose method for the measurement of local cerebral glucose utilization: Theory, procedures, and normal values in the conscious and anesthetized albino rat. J. Neurochem. 1977, 28, 897–916. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  220. Tapia, G.; Chenausky, S.D.; Arnal, A.; Negishi, K.; Navarro, V.I.; Balivada, S.; Khan, A.M. Hindbrain catecholaminergic and non-catecholaminergic neuronal populations rapidly recruited during glycemic challenge: A fluorescent cell-detecting deep learning model for high-throughput tabulation and atlas-based mapping. In Proceedings of the 2022 Neuroscience Meeting Planner, Annual Meeting of the Society for Neuroscience, San Diego, CA, USA, 12–16 November 2022; Society for Neuroscience: Washington, DC, USA, 2022. [Google Scholar]
  221. Bellingeri, C.F. De Medulla Spinali Nervisque ex ea Prodeuntibus Annotationes Anatomico-Physiologiae; Typographia Regia: Turin, Italy, 1823; Available online: https://archive.org/details/b21709142/page/n5/mode/2up (accessed on 21 May 2022).
  222. Procháska, G. Operum Minorum Anatomici, Physiologici et Pathologici Argumenti [2 parts]; Wappler et Beck: Vienna, Austria, 1800; Available online: http://dx.doi.org/10.25673/79580 (accessed on 21 May 2022).
  223. Reil, J.C. Fragmente über die Bildung des kleinen Gehirns im Menschen. Arch. Die Physiol. 1807, 8, 1–58. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Overview of tissue sampling employed for the experimental dataset. (A) A sagittal-plane view of the rat brain, adapted from the Brain Maps 4.0 (BM4.0) open-access rat brain atlas [81], which is the spatial model we used in this study. (B) An enlargement is presented of the boxed outline shown in (A), which provides details concerning the portions of the rhombic brain (His, 1893) [21] (RB) we studied (shaded in red). These include the locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1912) [82] (LC), area postrema (>1840) (AP), nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS), dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX), and hypoglossal nucleus (>1840) (XII). Portions of these structures were identified in tissue sections as closely registered to specific atlas levels from BM4.0 (shown as vertical blue lines). See the methods section for details.
Figure 1. Overview of tissue sampling employed for the experimental dataset. (A) A sagittal-plane view of the rat brain, adapted from the Brain Maps 4.0 (BM4.0) open-access rat brain atlas [81], which is the spatial model we used in this study. (B) An enlargement is presented of the boxed outline shown in (A), which provides details concerning the portions of the rhombic brain (His, 1893) [21] (RB) we studied (shaded in red). These include the locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1912) [82] (LC), area postrema (>1840) (AP), nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS), dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX), and hypoglossal nucleus (>1840) (XII). Portions of these structures were identified in tissue sections as closely registered to specific atlas levels from BM4.0 (shown as vertical blue lines). See the methods section for details.
Jcm 12 02483 g001
Figure 2. Increased recruitment of locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1912) [82] (LC) neurons 15 min after intravenous 2-deoxy-d-glucose (2-DG) administration, as deduced from a comparison of LC perikaryon profiles in coronal-plane tissue sections from saline- (A) and 2-DG-treated (B) subjects. (AiAiv): Subject K10-012 (saline-treated). The LC of the right hemisphere, as visible in a dark-field image (Ai), was immunostained for DBH (green; (Aii)) and for the activation marker, phospho-ERK1/2 (red, (Aiii)), but displayed virtually no signal for the latter molecule. (BiBiv): Subject K10-026 (2-DG-treated). In contrast, this subject displayed a robust phosopho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive signal in the LC (Biii), the boundaries of which were determined using a Nissl-stained tissue section from an adjacent tissue series collected from the same subject (bright-field photomicrograph, shown in (Bi). The maps in (Av,Bv) are of the LC from the BM4.0 reference atlas [81]. Scale bars in (Ai)/(Bi) and (Aii)/(Bii) each mark 20 μm and apply to the rest of the photomicrographs shown in this figure. Note, for (Ai), that the dark-field image of the same tissue reacted with immunofluorescence was used to identify boundaries for the LC in the brain of subject K10-012, rather than an adjacent Nissl-stained section, which was damaged during tissue processing. Headers for (A,B) note the inferred anteroposterior stereotaxic coordinate, expressed as millimeters from the bregma (β) skull suture. See the list of abbreviations at the end of this article for a description of those marked on the maps.
