Next Article in Journal
Clonal Hematopoiesis and the Risk of Hematologic Malignancies after Curative Therapies for Sickle Cell Disease
Next Article in Special Issue
Promoting Physical Activity and Reducing Sedentary Behavior to Prevent Chronic Diseases during the COVID Pandemic and Beyond
Previous Article in Journal
Clinical Update Findings about pH-Impedance Monitoring Features in Laryngopharyngeal Reflux Patients
Previous Article in Special Issue
Physical Activity, Positive and Negative Symptoms of Psychosis, and General Psychopathology among People with Psychotic Disorders: A Meta-Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sprint Interval Exercise Improves Cognitive Performance Unrelated to Postprandial Glucose Fluctuations at Different Levels of Normobaric Hypoxia

1
Faculty of Education, University of Macau, Macao, China
2
Institute of Physical Education, Huzhou University, Huzhou 313000, China
3
Faculty of Health Sciences and Sports, Macao Polytechnic University, Macao, China
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
J. Clin. Med. 2022, 11(11), 3159; https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm11113159
Submission received: 31 March 2022 / Revised: 30 May 2022 / Accepted: 30 May 2022 / Published: 2 June 2022

Abstract

:
Objective: The aim of our study was to examine cognition response to sprint interval exercise (SIE) against different levels of hypoxia. Research design and methods: 26 recreational active males performed SIE (20 × 6 s of all-out cycling bouts, 15 s of passive recovery) under normoxia (FIO2: 0.209), moderate hypoxia (FIO2: 0.154), and severe hypoxia (FIO2: 0.112) in a single-blinded crossover design. Cognitive function and blood glucose were assessed before and after 0, 10, 30, and 60 min of the SIE. Heart rate (HR), peripheral oxygen saturation (SpO2), and ratings of perceived exertion (RPE, the Borg 6–20-point scale) during each SIE trial were recorded before and immediately after every five cycling bouts, and after 0, 10, 30, and 60 min of the SIE. Results: All the three SIE trials had a significantly faster overall reaction time in the Stroop test at 10 min after exercise as compared to that of the baseline value (p = 0.003, ƞ2 = 0.606), and returned to normal after 60 min. The congruent RT at 10 min after SIE was significantly shorter than that of the baseline (p < 0.05, ƞ2 = 0.633), while the incongruent RT at both 10 min and 30 min were significantly shorter than that measured at baseline (p < 0.05, ƞ2 = 0.633). No significant differences in terms of accuracy were found across the three trials at any time points (p = 0.446, ƞ2 = 0.415). Blood glucose was significantly reduced at 10 min and was sustained for at least 60 min after SIE when compared to pre-exercise in all trials (p < 0.05). Conclusions: Acute SIE improved cognitive function regardless of oxygen conditions, and the sustained improvement following SIE could last for at least 10–30 min and was unaffected by the altered blood glucose level.

1. Introduction

High-intensity interval training (HIIT) has been promoted over the last decade as an effective form of exercise training for improving cardiovascular and metabolic function and mental health in active young adults [1,2] and patients with coronary artery disease [3]. The benefits of the lower volume and time commitment of HIIT in overcoming the time-related barrier to physical activity adherence show clear superiority compared to traditional endurance exercise. As such, extreme short (<10 s) bouts of all-out sprint interval exercise (SIE), classified as one form of HIIT, seems to be an attractive option for achieving exercise-related health benefits [4].
Hypoxic training has been widely adopted as an effective strategy to enhance exercise performance and physical fitness in athletes for decades. More recently, this training strategy has shown its beneficial effects on cardiometabolic health and weight management in untrained individuals [5,6,7,8]. Compared to training at sea level, hypoxic training could further enhance blood oxygen transport capacity and aerobic endurance, as reported in previous studies [8,9,10]. However, the optimal dose of hypoxic severity for health-enhancing strategies is yet to be determined in view of the widely varying hypoxic exposure duration and severity used in the experimental protocols in the previous studies. The discrepancies in methodology make generalization challenging. Protocols that elicit beneficial effects on cardiorespiratory, metabolic, and immune health without pathology are more likely to arise from hypoxic exposure to a 9–16% fraction of inspired O2 [11]. Under such conditions, the exaggerated reduction in oxygen saturation increased ventilatory demand in response to hypoxia as a compensatory effect [12], with a more severe hypoxic level during a short period of exposure most often leading to beneficial cardiovascular effects [11]. This suggested that the severity of hypoxia appears to be the pivotal factor to determine this critical dependency.
A number of studies have demonstrated that acute exposure to hypoxia can negatively alter human cognitive function due to reduced oxygen partial pressure [13]. Cognitive deterioration may occur in acute hypoxic exposure at altitudes above 6000 m (FiO2: 0.097) [14]. Physical activity has previously been shown to enhance cognitive function [15,16,17]. It has been found that SIE improved selective attention [18], lexical learning [19], and executive function [20] in adults, and such beneficial effects were greater [19] and lasted longer [20] when compared to those resulting from continuous aerobic exercise. Due to the discrepancies in severity and duration of hypoxic exposure, however, results from randomized control trials and reviews investigating exercise combined with hypoxia showed both beneficial [21,22,23,24,25,26,27] and detrimental effects [28,29,30,31,32,33,34] on cognitive performance. Notably, studies on cerebral oxygenation have reported that central oxygenation between normoxia and hypoxic conditions were different. Near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) measurement revealed that during incremental exercise under hypoxia, stages in low to high intensity elicit a larger degree of cerebral deoxygenation compared with normoxia, which may limit cognitive activity and exercise performance [35]. Investigations that used electroencephalography and transcranial magnetic stimulation reported that as the severity of the hypoxia increased, cerebral oxygenation became more predominant in influencing cognitive activity and exercise performance [36]. Our previous studies demonstrated that high-intensity exercise under moderate hypoxia (FiO2: 0.154) [34] and moderate-intensity exercise under severe hypoxia (FiO2: 0.12) [25] had no detrimental effect on cognitive function. More information about the impact of different hypoxic severities on cognitive function is needed.
Acute SIE improved postprandial glucose metabolism in healthy subjects [37], an obese population [38,39], and type 2 diabetes patients [40] immediately or up to 24 h after exercise. Hypoxia is known to improve glucose effectiveness, and is suggested to facilitate further improvement in insulin sensitivity when combined with exercise [41]. Glucose is the major energy resource for the brain when processing cognition-related information. The brain is sensitive to disruptions in energy supply, whereas a modest increase in blood glucose concentration was associated with better cognitive performance [42]. Research has also shown that difficult cognitive tasks, such as those involving executive function pertaining to the frontal cortex, were more susceptible to glycemic alteration [43]. Available data indicate that acute hypoglycemia, or when the blood glucose concentration was below 3 mmol∙L−1, would impair cognition performance [44]. Therefore, glucose fluctuations induced by exercise in hypoxic conditions may have a negative influence on cognitive performance.
Given the above, this study used a single-blinded crossover design to evaluate the effects of SIE under normoxia, moderate hypoxia (FIO2: 0.154, simulating an altitude of 2500 m), and severe hypoxia (FIO2: 0.112, simulating an altitude corresponding to 5000 m) on cognitive function. We particularly focused on whether the cognitive-improvement effects of SIE were impaired while performing under hypoxia as the severity of hypoxia increased, and whether blood glucose was associated with cognitive performance during exercise under hypoxia. Given that brain function and tissue integrity are dependent on a continuous and sufficient oxygen supply, we hypothesized that cognitive function would be impaired during exercise under hypoxia as the severity of hypoxia increased.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

