Linguistic Markers of Theory of Mind in Spontaneous Speech: A Narrative Review
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. Speech Markers of Theory of Mind
2.1. Mental State Terms
2.2. General Linguistic Ability and Vocabulary
2.3. Embedded Clauses
2.4. Referring Forms
2.5. Pragmatic Markers
2.6. Definitional Competence
3. Future Directions
4. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A. Summary of Study Characteristics
Citation | Paper Type(s) | Aim | Population | Place of Origin | Measures | Method |
Ruffman et al. (2002) | Quantitative Original Research | Investigates whether mothers’ use of mental state language predicts children’s later theory of mind understanding, beyond children’s own mental language, age, language ability, and SES. | n = 82 children (ages 2–5) and their mothers. White middle and upper-middle class, rural and urban UK areas. | United Kingdom | CELF language test (Linguistic Concept subtest at all time points) Theory of mind composite (false belief, desire–emotion, desire–action, emotion–situation [Time 1 only], justification, ambiguity, wicked desires [Time 3 only]) Picture task (mental state utterances: desire, emotion, cognition [think/know], modulation of assertion) Maternal education (7-point SES scale) | Longitudinal study with 3 time points over 1 year; picture description tasks, transcribed utterances coded; theory of mind tasks and standardized language tests administered. |
Dunn et al. (1991) | Quantitative Original Research | Examines individual differences in young children’s understanding of others’ emotions and beliefs and identifies early family-related predictors. | n = 50 children (mean age = 33 months at T1, 40 months at T2), their mothers, and older siblings; families from central Pennsylvania. | United States | Affective labeling task (identify emotions in facial expressions) Affective perspective-taking task (infer others’ emotions from scenarios) False-belief task (standard unexpected-location task) Conversational coding (feeling state talk, causal talk) Mean length of utterance (MLU) (child language complexity) Parent education (years of schooling) Occupational SES (parent job status) Family interaction rating scales (sensitivity, responsiveness, affective climate) | Longitudinal study with two home visits (at 33 and 40 months); observational recordings and rating scales at T1, standardized sociocognitive tasks at T2 (affective labeling, perspective-taking, false belief); discourse transcribed and coded; multiple regression analyses. |
Grazzani and Ornaghi (2012) | Quantitative Original Research | Investigates the relationships between children’s use and comprehension of mental state language and their theory of mind abilities. | n = 110 children (mean age = 9 years 7 months; aged 8–10), balanced by gender; recruited from Italian primary schools. | Italy | TAM-2 language test (verbal ability—metalinguistic awareness) Theory of mind tasks (second-order false-belief stories) Test of Emotion Comprehension (TEC) (belief-based and mixed emotions) Mental state verb comprehension task (definition selection for psychological verbs) Describe-a-friend task (use of mental state terms: emotion, cognition, volition) | Cross-sectional design; standardized tasks and written responses; measures administered individually; data analyzed using correlations and multiple regression. |
Barreto et al. (2018) | Quantitative Original Research | Examines the association between ToM and mental state talk at 55 months and their relationship with social competence and behavior at 69 months. | n = 73 preschool children (mean age = 55 months) and their mothers. | Portugal | Theory of mind tasks (Diverse Beliefs, Knowledge Access, Unexpected Contents I & II, Explicit False Belief, Unexpected Location) Picture book task (mother and child mental state terms: desire, cognition, emotion) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised Social Competence and Behaviour Evaluation Scale (teacher report: aggression, anxiety–withdrawal, competence) | Longitudinal design with 2 time points (T1 = 55 months, T2 = 69 months); lab-based interaction and assessments at T1; teacher report at T2; controlled for maternal education, age, verbosity, and child gender |
Tager-Flusberg (1992) | Quantitative Original Research | Investigates whether autistic children show deficits in the early acquisition of mental state language (desire, perception, emotion, cognition) compared to children with Down syndrome as an index of theory of mind development. | n = 12 children (6 autistic boys; 4 boys with Down syndrome, 2 girls with Down syndrome), matched on age and language level; families from various SES levels. | United States | SALT language transcript system (coded for utterances referencing desire, perception, emotion, cognition) Functional coding of mental state terms (e.g., conversational use vs. true mental state reference) Elaboration coding (causality, contrast with reality) Self/Other reference coding Leiter International Performance Scale (non-verbal IQ) Mean Length of Utterance (MLU) Index of Productive Syntax (IPSyn) Lexical Diversity (number of different word roots per 100 utterances) | Longitudinal study over 1–2 years; bimonthly home visits; 40–70 min recorded parent–child play sessions; transcripts coded using SALT system; utterances analyzed for psychological state terms and functions. |
Astington and Jenkins (1999) | Quantitative Original Research | Examines whether language competence predicts later theory of mind development or vice versa and whether syntax or semantics contribute differentially to this relation. | n = 59 children (29 girls, 30 boys), aged 2 years 9 months to 3 years 10 months at start; all from middle-class English-speaking homes; 4 spoke additional languages. | Canada | Test of Early Language Development (assesses general language ability, with separate subscores for syntax and semantics, both expressive and receptive) Theory of mind composite score (based on performance on three tasks: change-in-location false-belief task, unexpected-contents false-belief task, and appearance–reality task) Internal consistency reliability coefficients reported for both language and theory of mind measures at all time points | Longitudinal design with 3 testing points across 7 months; children assessed at beginning, middle, and end of nursery school year; hierarchical regression analyses used to examine predictive relations between language and theory of mind over time. |
Farrar and Maag (2002) | Quantitative Original Research | Examines whether children’s language abilities at 2 years of age (vocabulary and grammar) predict their theory of mind performance at 4 years of age. | n = 20 children (9 girls, 11 boys); tested at 24–27 months, and again at 4–5 years (mean age = 52 months). | United States | MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (MCDI) (parent-reported vocabulary size and grammatical complexity at 24 and 27 months) Mean Length of Utterance (MLU) (derived from naturalistic mother-child play sessions at 27 months, analyzed via CHILDES/CLAN system) Theory of mind tasks (assessing appearance-reality, false belief, and representational change; adapted from Gopnik & Astington, Welch-Ross) Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT-R) (general vocabulary ability at 4 years) Woodcock–Johnson Memory for Sentences (verbal memory at 4 years) | Longitudinal design: toddlers tested at 24 and 27 months (language), followed up at 4 years for theory of mind tasks; multiple correlational and partial correlation analyses conducted to assess associations between early language and later theory of mind. |
Slade and Ruffman (2005) | Quantitative Original Research | Examines longitudinal bidirectional relations between syntax, semantics, working memory, and theory of mind in preschool children. Tests whether syntax uniquely contributes to theory of mind development and whether working memory mediates the relation. | n = 44 preschool children (25 boys, 19 girls), mean age 3.8 years at Time 1; all native English speakers from 4 nurseries. | United Kingdom | Semantics: British Picture Vocabulary Scale Semantics: Linguistic Concepts subtest Syntax: Word Order Test (adapted from Caramazza & Zurif, 1976) Syntax: Embedded Clause Test (adapted from Ruffman et al., 2002) Theory of mind composite (two unexpected-transfer false-belief tasks and one unexpected-contents task) Working Memory task: modified Backwards Digit Span (adapted from Davis & Pratt, 1995) | Longitudinal design with two time points, 6 months apart; testing conducted individually at nursery; hierarchical regressions and cross-lagged analyses used to examine predictive relations; sensitivity analyses conducted with subsampling procedures. |
Milligan et al. (2007) | Meta-analysis | Quantifies the strength of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding in children under 7 years old; examines moderators such as type of language ability assessed, type of false-belief task, and direction of effect. | Meta-analysis of 104 studies; total n = 8891 children under 7 years old. | Multiple countries including United States, Canada, and United Kingdom | Language measures: receptive vocabulary (e.g., PPVT), general language, syntax, semantics (varies across studies) False-belief measures: standard false-belief tasks (unexpected transfer, unexpected contents, etc.) Moderator variables coded: age, type of language measure, type of false-belief task, direction of effect, clinical vs. non-clinical samples | Meta-analytic methods: effect sizes computed from correlations; moderator analyses conducted; controlled for age; cross-lagged effect sizes analyzed for directionality of effect. |
van Dijk et al. (2023) | Quantitative Original Research | Investigates the relation between language ability and theory of mind in children’s freely told narratives using natural language processing (NLP) tools and classification models. | n = 442 children (ages 4–12), recruited from primary schools, daycares, and community centers. | Netherlands | Character Depth (CD) coding system (labels stories into Actor, Agent, or Person levels reflecting mental state complexity) Lexical Complexity (LC) (calculated using perplexity based on lemma frequencies from the BasiScript corpus) Lexical Diversity (LD) (measured via Measure of Textual Lexical Diversity [MTLD]) Dependency Distance (DD) (syntactic complexity; mean dependency length via spaCy parsing) Clausal Complementation (CC) (number of sentential complements per utterance) Pragmatic Markers (PM) (use of deixis and definite articles as perspective indicators) Social Words (SOC) (LIWC social category word counts) Lemmas (binarized bag-of-words representations for classification) | Recorded live storytelling sessions during workshops; expert-annotated narratives for CD level; feature extraction using NLP tools; logistic regression classifier; model interpretation via Shapley values; 80/20 train-test split; robustness tested over 100 resampling splits. |
de Villiers and Pyers (2002) | Quantitative Original Research | Examines whether mastery of complex syntax, especially sentential complements, predicts children’s false-belief understanding over time. | n = 28 children (13 boys, 15 girls), aged 3–5 years old at the start of the study (range 3–1 to 3–10 years), recruited from local preschool and daycare centers. | United States | Memory for Complements task (elicitation of complement structures with mental verbs [think/believe] and communication verbs [say/tell], 12 items total) Spontaneous Speech Samples (analyzed using Index of Productive Syntax [IPSyn]: overall IPSyn, complements, complex sentences, sentence scores) Mean Length of Utterance (MLU) Medial Wh-question task (long-distance wh-questions testing complement understanding) False-belief tasks:
| Longitudinal study over 1 year with 4 testing periods; individual child assessments at daycare/preschool centers; multiple regression and pass/fail contingency analyses conducted to examine directionality between language and theory of mind measures. |
Zhang et al. (2023) | Quantitative Original Research | Investigates relations between syntactic complexity in Mandarin Chinese and theory of mind performance in schizophrenia patients versus healthy controls. | n = 90 total participants (51 schizophrenia patients, 39 healthy controls); all native Mandarin speakers of Han Chinese ethnicity. | Multiple countries including China, Denmark, Spain and United States | Animated Triangles Task (Abell et al., 2000) (used both for spontaneous speech elicitation and ToM scoring) Brüne’s Picture Sequencing Task (Brüne, 2003) (non-verbal ToM measure with 1st-, 2nd-, and 3rd-order false-belief questions) Brief Assessment of Cognition in Schizophrenia (BACS) (assesses verbal memory, working memory, motor speed, verbal fluency, attention, executive function) Linguistic Annotations (via CLAN system):
| Participants completed narrative descriptions during the Animated Triangles Task; speech was recorded and manually annotated; mixed-effects negative binomial regression models and Spearman correlations used to analyze relations between linguistic features, ToM, neurocognition, and clinical symptoms. |
Wiltschko (2024) | Position Piece/Review | Provides a linguistic critique of Zhang et al. (2023), arguing that their classification of embedded argument clauses in Mandarin is theoretically problematic and challenges their conclusions about syntax and theory of mind in schizophrenia. | N/A | Spain | N/A | Theoretical analysis of linguistic classifications used in Zhang et al. (2023); reviews linguistic literature on Mandarin ba-construction, argument/adjunct clause distinctions, and mental content representation; evaluates implications for ToM research in schizophrenia. |
K. Schroeder et al. (2021) | Quantitative Original Research | Investigates how intensionality, theory of mind (ToM), and complex syntax relate in children with autism spectrum conditions (ASCs) compared to typically developing (TD) peers and whether understanding embedded clauses predicts performance on metarepresentational tasks. | n = 50 children (25 ASC, 25 TD), mean age ≈ 9 years. All Catalan–Spanish bilinguals from Spain. Children were matched on VMA. ASC diagnoses confirmed via ICD-10 and ADOS. No intellectual disability in ASC group. | Spain | Sally–Anne Task (false-belief/true-belief understanding) Intensionality Task (dual-description reasoning with appearance-reality distinctions) Relative Clause Comprehension (e.g., “Show me the boy who hugs the girl”) Complement Clause Comprehension, subdivided into:
| Experimental design with two sessions (~25 min each) using picture selection tasks delivered via PowerPoint. Tasks assessed metarepresentational and syntactic abilities using matched visual formats across conditions. Participants’ responses analyzed using logistic mixed-effects regression. |
Lohmann and Tomasello (2003) | Quantitative Original Research | Examines whether different types of linguistic experience (specifically sentential complement syntax and perspective-shifting discourse) causally contribute to the development of false-belief understanding in young children. | n = 138 German children (aged 3 years 3 months to 3 years 10 months), from various SESs, all native German speakers. All participants had typical language development and no demonstration of false-belief understanding at pre-test. | Germany | Vocabulary Pretest (Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children) False Belief Pretest and Post-tests
| Children were randomly assigned to one of four training groups: full training (discourse + syntax), discourse-only, sentential-complement-only, or no-language. Each received three sessions over two weeks in their preschool. Training involved structured interactions using deceptive objects or stories, depending on group. Standardized pretests and post-tests assessed false-belief and complement syntax understanding. |
Durrleman et al. (2023) | Quantitative Original Research | Investigates whether training on sentential complements (e.g., “X says that…”) can improve ToM in children with autism spectrum disorder and to examine which individual characteristics predict training success. | n = 53 children, 33 with ASD (ages 5 years 7 months to 14 years 9 months) and 20 neurotypical (ages 3 years to 6 years). All were native French speakers. | Switzer-land and France | Verbal False-Belief Task (e.g., adapted Sally–Anne scenario) Low-Verbal False-Belief Task (minimizing linguistic demands) Diverse Desire and Belief Tasks (ToM precursors) Complements Comprehension Test (e.g., “Dad says that…”) Raven’s Coloured Progressive Matrices (RPM) Exalang 3–6 Scale (language and cognitive skills) Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS) (for ASD group) | Children completed pre-, post-, and delayed (4–6 weeks later) assessments. Training on sentential complements (especially communication verbs) was delivered via the DIRE iPad app over 4–6 weeks (8–12 sessions). Tasks used animated scenarios followed by selection or repetition activities targeting syntactic structures relevant to ToM. |
Gundel and Johnson (2013) | Case Study | Examines young children’s use of referring expressions (e.g., pronouns, articles, demonstratives) within the Givenness Hierarchy framework and explores how these usages reflect early-developing components of theory of mind. | Analyzed naturalistic speech data from 9 English-speaking children (aged 1 year 9 months to 2 years 8 months) from the CHILDES database. Data drawn from the Valian, Brown, and Bloom corpora. All were typically developing children observed in their home, daycare, or playroom. | United States | Spontaneous Speech Samples from CHILDES corpora Use of Referring Expressions, categorized as
| Corpus analysis of children’s spontaneous utterances coded for referring expressions and the cognitive status (e.g., in focus, activated, familiar) of the referents, based on the Givenness Hierarchy framework. Each referring form was analyzed for whether it matched the assumed mental state of the addressee, using established coding guidelines. Inter-coder reliability was 94%. |
Lorusso et al. (2007) | Case Study/Quantitative Original Research | Investigates how different linguistic profiles in individuals with genetic syndromes affect ToM indicators in narrative production and evaluates whether ToM development is more closely tied to language or cognitive development. | n = 42 individuals (6 with Cornelia de Lange syndrome, 6 with Williams syndrome, 6 with Down syndrome, and 24 typically developed). All participants were native Italian speakers. | Netherlands and Italy | Narrative Production Task using a 6-frame picture story ToM-related Indicators, including:
| Participants were asked to tell a story based on a picture sequence. Narratives were audio-recorded, transcribed, and analyzed for ToM-related language features. Statistical comparisons and correlations examined the influence of cognitive and linguistic factors across groups. |
Rubio-Fernandez (2021) | Position Piece/Review | Proposes that pragmatic markers (especially demonstratives and definite articles) form a critical link between language and ToM, enabling their co-development and co-evolution via a positive feedback loop across three timescales: language acquisition, use, and historical change. | N/A | United States | Reviews studies using the following: False-Belief Tasks (e.g., Sally–Anne task) Demonstrative Comprehension Tasks (e.g., “this” vs. “that” across contexts) Article Usage Tasks (e.g., definite vs. indefinite noun phrase production) Narrative and Discourse Analyses Cross-linguistic and Typological Comparisons Corpus and Developmental Data (e.g., CHILDES) | The author reviews cross-linguistic, psycholinguistic, and developmental research to support the hypothesis that pragmatic markers scaffold early ToM abilities like joint attention and perspective-taking, and that their evolution parallels ToM development across cultures and history. |
Cornaggia et al. (2024) | Quantitative Original Research | Explores the relationship between two metarepresentational abilities in adolescence (ToM and definitional competence) while also examining the effects of age and gender on these abilities. | n = 75 Italian adolescents (age range: 14–19 years). Participants attended three types of public high schools (lyceum, technical, and professional) in central Italy. All native Italian speakers, no neurodevelopmental or psychiatric disorders. | Italy | Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET)
| Participants completed both tasks in a single 1-h classroom session. Data were analyzed using t-tests, correlations, and hierarchical regressions to assess the effects of age, gender, and the predictive relationships between ToM (RMET) and definitional competence. |
Bianco et al. (2022) | Quantitative Original Research | Investigates how ageing affects two metarepresentational abilities (ToM and definitional competence) and whether ToM predicts definitional skill beyond the effects of age and education. | n = 74 participants (24 aged 21–55 years old, 25 aged 60–70 years old, and 25 aged 71–85 years old. | Italy | Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (Adult version) Co.De. Scale (Definitional Competence):
| Participants completed the Eyes Task and Co.De. Scale in a 1 h session at home. Scores were analyzed using ANOVAs, correlations, and hierarchical regressions to examine effects of age, education, and ToM on definitional ability. |
Paunov et al. (2022) | Quantitative Original Research | Examines whether language and ToM brain networks differentially track linguistic and mental state content in naturalistic stories. | n = 40 adults (ages 19–45, mean = 26 years) | United States | fMRI, naturalistic story listening, annotations of linguistic vs. mental state content | Participants completed fMRI while listening to naturalistic stories. Statistical analyses used mixed-effects linear regression to compare tracking of linguistic and mental state content across language and ToM regions of interest (ROIs). |
References
- Abell, F., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2000). Do triangles play tricks? Attribution of mental states to animated shapes in normal and abnormal development. Cognitive Development, 15(1), 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Antoniou, K. (2022, July 27–30). Multilingual and bi-dialectal irony processing. Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Toronto, ON, Canada. [Google Scholar]
- Apperly, I. (2011). Mindreaders: The cognitive basis of “theory of mind” (p. xii, 219). Psychology Press. [Google Scholar]
- Apperly, I. A. (2008). Beyond Simulation-Theory and Theory-Theory: Why social cognitive neuroscience should use its own concepts to study “theory of mind”. Cognition, 107(1), 266–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Apperly, I. A. (2012). What is “theory of mind”? Concepts, cognitive processes and individual differences. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(5), 825–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2009). Studies of adults can inform accounts of theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 190–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Astington, J. W., & Baird, J. A. (Eds.). (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind (p. xii, 355). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Astington, J. W., & Jenkins, J. M. (1999). A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Developmental Psychology, 35(5), 1311–1320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Astington, J. W., & Olson, D. R. (1990). Metacognitive and metalinguistic language: Learning to talk about thought. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 39(1), 77–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind (p. xxii, 171). The MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen, S. (2000). Theory of mind and autism: A review. International Review of Research in Mental Retardation, 23, 169–184. [Google Scholar]
- Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21(1), 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barreto, A. L., Osório, A., Baptista, J., Fearon, P., & Martins, C. (2018). Association between theory of mind and mental state talk in preschoolers and later social competence and behaviour. Infant and Child Development, 27(2), e2060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bartsch, K., & Wellman, H. (1989). Young children’s attribution of action to beliefs and desires. Child Development, 60(4), 946–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berguno, G., & Bowler, D. M. (2004). Communicative interactions, knowledge of a second language, and theory of mind in young children. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 165(3), 293–309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Białecka, M., Wodniecka, Z., Muszyńska, K., Szpak, M., & Haman, E. (2024). Both L1 and L2 proficiency impact ToM reasoning in children aged 4 to 6. Painting a more nuanced picture of the relation between bilingualism and ToM. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 27(3), 400–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bianco, F., Castelli, I., & Belacchi, C. (2022). Changes of meta-representational skills in ageing: First empirical evidence on the relation between metalinguistic competence and attributions of mental states. Journal of Language and Education, 8(3), 40–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brüne, M. (2003). Theory of mind and the role of IQ in chronic disorganized schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 60(1), 57–64. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brüne, M. (2005). “Theory of mind” in schizophrenia: A review of the literature. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31(1), 21–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Buchanan, D., Pexman, P. M., & Titone, D. (2021). The psychology of saying what you don’t mean [special issue]. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 75(2), 93–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caramazza, A., & Zurif, E. B. (1976). Dissociation of algorithmic and heuristic processes in language comprehension: Evidence from aphasia. Brain and Language, 3, 572–582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cornaggia, A., Bianco, F., Castelli, I., & Belacchi, C. (2024). Assessing metarepresentational abilities in adolescence: An exploratory study on relationships between definitional competence and theory of mind. Frontiers in Psychology, 15, 1456432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Davis, H. L., & Pratt, C. (1995). The development of children’s theory of mind: The working memory explanation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 47, 25–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- de Boer, J. N., Brederoo, S. G., Voppel, A. E., & Sommer, I. E. C. (2020). Anomalies in language as a biomarker for schizophrenia. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 33(3), 212–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- de Villiers, J. G., & Pyers, J. E. (2002). Complements to cognition: A longitudinal study of the relationship between complex syntax and false-belief-understanding. Cognitive Development, 17(1), 1037–1060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dumontheil, I., Apperly, I. A., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence. Developmental Science, 13(2), 331–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dunn, J., Brown, J., Slomkowski, C., Tesla, C., & Youngblade, L. (1991). Young Children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: Individual differences and their antecedents. Child Development, 62(6), 1352–1366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Durrleman, S., Bentea, A., Prisecaru, A., Thommen, E., & Delage, H. (2023). Training syntax to enhance theory of mind in children with ASD. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 53(6), 2444–2457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dwyer, K., David, A. S., McCarthy, R., McKenna, P., & Peters, E. (2020). Linguistic alignment and theory of mind impairments in schizophrenia patients’ dialogic interactions. Psychological Medicine, 50(13), 2194–2202. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ensink, K., & Mayes, L. C. (2010). The development of mentalisation in children from a theory of mind perspective. Psychoanalytic Inquiry, 30(4), 301–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ensor, R., & Hughes, C. (2008). Content or connectedness? Mother–child talk and early social understanding. Child Development, 79(1), 201–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farrar, M. J., & Maag, L. (2002). Early language development and the emergence of a theory of mind. First Language, 22(2), 197–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farris, M. S., Devoe, D. J., & Addington, J. (2020). Attrition rates in trials for adolescents and young adults at clinical high-risk for psychosis: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 14(5), 515–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frank, C. K. (2018). Reviving pragmatic theory of theory of mind. AIMS Neuroscience, 5(2), 116–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds—A biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692–1695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2012). Mechanisms of social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 287–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2021). Mapping mentalising in the brain. In M. Gilead, & K. N. Ochsner (Eds.), The neural basis of mentalizing (pp. 17–45). Springer International Publishing. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gleitman, L. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 1(1), 3–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goetz, P. J. (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(1), 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gordon, K. R. (2016). High proficiency across two languages is related to better mental state reasoning for bilingual children. Journal of Child Language, 43(2), 407–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grazzani, I., & Ornaghi, V. (2012). How do use and comprehension of mental-state language relate to theory of mind in middle childhood? Cognitive Development, 27(2), 99–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gullifer, J. W., & Titone, D. (2020). Characterizing the social diversity of bilingualism using language entropy. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 23(2), 283–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gundel, J. K., Hedberg, N., & Zacharski, R. (1993). Cognitive status and the form of referring expressions in discourse. Language, 69(2), 274–307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gundel, J. K., & Johnson, K. (2013). Children’s use of referring expressions in spontaneous discourse: Implications for theory of mind development. Journal of Pragmatics, 56, 43–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gundel, J. K., Ntelitheos, D., & Kowalsky, M. (2007). Children’s use of referring expressions: Some implications for theory of mind. ZAS Papers in Linguistics, 48, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harris, L. T. (2017). Invisible mind: Flexible social cognition and dehumanization. MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Jenkins, J. M., & Astington, J. W. (1996). Cognitive factors and family structure associated with theory of mind development in young children. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 70–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Khurana, D., Koli, A., Khatter, K., & Singh, S. (2023). Natural language processing: State of the art, current trends and challenges. Multimedia Tools and Applications, 82(3), 3713–3744. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koster-Hale, J., & Saxe, R. (2013). Theory of mind: A neural prediction problem. Neuron, 79(5), 836–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kovács, Á. M. (2009). Early bilingualism enhances mechanisms of false-belief reasoning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 48–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kovács, Á. M. (2011). Early bilingualism and theory of mind: Bilinguals’ advantage in dealing with conflicting mental representations. In Access to language and cognitive development (pp. 192–218). Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kulke, L., & Hinrichs, M. A. B. (2021). Implicit theory of mind under realistic social circumstances measured with mobile eye-tracking. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 1215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lee, H. (1960). To kill a mockingbird. J. B. Lippincott & Co. [Google Scholar]
- Lewis, C., Freeman, N. H., Kyriakidou, C., Maridaki-Kassotaki, K., & Berridge, D. M. (1996). Social influences on false belief access: Specific sibling influences or general apprenticeship? Child Development, 67(6), 2930–2947. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lohmann, H., & Tomasello, M. (2003). The role of language in the development of false belief understanding: A training study. Child Development, 74(4), 1130–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lorusso, M. L., Galli, R., Libera, L., Gagliardi, C., Borgatti, R., & Hollebrandse, B. (2007). Indicators of theory of mind in narrative production: A comparison between individuals with genetic syndromes and typically developing children. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 21(1), 37–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mackinley, M., Chan, J., Ke, H., Dempster, K., & Palaniyappan, L. (2021). Linguistic determinants of formal thought disorder in first episode psychosis. Early Intervention in Psychiatry, 15(2), 344–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meijering, B., van Rijn, H., Taatgen, N. A., & Verbrugge, R. (2012). What eye movements can tell about theory of mind in a strategic game. PLoS ONE, 7(9), e45961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Miller, C. A. (2006). Developmental relationships between language and theory of mind. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15(2), 142–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Milligan, K., Astington, J. W., & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development, 78(2), 622–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Navarro, E., & Conway, A. R. (2021). Adult bilinguals outperform monolinguals in theory of mind. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(11), 1841–1851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Navarro, E., DeLuca, V., & Rossi, E. (2022). It takes a village: Using network science to identify the effect of individual differences in bilingual experience for theory of mind. Brain Sciences, 12(4), 487. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Navarro, E., & Rossi, E. (2024). Inhibitory control partially mediates the relationship between metalinguistic awareness and perspective-taking. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 53(5), 71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nguyen, T. K., & Astington, J. W. (2014). Reassessing the bilingual advantage in theory of mind and its cognitive underpinnings. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 17(2), 396–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nielsen, M., & Dissanayake, C. (2000). An investigation of pretend play, mental state terms and false belief understanding: In search of a metarepresentational link. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 18(4), 609–624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paunov, A. M., Blank, I. A., Jouravlev, O., Mineroff, Z., Gallée, J., & Fedorenko, E. (2022). Differential tracking of linguistic vs. mental state content in naturalistic stimuli by language and theory of mind (ToM) brain networks. Neurobiology of Language, 3(3), 413–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perner, J., Leekam, S. R., & Wimmer, H. (1987). Three-year-olds’ difficulty with false belief: The case for a conceptual deficit. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5(2), 125–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Perner, J., Ruffman, T., & Leekam, S. R. (1994). Theory of mind is contagious: You catch it from your sibs. Child Development, 65(4), 1228–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peterson, C. C. (2000). Kindred spirits: Influences of siblings’ perspectives on theory of mind. Cognitive Development, 15(4), 435–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Premack, D., & Woodruff, G. (1978). Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 515–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rubio-Fernandez, P. (2021). Pragmatic markers: The missing link between language and theory of mind. Synthese, 199(1), 1125–1158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rubio-Fernández, P., & Glucksberg, S. (2012). Reasoning about other people’s beliefs: Bilinguals have an advantage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(1), 211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rubio-Fernández, P., Mollica, F., Oraa Ali, M., & Gibson, E. (2019). How do you know that? Automatic belief inferences in passing conversation. Cognition, 193, 104011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ruffman, T., Slade, L., & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers’ mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development, 73(3), 734–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Saxe, R., Carey, S., & Kanwisher, N. (2004). Understanding other minds: Linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annual Review of Psychology, 55(1), 87–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people. The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. NeuroImage, 19(4), 1835–1842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schroeder, K., Durrleman, S., Çokal, D., Sanfeliu Delgado, A., Masana Marin, A., & Hinzen, W. (2021). Relations between intensionality, theory of mind and complex syntax in autism spectrum conditions and typical development. Cognitive Development, 59, 101071. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schroeder, S. R. (2018). Do bilinguals have an advantage in theory of mind? A meta-analysis. Frontiers in Communication, 3, 36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schurz, M., Radua, J., Tholen, M. G., Maliske, L., Margulies, D. S., Mars, R. B., Sallet, J., & Kanske, P. (2021). Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: A neuroimaging meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind. Psychological bulletin, 147(3), 293–337. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sebastian, C. L., Fontaine, N. M., Bird, G., Blakemore, S. J., De Brito, S. A., McCrory, E. J., & Viding, E. (2012). Neural processing associated with cognitive and affective theory of mind in adolescents and adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 53–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slade, L., & Ruffman, T. (2005). How language does (and does not) relate to theory of mind: A longitudinal study of syntax, semantics, working memory and false belief. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 117–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slaughter, V. (2015). Theory of mind in infants and young children: A review. Australian Psychologist, 50(3), 169–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sundaray, S., Marinis, T., & Bose, A. (2018). Comprehending non-literal language: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Symeonidou, I., Dumontheil, I., Chow, W. Y., & Breheny, R. (2016). Development of online use of theory of mind during adolescence: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 149, 81–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tager-Flusberg, H. (1992). Autistic children’s talk about psychological states: Deficits in the early acquisition of a theory of mind. Child Development, 63(1), 161–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tiv, M., Kutlu, E., O’Regan, E., & Titone, D. (2022). Bridging people and perspectives: General and language-specific social network structure predict mentalizing across diverse sociolinguistic contexts. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Expérimentale, 76(4), 235. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tiv, M., O’Regan, E., & Titone, D. (2021). In a bilingual state of mind: Investigating the continuous relationship between bilingual language experience and mentalizing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 24(5), 918–931. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tiv, M., O’Regan, E., & Titone, D. (2023). The role of mentalizing capacity and ecological language diversity on irony comprehension in bilingual adults. Memory & Cognition, 51(2), 253–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomasello, M. (2008). Origins of human communication (p. xiii, 393). MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- van Dijk, B., Spruit, M., & van Duijn, M. (2023). Theory of mind in freely-told children’s narratives: A classification approach. In Findings of the Association for Computational Linguistics: ACL 2023 (pp. 12979–12993). Association for Computational Linguistics. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wellman, H. M. (2018). Theory of mind: The state of the art. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15(6), 728–755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wiltschko, M. (2024). Is grammar affected in Schizophrenia? Psychiatry Research, 339, 116061. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13(1), 103–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zaher, F., Diallo, M., Achim, A. M., Joober, R., Roy, M. A., Demers, M. F., Subramanian, P., Lavigne, K. M., Lepage, M., Gonzalez, D., & Zeljkovic, I. (2024). Speech markers to predict and prevent recurrent episodes of psychosis: A narrative overview and emerging opportunities. Schizophrenia Research, 266, 205–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, H., Parola, A., Zhou, Y., Wang, H., Bliksted, V., Fusaroli, R., & Hinzen, W. (2023). Linguistic markers of psychosis in Mandarin Chinese: Relations to theory of mind. Psychiatry Research, 325, 115253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Type of Marker | Description | Population Studied | Key Findings |
---|---|---|---|
Mental State Terms | Words that describe internal mental processes such as thoughts, feelings, desires, perceptions, or intentions (e.g., think, wish, pretend, hate). | Children: between 53 and 60 months (Barreto et al., 2018); between 8 and 10 years old (Grazzani & Ornaghi, 2012); with autism, between 3 and 7 years old (Tager-Flusberg, 1992). | Exposure to and use of mental state terms improves ToM ability (e.g., Barreto et al., 2018; Grazzani & Ornaghi, 2012). Deficits noted in autism (Tager-Flusberg, 1992). |
General Linguistic Ability | Broader language skills such as syntax, vocabulary, and memory for complements. | Children; 3 years old (Astington & Jenkins, 1999); 2 years old (Farrar & Maag, 2002); 4 to 12 years old (van Dijk et al., 2023); under age 7 (Milligan et al., 2007). | Early language abilities (esp. syntax and vocabulary size) linked to later ToM performance (Astington & Jenkins, 1999; Farrar & Maag, 2002) and are proxies for assessing ToM development (Milligan et al., 2007; van Dijk et al., 2023). |
Embedded Clauses | Complex sentence structures embedding one proposition within another (e.g., She thinks that he is lying). | Children; 3 to 5 years old (de Villiers & Pyers, 2002); 3 years old (Lohmann & Tomasello, 2003); 8 years old (Durrleman et al., 2023). | Understanding and using embedded clauses is a prerequisite for and is linked to ToM (de Villiers & Pyers, 2002). Training with embedded clauses enhances false-belief reasoning (Lohmann & Tomasello, 2003) and shown effective in ASD interventions (Durrleman et al., 2023). |
Referring Forms | Use of definite articles (e.g., the), indefinite articles (e.g., a, an), demonstrative determiners (e.g., this, that, these, those), and pronouns (e.g., he, she, it, they, we). | Children; between 1 and 9 years old (Gundel & Johnson, 2013); 6 and 14 years old (Lorusso et al., 2007). | Proxy for mentalizing abilities (Gundel & Johnson, 2013). Inappropriate use in conditions with ToM deficits (Lorusso et al., 2007). |
Pragmatic Markers | Contextual linguistic elements like demonstratives (e.g., this vs. that) and articles (e.g., a vs. the). | Children; 4 to 12 years old (van Dijk et al., 2023). | Likely indicators of ToM competence (Rubio-Fernandez, 2021); linked to character complexity in narratives (van Dijk et al., 2023). |
Definitional Competence | Ability to produce explicit, accurate definitions of words. | Adults, 21 to 85 years old (Bianco et al., 2022); adolescents, 14 to 19 years old (Cornaggia et al., 2024). | Performance on definitional competence tasks is linked to better ToM performance (Bianco et al., 2022; Cornaggia et al., 2024). |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
El Mouslih, C.; Hodgins, V.; Palaniyappan, L.; Titone, D.A. Linguistic Markers of Theory of Mind in Spontaneous Speech: A Narrative Review. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 1016. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081016
El Mouslih C, Hodgins V, Palaniyappan L, Titone DA. Linguistic Markers of Theory of Mind in Spontaneous Speech: A Narrative Review. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(8):1016. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081016
Chicago/Turabian StyleEl Mouslih, Chaimaa, Vegas Hodgins, Lena Palaniyappan, and Debra A. Titone. 2025. "Linguistic Markers of Theory of Mind in Spontaneous Speech: A Narrative Review" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 8: 1016. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081016
APA StyleEl Mouslih, C., Hodgins, V., Palaniyappan, L., & Titone, D. A. (2025). Linguistic Markers of Theory of Mind in Spontaneous Speech: A Narrative Review. Behavioral Sciences, 15(8), 1016. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15081016