Figure 2. Increased recruitment of locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1912) [82] (LC) neurons 15 min after intravenous 2-deoxy-d-glucose (2-DG) administration, as deduced from a comparison of LC perikaryon profiles in coronal-plane tissue sections from saline- (A) and 2-DG-treated (B) subjects. (AiAiv): Subject K10-012 (saline-treated). The LC of the right hemisphere, as visible in a dark-field image (Ai), was immunostained for DBH (green; (Aii)) and for the activation marker, phospho-ERK1/2 (red, (Aiii)), but displayed virtually no signal for the latter molecule. (BiBiv): Subject K10-026 (2-DG-treated). In contrast, this subject displayed a robust phosopho-ERK1/2-immunoreactive signal in the LC (Biii), the boundaries of which were determined using a Nissl-stained tissue section from an adjacent tissue series collected from the same subject (bright-field photomicrograph, shown in (Bi). The maps in (Av,Bv) are of the LC from the BM4.0 reference atlas [81]. Scale bars in (Ai)/(Bi) and (Aii)/(Bii) each mark 20 μm and apply to the rest of the photomicrographs shown in this figure. Note, for (Ai), that the dark-field image of the same tissue reacted with immunofluorescence was used to identify boundaries for the LC in the brain of subject K10-012, rather than an adjacent Nissl-stained section, which was damaged during tissue processing. Headers for (A,B) note the inferred anteroposterior stereotaxic coordinate, expressed as millimeters from the bregma (β) skull suture. See the list of abbreviations at the end of this article for a description of those marked on the maps.
Jcm 12 02483 g002
Figure 3. A higher-magnification view (cf. Figure 2) of cellular activation observed in the locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1812) (LC) [82] 15 min after 2-DG administration. Subject K10-027. (A) LC perikaryon profiles labeled for dopamine β-hydroxylase (DBH, green) and (B) phospho-ERK1/2 (pERK1/2, red) appear to co-label the same perikaryon profiles ((D), arrows). (C) Co-labeled perikaryon profiles in the LC (identically marked by arrows as in (D)) with the A488 (green) channel set at 35% opacity renders visible the subtle, graded activation profiles of phospho-ERK1/2+ signal. Arrowheads (green in (A); red in (B)) mark single-labeled neurites immunoreactive for DBH or pERK1/2, respectively. Scale bars in (B,D) mark 50 μm and apply to all photomicrographs.
Figure 3. A higher-magnification view (cf. Figure 2) of cellular activation observed in the locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1812) (LC) [82] 15 min after 2-DG administration. Subject K10-027. (A) LC perikaryon profiles labeled for dopamine β-hydroxylase (DBH, green) and (B) phospho-ERK1/2 (pERK1/2, red) appear to co-label the same perikaryon profiles ((D), arrows). (C) Co-labeled perikaryon profiles in the LC (identically marked by arrows as in (D)) with the A488 (green) channel set at 35% opacity renders visible the subtle, graded activation profiles of phospho-ERK1/2+ signal. Arrowheads (green in (A); red in (B)) mark single-labeled neurites immunoreactive for DBH or pERK1/2, respectively. Scale bars in (B,D) mark 50 μm and apply to all photomicrographs.
Jcm 12 02483 g003
Figure 4. Increased recruitment of locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1812) (LC) [82] neurons 15 min after 2-DG administration. Abercrombie-corrected [106] perikaryon profile counts from both saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects are represented as box plots displaying the total numbers of phospho-ERK1/2- or DBH-immunoreactive profiles, or only the single- and double-labeled profiles, from tissue sections identified to be in register with BM4.0 atlas level 51 (map, upper right). An asterisk (*) denotes statistical significance between groups, FDR-adjusted p < 0.05. See Tables S1 and S2 for details. On the box plots, potential outliers are presented as open circles.
Figure 4. Increased recruitment of locus ceruleus (Wenzel and Wenzel, 1812) (LC) [82] neurons 15 min after 2-DG administration. Abercrombie-corrected [106] perikaryon profile counts from both saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects are represented as box plots displaying the total numbers of phospho-ERK1/2- or DBH-immunoreactive profiles, or only the single- and double-labeled profiles, from tissue sections identified to be in register with BM4.0 atlas level 51 (map, upper right). An asterisk (*) denotes statistical significance between groups, FDR-adjusted p < 0.05. See Tables S1 and S2 for details. On the box plots, potential outliers are presented as open circles.
Jcm 12 02483 g004
Figure 5. Glycemic challenge is associated with the recruitment of dorsal medulla (Winslow, 1733) [42] (MY) neurons mapped to atlas level 67 of Brain Maps 4.0 (BM4.0) [81]. Cytoarchitecture- and chemoarchitecture-based spatial analysis of distributed phosphorylated MAP kinase (pERK1/2) signal within dorsal RB neurons at the level of the nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS) (BM4.0 level 67). (A,B) The panels display bright-field Nissl photomicrographs (i), single- (iiiv) and dual-channel (viviii) fluorescence images, and BM4.0 atlas maps (v) for saline- ((A); Subject K10-011) and 2-DG-treated ((B); Subject K10-026) subjects of individual immunoreactivity patterns for dopamine β-hydroxylase (DBH, green), phospho-ERK1/2 (pERK1/2, red), and choline acetyltransferase (ChAT, blue). The patterns fall near and within the boundaries for the NTS, dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX) and hypoglossal nucleus (>1840) (XII). Note that the NTS label is colored green to signify its boundary determination using the pattern of immunoreactivity observed for DBH in the green channel, and DMX and XII labels are colored blue to signify their boundary determinations using the ChAT-immunoreactive signal in the far-red channel. Tissue is shown in the coronal plane. Scale bars in (i,ii), which mark 20 μm, and the medial orientation noted for the maps in (v), apply to all photomicrographs. Arrows in (Biii) mark perikaryon profiles immunoreactive for pERK1/2 in the NTS. Asterisks in (Aiv,Biv) mark the locations of masks placed over the ventricular space using Adobe Photoshop to cover visually distracting autofluorescence observed under the far-red channel from the mounting medium; this mask is used for all images containing a far-red (blue-colored) signal (thus, this also applies to (Avii/Aviii) and (Bvii/Bviii)). Headers for (A,B) note the inferred anteroposterior stereotaxic coordinate, expressed as millimeters from the bregma (β) skull suture. See the list of abbreviations at the end of this article for a description of those marked on the maps.
Figure 5. Glycemic challenge is associated with the recruitment of dorsal medulla (Winslow, 1733) [42] (MY) neurons mapped to atlas level 67 of Brain Maps 4.0 (BM4.0) [81]. Cytoarchitecture- and chemoarchitecture-based spatial analysis of distributed phosphorylated MAP kinase (pERK1/2) signal within dorsal RB neurons at the level of the nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS) (BM4.0 level 67). (A,B) The panels display bright-field Nissl photomicrographs (i), single- (iiiv) and dual-channel (viviii) fluorescence images, and BM4.0 atlas maps (v) for saline- ((A); Subject K10-011) and 2-DG-treated ((B); Subject K10-026) subjects of individual immunoreactivity patterns for dopamine β-hydroxylase (DBH, green), phospho-ERK1/2 (pERK1/2, red), and choline acetyltransferase (ChAT, blue). The patterns fall near and within the boundaries for the NTS, dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX) and hypoglossal nucleus (>1840) (XII). Note that the NTS label is colored green to signify its boundary determination using the pattern of immunoreactivity observed for DBH in the green channel, and DMX and XII labels are colored blue to signify their boundary determinations using the ChAT-immunoreactive signal in the far-red channel. Tissue is shown in the coronal plane. Scale bars in (i,ii), which mark 20 μm, and the medial orientation noted for the maps in (v), apply to all photomicrographs. Arrows in (Biii) mark perikaryon profiles immunoreactive for pERK1/2 in the NTS. Asterisks in (Aiv,Biv) mark the locations of masks placed over the ventricular space using Adobe Photoshop to cover visually distracting autofluorescence observed under the far-red channel from the mounting medium; this mask is used for all images containing a far-red (blue-colored) signal (thus, this also applies to (Avii/Aviii) and (Bvii/Bviii)). Headers for (A,B) note the inferred anteroposterior stereotaxic coordinate, expressed as millimeters from the bregma (β) skull suture. See the list of abbreviations at the end of this article for a description of those marked on the maps.
Jcm 12 02483 g005aJcm 12 02483 g005b
Figure 6. Increased recruitment of BM4.0 atlas level-67-registered nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS) neurons 15 min after 2-DG administration. Abercrombie-corrected [106] perikaryon profile counts from both saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects are represented as box plots displaying the total numbers of phospho-ERK1/2- or DBH-immunoreactive profiles, or only the single- and double-labeled profiles, from tissue sections identified to be in register with BM4.0 atlas level 67 (map, upper right). An asterisk (*) denotes statistical significance between groups, FDR-adjusted p < 0.05. See Tables S4 and S5 for details. On the box plots, potential outliers are presented as open circles.
Figure 6. Increased recruitment of BM4.0 atlas level-67-registered nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS) neurons 15 min after 2-DG administration. Abercrombie-corrected [106] perikaryon profile counts from both saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects are represented as box plots displaying the total numbers of phospho-ERK1/2- or DBH-immunoreactive profiles, or only the single- and double-labeled profiles, from tissue sections identified to be in register with BM4.0 atlas level 67 (map, upper right). An asterisk (*) denotes statistical significance between groups, FDR-adjusted p < 0.05. See Tables S4 and S5 for details. On the box plots, potential outliers are presented as open circles.
Jcm 12 02483 g006
Figure 7. 2-DG administration, at a group level, is not associated with an alteration of activation patterns in the dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX) perikaryon profiles registered to BM4.0 atlas level 67. Abercrombie-corrected [106] perikaryon profile counts from both saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects are represented as box plots displaying the total numbers of phospho-ERK1/2- or ChAT- or DBH-immunoreactive profiles, or only the single-, double-, and triple-labeled profiles, from tissue sections identified to be in register with BM4.0 atlas level 67 (map, upper right). Differences in the numbers of perikaryon profile were not significant; see Tables S3 and S4 for a statistical summary.
Figure 7. 2-DG administration, at a group level, is not associated with an alteration of activation patterns in the dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX) perikaryon profiles registered to BM4.0 atlas level 67. Abercrombie-corrected [106] perikaryon profile counts from both saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects are represented as box plots displaying the total numbers of phospho-ERK1/2- or ChAT- or DBH-immunoreactive profiles, or only the single-, double-, and triple-labeled profiles, from tissue sections identified to be in register with BM4.0 atlas level 67 (map, upper right). Differences in the numbers of perikaryon profile were not significant; see Tables S3 and S4 for a statistical summary.
Jcm 12 02483 g007
Figure 8. Glycemic challenge is associated with the recruitment of dorsal medulla (Winslow, 1733) [42] (MY) neurons mapped to atlas level 69 of Brain Maps 4.0 (BM4.0) [81] at the level of the area postrema (>1840) (AP). Cytoarchitecture- and chemoarchitecture-based spatial analysis of immunoreactivity patterns for dopamine β-hydroxylase (DBH, green), phospho-ERK1/2 (pERK1/2, red), and choline acetyltransferase (ChAT, blue) within dorsal MY neurons. (A,B) The panels display coronal-plane Nissl photomicrographs (i), single- (iiiv) and dual-channel (viviii) fluorescence images, and BM4.0 atlas maps (v) for saline- ((A); Subject K10-011) and 2-DG-treated ((B); Subject K10-026) subjects. The patterns fall near or within the boundaries for the AP, nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS), dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX), and hypoglossal nucleus (>1840) (XII). Arrows in (Aiii,Biii) mark perikaryon profiles immunoreactive for pERK1/2 in the NTS. Colors for the AP, NTS, DMX, and XII labels match those of the labeling patterns used to help define their boundaries. Scale bars in (i,ii), which mark 20 μm, and the medial orientation noted in (v), apply to all images. Headers for (A,B) note the inferred anteroposterior stereotaxic coordinate, expressed as millimeters from the bregma (β) skull suture. See the list of abbreviations at the end of this article for a description of those marked on the maps.
Figure 8. Glycemic challenge is associated with the recruitment of dorsal medulla (Winslow, 1733) [42] (MY) neurons mapped to atlas level 69 of Brain Maps 4.0 (BM4.0) [81] at the level of the area postrema (>1840) (AP). Cytoarchitecture- and chemoarchitecture-based spatial analysis of immunoreactivity patterns for dopamine β-hydroxylase (DBH, green), phospho-ERK1/2 (pERK1/2, red), and choline acetyltransferase (ChAT, blue) within dorsal MY neurons. (A,B) The panels display coronal-plane Nissl photomicrographs (i), single- (iiiv) and dual-channel (viviii) fluorescence images, and BM4.0 atlas maps (v) for saline- ((A); Subject K10-011) and 2-DG-treated ((B); Subject K10-026) subjects. The patterns fall near or within the boundaries for the AP, nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS), dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX), and hypoglossal nucleus (>1840) (XII). Arrows in (Aiii,Biii) mark perikaryon profiles immunoreactive for pERK1/2 in the NTS. Colors for the AP, NTS, DMX, and XII labels match those of the labeling patterns used to help define their boundaries. Scale bars in (i,ii), which mark 20 μm, and the medial orientation noted in (v), apply to all images. Headers for (A,B) note the inferred anteroposterior stereotaxic coordinate, expressed as millimeters from the bregma (β) skull suture. See the list of abbreviations at the end of this article for a description of those marked on the maps.
Jcm 12 02483 g008aJcm 12 02483 g008b
Figure 9. Increased recruitment of dorsal medulla (Winslow, 1733) [42] (MY) neurons 15 min after intravenous 2-deoxy-d-glucose (2-DG) administration relative to saline-treated subjects. Single-channel (A,B), dual-channel (A’,B’), and triple-channel (A”,B”) coronal-plane images, from saline- ((A,A’,A”); Subject K10-007) and 2-DG-treated (B,B’,B”); Subject K10-027) subjects, of individual immunoreactivity patterns for dopamine β-hydroxylase (DBH; green), phospho-ERK1/2 (pERK1/2; red), and choline acetyltransferase (ChAT; blue) in the area postrema (>1840) (AP), nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS), dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX), and hypoglossal nucleus (>1840) (XII), in the vicinity of the central canal of medulla (>1840) (Cmd). Individual single-channel images are shown in (A,B) for DBH signal ((Ai,Bi)), phospho-ERK1/2 signal (Aii,Bii), and ChAT signal (Aiii,Biii). Similarly, pairs of signals are shown in dual-channel images in (A’,B’): DBH with phospho-ERK1/2 (A’i,B’i), DBH with ChAT (A’ii,B’ii), and ChAT with phospho-ERK1/2 (A’iii,B’iii). Finally, triple-channel images of all signals combined (DBH, phospho-ERK1/2, ChAT) are shown in (A”i,B”i), with enlargements of the boxed areas designated as (A”ii,B”ii), respectively, with perikaryon profiles dual-labeled for phospho-ERK1/2 and either DBH or ChAT marked by arrows. Note that the AP and NTS labels are colored green to signify their boundary determinations using the pattern of immunoreactivity observed for DBH in the green channel, and DMX and XII locations are labeled in blue to signify their boundary determinations using the ChAT immunoreactive signal in the far-red channel. See the methods section for more details. Phospho-ERK1/2 immunoreactivity (red) is also present in NTS perikaryon profiles of unidentified phenotype (see dashed circles in (B”ii)) in 2-DG-treated but not saline-treated subjects.
Figure 9. Increased recruitment of dorsal medulla (Winslow, 1733) [42] (MY) neurons 15 min after intravenous 2-deoxy-d-glucose (2-DG) administration relative to saline-treated subjects. Single-channel (A,B), dual-channel (A’,B’), and triple-channel (A”,B”) coronal-plane images, from saline- ((A,A’,A”); Subject K10-007) and 2-DG-treated (B,B’,B”); Subject K10-027) subjects, of individual immunoreactivity patterns for dopamine β-hydroxylase (DBH; green), phospho-ERK1/2 (pERK1/2; red), and choline acetyltransferase (ChAT; blue) in the area postrema (>1840) (AP), nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS), dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX), and hypoglossal nucleus (>1840) (XII), in the vicinity of the central canal of medulla (>1840) (Cmd). Individual single-channel images are shown in (A,B) for DBH signal ((Ai,Bi)), phospho-ERK1/2 signal (Aii,Bii), and ChAT signal (Aiii,Biii). Similarly, pairs of signals are shown in dual-channel images in (A’,B’): DBH with phospho-ERK1/2 (A’i,B’i), DBH with ChAT (A’ii,B’ii), and ChAT with phospho-ERK1/2 (A’iii,B’iii). Finally, triple-channel images of all signals combined (DBH, phospho-ERK1/2, ChAT) are shown in (A”i,B”i), with enlargements of the boxed areas designated as (A”ii,B”ii), respectively, with perikaryon profiles dual-labeled for phospho-ERK1/2 and either DBH or ChAT marked by arrows. Note that the AP and NTS labels are colored green to signify their boundary determinations using the pattern of immunoreactivity observed for DBH in the green channel, and DMX and XII locations are labeled in blue to signify their boundary determinations using the ChAT immunoreactive signal in the far-red channel. See the methods section for more details. Phospho-ERK1/2 immunoreactivity (red) is also present in NTS perikaryon profiles of unidentified phenotype (see dashed circles in (B”ii)) in 2-DG-treated but not saline-treated subjects.
Jcm 12 02483 g009
Figure 10. 2-DG administration is associated with a marked alteration of activation patterns in nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS) perikaryon profiles anatomically registered to BM4.0 atlas level 69. Abercrombie-corrected [106] perikaryon profile counts from both saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects are represented as box plots displaying the total numbers of phospho-ERK1/2- or DBH-immunoreactive profiles, or only the single- and double-labeled profiles, from tissue sections identified to be in register with BM4.0 atlas level 69 (map, upper right). An asterisk (*) denotes statistical significance between groups with an FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; a double asterisk (**) denotes an FDR-adjusted p < 0.01. See Tables S5 and S6 for details. On the box plots, potential outliers are presented as open circles.
Figure 10. 2-DG administration is associated with a marked alteration of activation patterns in nucleus of solitary tract (>1840) (NTS) perikaryon profiles anatomically registered to BM4.0 atlas level 69. Abercrombie-corrected [106] perikaryon profile counts from both saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects are represented as box plots displaying the total numbers of phospho-ERK1/2- or DBH-immunoreactive profiles, or only the single- and double-labeled profiles, from tissue sections identified to be in register with BM4.0 atlas level 69 (map, upper right). An asterisk (*) denotes statistical significance between groups with an FDR-adjusted p < 0.05; a double asterisk (**) denotes an FDR-adjusted p < 0.01. See Tables S5 and S6 for details. On the box plots, potential outliers are presented as open circles.