The study was approved by the Research Ethics Panel of the University of Macau, and all experimental procedures were in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki. Advertisements including research purposes and inclusion criteria were posted on the e-bulletin board of the university to recruit recreational active males. The inclusion criteria were: (1) a maximal oxygen uptake ( V . O2max) level of over 40 mL∙kg−1∙min−1 but lower than 55 mL∙kg−1∙min−1; (2) right-hand dominant; (3) nonhighland resident (above 1000 m); (4) without prior experience in hypoxic training; (5) free of any known neurological, cardiovascular, and pulmonary disorders; and (6) free from color blindness or abnormal vision. Smokers or those taking medication or having any physical barriers to performing SIE under hypoxia were excluded. Volunteers who were interested in this study were required to perform an incremental ramp test in the kinesiology lab to determine their V . O2max and eligibility.
An a priori power analysis was conducted using G*Power Version 3.1 to estimate the sample size. When the effect size was set at medium (f = 0.25), 21 participants would be needed to detect a significant difference in a two-way repeated-measures ANOVA with a power of 80% and a significance level of 5%. Considering a 20% dropout rate, 26 recreational active males were invited to participate in this study. All participants were informed of the experimental procedures and potential risks, and provided a written consent prior to participation. This study was conducted from October 2019 to September 2020. A total of 20 recreational active males (age: 21.4 ± 2.0 y, stature: 175.9 ± 9.1 cm, body mass: 68.7 ± 11.4 kg, body mass index (BMI): 22.1 ± 2.1 kg·m−2, maximal oxygen uptake ( V . O2max): 42.9 ± 1.3 mL·kg−1·min−1) completed all experimental trials and required measurements (Figure 1).

2.2. Experimental Design

This study included a familiarization session and three experimental trials. During the familiarization session, participants were familiarized with the experimental procedures and practiced the SIE protocol and the Stroop test.
After the familiarization session, the participants accomplished three main trials at the same time on different days, namely an SIE trial under normoxia (N), an SIE trial under normobaric moderate hypoxia (M, FIO2: 0.154, simulated at an altitude of 2500 m), and an SIE trial under severe hypoxia (S, FIO2: 0.112, simulating an altitude corresponding to 5000 m). The three trials were assigned in a random and counterbalanced order, and were interspersed by three to seven days of washout period.
On the day of trials, participants were required to report to the lab before 17:50 to perform a baseline blood glucose test (using glucose and lactate analyzer, Biosen C-Line, EKF diagnostics, Barleben, Germany) and a Stroop test after a 10 min rest. Then, a standard dinner was provided to them at 18:00. A modified gas-mixing system (Everest Summit II Hypoxic Generator, New York, NY, USA) was used to generate normoxic or hypoxic gases, and the normoxic or hypoxic gas mixtures were delivered to participants through a breathing mask and tubes. At 19:30, participants were fitted with the breathing mask connected to the gas-mixing system, and they rested on the seat for 25 min. Then, a pre-exercise test of blood glucose and the Stroop test were performed. The SIE session (5 min warmup, 7 min exercise, and 3 min cooldown) began at 20:00 under the conditions of normobaric normoxia (i.e., the N trial) or normobaric hypoxia (i.e., the M and S trials). Stroop tests were carried out subsequent to the measurement of blood glucose levels at 10, 30, and 60 min after SIE. The breathing mask was taken off at 20:30.
Participants were unaware of the normoxic or hypoxic condition of the experimental trials, and they were required to guess the oxygen condition of the SIE trial by answering the question “Under what condition do you think you were exercising, normoxia, moderate hypoxia or severe hypoxia (i.e., sea level, 2500 m or 5000 m)?” However, no confirmation was given to the participants in order to avoid a potential influence on the subsequent trials.

2.3. Sprint Interval Exercise

The 7 min SIE trial consisted of 20 repetitions for 6 s of high-intensity cycling bouts interspersed with 15 s of passive recovery. Participants pedaled maximally against a load equivalent to 7.5% of their body mass during the 6 s work durations, and underwent passive recovery during the 15 s rest periods on a cycle ergometer (Monark 839E, Vansbro, Sweden). Heart rate (HR) and peripheral oxygen saturation (SpO2) during each SIE trial were recorded continuously by a pulse oximeter (Radical-7 Pulse CO-Oximeter, Masimo, Irvine, CA, USA), while ratings of perceived exertion (RPE, the Borg 6–20-point scale) were recorded before and immediately after every five cycling bouts, as well as 10 min, 30 min, and 60 min after the SIE. Participants were introduced to the scale and given standardized instructions on how to rate their overall sensations of effort (including the feeling of peripheral working muscles and joints, central cardiovascular and respiratory systems, and the central nervous system [45]) before each exercise session. Peak power, average power, and the fatigue index were calculated using Monark Anaerobic Test software (Sports Medicine Industries, Inc., St. Cloud, MN, USA). The fatigue index was analyzed to determine the level of fatigue during the anaerobic exercise [46]. A higher fatigue index indicated a lower ability to maintain anaerobic performance over a series of sprints. The exercise HR was estimated using the average HR during the entire exercise session. The HRmax was determined as the highest value attained when the V . O2max test was performed during the familiarization session. The percentage of HRmax was estimated using the ratio between exercise HR and HRmax.

2.4. Cognitive Test

For the cognitive task, the color–word matching Stroop test [47] was adopted to reflect prefrontal cortex function. A laptop with a pre-established E-Prime program was used to administrate the Stroop test. For each trial, two lines of letters were displayed on the laptop screen, and participants were instructed to determine whether the color of the letters on the top line matched the name of the color displayed on the bottom line (Figure 2), and to input their response by pressing “F” or “J” buttons to provide a “yes” or “no” choice with their index fingers. Each experimental session consisted of 30 trials, including 10 neutral trials, 10 congruent trials, and 10 incongruent trials, which appeared in a random order. For the neutral trial, the top line contained a group of Xs (XXXX) printed in red, green, blue, or yellow, and the bottom line contained the word “RED”, “GREEN”, “BLUE,” or “YELLOW” printed in black. For the congruent trial, the top line contained the word “RED”, “GREEN”, “BLUE,” or “YELLOW” printed in a congruent color. For the incongruent trial, the word “RED”, “GREEN”, “BLUE,” or “YELLOW” on the top line was printed in an incongruent color. All the word stimuli were presented in Chinese. To achieve sequential visual attention, the upper line was displayed 100 ms before the lower line (Schroeter et al., 2002). The rate of correct answers assigned to “yes” or “no” was 50% each. The stimulus disappeared from the screen when a response was given, or remained on the screen for 2 s. The correct answer rate (ACC) and reaction time (RT) for each type of trial (i.e., neutral, congruent, and incongruent trials) were calculated to measure the prefrontal cortex function.

2.5. Blood Glucose Measures

Arterialized venous blood samples from capillary finger-prick samples were collected before meals (baseline) and at 90 min (just before exercise), 120 min (10 min after exercise), 135 (30 min after exercise), and 150 min (60 min after exercise) after meals. Duplicate measures were analyzed with a portable glucometer (ACCU-CHEK Active, Roche Diagnostics, Indianapolis, IN, USA) that met the accuracy standards outlined by ISO 15197. If the measurement error between the first two blood samples exceeded 1.0 mmol∙L−1, one or two additional samples were taken. The average measurement from the two blood samples with the least variance was documented as the glucose concentration for the corresponding time point.

2.6. Physical Activity and Diet

In order to exclude the potential influence of diet and daily physical activity on outcome variables, six standard meals (i.e., 3 lunches and 3 dinners) were provided to each participant on the day of trials that included 1600–1620 kcal of total energy, with carbohydrate, protein, and fat accounting for approximately 60%, 10%, and 30%, respectively, of the total energy intake. Lunch and dinner were provided at 1:00 p.m. and 6:00 p.m., respectively, and the participants were required to finish in 15 min. Moreover, participants were instructed to refrain from coffee and alcohol, as well as strenuous exercise, for 48 h before the experiment days. Routine physical activities were assessed using pedometers (Yamax Digi-Walker SW-200, Tokyo, Japan) on one day before and on the day of the experiment.

2.7. Statistical Analyses

The PASW software (Release 22.0; IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) was used for statistical analyses. Prior to main statistical analyses, the Shapiro–Wilk test was conducted to confirm the normal distribution of the outcome variables. The data on cognitive function and psychological responses were analyzed using a two-way analysis of variance with two within-factors of condition (N, M, and S), and time (baseline; pre-exercise; and 10, 30, and 60 min after exercise). Significant interactions were further analyzed using a one-way repeated-measure test and/or paired test when necessary. The effect size of partial eta squared (η2) was calculated to determine the main and interaction effects. The effect size was considered small if η2 < 0.06 and large if η2 > 0.14 [48]. Cohen’s d values were used to access the effect sizes for the difference between the exercise trial and the control, and scores of 0.2, 0.5, and >0.8 were classified as small, moderate, and large, respectively [49]. All data are expressed as the mean ± standard deviation (SD), and p < 0.05 was taken as the level of statistical significance.

3. Results

3.1. Daily Activity and Baseline Values

No differences in routine physical activities were found among the three trials one day before (N vs. M vs. S = 8874 ± 3365 vs. 9427 ± 5112 vs. 8617 ± 4008 steps) and the experimental day (N vs. M vs. S = 7572 ± 3334 vs. 7536 ± 4231 vs. 7293 ± 2607 steps), despite fewer steps on the experimental day (p > 0.05).
The percentages of correct estimation of oxygen conditions were 30.0% in normoxia (N), 70.4% in moderate hypoxia (M), and 65% in severe hypoxia (S), indicating that the participants could not recognize the oxygen conditions (p = 0.06). Baseline measurements for cognitive function and blood glucose were taken at time points of baseline and pre-exercise, as shown in Table 1. As expected, the pre-exercise SpO2 was significantly lower in hypoxia compared to normoxia (p < 0.001), confirming the hypoxic conditions. Overall RT (p = 0.952) and ACC (p = 0.346) were not significantly different between baseline and pre-exercise (Figure 3 and Figure 4).

3.2. Exercise Performance and Physiological Responses

Peak power output was significantly lower in the S trial compared to that in the N trial (p < 0.01, Table 2). Mean power output was significantly higher in the M (6.1 ± 0.7 W∙kg−1) and S (5.9 ± 0.9 W∙kg−1) trials compared to that in N (6.4 ± 0.8 W∙kg−1, p < 0.01). There were no differences in the fatigue index, exercise HR, and percentage of HRmax among the three trials (p > 0.05).
The physiological responses (SpO2, HR, and RPE20) to SIE in normoxia and hypoxia are displayed in Table 1. There were significant interaction effects (F = 119.374, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.992) on SpO2 among the three trials. Specifically, SpO2 was significantly lower in the M and S trials compared to the N trial at pre-exercise, and immediately and 10 min after SIE (F = 365.884, p <0.001, ƞ2 = 0.976). In the M and S trials, SpO2 was significantly lower at the time points of pre-exercise, immediately, and 10 min postexercise compared to that of the baseline (F = 446.551, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.993). Significant interaction effects were also found in the HR levels (F = 4.682, p = 0.011, ƞ2 = 0.824); the HR before exercise was higher in the M and S trials compared to the N trial (F = 11.082, p = 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.552), and the HR levels measured in the later periods were lower than those measured in the preceding periods during the N, M, and S trials (F = 1613.675, p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.998). There were no differences in self-reported RPE between the three oxygen conditions (i.e., normoxia, 2500 m hypoxia, and 5000 m hypoxia; p = 0.993, ƞ2 < 0.001), suggesting that the three trails induced similar physical exertion at the same time point. However, participants perceived more effort immediately after exercise compared to that perceived at baseline and pre-exercise (p < 0.001, ƞ2 = 0.966).

3.3. Cognitive Function and Blood Glucose

After SIE, the overall reaction time of the Stroop test was significantly reduced at 10 min after exercise as compared to that of the baseline value (p = 0.003, ƞ2 = 0.606), which was significantly higher at Post 60 when compared to Post 10 (Figure 3A). As for particular trials, the reaction time at 10 min after SIE was significantly shorter than that of the baseline in congruent trials (p < 0.05; Figure 3B), while the reaction times at both 10 min and 30 min after SIE were significantly shorter than that measured at baseline in incongruent trials (p < 0.05; Figure 3C). In terms of accuracy, no significant differences were found across the three trials at any time points (p = 0.446, ƞ2 = 0.415, Figure 4). Additionally, blood glucose was significantly reduced at 10 min and 30 min after SIE when compared to pre-exercise in all trials (p < 0.05, Figure 5).

4. Discussion

This study was the first trial to demonstrate the effects of SIE under normoxia and moderate and severe hypoxia on cognitive function and metabolic health in active males. Our result showed that acute SIE, regardless of oxygen conditions, facilitated cognitive function, and the lasting effect was sustained for at least 10–30 min. We also demonstrated that the RT improvement was unaffected by glucose alterations.
The main findings of this study were that SIE with or without hypoxia decreased RT, and this improvement did not sacrifice ACC in both conditions, indicating that this effect could improve cognitive function independently if it was normoxia, moderate hypoxia, or severe hypoxia. No detrimental effect was observed under exposure to a significant hypoxic-induced oxidative stress condition, which was strongly associated with greater reductions in cognitive function [32] by inducing low partial pressure of oxygen (PaO2) levels, indicated by the SpO2 decline from 98% to 89% and 78% in the moderate hypoxic SIE trial and severe hypoxic SIE trial, respectively, which may have been due to a compensation of the benefit of exercise-induced cerebral blood flow. It has been suggested that increased CBF and cerebral metabolism that maintained oxygen delivery to the brain during exercise are associated with cognitive improvement. Another possible reason for the stable cognitive performance may be attributed to the delivery of reserve oxygen. Under severe hypoxia, peak power and average power were significantly lower than N, with a small to medium Cohen’s d effect size (d = 0.37) of the fatigue index. A higher fatigue index in severe hypoxic SIE showed that sprinting under severe hypoxia made it more difficult to maintain the power than in other conditions, which induced a significant drop in the power output. It is plausible that the decrement in the performance was associated with the delivery of reserve oxygen to maintain homeostasis in the whole-body biological requirement and cerebral function, other than the exercising skeletal muscle, to prevent cognitive impairment.
In our study, overall RT and RT in congruent tasks improved following SIE regardless of oxygen conditions, and the effect was sustained for 10 min during the recovery phase and then returned to the baseline value; whereas the incongruent RT, a more sensitive type of task to detect the performance decrement based on the premise that it activated the neurobehavioral probe for the region associated with selective attention in a more challenging way [50], persisted for 30 min in both the normoxic and hypoxic SIE trials and returned to the baseline value after 60 min. In line with our results, SIE was previously found to improve cognitive performance as determined by a Stroop test, and the effect persisted for up to 30 min [20]; while another experimental study reported that intermittent exercise improved inhibitory control as assessed by a flanker test, and it was sustained for at least 60 min [51]. However, a negative effect was also found in a prolonged exercise that exposed the participants to an altitude of 5486 m, as assessed using visual perception, for up to 24 h [52]. Possible pathways by which acute hypoxic SIE may improve cognition included increasing cerebral blood flow [53] and enhancing the arousal level [20] and neurometabolism [54] in the brain to a positive level that led to an improvement in cognitive performance. Taken together, despite inconclusive results and an inapparent effect between oxygen conditions and different levels of hypoxia, the lasting effect of hypoxic SIE on cognitive function was significant in this study, regardless of oxygen conditions.
In our study, all trials showed a significant rise in blood glucose value at 90 min after meal ingestion and a significant drop after SIE and hypoxic SIE, and maintained a level lower than the pre-exercise value for at least 60 min. Our results were consistent with a recent study in healthy subjects that suggested that blood glucose remained stable under a hypoxic condition [55]. A recent review demonstrated that the mobilization of energy provisions augmented by hypoxic-induced physiological demand resulted in gluconeogenesis and glycogenolysis [56], and favored the energy balance in the competition of glucose demand. Others reported that exercise under hypoxia was expected to be efficient in glucose metabolism because it would further activate the exercise-induced glucose transport pathway, especially in SIE, which is characterized by its nature of high exercise intensity [57,58]. A possible mechanism is that lower oxygen availability would increase the higher proportion of glucose metabolism activated by sympathetic nervous system [59,60]. Our observations of cognitive performance during pre- and postprandial blood glucose excursion showed that cognitive performance was maintained as stable while postprandial glucose levels rose. In addition, the beneficial effects of SIE and the two levels of hypoxic SIE on cognitive performance were unaffected by the glucose drop at the postexercise time point compared to the preprandial glucose baseline value. Overall, our study showed that normoxic SIE and two different levels of hypoxic SIE significantly improved cognitive function, accompanied and unaffected by the altered blood glucose level.
There were several limitations to this study. Firstly, it was technically difficult to measure Post 0 due to the nature of SIE, so that immediate response was lacking in this study. In addition, the main effect of hypoxia needs to be clarified by a seated nonexercising trial with participants exposed to hypoxia for the same duration of time of the experimental trials. Our strengths were that we observed cognitive performance during a prescribed physiological state at successive multiple time points. This study provided evidence that cognitive improvement remains stable at both normoxic SIE and two levels of hypoxia, and it is also unaffected during blood glucose excursions. We used prolonged time points to investigate the lasting effects on cognition and blood glucose, which helped to provide information on the responsiveness of cognitive function following hypoxic SIE over successive time points, and how glycemic changes influenced cognition.

5. Conclusions

Similar positive effects on cognitive function were found in normoxia, moderate hypoxia, and severe hypoxia following acute bouts of SIE. Cognitive improvements lasted for at least 10–30 min, with neither losing accuracy nor being affected by an altered blood glucose level. Further research is warranted to elucidate the effects of different levels of hypoxia on cognitive performance with multiple tasks and gather data on cerebral blood flow to provide insight into brain activity, as well as the role of blood glucose in relation to the cognitive performance in active young adults, to better clarify the underlying mechanisms for the interaction between cognition and glucose metabolism. This study has implications for the use of SIE and hypoxia as a time-saving and more effective strategy to enhance physiological health [8,9,10] without cognitive impairment.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.N. and Z.K.; methodology, O.-K.L. and Z.K.; software, S.S. and Q.S.; validation, O.-K.L. and Z.K.; formal analysis, S.S., J.N. and Q.S.; investigation, O.-K.L. and Z.K.; resources, Z.K.; data curation, O.-K.L.; writing—original draft preparation, O.-K.L., S.S. and J.N.; writing—review and editing, all authors; visualization, S.S. and Q.S.; supervision, Z.K.; project administration, O.-K.L.; funding acquisition, Z.K. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the University of Macau under grant number MYRG2018-00216-FED. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the UM.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of University of Macau (protocol code MYRG2018-00216-FED; date of approval 2 May 2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Written informed consent was obtained from the patient(s) to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available upon request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to the wishes of some of the subjects.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the participants of this study for their time.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Costigan, S.; Eather, N.; Plotnikoff, R.; Hillman, C.; Lubans, D. High intensity interval training on cognitive and mental health in adolescents. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2017, 20, e108–e109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Gibala, M.J.; McGee, S.L. Metabolic Adaptations to Short-Term High-Intensity Interval Training: A Little Pain for a Lot of Gain? Exerc. Sport Sci. Rev. 2008, 36, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Warburton, D.E.R.; McKenzie, D.C.; Haykowsky, M.J.; Taylor, A.; Shoemaker, P.; Ignaszewski, A.P.; Chan, S.Y. Effectiveness of High-Intensity Interval Training for the Rehabilitation of Patients With Coronary Artery Disease. Am. J. Cardiol. 2005, 95, 1080–1084. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Sloth, M.; Sloth, D.; Overgaard, K.; Dalgas, U. Effects of sprint interval training on VO2max and aerobic exercise performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis: Effects of Sprint Interval Training on VO2max. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2013, 23, e341–e352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Camacho-Cardenosa, A.; Camacho-Cardenosa, M.; Brooks, D.; Timón, R.; Olcina, G.; Brazo-Sayavera, J. Effects training in hypoxia on cardiometabolic parameters in obese people: A systematic review of randomized controlled trial. Aten. Primaria. 2019, 51, 397–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Hobbins, L.G.; Hunter, S.; Gaoua, N.; Girard, O. Normobaric hypoxic conditioning to maximize weight loss and ameliorate cardio-metabolic health in obese populations: A systematic review. Am. J. Physiol. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2017, 313, R251–R264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  7. Kong, Z.; Sun, S.; Liu, M.; Shi, Q. Short-Term High-Intensity Interval Training on Body Composition and Blood Glucose in Overweight and Obese Young Women. J. Diabetes Res. 2016, 2016, 4073618. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kong, Z.; Shi, Q.; Nie, J.; Tong, T.K.; Song, L.; Yi, L.; Hu, Y. High-Intensity Interval Training in Normobaric Hypoxia Improves Cardiorespiratory Fitness in Overweight Chinese Young Women. Front. Physiol. 2017, 8, 175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Camacho-Cardenosa, A.; Camacho-Cardenosa, M.; Brazo-Sayavera, J.; Timón, R.; González-Custodio, A.; Olcina, G. Repeated sprint in hypoxia as a time-metabolic efficient strategy to improve physical fitness of obese women. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2020, 120, 1051–1061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Camacho-Cardenosa, A.; Camacho-Cardenosa, M.; Brazo-Sayavera, J.; Burtscher, M.; Timón, R.; Olcina, G. Effects of High-Intensity Interval Training Under Normobaric Hypoxia on Cardiometabolic Risk Markers in Overweight/Obese Women. High Alt. Med. Biol. 2018, 19, 356–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Navarrete-Opazo, A.; Mitchell, G.S. Therapeutic potential of intermittent hypoxia: A matter of dose. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2014, 307, R1181–R1197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Powell, F.L.; Milsom, W.K.; Mitchell, G.S. Time domains of the hypoxic ventilatory response. Respir. Physiol. 1998, 112, 123–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. McMorris, T.; Hale, B.J.; Barwood, M.; Costello, J.; Corbett, J. Effect of acute hypoxia on cognition: A systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2017, 74, 225–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Petrassi, F.A.; Hodkinson, P.D.; Walters, P.L.; Gaydos, S.J. Hypoxic hypoxia at moderate altitudes: Review of the state of the science. Aviat. Space Environ. Med. 2012, 83, 975–984. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Chang, Y.K.; Labban, J.D.; Gapin, J.I.; Etnier, J.L. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Brain Res. 2012, 1453, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Lambourne, K.; Tomporowski, P. The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: A meta-regression analysis. Brain Res. 2010, 1341, 12–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Rąglewska, P. Dancing as Non-Pharmacological Treatment for Healthy Aging in the COVID-19 Era; a Gerontological Perspective. TSS 2021, 28, 173–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Alves, C.R.R.; Tessaro, V.H.; Teixeira, L.A.C.; Murakava, K.; Roschel, H.; Gualano, B.; Takito, M.Y. Influence of Acute High-Intensity Aerobic Interval Exercise Bout on Selective Attention and Short-Term Memory Tasks. Percept. Mot. Ski. 2014, 118, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Winter, B.; Breitenstein, C.; Mooren, F.C.; Voelker, K.; Fobker, M.; Lechtermann, A.; Krueger, K.; Fromme, A.; Korsukewitz, C.; Floel, A.; et al. High impact running improves learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2007, 87, 597–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Tsukamoto, H.; Suga, T.; Takenaka, S.; Tanaka, D.; Takeuchi, T.; Hamaoka, T.; Isaka, T.; Hashimoto, T. Greater impact of acute high-intensity interval exercise on post-exercise executive function compared to moderate-intensity continuous exercise. Physiol. Behav. 2016, 155, 224–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Ando, S.; Hatamoto, Y.; Sudo, M.; Kiyonaga, A.; Tanaka, H.; Higaki, Y. The Effects of Exercise Under Hypoxia on Cognitive Function. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e63630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  22. de Aquino-Lemos, V.; Santos, R.V.T.; Antunes, H.K.M.; Lira, F.S.; Bittar, I.G.L.; Caris, A.V.; Tufik, S.; de Mello, M.T. Acute physical exercise under hypoxia improves sleep, mood and reaction time. Physiol. Behav. 2016, 154, 90–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Komiyama, T.; Sudo, M.; Higaki, Y.; Kiyonaga, A.; Tanaka, H.; Ando, S. Does moderate hypoxia alter working memory and executive function during prolonged exercise? Physiol. Behav. 2015, 139, 290–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Komiyama, T.; Katayama, K.; Sudo, M.; Ishida, K.; Higaki, Y.; Ando, S. Cognitive function during exercise under severe hypoxia. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 10000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Lei, O.-K.; Kong, Z.; Loprinzi, P.D.; Shi, Q.; Sun, S.; Zou, L.; Hu, Y.; Nie, J. Severe Hypoxia Does Not Offset the Benefits of Exercise on Cognitive Function in Sedentary Young Women. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Seo, Y.; Burns, K.; Fennell, C.; Kim, J.-H.; Gunstad, J.; Glickman, E.; McDaniel, J. The Influence of Exercise on Cognitive Performance in Normobaric Hypoxia. High Alt. Med. Biol. 2015, 16, 298–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Jung, M.; Zou, L.; Yu, J.J.; Ryu, S.; Kong, Z.; Yang, L.; Kang, M.; Lin, J.; Li, H.; Smith, L.; et al. Does exercise have a protective effect on cognitive function under hypoxia? A systematic review with meta-analysis. J. Sport Health Sci. 2020, 9, 562–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Bouak, F.; Vartanian, O.; Hofer, K.; Cheung, B. Acute Mild Hypoxic Hypoxia Effects on Cognitive and Simulated Aircraft Pilot Performance. Aerosp. Med. Hum. Perform. 2018, 89, 526–535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Dobashi, S.; Horiuchi, M.; Endo, J.; Kiuchi, M.; Koyama, K. Cognitive Function and Cerebral Oxygenation During Prolonged Exercise Under Hypoxia in Healthy Young Males. High Alt. Med. Biol. 2016, 17, 214–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Kim, C.-H.; Ryan, E.J.; Seo, Y.; Peacock, C.; Gunstad, J.; Muller, M.D.; Ridgel, A.L.; Glickman, E.L. Low intensity exercise does not impact cognitive function during exposure to normobaric hypoxia. Physiol. Behav. 2015, 151, 24–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Lefferts, W.K.; Babcock, M.; Tiss, M.J.; Ives, S.J.; White, C.N.; Brutsaert, T.D.; Heffernan, K.S. Effect of hypoxia on cerebrovascular and cognitive function during moderate intensity exercise. Physiol. Behav. 2016, 165, 108–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Ochi, G.; Kanazawa, Y.; Hyodo, K.; Suwabe, K.; Shimizu, T.; Fukuie, T.; Byun, K.; Soya, H. Hypoxia-induced lowered executive function depends on arterial oxygen desaturation. J. Physiol. Sci. 2018, 68, 847–853. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Shannon, O.M.; Duckworth, L.; Barlow, M.J.; Deighton, K.; Matu, J.; Williams, E.L.; Woods, D.; Xie, L.; Stephan, B.C.M.; Siervo, M.; et al. Effects of Dietary Nitrate Supplementation on Physiological Responses, Cognitive Function, and Exercise Performance at Moderate and Very-High Simulated Altitude. Front. Physiol. 2017, 8, 401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Sun, S.; Loprinzi, P.D.; Guan, H.; Zou, L.; Kong, Z.; Hu, Y.; Shi, Q.; Nie, J. The Effects of High-Intensity Interval Exercise and Hypoxia on Cognition in Sedentary Young Adults. Medicina 2019, 55, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Subudhi, A.W.; Dimmen, A.C.; Roach, R.C. Effects of acute hypoxia on cerebral and muscle oxygenation during incremental exercise. J. Appl. Physiol. 2007, 103, 177–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Goodall, S.; Twomey, R.; Amann, M. Acute and chronic hypoxia: Implications for cerebral function and exercise tolerance. Fatigue 2014, 2, 73–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Cockcroft, E.J.; Williams, C.A.; Weaver, H.; O’Connor, A.; Jackman, S.R.; Armstrong, N.; Barker, A.R. Acute Exercise and Insulin Sensitivity in Boys: A Time-Course Study. Int. J. Sports Med. 2017, 38, 967–974. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Kong, Z.; Shi, Q.; Sun, S.; Tong, T.K.; Zhang, H.; Nie, J. High-intensity interval exercise lowers postprandial glucose concentrations more in obese adults than lean adults. Prim. Care Diabetes 2019, 13, 568–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Nie, J.; Kong, Z.; Baker, J.S.; Tong, T.K.; Lei, S.H.; Shi, Q. Acute changes in glycemic homeostasis in response to brief high-intensity intermittent exercise in obese adults. J. Exerc. Sci. Fit. 2012, 10, 97–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Gillen, J.B.; Little, J.P.; Punthakee, Z.; Tarnopolsky, M.A.; Riddell, M.C.; Gibala, M.J. Acute high-intensity interval exercise reduces the postprandial glucose response and prevalence of hyperglycaemia in patients with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Obes. Metab. 2012, 14, 575–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Mackenzie, R.; Maxwell, N.; Castle, P.; Brickley, G.; Watt, P. Acute hypoxia and exercise improve insulin sensitivity (SI2*) in individuals with type 2 diabetes. Diabetes/Metab. Res. Rev. 2011, 27, 94–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Gold, P.E. Role of glucose in regulating the brain and cognition. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 1995, 61, 987S–995S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Benton, D.; Parker, P.Y.; Donohoe, R.T. The supply of glucose to the brain and cognitive functioning. J. Biosoc. Sci. 1996, 28, 463–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Graveling, A.J.; Deary, I.J.; Frier, B.M. Acute Hypoglycemia Impairs Executive Cognitive Function in Adults With and Without Type 1 Diabetes. Diabetes Care 2013, 36, 3240–3246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Borg, G.A. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1982, 14, 377–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. William, B. Gene Adams Exercise Physiology Laboratory Manual; McGraw-Hill Education: New York, NY, USA, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  47. Yanagisawa, H.; Dan, I.; Tsuzuki, D.; Kato, M.; Okamoto, M.; Kyutoku, Y.; Soya, H. Acute moderate exercise elicits increased dorsolateral prefrontal activation and improves cognitive performance with Stroop test. NeuroImage 2010, 50, 1702–1710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Levine, T.R.; Hullett, C.R. Eta Squared, Partial Eta Squared, and Misreporting of Effect Size in Communication Research. Hum. Commun. Res. 2002, 28, 612–625. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Routledge: London, UK, 2013; ISBN 978-1-134-74270-7. [Google Scholar]
  50. Carter, C.S.; Mintun, M.; Cohen, J.D. Interference and Facilitation Effects during Selective Attention: An H215O PET Study of Stroop Task Performance. NeuroImage 1995, 2, 264–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Ludyga, S.; Pühse, U.; Lucchi, S.; Marti, J.; Gerber, M. Immediate and sustained effects of intermittent exercise on inhibitory control and task-related heart rate variability in adolescents. J. Sci. Med. Sport 2018, 22, 96–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Phillips, J.B.; Hørning, D.; Funke, M.E. Cognitive and Perceptual Deficits of Normobaric Hypoxia and the Time Course to Performance Recovery. Aerosp. Med. Hum. Perform. 2015, 86, 357–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Ogoh, S.; Ainslie, P.N. Cerebral blood flow during exercise: Mechanisms of regulation. J. Appl. Physiol. 2009, 107, 1370–1380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  54. McMorris, T.; Collard, K.; Corbett, J.; Dicks, M.; Swain, J.P. A test of the catecholamines hypothesis for an acute exercise–cognition interaction. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 2008, 89, 106–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Chan, J.S.; Chiew, A.E.; Rimke, A.N.; Chan, G.; Rampuri, Z.H.; Kozak, M.D.; Boulé, N.G.; Steinback, C.D.; Davenport, M.H.; Day, T.A. Blood glucose concentration is unchanged during exposure to acute normobaric hypoxia in healthy humans. Physiol. Rep. 2021, 9, e14932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Katayama, K.; Goto, K.; Ishida, K.; Ogita, F. Substrate utilization during exercise and recovery at moderate altitude. Metabolism 2010, 59, 959–966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Wadley, G.D.; Lee-Young, R.S.; Canny, B.J.; Wasuntarawat, C.; Chen, Z.P.; Hargreaves, M.; Kemp, B.E.; McConell, G.K. Effect of exercise intensity and hypoxia on skeletal muscle AMPK signaling and substrate metabolism in humans. Am. J. Physiol. Endocrinol. Metab. 2006, 290, E694–E702. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Kelly, K.R.; Williamson, D.L.; Fealy, C.E.; Kriz, D.A.; Krishnan, R.K.; Huang, H.; Ahn, J.; Loomis, J.L.; Kirwan, J.P. Acute altitude-induced hypoxia suppresses plasma glucose and leptin in healthy humans. Metabolism 2010, 59, 200–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  59. Chiasson, J.-L.; Shikama, H.; Chu, D.T.W.; Exton, J.H. Inhibitory Effect of Epinephrine on Insulin-stimulated Glucose Uptake by Rat Skeletal Muscle. J. Clin. Investig. 1981, 68, 706–713. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Goto, K.; Morishima, T.; Kurobe, K.; Huang, Z.; Ogita, F. Augmented Carbohydrate Oxidation under Moderate Hypobaric Hypoxia Equivalent to Simulated Altitude of 2500 m. Tohoku J. Exp. Med. 2015, 236, 163–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Flow diagram of the study.
Figure 1. Flow diagram of the study.
Jcm 11 03159 g001
Figure 2. Instances of single trials for the neutral, congruent, and incongruent conditions of the color–word matching Stroop task.
Figure 2. Instances of single trials for the neutral, congruent, and incongruent conditions of the color–word matching Stroop task.
Jcm 11 03159 g002
Figure 3. Overall (A), congruent (C-RT, B), and incongruent reaction time (INC-RT, C) in the Stroop test in responses to sprint interval exercise (SIE) under normoxia (20.9% O2) and hypoxia (15.4%O2 and 11.2%). The shaded part indicates oxygen condition. N: SIE in normoxia; M: SIE in moderate hypoxia; S: SIE in severe hypoxia; Pre: pre-exercise; Post 10: 10 min after exercise; Post 30: 30 min after exercise; Post 60: 60 min after exercise. † p < 0.05 vs. baseline; ‡ p < 0.05 vs. Pre; § p < 0.05 vs. Post 10; ¥ p < 0.05 vs. Post 30.
Figure 3. Overall (A), congruent (C-RT, B), and incongruent reaction time (INC-RT, C) in the Stroop test in responses to sprint interval exercise (SIE) under normoxia (20.9% O2) and hypoxia (15.4%O2 and 11.2%). The shaded part indicates oxygen condition. N: SIE in normoxia; M: SIE in moderate hypoxia; S: SIE in severe hypoxia; Pre: pre-exercise; Post 10: 10 min after exercise; Post 30: 30 min after exercise; Post 60: 60 min after exercise. † p < 0.05 vs. baseline; ‡ p < 0.05 vs. Pre; § p < 0.05 vs. Post 10; ¥ p < 0.05 vs. Post 30.
Jcm 11 03159 g003
Figure 4. Overall (A), congruent (C-ACC, B), and incongruent accuracy (INC-ACC, C) in the Stroop test in responses to sprint interval exercise (SIE) under normoxia (20.9% O2) and hypoxia (15.4%O2, and 11.2%). The shaded part indicates oxygen condition. N: SIE in normoxia; M: SIE in moderate hypoxia; S: SIE in severe hypoxia; Pre: pre-exercise; Post 10: 10 min after exercise; Post 30: 30 min after exercise; Post 60: 60 min after exercise.
Figure 4. Overall (A), congruent (C-ACC, B), and incongruent accuracy (INC-ACC, C) in the Stroop test in responses to sprint interval exercise (SIE) under normoxia (20.9% O2) and hypoxia (15.4%O2, and 11.2%). The shaded part indicates oxygen condition. N: SIE in normoxia; M: SIE in moderate hypoxia; S: SIE in severe hypoxia; Pre: pre-exercise; Post 10: 10 min after exercise; Post 30: 30 min after exercise; Post 60: 60 min after exercise.
Jcm 11 03159 g004
Figure 5. Glucose responses to sprint interval exercise (SIE) under normoxia (20.9% O2) and hypoxia (15.4%O2, and 11.2%). The shaded part indicates oxygen condition. N: SIE in normoxia; M: SIE in moderate hypoxia; S: SIE in severe hypoxia; Pre: pre-exercise; Post 10: 10 min after exercise; Post 30: 30 min after exercise; Post 60: 60 min after exercise. † p < 0.05 vs. baseline; ‡ p < 0.05 vs. Pre; § p < 0.05 vs. Post 10; ¥ p < 0.05 vs. Post 30.
Figure 5. Glucose responses to sprint interval exercise (SIE) under normoxia (20.9% O2) and hypoxia (15.4%O2, and 11.2%). The shaded part indicates oxygen condition. N: SIE in normoxia; M: SIE in moderate hypoxia; S: SIE in severe hypoxia; Pre: pre-exercise; Post 10: 10 min after exercise; Post 30: 30 min after exercise; Post 60: 60 min after exercise. † p < 0.05 vs. baseline; ‡ p < 0.05 vs. Pre; § p < 0.05 vs. Post 10; ¥ p < 0.05 vs. Post 30.
Jcm 11 03159 g005
Table 1. Physiological responses before and after SIE in normoxia and hypoxia.
Table 1. Physiological responses before and after SIE in normoxia and hypoxia.
NMS
SpO2 (%)
 Baseline98 ± 198 ± 198 ± 1
 Pre98 ± 389 ± 3 *,a78 ± 4 *,**,a,b
 Post 096 ± 384 ± 5 *,a,b74 ± 58 *,**,a,b
 Post 1097 ± 189 ± 4 *,a79 ± 5 *,**,a
 Post 3097 ± 197 ± 2 b,c,d96 ± 3 b,c,d
 Post 6097 ± 197 ± 2 b,c,d97 ± 2 b,c,d
HR (bpm)
 Baseline63 ± 762 ± 664 ± 5
 Pre65 ± 672 ± 8 *,a79 ± 10 *,**,a,b
 Post 0167 ± 9 a,b168 ± 11 a,b168 ± 7 a,b
 Post 1092 ± 11 a,b,c94 ± 16 a,b,c95 ± 12 a,b,c
 Post 3086 ± 11 a,b,c,d86 ± 13 a,b,c,d87 ± 11 a,b,c,d
 Post 6081 ± 10 a,b,c,d,e80 ± 11 a,b,c,d,e80 ±9 a,b,c,d,e
RPE20
 Baseline6 ± 06 ± 16 ± 0
 Pre7 ± 17 ± 17 ± 1
 Post 017 ± 2 a,b18 ± 2 a,b17 ± 2 a,b
 Post 108 ± 2 c8 ± 2 c8 ± 2 c
 Post 307 ± 2 c7 ± 1 c7 ± 2 c
 Post 607 ± 1 c7 ± 1 c6 ± 1 c
N: sprint interval exercise in normoxia; M: sprint interval exercise in moderate hypoxia; S: sprint interval exercise in severe hypoxia; Pre: pre-exercise; Post 0: immediately after exercise; Post 10: 10 min after exercise; Post 30: 30 min after exercise; Post 60: 60 min after exercise. * p <0.05 vs. N, ** p < 0.05 vs. S; a p < 0.05 vs. baseline; b p < 0.05 vs. Pre; c p < 0.05 vs. Post 0; d p < 0.05 vs. Post 10; e p < 0.05 vs. Post 30.
Table 2. Variables of SIE in normoxia, moderate hypoxia, and severe hypoxia.
Table 2. Variables of SIE in normoxia, moderate hypoxia, and severe hypoxia.
NMS
Peak power (W/kg)11.6 ± 1.911.4 ± 1.89.9 ± 2.8 *,#
Average power (W/kg)6.4 ± 0.86.1 ± 0.7 *5.9 ± 0.9 *
Fatigue index (%)45.0 ± 13.648.8 ± 14.951.0 ± 18.4
Exercise HR (bpm)169 ± 9169 ± 11168 ± 9
%HRmax (%)93.1 ± 4.393.2 ± 5.492.8 ± 4.6
N: sprint interval exercise in normoxia; M: sprint interval exercise in moderate hypoxia; S: sprint interval exercise in severe hypoxia. * p < 0.05 vs. N; # p < 0.05 vs. M.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Lei, O.-K.; Sun, S.; Nie, J.; Shi, Q.; Kong, Z. Sprint Interval Exercise Improves Cognitive Performance Unrelated to Postprandial Glucose Fluctuations at Different Levels of Normobaric Hypoxia. J. Clin. Med. 2022, 11, 3159. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm11113159

AMA Style

Lei O-K, Sun S, Nie J, Shi Q, Kong Z. Sprint Interval Exercise Improves Cognitive Performance Unrelated to Postprandial Glucose Fluctuations at Different Levels of Normobaric Hypoxia. Journal of Clinical Medicine. 2022; 11(11):3159. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm11113159

Chicago/Turabian Style

Lei, On-Kei, Shengyan Sun, Jinlei Nie, Qingde Shi, and Zhaowei Kong. 2022. "Sprint Interval Exercise Improves Cognitive Performance Unrelated to Postprandial Glucose Fluctuations at Different Levels of Normobaric Hypoxia" Journal of Clinical Medicine 11, no. 11: 3159. https://doi.org/10.3390/jcm11113159

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop