Somatic Marker Production Deficits Do Not Explain the Relationship between Psychopathic Traits and Utilitarian Moral Decision Making
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Materials and Methods
3. Results
3.1. Correlations
3.2. Mediation Analyses
4. Discussion
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
References
- Blair, R.J.R. The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2013, 14, 786–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gao, Y.; Tang, S. Psychopathic personality and utilitarian moral judgment in college students. J. Crim. Justice 2013, 41, 342–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Gillespie, S.M.; Mitchell, I.J.; Satherley, R.-M.; Beech, A.R.; Rotshtein, P. Relations of distinct psychopathic personality traits with anxiety and fear: Findings from offenders and non-offenders. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0143120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Waller, R.; Gardner, F.; Hyde, L.W. What are the associations between parenting, callous–unemotional traits, and antisocial behavior in youth? A systematic review of evidence. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2013, 33, 593–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Zeier, J.D.; Newman, J.P. Feature-based attention and conflict monitoring in criminal offenders: Interactive relations of psychopathy with anxiety and externalizing. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 2013, 122, 797–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Stevens, G.W.; Deuling, J.K.; Armenakis, A.A. Successful psychopaths: Are they unethical decision-makers and why? J. Bus. Ethics 2012, 105, 139–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Glenn, A.L.; Koleva, S.; Iyer, R.; Graham, J.; Ditto, P.H. Moral identity in psychopathy. Judgm. Decis. Mak. 2010, 5, 497–505. [Google Scholar]
- Foot, P. The problem of abortion and the doctrine of double effect. Oxf. Rev. 1967, 5, 5–15. [Google Scholar]
- Cushman, F.; Young, L.; Hauser, M. The role of conscious reasoning and intuition in moral judgment: Testing three principles of harm. Psychol. Sci. 2006, 17, 1082–1089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greene, J.D.; Sommerville, R.B.; Nystrom, L.E.; Darley, J.M.; Cohen, J.D. An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science 2001, 293, 2105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Conway, P.; Gawronski, B. Deontological and utilitarian inclinations in moral decision making: A process dissociation approach. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 104, 216–235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Blair, R.J.R. A cognitive developmental approach to morality: Investigating the psychopath. Cognition 1995, 57, 1–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blair, R.J.R.; White, S.F.; Meffert, H.; Hwang, S. Emotional learning and the development of differential moralities: Implications from research on psychopathy. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2013, 1299, 36–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Bechara, A.; Damasio, A.R. The somatic marker hypothesis: A neural theory of economic decision. Games Econ. Behav. 2005, 52, 336–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Damasio, A. Testing the somatic marker hypothesis. In Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain; Penguin: New York, NY, USA, 1994; pp. 205–222. [Google Scholar]
- Li, X.; Lu, Z.-L.; D’Argembeau, A.; Ng, M.; Bechara, A. The Iowa Gambling Task in fMRI images. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2009, 31, 410–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Paulus, M.P.; Rogalsky, C.; Simmons, A.; Feinstein, J.S.; Stein, M.B. Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism. NeuroImage 2003, 19, 1439–1448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poppa, T.; Bechara, A. The somatic marker hypothesis: Revisiting the role of the ‘body-loop’ in decision-making. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2018, 19, 61–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dawson, M.E.; Schell, A.M.; Courtney, C.G. The skin conductance response, anticipation, and decision-making. J. Neurosci. Psychol. Econ. 2011, 4, 111–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wright, R.J.; Rakow, T. Don’t sweat it: Re-examining the somatic marker hypothesis using variants of the Balloon Analogue Risk Task. Decision 2017, 4, 52–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hein, G.; Lamm, C.; Brodbeck, C.; Singer, T. Skin Conductance Response to the Pain of Others Predicts Later Costly Helping. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e22759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Cushman, F.; Gray, K.; Gaffey, A.; Mendes, W.B. Simulating murder: The aversion to harmful action. Emotion 2012, 12, 2–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- van Honk, J.; Hermans, E.J.; Putman, P.; Montagne, B.; Schutter, D.J. Defective somatic markers in sub-clinical psychopathy. Neuroreport Rapid Commun. Neurosci. Res. 2002, 13, 1025–1027. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Fanti, K.A.; Kyranides, M.N.; Georgiou, G.; Petridou, M.; Colins, O.F.; Tuvblad, C.; Andershed, H. Callous-unemotional, impulsive-irresponsible, and grandiose-manipulative traits: Distinct associations with heart rate, skin conductance, and startle responses to violent and erotic scenes: Psychopathic traits and physiological measures. Psychophysiology 2017, 54, 663–672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hare, R.D.; Craigen, D. Psychopathy and Physiological Activity In a Mixed-Motive Game Situation. Psychophysiology 1974, 11, 197–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ragsdale, K.A.; Mitchell, J.C.; Cassisi, J.E.; Bedwell, J.S. Comorbidity of schizotypy and psychopathy: Skin conductance to affective pictures. Psychiatry Res. 2013, 210, 1000–1007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McDonald, M.; Defever, A.; Navarrete, C. Killing for the greater good: Action aversion and the emotional inhibition of harm in moral dilemmas. Evol. Hum. Behav. 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moretto, G.; Làdavas, E.; Mattioli, F.; Di Pellegrino, G. A psychophysiological investigation of moral judgment after ventromedial prefrontal damage. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2010, 22, 1888–1899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bartels, D.M.; Pizarro, D.A. The mismeasure of morals: Antisocial personality traits predict utilitarian responses to moral dilemmas. Cognition 2011, 121, 154–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koenigs, M.; Kruepke, M.; Zeier, J.; Newman, J.P. Utilitarian moral judgment in psychopathy. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 2012, 7, 708–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Djeriouat, H.; Trémolière, B. The Dark Triad of personality and utilitarian moral judgment: The mediating role of Honesty/Humility and Harm/Care. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2014, 67, 11–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kahane, G.; Everett, J.A.C.; Earp, B.D.; Farias, M.; Savulescu, J. ‘Utilitarian’ judgments in sacrificial moral dilemmas do not reflect impartial concern for the greater good. Cognition 2015, 134, 193–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tassy, S.; Deruelle, C.; Mancini, J.; Leistedt, S.; Wicker, B. High levels of psychopathic traits alters moral choice but not moral judgment. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Crockett, M.J.; Clark, L.; Hauser, M.D.; Robbins, T.W. Serotonin selectively influences moral judgment and behavior through effects on harm aversion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2010, 107, 17433–17438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- López, R.; Poy, R.; Patrick, C.J.; Moltó, J. Deficient fear conditioning and self-reported psychopathy: The role of fearless dominance: Fear conditioning and fearless dominance. Psychophysiology 2013, 50, 210–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rothemund, Y.; Ziegler, S.; Hermann, C.; Gruesser, S.M.; Foell, J.; Patrick, C.J.; Flor, H. Fear conditioning in psychopaths: Event-related potentials and peripheral measures. Biol. Psychol. 2012, 90, 50–59. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Miller, J.D.; Gaughan, E.T.; Pryor, L.R. The Levenson Self-Report Psychopathy Scale: An examination of the personality traits and disorders associated with the LSRP factors. Assessment 2008, 15, 450–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Levenson, M.R.; Kiehl, K.A.; Fitzpatrick, C.M. Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 68, 151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Greene, J.D. Why are VMPFC patients more utilitarian? A dual-process theory of moral judgment explains. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2007, 11, 322–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Crone, E.A.; Van Der Molen, M.W. Development of decision making in school-aged children and adolescents: Evidence from heart rate and skin conductance analysis. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 1288–1301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hubbard, J.A.; Smithmyer, C.M.; Ramsden, S.R.; Parker, E.H.; Flanagan, K.D.; Dearing, K.F.; Relyea, N.; Simons, R.F. Observational, physiological, and self–report measures of children’s anger: Relations to reactive versus proactive aggression. Child Dev. 2002, 73, 1101–1118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Patil, I.; Cogoni, C.; Zangrando, N.; Chittaro, L.; Silani, G. Affective basis of judgment-behavior discrepancy in virtual experiences of moral dilemmas. Soc. Neurosci. 2014, 9, 94–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Faul, F.; Erdfelder, E.; Lang, A.-G.; Buchner, A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav. Res. Methods 2007, 39, 175–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bürkner, P.-C. brms: An R Package for Bayesian Multilevel Models Using Stan. J. Stat. Softw. 2017, 80, 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Carpenter, B.; Gelman, A.; Hoffman, M.D.; Lee, D.; Goodrich, B.; Betancourt, M.; Brubaker, M.; Guo, J.; Li, P.; Riddell, A. Stan: A Probabilistic Programming Language. J. Stat. Softw. 2017, 76, 1–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lüdecke, D. sjstats: Collection of Convenient Functions for Common Statistical Computations Version 0.18.0. 2020. Available online: https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/sjstats/index.html (accessed on 1 January 2020).
- Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2013; ISBN 1-60918-230-8. [Google Scholar]
- Hayes, A.F. Beyond Baron and Kenny: Statistical mediation analysis in the new millennium. Commun. Monogr. 2009, 76, 408–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Heino, M.T.J.; Vuorre, M.; Hankonen, N. Bayesian evaluation of behavior change interventions: A brief introduction and a practical example. Health Psychol. Behav. Med. 2018, 6, 49–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Miočević, M.; Gonzalez, O.; Valente, M.J.; MacKinnon, D.P. A tutorial in Bayesian potential outcomes mediation analysis. Struct. Equ. Model. Multidiscip. J. 2018, 25, 121–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baguley, T.; Kaye, W. Review of: Understanding psychology as a science: An introduction to scientific and statistical inference, by Z. Dienes. Br. J. Math. Stat. Psychol. 2010, 63, 695–698. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dienes, Z.; Mclatchie, N. Four reasons to prefer Bayesian analyses over significance testing. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2018, 25, 207–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Nuijten, M.B.; Wetzels, R.; Matzke, D.; Dolan, C.V.; Wagenmakers, E.-J. A default Bayesian hypothesis test for mediation. Behav. Res. Methods 2015, 47, 85–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jeffreys, H. Theory of Probability, 3rd ed.; The Clarendon Press: Oxford, UK, 1961. [Google Scholar]
- Dienes, Z.; Coulton, S.; Heather, N. Using Bayes factors to evaluate evidence for no effect: Examples from the SIPS project. Addiction 2018, 113, 240–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Patil, I. Trait psychopathy and utilitarian moral judgement: The mediating role of action aversion. J. Cogn. Psychol. 2015, 27, 349–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hare, R.D. Manual for the Revised Psychopathy Checklist, 2nd ed.; Multi-Health Systems: Tor, ON, Canada, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Welsh, G.S. Factor dimensions A and R. In Basic Readings on the MMPI in Psychology and Medicine; University of Minnesota Press: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 1956; pp. 264–281. [Google Scholar]
- Brinkley, C.A.; Schmitt, W.A.; Smith, S.S.; Newman, J.P. Construct validation of a self-report psychopathy scale: Does Levenson’s self-report psychopathy scale measure the same constructs as Hare’s psychopathy checklist-revised? Personal. Individ. Differ. 2001, 31, 1021–1038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lykken, D.T. A study of anxiety in the sociopathic personality. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1957, 55, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yildirim, B.O.; Derksen, J.J.L. Clarifying the heterogeneity in psychopathic samples: Towards a new continuum of primary and secondary psychopathy. Aggress. Violent Behav. 2015, 24, 9–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chen, H.; Cohen, P.; Chen, S. How Big is a Big Odds Ratio? Interpreting the Magnitudes of Odds Ratios in Epidemiological Studies. Commun. Stat. - Simul. Comput. 2010, 39, 860–864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lilienfeld, S.O.; Widows, M.R. PPI-R: Psychopathic Personality Inventory Revised: Professional Manual; Psychological Assessment Resources, Incorporated: Odessa, FL, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
- Navarrete, C.D.; McDonald, M.M.; Mott, M.L.; Asher, B. Virtual morality: Emotion and action in a simulated three-dimensional “trolley problem”. Emotion 2012, 12, 364–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Balash, J.; Falkenbach, D.M. The ends justify the meanness: An investigation of psychopathic traits and utilitarian moral endorsement. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2018, 127, 127–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dindo, L.; Fowles, D. Dual temperamental risk factors for psychopathic personality: Evidence from self-report and skin conductance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2011, 100, 557–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ling, S.; Raine, A.; Gao, Y.; Schug, R. The mediating role of emotional intelligence on the autonomic functioning – Psychopathy relationship. Biol. Psychol. 2018, 136, 136–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veit, R.; Konicar, L.; Klinzing, J.G.; Barth, B.; Yilmaz, Ö.; Birbaumer, N. Deficient fear conditioning in psychopathy as a function of interpersonal and affective disturbances. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2013, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Benning, S.D.; Patrick, C.J.; Salekin, R.T.; Leistico, A.-M.R. Convergent and discriminant validity of psychopathy factors assessed via self-report: A Comparison of three instruments. Assessment 2005, 12, 270–289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conway, P.; Goldstein-Greenwood, J.; Polacek, D.; Greene, J.D. Sacrificial utilitarian judgments do reflect concern for the greater good: Clarification via process dissociation and the judgments of philosophers. Cognition 2018, 179, 241–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gawronski, B.; Armstrong, J.; Conway, P.; Friesdorf, R.; Hütter, M. Consequences, norms, and generalized inaction in moral dilemmas: The CNI model of moral decision-making. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2017, 113, 343–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Crone, D.L.; Laham, S.M. Utilitarian preferences or action preferences? De-confounding action and moral code in sacrificial dilemmas. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2017, 104, 476–481. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Körner, A.; Deutsch, R.; Gawronski, B. Using the CNI model to investigate individual differences in moral dilemma judgments. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gleichgerrcht, E.; Young, L. Low levels of empathic concern predict utilitarian moral judgment. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e60418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Duke, A.A.; Bègue, L. The drunk utilitarian: Blood alcohol concentration predicts utilitarian responses in moral dilemmas. Cognition 2015, 134, 121–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ali, F.; Amorim, I.S.; Chamorro-Premuzic, T. Empathy deficits and trait emotional intelligence in psychopathy and Machiavellianism. Personal. Individ. Differ. 2009, 47, 758–762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Newman, J.P.; Baskin-Sommers, A.R. Early selective attention abnormalities in psychopathy. In Cognitive Neuroscience of Attention; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 421–440. [Google Scholar]
1 | 2 | 3 | 4 | 5 | 6 | 7 | 8 | 9 | 10 | 11 | 12 | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 | Sex | 1 | |||||||||||
2 | LSRP Total | −0.236 * | 1 | ||||||||||
3 | LSRP Primary | −0.276 ** | 0.883 ** | 1 | |||||||||
4 | LSRP Secondary | −0.048 | 0.676 ** | 0.251 * | 1 | ||||||||
5 | SCL-R Neutral | −0.14 | −0.05 | −0.065 | 0.008 | 1 | |||||||
6 | SCL-R Impersonal | 0.145 | −0.05 | −0.129 | 0.096 | 0.291 ** | 1 | ||||||
7 | SCL-R Personal | −0.08 | −0.05 | −0.086 | 0.024 | 0.351 ** | 0.266 ** | 1 | |||||
8 | SCL-R Moral | 0.039 | −0.07 | −0.136 | 0.075 | 0.404 ** | 0.788 ** | 0.804 ** | 1 | ||||
9 | Yes Neutral | −0.065 | 0.054 | −0.022 | 0.146 | 0.077 | 0.034 | −0.025 | 0.005 | 1 | |||
10 | Yes Impersonal | −0.021 | 0.239 * | 0.223 * | 0.142 | 0.186 | 0.039 | 0.074 | 0.071 | 0.016 | 1 | ||
11 | Yes Personal | 0.077 | 0.197 † | 0.312 ** | −0.085 | −0.15 | −0.09 | −0.266 ** | −0.225 * | −0.229 * | 0.204 * | 1 | |
12 | Yes Moral | 0.05 | 0.269 ** | 0.352 ** | 0 | −0.03 | −0.05 | −0.171 | −0.14 | −0.17 | 0.637 ** | 0.884 ** | 1 |
Mean | 1.71 | 50.2 | 29.51 | 20.69 | −0.35 | −0.19 | −0.29 | −0.24 | 0.8 | 0.39 | 0.51 | 0.45 | |
SD | 0.46 | 9.16 | 6.97 | 4.44 | 0.47 | 0.32 | 0.33 | 0.26 | 0.15 | 0.17 | 0.28 | 0.18 | |
Minimum | 1 | 29 | 18 | 10 | −2.06 | −1.33 | −1.35 | −1.11 | 0.4 | 0 | 0 | 0.1 | |
Maximum | 2 | 71 | 47 | 32 | 0.57 | 0.41 | 0.37 | 0.26 | 1 | 0.8 | 1 | 0.8 |
Hypothesized Mediator (M) | Independent Variable (IV) Effect on M (a) | Association of M with Outcome Variable (b) | Direct Effect of IV on M (c’) | Indirect Effect | Proportion Mediated | BF10 | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Independent variable | Estimate | 95% CI | Estimate | 95% CI | Estimate | 95% CI | Estimate | 95% CI | % | |
SCL-Reactivity during impersonal moral dilemmas | ||||||||||
Primary psychopathy | −0.0061 | (−0.0158, 0.0039) | 0.0355 | (−0.0786, 0.1492) | 0.0061 | (0.0007, 0.0113)* | −0.0002 | (−0.0014, 0.0006) | −2.04% | 0.079 |
Secondary psychopathy | 0.0076 | (−0.0081, 0.0234) | 0.0119 | (−0.0977, 0.1245) | 0.0056 | (−0.0026, 0.0139) | 0.0001 | (−0.0012, 0.0014) | 0.33% | 0.149 |
Total psychopathy | −0.0016 | (−0.0091, 0.0061) | 0.0246 | (−0.0872, 0.1362) | 0.0047 | (0.0007, 0.0086)* | 0.0000 | (−0.0007, 0.0005) | −0.22% | 0.082 |
SCL-Reactivity during personal moral dilemmas | ||||||||||
Primary psychopathy | −0.0041 | (−0.0144, 0.0065) | −0.1913 | (−0.3530, −0.0215)* | 0.0121 | (0.0040, 0.0201)* | 0.0008 | (−0.0013, 0.0036) | 4.94% | 0.217 |
Secondary psychopathy | 0.0027 | (−0.0132, 0.0186) | −0.2107 | (−0.3774, −0.0403)* | −0.0046 | (−0.0177, 0.0087) | −0.0006 | (−0.0048, 0.0031) | 8.37% | 0.375 |
Total psychopathy | −0.0018 | (−0.0096, 0.0061) | −0.2037 | (−0.3716, −0.0304)* | 0.0057 | (−0.0008, 0.0121) | 0.0004 | (−0.0013, 0.0023) | 4.47% | 0.278 |
SCL-Reactivity during all moral dilemmas | ||||||||||
Primary psychopathy | −0.0051 | (−0.0132, 0.0031) | −0.0576 | (−0.1979, 0.0832) | 0.0091 | (0.0036, 0.0143)* | 0.0003 | (−0.0006, 0.0016) | 1.92% | 0.117 |
Secondary psychopathy | 0.0050 | (−0.0070, 0.0171) | −0.0905 | (−0.2443, 0.0568) | 0.0007 | (−0.0077, 0.0094) | −0.0005 | (−0.0025, 0.0009) | −94.07% | 0.917 |
Total psychopathy | −0.0016 | (−0.0078, 0.0046) | −0.0795 | (−0.2163, 0.0617) | 0.0052 | (0.0013, 0.0092)* | 0.0001 | (−0.0005, 0.0010) | 1.16% | 0.117 |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Fagan, S.E.; Kofler, L.; Riccio, S.; Gao, Y. Somatic Marker Production Deficits Do Not Explain the Relationship between Psychopathic Traits and Utilitarian Moral Decision Making. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 303. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050303
Fagan SE, Kofler L, Riccio S, Gao Y. Somatic Marker Production Deficits Do Not Explain the Relationship between Psychopathic Traits and Utilitarian Moral Decision Making. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(5):303. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050303
Chicago/Turabian StyleFagan, Shawn E., Liat Kofler, Sarah Riccio, and Yu Gao. 2020. "Somatic Marker Production Deficits Do Not Explain the Relationship between Psychopathic Traits and Utilitarian Moral Decision Making" Brain Sciences 10, no. 5: 303. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050303
APA StyleFagan, S. E., Kofler, L., Riccio, S., & Gao, Y. (2020). Somatic Marker Production Deficits Do Not Explain the Relationship between Psychopathic Traits and Utilitarian Moral Decision Making. Brain Sciences, 10(5), 303. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10050303