Jcm 12 02483 g010
Figure 11. 2-DG administration does not alter activation patterns in dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX) cells registered to BM4.0 atlas level 69. Abercrombie-corrected [106] perikaryon profile counts from both saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects are represented as box plots displaying the total numbers of phospho-ERK1/2-, ChAT-, or DBH-immunoreactive profiles, or only the single-, double-, and triple-labeled profiles, from tissue sections identified to be in register with BM4.0 atlas level 69 (map, upper right). Perikaryon profile count differences were not significant; see Tables S5 and S6 for a statistical summary. On the box plots, potential outliers are presented as open circles.
Figure 11. 2-DG administration does not alter activation patterns in dorsal motor nucleus of vagus nerve (>1840) (DMX) cells registered to BM4.0 atlas level 69. Abercrombie-corrected [106] perikaryon profile counts from both saline- and 2-DG-treated subjects are represented as box plots displaying the total numbers of phospho-ERK1/2-, ChAT-, or DBH-immunoreactive profiles, or only the single-, double-, and triple-labeled profiles, from tissue sections identified to be in register with BM4.0 atlas level 69 (map, upper right). Perikaryon profile count differences were not significant; see Tables S5 and S6 for a statistical summary. On the box plots, potential outliers are presented as open circles.
Jcm 12 02483 g011
Table 1. Antibody combinations.
Table 1. Antibody combinations.
Reagent aAntigen or ConjugateHostTypeSource b, Catalog #Titer cRRIDReaction
(h, temp.) d
Reaction Series 1
PrimaryPhospho-ERK1/2 eRbPoly IgGCST, 9101 j1:1000AB_33164664–66, 4 °C
SecondaryRabbit IgG (H + L) fDkCy3-labeledJIL, 711-165-152 k1:500AB_23074435–6, 4 °C
PrimaryDBHMsMono IgGM, MAB308 l1:10,000AB_224574064–66, 4 °C
SecondaryMouse IgG (H + L) gDkBiotinylatedJIL, 715-065-150 m1:500AB_23074385–6, 4 °C
ConjugateStreptavidin A488-labeledTF, S-11223 n1:2000na1, 4 °C
PrimaryHuman placental ChATGtPoly IgGM, AB144P o1:6000AB_207975164–66, 4 °C
SecondaryGoat IgG (H+L)hDkA647-labeledJIL, 705-605-147 p1:500AB_23404375–6, 4 °C
Counterstain DAPI iRNA labelTF, D1306 q1:4000na0.5, RT
Reaction Series 2
PrimaryPhospho-ERK1/2 eRbPoly IgGCST, 9101 j1:1000AB_33164660–65, 4 °C
SecondaryRabbit IgG (H + L) fDkBiotinylatedJIL, 711-065-152 ^1:500AB_23405934–5, 4 °C
ConjugateStreptavidin A488-labeledTF, S-11223 n1:2000na1, 4 °C
PrimaryDBHMsMono IgGM, MAB308 l1:10,000AB_224574060–65, 4 °C
SecondaryMouse IgG (H + L) gDkCy3-labeledJIL, 715-165-150 ^1:500AB_23408134–5, 4 °C
PrimaryHuman placental ChATGtPoly IgGM, AB144P o1:6000AB_207975160–65, 4 °C
SecondaryGoat IgG (H + L) hDkA647-labeledJIL, 705-605-147 p1:500AB_23404374–5, 4 °C
^ Lot # not available. a Reagents used within a common reaction set are grouped by reaction series. Each series represents a common set of reagents applied to one series of tissue sections. b See abbreviations below for full names of suppliers. c The dilutions listed are calculated from the suppliers’ stock. All secondaries and conjugate stocks from suppliers were diluted 1:2 in glycerol (i.e., 50% glycerol, 50% buffer), and the dilution listed (e.g., 1:500) is the final dilution. Thus, for example, we calculated a 1:250 dilution of the 1:2 working stock to obtain the final 1:500 dilution. d The total duration of incubation (in hours) is listed, followed by the temperature at which the incubation proceeded. In many cases, the duration is expressed as a range, based on the exact parameters of reactions run on separate occasions. e Phospho-ERK1/2 antibodies recognize the phosphoryl modifications of p44/42 MAP kinases on residues Thr202 and Tyr204 (Thr183 and Tyr185 in rat ERK2). f According to the supplier’s information, this affinity-purified secondary antibody displayed minimal cross-reactivity with bovine, chicken, goat, guinea pig, Syrian hamster, horse, human, mouse, rat, or sheep serum proteins. g According to the supplier’s information, this affinity-purified secondary antibody displayed minimal cross-reactivity with bovine, chicken, goat, guinea pig, Syrian hamster, horse, human, rabbit, or sheep serum proteins. h According to the supplier’s information, this affinity-purified secondary antibody displayed minimal cross-reactivity with chicken, guinea pig, Syrian hamster, horse, human, mouse, rabbit, or rat serum proteins. i For some, but not all, reactions using this combination of reagents, 4,6 diamidino-2-phenylindole dihydrochloride (DAPI), was used as a counterstain. j Lot #19 or #27 (run by SDC); Lot #30 (run by VIN). k Lot #131748 (run by VIN). l Lot #2029625 (run by SDC); Lot #3736685 (run by VIN). m Lot #107814 (run by VIN). n Lot #1103102 (runs by SDC, VIN). o Lot #NG1825168 (run by SDC); Lot #3233120 (run by VIN). p Lot #103972 (run by SDC); Lot #116978 (run by VIN). q Lot#1159932 (run by SDC). Abbreviations: A488, Alexa 488; A647, Alexa 647; ChAT, choline acetyltransferase; CST, Cell Signaling Technology, Inc.; DBH, dopamine beta-hydroxylase; Dk, donkey; ERK1/2, extracellularly regulated kinases 1 and 2; Gt, goat; (H + L), immunoglobulin heavy and light chains; IgG, immunoglobulin G; JIL, Jackson ImmunoResearch Labs; M, MilliporeSigma; Mono, monoclonal; Ms, mouse; na, not applicable; Poly, polyclonal; Rb, rabbit; RT, room temperature; TF, Thermo Fisher.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Tapia, G.P.; Agostinelli, L.J.; Chenausky, S.D.; Padilla, J.V.S.; Navarro, V.I.; Alagh, A.; Si, G.; Thompson, R.H.; Balivada, S.; Khan, A.M. Glycemic Challenge Is Associated with the Rapid Cellular Activation of the Locus Ceruleus and Nucleus of Solitary Tract: Circumscribed Spatial Analysis of Phosphorylated MAP Kinase Immunoreactivity. J. Clin. Med. 2023, 12, 2483. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12072483

AMA Style

Tapia GP, Agostinelli LJ, Chenausky SD, Padilla JVS, Navarro VI, Alagh A, Si G, Thompson RH, Balivada S, Khan AM. Glycemic Challenge Is Associated with the Rapid Cellular Activation of the Locus Ceruleus and Nucleus of Solitary Tract: Circumscribed Spatial Analysis of Phosphorylated MAP Kinase Immunoreactivity. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2023; 12(7):2483. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12072483

Chicago/Turabian Style

Tapia, Geronimo P., Lindsay J. Agostinelli, Sarah D. Chenausky, Jessica V. Salcido Padilla, Vanessa I. Navarro, Amy Alagh, Gabriel Si, Richard H. Thompson, Sivasai Balivada, and Arshad M. Khan. 2023. "Glycemic Challenge Is Associated with the Rapid Cellular Activation of the Locus Ceruleus and Nucleus of Solitary Tract: Circumscribed Spatial Analysis of Phosphorylated MAP Kinase Immunoreactivity" Journal of Clinical Medicine 12, no. 7: 2483. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm12072483

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop