Working Memory and Instructional Fit: Reintroducing Aptitude–Treatment Interaction in Education Research
Abstract
1. Introduction
2. WM and Cognitive Performance
3. WM and Academic Achievement
3.1. Role of WM in Academic Domains
3.1.1. Reading Comprehension
3.1.2. Written Expression
3.1.3. Second Language Acquisition
3.1.4. Mathematics and Problem-Solving
3.1.5. Science Learning and Reasoning
3.2. Role of WM in Classroom Skills
3.2.1. Classroom Reasoning and Participation
3.2.2. Note-Taking and Following Instructions
3.2.3. Classroom Management and Behavior Regulation
4. The ATI Framework in Educational Research
4.1. The Evolution and Importance of the ATI Framework
4.2. Methodological Approaches in ATI Research
4.3. Practical Considerations in ATI Research
5. WM and Instructional Strategies: An ATI Perspective
5.1. Multimedia Learning
5.2. Test-Enhanced Learning (Retrieval Practice)
5.3. Spaced Learning (Distributed Practice)
5.4. Interleaved Practice
5.5. Worked Examples and Explicit Instruction
5.6. Instructional Guidance: Inquiry vs. Direct Instruction
5.7. Flipped Classrooms
5.8. Feedback (Types and Delivery)
6. WM, Metacognition, and Retrieval Cues
6.1. Metacognitive Monitoring and Strategies
6.2. Retrieval Cues and Search Processes in WM
7. Integrating ATI Research into Educational Practice
7.1. Guidelines for Educators
7.1.1. Assessment and Classroom Collaboration in ATI-Based Instruction
7.1.2. Adapting Group Instruction Without Stigmatization
7.2. Guidelines for Researchers
8. Future Directions for ATI and WM in Education
9. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Abbreviations
ATI | aptitude–treatment interaction |
WM | working memory |
WMC | working memory capacity |
References
- Agarwal, P. K., Finley, J. R., Rose, N. S., & Roediger, H. L., III. (2017). Benefits from retrieval practice are greater for students with lower working memory capacity. Memory, 25(6), 764–771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Alloway, T. P., & Alloway, R. G. (2010). Investigating the predictive roles of working memory and IQ in academic attainment. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106(1), 20–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Alloway, T. P., & Copello, E. (2013). Working memory: The connected intelligence. Psychology Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baddeley, A. D. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4(10), 829–839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bergmann, J., & Sams, A. (2012). Flip your classroom: Reach every student in every class every day. International Society for Technology in Education. [Google Scholar]
- Bjork, R. A., Dunlosky, J., & Kornell, N. (2013). Self-regulated learning: Beliefs, techniques, and illusions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 417–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Brewer, G. A., & Unsworth, N. (2012). Individual differences in the effects of retrieval from long-term memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 66(3), 407–415. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bui, D. C., Maddox, G. B., & Balota, D. A. (2013). The roles of working memory and intervening task difficulty in determining the benefits of repetition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(2), 341–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burgess, G. C., Gray, J. R., Conway, A. R. A., & Braver, T. S. (2011). Neural mechanisms of interference control underlie the relationship between fluid intelligence and working memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(4), 674–692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cepeda, N. J., Pashler, H., Vul, E., Wixted, J. T., & Rohrer, D. (2006). Distributed practice in verbal recall tasks: A review and quantitative synthesis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(3), 354–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, R. E., Kirschner, P. A., & Sweller, J. (2012). Putting students on the path to learning: The case for fully guided instruction. American Educator, 36(1), 5–11. Available online: https://eric.ed.gov/?id=EJ971752 (accessed on 10 May 2025).
- Cowan, N. (2017). The many faces of working memory and short-term storage. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(4), 1158–1170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cronbach, L. J., & Snow, R. E. (1977). Aptitudes and instructional methods: A handbook for research on interactions. Irvington Publishers. [Google Scholar]
- Delaney, P. F., Godbole, N. R., Holden, L. R., & Chang, Y. (2017). Working memory capacity and the spacing effect in cued recall. Memory, 26(6), 784–797. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Diningrat, S. W. M., Setyosari, P., Ulfa, S., & Widiati, U. (2023). The effect of an extended flipped classroom model for fully online learning and its interaction with working memory capacity on students’ reading comprehension. Journal of New Approaches in Educational Research, 12(1), 78–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Draheim, C., Hicks, K. L., & Engle, R. W. (2016). Combining reaction time and accuracy: The relationship between working memory capacity and task switching as a case example. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(1), 133–155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2005). Self-discipline outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of adolescents. Psychological Science, 16(12), 939–944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dunlosky, J., & Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognition. Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2019). How cognitive psychology can inform evidence-based education reform. In J. Dunlosky, & K. A. Rawson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of cognition and education (pp. 1–14). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Dunlosky, J., & Thiede, K. W. (2013). Four cornerstones of calibration research: Why understanding students’ judgments can improve their achievement. Learning and Instruction, 24, 58–61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Engle, R. W. (2018). Working memory and executive attention: A revisit. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(2), 190–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Finn, A. S., Kraft, M. A., West, M. R., Leonard, J. A., Bish, C. E., Martin, R. E., Sheridan, M. A., Gabrieli, C. F., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2015). Cognitive skills, student achievement tests, and schools. Psychological Science, 26(6), 736–749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Foster, J. L., Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., Hicks, K. L., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2015). Shortened complex span tasks can reliably measure working memory capacity. Memory & Cognition, 43(2), 226–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Friso-van den Bos, I., van der Ven, S. H., Kroesbergen, E. H., & van Luit, J. E. (2015). Working memory and mathematics in primary school children: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 10, 29–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fung, W., & Swanson, H. L. (2017). Working memory components that predict word problem solving: Is it merely a function of reading, calculation, and fluid intelligence? Memory & Cognition, 45(5), 804–823. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fyfe, E. R., DeCaro, M. S., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2015). When feedback is cognitively demanding: The importance of working memory capacity. Instructional Science, 43(1), 73–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fyfe, E. R., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2017). Mathematics practice without feedback: A desirable difficulty in a classroom setting. Instructional Science, 45(2), 177–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gathercole, S. E., & Alloway, T. P. (2008). Working memory and learning: A practical guide for teachers. Sage Publications. [Google Scholar]
- Geary, D. C., Hoard, M. K., Nugent, L., & Bailey, D. H. (2017). Mathematical cognition deficits in children with learning disabilities and persistent low achievement: A five-year longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(5), 680–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ginns, P. (2005). Meta-analysis of the modality effect. Learning and Instruction, 15(4), 313–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffin, T. D., Wiley, J., & Thiede, K. W. (2008). Individual differences, rereading, and self-explanation: Concurrent processing and cue validity as constraints on metacomprehension accuracy. Memory & Cognition, 36(1), 93–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guzman-Munoz, F. J. (2016). The advantage of mixing examples in inductive learning: A comparison of three hypotheses. Educational Psychology, 37(4), 421–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hattie, J., & Timperley, H. (2007). The power of feedback. Review of Educational Research, 77(1), 81–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holmes, J., & Gathercole, S. E. (2014). Taking working memory training from the laboratory into schools. Educational Psychology, 34(4), 440–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ilkowska, M., & Engle, R. W. (2010). Trait and state differences in working memory capacity. In A. Gruszka, G. Matthews, & B. Szymura (Eds.), Handbook of individual differences in cognition: Attention, memory, and executive control (pp. 295–320). Springer. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Perrig, W. J. (2008). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(19), 6829–6833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Juffs, A., & Harrington, M. (2011). Aspects of working memory in L2 learning. Language Teaching, 44(2), 137–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalyuga, S. (2007). Expertise reversal effect and its implications for learner-tailored instruction. Educational Psychology Review, 19(4), 509–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalyuga, S. (2009). Managing cognitive load in adaptive multimedia learning. IGI Global. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kalyuga, S., & Plass, J. L. (2017). Cognitive load as a local characteristic of cognitive processes: Implications for measurement approaches. In R. Z. Zheng (Ed.), Cognitive load measurement and application: A theoretical framework for meaningful research and practice (pp. 59–74). Routledge. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kim, Y.-S. G. (2016). Direct and mediated effects of language and cognitive skills on comprehension of oral narrative texts (listening comprehension) for children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 101–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., & Clark, R. E. (2006). Why minimal guidance during instruction does not work: An analysis of the failure of constructivist, discovery, problem-based, experiential, and inquiry-based teaching. Educational Psychologist, 41(2), 75–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kirschner, P. A., Sweller, J., Kirschner, F., & Zambrano, R. J. (2018). From cognitive load theory to collaborative cognitive load theory. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 13(2), 213–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Koedinger, K. R., Corbett, A. T., & Perfetti, C. (2012). The knowledge-learning-instruction framework: Bridging the science-practice chasm to enhance robust student learning. Cognitive Science, 36(5), 757–798. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koriat, A. (2007). Metacognition and consciousness. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 289–325). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kyllonen, P. C., & Christal, R. E. (1990). Reasoning ability is (little more than) working-memory capacity?! Intelligence, 14(4), 389–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Li, S. (2013). The interactions between the effects of implicit and explicit feedback and individual differences in language analytic ability and working memory. Modern Language Journal, 97(3), 634–654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Limpo, T., & Alves, R. A. (2013). Modeling writing development: Contribution of transcription and self-regulation to Portuguese students’ text generation quality. Journal of Educational Psychology, 105(2), 401–413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Linck, J. A., Osthus, P., Koeth, J. T., & Bunting, M. F. (2014). Working memory and second language comprehension and production: A meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(4), 861–883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Logie, R. H., Camos, V., & Cowan, N. (Eds.). (2021). Working memory: The state of the science. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lusk, D. L., Evans, A. D., Jeffrey, T. R., Palmer, K. R., Wikstrom, C. S., & Doolittle, P. E. (2009). Multimedia learning and individual differences: Mediating the effects of working memory capacity with segmentation. British Journal of Educational Technology, 40(4), 636–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Martinussen, R., & Major, A. (2011). Working memory weaknesses in students with ADHD: Implications for instruction. Theory into Practice, 50(1), 68–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayer, R. E. (2017). Using multimedia for e-learning. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 33(5), 403–423. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayer, R. E., & Moreno, R. (2003). Nine ways to reduce cognitive load in multimedia learning. Educational Psychologist, 38(1), 43–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melby-Lervåg, M., Redick, T. S., & Hulme, C. (2016). Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer”: Evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 512–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meyer, A., Rose, D. H., & Gordon, D. (2014). Universal design for learning: Theory and practice. CAST Professional Publishing. [Google Scholar]
- Oberauer, K. (2019). Working memory and attention—A conceptual analysis and review. Journal of Cognition, 2(1), 36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Olive, T. (2014). Toward a parallel and cascading model of the writing system: A review of research on writing processes coordination. Journal of Writing Research, 5(2), 173–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 122–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2020). Cognitive-load theory: Methods to manage working memory load in the learning of complex tasks. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(4), 394–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1(2), 117–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pellicano, E. (2012). The development of executive function in autism. Autism Research and Treatment, 2012, 146132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peng, P., Barnes, M., Wang, C., Wang, W., Li, S., Swanson, H. L., Dardick, W., & Tao, S. (2018). A meta-analysis on the relation between reading and working memory. Psychological Bulletin, 144(1), 48–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peng, P., & Fuchs, D. (2016). A meta-analysis of working memory deficits in children with learning difficulties: Is there a difference between verbal domain and numerical domain? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 49(1), 3–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Peng, P., Namkung, J., Barnes, M., & Sun, C. (2016). A meta-analysis of mathematics and working memory: Moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(4), 455–473. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Peverly, S. T., Vekaria, P. C., Reddington, L. A., Sumowski, J. F., Johnson, K. R., & Ramsay, C. M. (2013). The relationship of handwriting speed, working memory, language comprehension and outlines to lecture note-taking and test-taking among college students. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 27, 115–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Preacher, K. J., & Sterba, S. K. (2018). Aptitude-by-treatment interactions in research on educational interventions. Exceptional Children, 85(2), 248–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Putnam, A. L., Sungkhasettee, V. W., & Roediger, H. L. (2016). Optimizing learning in college: Tips from cognitive psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(5), 652–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Redick, T. S., Broadway, J. M., Meier, M. E., Kuriakose, P. S., Unsworth, N., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2016). Measuring working memory capacity with automated complex span tasks. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 32(3), 164–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Renkl, A. (2014a). The worked example principle in multimedia learning. In R. E. Mayer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of multimedia learning (2nd ed., pp. 391–412). Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Renkl, A. (2014b). Toward an instructionally oriented theory of example-based learning. Cognitive Science, 38(1), 1–37. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Richland, L. E., & Burchinal, M. R. (2013). Early executive function predicts reasoning development. Psychological Science, 24(1), 87–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Richland, L. E., Zur, O., & Holyoak, K. J. (2007). Cognitive supports for analogies in the mathematics classroom. Science, 316(5828), 1128–1129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roediger, H. L., & Butler, A. C. (2011). The critical role of retrieval practice in long-term retention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(1), 20–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rohrer, D., & Pashler, H. (2010). Recent research on human learning challenges conventional instructional strategies. Educational Researcher, 39(5), 406–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rohrer, D., & Taylor, K. (2007). The shuffling of mathematics problems improves learning. Instructional Science, 35(6), 481–498. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosenshine, B., & Meister, C. (1994). Reciprocal teaching: A review of the research. Review of Educational Research, 64(4), 479–530. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2020). Working memory training in typically developing children: A multilevel meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(2), 423–434. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sana, F., Yan, V. X., & Kim, J. A. (2017). Study sequence matters for the inductive learning of cognitive concepts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 109(1), 84–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sana, F., Yan, V. X., Kim, J. A., Bjork, E. L., & Bjork, R. A. (2018). Does working memory capacity moderate the interleaving benefit? Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 7(3), 361–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanchez, C. A., & Wiley, J. (2006). An examination of the seductive details effect in terms of working memory capacity. Memory & Cognition, 34(2), 344–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sanchez, C. A., & Wiley, J. (2009). To Scroll or not to scroll: Scrolling, working memory capacity, and comprehending complex texts. Human Factors, 51(5), 730–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schmidt-Weigand, F., & Scheiter, K. (2011). The role of spatial descriptions in learning from multimedia. Computers in Human Behavior, 27(1), 22–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schraw, G. (2006). Promoting self-regulation in science education: Metacognition as part of a broader perspective on learning. Research in Science Education, 36(1–2), 111–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., & Engle, R. W. (2016). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence: Maintenance and disengagement. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(6), 771–799. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shipstead, Z., Lindsey, D. R. B., Marshall, R. L., & Engle, R. W. (2014). The mechanisms of working memory capacity: Primary memory, secondary memory, and attention control. Journal of Memory and Language, 72, 116–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snyder, H. R., Miyake, A., & Hankin, B. L. (2015). Advancing understanding of executive function impairments and psychopathology: Bridging the gap between clinical and cognitive approaches. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suzuki, Y., Yokosawa, S., & Aline, D. (2020). The role of working memory in blocked and interleaved grammar practice: Proceduralization of L2 syntax. Language Teaching Research, 26(4), 671–695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Swanson, H. L., Lussier, C. M., & Orosco, M. J. (2013). Cognitive strategies, working memory, and growth in word problem solving in children with math difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 48(4), 339–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Swanson, H. L., Orosco, M. J., & Kudo, M. (2016). Does growth in the executive system of working memory underlie growth in literacy for bilingual children with and without reading disabilities? Journal of Learning Disabilities, 50(4), 386–407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J. (1988). Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning. Cognitive Science, 12, 257–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sweller, J. (2017). Cognitive load theory: Research that teachers really need to understand. Centre for Education Statistics and Evaluation. Available online: https://education.nsw.gov.au/content/dam/main-education/about-us/educational-data/cese/2017-cognitive-load-theory.pdf (accessed on 10 May 2025).
- Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J., & Paas, F. (2019). Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educational Psychology Review, 31(2), 261–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tarchi, C., Ruffini, C., & Pecini, C. (2024). Executive functions and multiple-text comprehension. Learning and Individual Differences, 108, 102566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tolmie, A. K., Ghazali, Z., & Morris, S. (2016). Children’s science learning: A core skills approach. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(3), 481–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tse, C. S., & Pu, X. (2012). The effectiveness of test-enhanced learning depends on trait test anxiety and working-memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 18(3), 253–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2007). The nature of individual differences in working memory capacity: Active maintenance in primary memory and controlled search from secondary memory. Psychological Review, 114(1), 104–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Unsworth, N., Heitz, R. P., Schrock, J. C., & Engle, R. W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research Methods, 37(3), 498–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Unsworth, N., Miller, A. L., & Robison, M. K. (2021). Are individual differences in attention control related to working memory capacity? A latent variable mega-analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 150(7), 1332–1357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, N., & Robison, M. K. (2020). Working memory capacity and sustained attention: A cognitive-energetic perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(1), 77–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, N., Spillers, G. J., & Brewer, G. A. (2012a). Working memory capacity and retrieval from long-term memory: The role of controlled search. Memory & Cognition, 40(2), 242–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Unsworth, N., Spillers, G. J., & Brewer, G. A. (2012b). Working memory capacity and retrieval limitations from long-term memory: An examination of differences in accessibility. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65(12), 2397–2410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van de Pol, J., Volman, M., & Beishuizen, J. (2010). Scaffolding in teacher-student interaction: A decade of research. Educational Psychology Review, 22(3), 271–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vanderberg, R., & Swanson, H. L. (2007). Which components of working memory are important in the writing process? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20(7), 721–752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Sweller, J. (2010). Cognitive load theory in health professional education: Design principles and strategies. Medical Education, 44(1), 85–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: Conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1(1), 3–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vogel, S., & Schwabe, L. (2016). Learning and memory under stress: Implications for the classroom. npj Science of Learning, 1(1), 16011. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wiley, J., & Jarosz, A. F. (2012). Working memory capacity, attentional focus, and problem solving. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(4), 258–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xu, Z., Adam, K. C., Fang, X., & Vogel, E. K. (2018). The reliability and stability of visual working memory capacity. Behavior Research Methods, 50(2), 576–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhang, Y., Tolmie, A., & Gordon, R. (2023). The relationship between working memory and arithmetic in primary school children: A meta-analysis. Brain Sciences, 13(1), 22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2025 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Sana, F.; Fenesi, B. Working Memory and Instructional Fit: Reintroducing Aptitude–Treatment Interaction in Education Research. Behav. Sci. 2025, 15, 765. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060765
Sana F, Fenesi B. Working Memory and Instructional Fit: Reintroducing Aptitude–Treatment Interaction in Education Research. Behavioral Sciences. 2025; 15(6):765. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060765
Chicago/Turabian StyleSana, Faria, and Barbara Fenesi. 2025. "Working Memory and Instructional Fit: Reintroducing Aptitude–Treatment Interaction in Education Research" Behavioral Sciences 15, no. 6: 765. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060765
APA StyleSana, F., & Fenesi, B. (2025). Working Memory and Instructional Fit: Reintroducing Aptitude–Treatment Interaction in Education Research. Behavioral Sciences, 15(6), 765. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs15060765