Next Article in Journal
Workaholism Scales: Some Challenges Ahead
Next Article in Special Issue
Lifelong Fitness in Ambulatory Children and Adolescents with Cerebral Palsy I: Key Ingredients for Bone and Muscle Health
Previous Article in Journal
Gratitude and Life Satisfaction among Older Adults in Saudi Arabia: Social Support and Enjoyment of Life as Mediators
Previous Article in Special Issue
Lifelong Fitness in Ambulatory Children and Adolescents with Cerebral Palsy II: Influencing the Trajectory
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Task-Related Differences in End-Point Kinematics in School-Age Children with Typical Development

1
Division of Physical Therapy, School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, College of Medicine, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
2
Department of Biomedical Engineering, College of Engineering, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
3
Department of Physical Therapy, Federal University of São Carlos, São Carlos 13565-905, SP, Brazil
4
Division of Medical Dietetics, School of Health and Rehabilitation Sciences, College of Medicine, The Ohio State University, Columbus, OH 43210, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Behav. Sci. 2023, 13(7), 528; https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070528
Submission received: 31 May 2023 / Revised: 19 June 2023 / Accepted: 20 June 2023 / Published: 23 June 2023

Abstract

:
Understanding whether and how children with typical development adapt their reaches for different functional tasks could inform a more targeted design of rehabilitation interventions to improve upper extremity function in children with motor disabilities. This prospective study compares timing and coordination of a reach-to-drink, reach-to-eat, and a bilateral reaching task in typically developing school-aged children. Average speed, straightness, and smoothness of hand movements were measured in a convenience sample of 71 children, mean age 8.77 ± 0.48 years. Linear mixed models for repeated measures compared the variables by task, phases of the reach, task x phase interactions, and dominant versus non-dominant hands. There were significant main effects for task and phase, significant task x phase interactions (p < 0.05), and a significant difference between the dominant and non-dominant hand for straightness. Hand movements were fastest and smoothest for the reach-to-eat task, and least straight for the bilateral reaching task. Hand movements were also straighter in the object transport phases than the prehension and withdrawal phases. These results indicate that children with typical development change their timing and coordination of reach based on the task they are performing. These results can inform the design of rehabilitation interventions targeting arm and hand function.

1. Introduction

Functional reach is essential for children to complete tasks like feeding, self-care, school participation, and leisure activities. Intentional reach is essential for driving other areas of development, such as cognitive and fine motor, in young children. Intentional reach-to-grasp skills emerge between 4–6 months of age in typical development as infants begin reaching for toys [1,2] and reach adult-like performance by 8–11 years [3,4,5]. Reaching and grasping are challenging skills for many children with motor disabilities, such as cerebral palsy (CP), and often a major focus of rehabilitation. The most common measures of functional upper extremity use in this population apply clinical observation and subjective scoring of performance, which have limitations in precision and rater bias [6,7]. Kinematic variables collected using three-dimensional (3D) motion capture offer an objective and quantitative measurement of upper extremity movements in early child development [3,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15,16] such as reach and grasp. Timing and coordination of reach and grasp in infants and children are well-documented in the literature [3,5,9,14,16,17,18,19,20].
When quantifying upper extremity movement, a reach is typically defined as a movement towards an object, ending when the hand contacts the object. Straightness and smoothness are variables that quantify the coordination of a reach [17]. Straightness describes the trajectory of the hand, with a straighter movement following a shorter trajectory from the start- to end-point of the movement. Smoothness describes the shape of the velocity profile of the hand, with fewer peaks in the velocity profile indicating a smoother movement. A volitional reach in a healthy adult typically presents with a single velocity peak and a near-straight trajectory with a single, shallow curve [18]. Speed of the reach is calculated to measure timing of the reach movement and both average velocity and peak velocity are frequently used. Speed of a reach increases with maturity [3].
In the past decade, a few studies have applied 3D kinematics to objectively measure reach in a functional task, such as reaching for food to eat or a cup to drink [17,19,20,21]. Butler et al. [17,20] published the Reach and Grasp Cycle, a protocol developed to objectively measure upper extremity movement during a functional reach-to-drink task. Butler et al. [17,20] and Machado et al. [19] have applied this protocol in samples of children with typical development and children with cerebral palsy with upper extremity motor impairments. Hung et al. [21] used a similar reach-grasp-eat task, which involved reaching to eat a cracker, to evaluate functional upper extremity movement in children with CP. These studies provide detailed information about reach and grasp during discrete functional tasks in populations of children with typical development and CP. They did not compare differences in reach kinematics with the type of task and use of the dominant versus non-dominant upper extremity. Previous research in adults has investigated differences in kinematics when reaching for different objects; however, these did not include functional, multi-phase reaching tasks [22,23,24]. This study aims to fill the gap in understanding about how children with typical development change their reaches for different functional tasks. This will inform more precise treatment planning for the improvement of functional upper extremity use and coordination in children with motor impairments.
We adapted the set-up, procedure, and movement sequence from the Reach and Grasp Cycle [20] to three different tasks: a reach-to-drink, reach-to-eat, and a bilateral reach. Our purpose was to measure kinematics of functional reach in typically developing school-aged children when performing various everyday tasks to identify differences in spatiotemporal characteristics of reach. We measured straightness, smoothness, and average speed of hand movement for each phase of the Reach and Grasp Cycle. We hypothesized that there would be significant differences in all three kinematic variables by type of task, phases of the Reach and Grasp Cycle, and between the dominant and non-dominant hands for unilateral tasks. Specifically, we expected the initial reach (prehension) to be less straight, less smooth, and slower than the other 3 phases. We expected movements in the first 3 phases to be straighter, smoother, and slower in the reach-to-drink task compared to the other two tasks. We also expected movements to be less straight, less smooth, and slower in the bilateral reach, due to the bilateral coordination aspect. Last, we expected movements with the dominant hand to be straighter, smoother, and faster than with the non-dominant hand.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants were 71 typically developing children (38 male, 33 female), 7–10 years old (mean 8.77 ± 0.48), with 10 left-hand dominant and 61 right-hand dominant, as determined by the child’s preferred writing hand. Participants were a convenience sample of children recruited from low-resource neighborhoods in Central Ohio, USA. Children were excluded if they were unable to follow instructions given in English, or if they had a motor impairment that prevented them from being able to perform the reach and grasp tasks. Given the exploratory nature of this investigation, the sample size was determined based on recommendations for normative datasets in pediatric populations [25].
This study was approved by the Institutional Review Board at The Ohio State University (2017B0110). Parents or legal guardians provided informed consent, and all child participants provided verbal assent.

2.2. Procedure

Participants were assessed at the Pediatric and Rehabilitation (PEARL) Laboratory at The Ohio State University. Retroreflective markers (8 mm) were placed on the children on their sternum, bilateral acromia, lateral epicondyles, radial and ulnar styloid processes, and heads of the third metacarpals. Markers were also placed on the 4 corners of the table, the back of the chair, and the objects that the children were reaching for (Figure 1). Children were seated at the table with the chair positioned so that their hips, knees, and ankles were flexed to 90°. The objects for which children were reaching (a 5.6 cm diameter cup of water, a 4.6 cm diameter Ritz cracker, or a 21.6 cm diameter ball) were positioned in front of the child at 75% of their arm’s length away, consistent with the set-up from Butler et al. and Machado et al. [19,20]. This position was marked with tape on the table for each participant. The participants began with their hands resting on the table, shoulders neutral, elbows flexed to 90°, and wrists neutral. The resting hand placement was marked on the table with hand-shaped outlines, within which the children placed their hands. For the reach-to-drink and reach-to-eat tasks, children were instructed: “With your [left/right] hand, reach for the [object], pick up the [object], take a [drink/bite], return the [object] to the marked position on the table, and return your hand to the start position. Do this twice.” For the bilateral task, the children were instructed: “Reach for the ball with both hands, pick it up, touch it to your chin, return the ball to the marked position, and return your hands to the start position. Do this twice.” Participants were allowed one practice trial per hand with each object. Two repetitions were recorded for each condition with a 10-camera VICON Motion Capture system at 120 Hz and filtered with a low-pass Butterworth filter at 4 Hz.

2.3. Data Analysis

Phases of the movement (Figure 2) were visually identified and marked as events in the Vicon recordings. The task was divided into the following 4 phases of movement, based on the Reach and Grasp Cycle created by Butler et al. [20]:
  • Prehension: The movement begins with the hand(s) in the marked start position and ends when the hand(s) contact the object.
  • Transport 1: The movement begins with the hand(s) lifting the object from the table and ends with the object contacting the mouth or chin.
  • Transport 2: The movement begins with the object leaving the mouth or chin and ends with the object contacting the table at its marked position.
  • Withdrawal: The movement begins with the hand(s) releasing the object and ends with the hands contacting the marked start position on the table.
A custom MATLAB script was used to calculate spatial and temporal variables of reach for each phase of the Reach and Grasp Cycle. The variables were calculated based on the hand position, which was defined as the center point between the markers on the 3rd metacarpal, ulnar styloid process, and radial styloid process. The spatial and temporal variables were defined as:
  • Straightness (ratio): ratio of the hand path length (total path the hand travels) to the movement length (difference between start- and end-points of the movement), with a value closer to one indicating a straighter movement.
  • Smoothness (count): measured as the number of velocity peaks in a movement, with fewer velocity peaks indicating a smoother movement.
  • Average speed (mm/s): calculated at each position of the marker using a 4-point central difference numerical differentiation.
A generalized linear mixed model for repeated measures for Gaussian distributions was applied using the SAS GLIMMIX procedure to compare each kinematic variable by task, phase, task x phase interactions, and dominant versus non-dominant hand for unilateral tasks (reach-to-drink and reach-to-eat). The model was created using a restricted maximum likelihood estimation technique, and the Kenward–Roger degrees of freedom method and fixed effects standard error adjustment. The alpha level was set to α = 0.05. This model was chosen because it accounts for correlations between dependent variables, as well as a response that is not normally distributed. This is the case for straightness ratio and smoothness, for which values can only be >1. Least squared means were calculated for post hoc testing of task x phase interactions. The Bonferroni correction was applied for post hoc comparisons, setting α = 0.0005.

3. Results

Means and standard deviation for kinematic variables of interest by task, type, and phase were calculated (Table 1).

3.1. Straightness Ratio

There were significant main effects of straightness ratio for type of task (F(2) = 107.26, p < 0.0001), phase of task (F(3) = 801.98, p < 0.0001), type x phase interaction (F(6) = 12.59, p < 0.0001), and hand dominance (F(1) = 13.05, p = 0.0003), with straighter movement on the dominant side (Table 2; Figure 3a). Post hoc analysis revealed significant differences (p < 0.0005) for all comparisons, except: the prehension, transport 1, and transport 2 phases between the reach-to-drink versus the reach-to-eat tasks; the transport 1 and withdrawal phases between the bilateral versus reach-to-eat tasks; and within the reach-to-drink and reach-to-eat tasks, the transport 1 versus transport 2 phases; these comparisons showed no significant differences (Table 3).

3.2. Smoothness

There was a significant main effect of smoothness for type of task (F(2) = 21.06, p < 0.0001). Main effects for phase, type x phase interaction, and dominant versus non-dominant hand were not significant (Table 2; Figure 3b). Post hoc analysis revealed that the reaching movements for the bilateral task were significantly less smooth than in the reach-to-eat task for transport 2 (p < 0.0005). The reaching movement for transport 2 was also significantly less smooth than withdrawal for the bilateral task (p = 0.0005). All other comparisons were not significantly different (Table 3).

3.3. Speed

There were significant main effects of speed for type of task (F(2) = 176.31, p < 0.0001), phase of task (F(3) = 64.87, p < 0.0001), and type x phase interaction (F(6) = 25.38, p < 0.0001), but not for dominant versus non-dominant hand (Table 2; Figure 3c). Post hoc testing revealed the following phase differences within each task: reaching movements in the prehension phase were faster than transport 2 and withdrawal, and reaching movements in the transport 1 phase were faster than transport 2; for the reach-to-eat task, reaching movements were faster in the prehension phase than the three other phases, and withdrawal was slower than all three other phases; reaching movements in the bilateral reach were significantly slower in the withdrawal phase than all other phases (Table 3). The following differences between tasks by phase were also found: for prehension and transport 1, reach movement was faster in both the bilateral and reach-to-eat tasks versus the reach-to-drink task; reaching movement in the reach-to-eat task was also significantly faster than the bilateral task for both transport phases.

4. Discussion

The objective of this study was to evaluate the coordination and timing of reaching movements in typically developing school-aged children when performing functional reaching tasks, applying the Reach and Grasp Cycle [20]. We were able to measure the straightness, smoothness, and speed of reaching movements of 71 children performing three different tasks: reach-to-drink, reach-to-eat, and a bilateral reach. The results from this study support our main hypothesis, indicating that reaching movement varies significantly based on the task being performed, even when the individual is following the same reaching pattern of the Reach and Grasp Cycle [20]. Support for our specific hypotheses was mixed. The prehension movement was slower and straighter than the two transport phases, as predicted, but not the withdrawal phase. Additionally, there was no significant difference in smoothness between the phases. This might have been due to a ceiling effect, as the mean was close to 1 for all phases. As we expected, bilateral movements were less straight than the other two tasks. The bilateral movements were also slower than the reach-to-eat movements, but not reach-to-drink, providing partial support for that hypothesis. Last, the dominant hand did move significantly straighter than the non-dominant hand, but not smoother nor faster. These results highlight the complex interplay of task and object characteristics that impact the timing and coordination of reaching behaviors in children. Since most of the research on the impacts of varying task and object constraints on reach kinematics has focused on an adult population, we will compare and contrast our results with those.
Research in adults has demonstrated changes in reaching behaviors with different task constraints. For example, when adults reach for an empty cup of water, their movements are significantly faster than reaches toward a full cup of water [24,27]. It is not yet known if these effects are the same in a pediatric population. In support of our hypotheses, some of the results in this study were consistent with these findings, such as: reaches were faster in the prehension and transport 1 phases for the reach-to-eat and bilateral tasks than for the reach-to-drink task. This is likely due to greater constraints on the reach-to-drink task, with the need for precision to keep the water in the cup. Consistent with this theory, movements in the two transport phases were straightest for the reach-to-drink task, although only significantly different from the bilateral task. This type of task requires feedback motor control to maintain a straight movement to keep the water from spilling out of the cup [23], meaning that the sensory systems are providing real-time feedback on the position of the cup, water, hand, etc., which the motor system then uses to guide the movement of the upper extremity to complete the task successfully. The children likely slowed their speed in the reach-to-drink task to allow time for this sensory processing to occur.
Changes in object size also affects reach speed in adults performing a reach and grasp task, with slower reaches toward smaller objects [23,28]. In contrast, children in this study had the fastest reaching speeds in the reach-to-eat task, even though the cracker was the smallest of the three objects. In this instance, there are likely other factors that had greater influence on reach behavior than object size. One factor might be that reaching for a cup full of water requires more precision and visuomotor feedback than reaching for a small object, as described previously. An additional factor could be motivation to complete the task. Motivation and reward can impact movement speed, amplitude, and variability in multiple types of movement tasks in adults [22,29,30,31]. In the present study, it is possible that the children found the reward of eating a cracker to be more motivating than drinking water or touching a ball to their chin. This would be consistent with previous research, which indicated that reaching movements were faster and less variable for tasks that offered a reward, compared to tasks with no reward [22,29].
The differences between phases of the Reach and Grasp Cycle in this study were partially consistent with our hypotheses and might indicate that familiarity with specific movements has a strong influence on timing and coordination of the reach. For example, reaches were fastest and straightest in the object transport phases, which is consistent with previous research and our predictions [17,32]. It is possible that these movements, or at least transport 1, can be completed using primarily feedforward control, which is much faster than feedback control [33]. Feedforward motor control relies on stored motor representations, which are created over many repetitions of a motor task, rather than from processing of sensory information in real time. Children bring objects to and from their mouth many times a day; however, the position of the object they are reaching for is variable, and thus the prehension phase requires more feedback control to accomplish. We had also hypothesized that movements would be faster and straighter in the withdrawal phase compared to the prehension phase, which was not supported by our results. It is possible that in the context of this study procedure, participants needed to use feedback control for the withdrawal phase as well, because there was a marked position on which they needed to place their hands. Overall, it is clear that there is a complex interplay of task and object characteristics that impact timing and coordination of reaching behaviors in children via motor control mechanisms.
Last, we found that the dominant hand movements were significantly straighter than the non-dominant hand movements, as we expected. Contrary to our hypothesis, and contrary to studies in adults, we found no differences in speed or smoothness of movement between the dominant and non-dominant hands [34,35]. Given the age and typical development of the participants (7–12 years old), we would expect them to demonstrate a strong hand preference, and our result partially supported this [36]. It is possible that this procedure is not sensitive enough to detect the difference in speed and smoothness between the dominant and non-dominant hands in typical development; however, Butler et al. have shown that it can detect differences between typical and motor-impaired movement [17]. Future investigations should determine the magnitude of difference required to detect a meaningful change for each of these kinematic variables in the Reach and Grasp Cycle. This would help determine whether measuring kinematics of functional reaching tasks could be used to detect clinically important improvements with rehabilitation.

5. Conclusions

Kinematic measurement of coordination and timing of reaching movement in functional reaching tasks is a promising objective, quantitative measure of upper extremity function in a pediatric population, as it can provide detailed information about movement timing and coordination. Results from this study provide insight into how task familiarity, constraints, and motivation might impact the coordination and timing of reach in typically developing children, and the role that motor control systems might play in those differences. This information could be used to further refine clinical rehabilitation approaches for pediatric populations with upper extremity motor impairment. Application to clinical practice could include: (1) routinely using kinematics as an outcome measure to track progress; (2) using information about timing and coordination and task constraints to target improvements in skills that are inherent to participation in daily life activities. Some examples of precise treatment planning based on the results of this study would be to choose larger target objects or more motivating tasks to train increased reach speed, and to use tasks that require feedback control, such as transporting a cup of water, to train straightness and smoothness. Future research could further investigate the impact of specific task characteristics on kinematics in a reach and grasp task in children. An important next step would be to determine a minimal clinically important difference for average speed, smoothness, and straightness of hand movement in the Reach and Grasp Cycle. In the future, data could be used as a normative comparison for age-matched children with motor disability to measure improvements in everyday reaching function with targeted upper extremity therapy interventions.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.M., J.C.H. and E.T.; methodology, J.M., J.C.H., A.M.W.C., E.T. and D.R.; software, D.R.; validation, J.M., D.R. and A.M.W.C.; formal analysis, J.M. and D.R.; investigation, J.M.; resources, C.K.S. and A.M.W.C.; data curation, J.M., C.K.S. and D.R.; writing—original draft preparation, J.M.; writing—review and editing, J.C.H., D.R., A.M.W.C., C.K.S. and E.T.; visualization, J.M.; supervision, J.C.H., A.M.W.C., E.T. and C.K.S.; project administration, J.M. and C.K.S.; funding acquisition, C.K.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by United States Department of Agriculture, grant number 2017-68001-26353; the Foundation for Physical Therapy Research Combined Promotion of Doctoral Studies II-New Investigator Fellowship Initiative; the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development award number 5F30HD104379-02; and C-PROGRESS award number P2CHD101912-04.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of The Ohio State University (protocol code 2017B0110, approved 6 July 2017).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study. Written informed consent has been obtained from the patient(s) to publish this paper.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author.

Acknowledgments

Additional contributions: Gardenia Barbosa, Rachel Bican, Kim Scott, and Thais Cabral for their contributions to protocol design and data collection.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Coluccini, M.; Maini, E.S.; Martelloni, C.; Sgandurra, G.; Cioni, G. Kinematic characterization of functional reach to grasp in normal and in motor disabled children. Gait Posture 2007, 25, 493–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Thelen, E.; Corbetta, D.; Spencer, J.P. Development of reaching during the first year: Role of movement speed. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1996, 22, 1059–1076. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Simon-Martinez, C.; dos Santos, G.L.; Jaspers, E.; Vanderschueren, R.; Mailleux, L.; Klingels, K.; Ortibus, E.; Desloovere, K.; Feys, H. Age-related changes in upper limb motion during typical development. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0198524. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Schneiberg, S.; Sveistrup, H.; McFadyen, B.; McKinley, P.; Levin, M.F. The development of coordination for reach-to-grasp movements in children. Exp. Brain Res. 2002, 146, 142–154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Gilliaux, M.; Dierckx, F.; Vanden Berghe, L.; Lejeune, T.M.; Sapin, J.; Dehez, B.; Stoquart, G.; Detrembleur, C. Age Effects on Upper Limb Kinematics Assessed by the REAplan Robot in Healthy School-Aged Children. Ann. Biomed. Eng. 2015, 43, 1123–1131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. DeMatteo, C.; Law, M.; Russell, D.; Pollock, N.; Rosenbaum, P.; Walter, S. QUEST: Quality of Upper Extremity Skills Test; McMaster University, CanChild Centre for Childhood Disability Research: Hamilton, ON, Canada, 1992. [Google Scholar]
  7. Krumlinde-Sundholm, L.; Eliasson, A.C. Development of the assisting hand assessment: A Rasch-built measure intended for children with unilateral upper limb impairments. Scand. J. Occup. Ther. 2003, 10, 16–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Chen, C.-Y.; Tafone, S.; Lo, W.; Heathcock, J.C. Perinatal stroke causes abnormal trajectory and laterality in reaching during early infancy. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2015, 38, 301–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Bhat, A.N.; Galloway, J.C. Toy-oriented changes during early arm movements: Hand kinematics. Infant Behav. Dev. 2006, 29, 358–372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Corbetta, D.; Thelen, E. Lateral Biases and Fluctuations in Infants’ Spontaneous Arm Movements and Reaching. Dev. Psychobiol. 1999, 34, 237–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Rachwani, J.; Santamaria, V.; Saavedra, S.L.; Wood, S.; Porter, F.; Woollacott, M.H. Segmental trunk control acquisition and reaching in typically developing infants. Exp. Brain Res. 2013, 228, 131–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Nelson, E.L.; Konidaris, G.D.; Berthier, N.E. Hand preference status and reach kinematics in infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 2014, 37, 615–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Marchi, V.; Belmonti, V.; Cecchi, F.; Coluccini, M.; Ghirri, P.; Grassi, A.; Sabatini, A.M.; Guzzetta, A. Movement analysis in early infancy: Towards a motion biomarker of age. Early Hum. Dev. 2020, 142, 104942. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Lynch, A.; Lee, H.M.; Bhat, A.; Galloway, J.C. No stable arm preference during the pre-reaching period: A comparison of right and left hand kinematics with and without a toy present. Dev. Psychobiol. 2008, 50, 390–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Karch, D.; Kim, K.; Wochner, K.; Pietz, J.; Dickhaus, H.; Philippi, H. Quantification of the segmental kinematics of spontaneous infant movements. J. Biomech. 2008, 41, 2860–2867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Lee, M.; Ranganathan, R.; Newell, K.M. Changes in Object-Oriented Arm Movements that Precede the Transition to Goal-Directed Reaching in Infancy. Dev. Psychobiol. 2011, 53, 685–693. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Butler, E.E.; Ladd, A.L.; LaMont, L.E.; Rose, J. Temporal-spatial parameters of the upper limb during a Reach & Grasp Cycle for children. Gait Posture 2010, 32, 301–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Morasso, P. Spatial Control of Arm Movements. Exp. Brain Res. 1981, 42, 223–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Machado, L.R.; Heathcock, J.; Carvalho, R.P.; Pereira, N.D.; Tudella, E. Kinematic characteristics of arm and trunk when drinking from a glass in children with and without cerebral palsy. Clin. Biomech. 2019, 63, 201–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Butler, E.E.; Ladd, A.L.; Louie, S.A.; LaMont, L.E.; Wong, W.; Rose, J. Three-dimensional kinematics of the upper limb during a Reach and Grasp Cycle for children. Gait Posture 2010, 32, 72–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Hung, Y.-C.; Henderson, E.R.; Akbasheva, F.; Valte, L.; Ke, W.S.; Gordon, A.M. Planning and coordination of a reach-grasp-eat task in children with hemiplegia. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2012, 33, 1649–1657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Summerside, E.M.; Shadmehr, R.; Ahmed, A.A. Vigor of reaching movements: Reward discounts the cost of effort. J. Neurophysiol. 2018, 119, 2347–2357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Berthier, N.E.; Clifton, R.K.; Gullapalli, V.; McCall, D.D.; Robin, D.J. Visual Information and Object Size in the Control of Reaching. J. Mot. Behav. 1996, 28, 187–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Flindall, J.W.; Doan, J.B.; Gonzalez, C.L.R. Manual asymmetries in the kinematics of a reach-to-grasp action. Laterality Asymmetries Body Brain Cogn. 2014, 19, 489–507. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Bridges, A.J.; Holler, K.A. How Many is Enough? Determining Optimal Sample Sizes for Normative Studies in Pediatric Neuropsychology. Child Neuropsychol. 2007, 13, 528–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Allen, M.; Poggiali, D.; Whitaker, K.; Marshall, T.R.; Kievit, R.A. Raincloud plots: A multi-platform tool for robust data visualization. Wellcome Open Res. 2019, 4, 63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Nelson, E.L.; Berthier, N.E.; Konidaris, G.D. Handedness and Reach-to-Place Kinematics in Adults: Left-Handers Are Not Reversed Right-Handers. J. Mot. Behav. 2018, 50, 381–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Grosskopf, A.; Kuhtz-Buschbeck, J.P. Grasping with the left and right hand: A kinematic study. Exp. Brain Res. 2006, 168, 230–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Bryden, P.J.; Roy, E.A. Preferential reaching across regions of hemispace in adults and children. Dev. Psychobiol. 2006, 48, 121–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Steenbergen, B. Achieving Coordination in Prehension: Joint Freezing and Postural Contributions. J. Mot. Behav. 1995, 27, 333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Gentilucci, M. Object motor representation and reaching-grasping control. Neuropsychologia 2002, 40, 1139–1153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Mosberger, A.C.; De Clauser, L.; Kasper, H.; Schwab, M.E. Motivational state, reward value, and Pavlovian cues differentially affect skilled forelimb grasping in rats. Learn. Mem. 2016, 23, 289–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Takikawa, Y.; Kawagoe, R.; Itoh, H.; Nakahara, H.; Hikosaka, O. Modulation of saccadic eye movements by predicted reward outcome. Exp. Brain Res. 2002, 142, 284–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Mir, P.; Trender-Gerhard, I.; Edwards, M.J.; Schneider, S.A.; Bhatia, K.P.; Jahanshahi, M. Motivation and movement: The effect of monetary incentive on performance speed. Exp. Brain Res. 2011, 209, 551–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Mackey, A.H.; Walt, S.E.; Stott, N.S. Deficits in upper-limb task performance in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy as defined by 3-dimensional kinematics. Arch. Phys. Med. Rehabil. 2006, 87, 207–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Seidler, R.D.; Noll, D.C.; Thiers, G. Feedforward and feedback processes in motor control. Neuroimage 2004, 22, 1775–1783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Marker placement and child positioning; (a) view from behind; (b) view from front.
Figure 1. Marker placement and child positioning; (a) view from behind; (b) view from front.
Behavsci 13 00528 g001
Figure 2. Four phases of the reaching task: (1) Prehension; (2) Transport 1; (3) Transport 2; (4) Withdrawal.
Figure 2. Four phases of the reaching task: (1) Prehension; (2) Transport 1; (3) Transport 2; (4) Withdrawal.
Behavsci 13 00528 g002
Figure 3. Raincloud plot by task and phase of straightness ratio (a), smoothness (b), and speed (c) [26].
Figure 3. Raincloud plot by task and phase of straightness ratio (a), smoothness (b), and speed (c) [26].
Behavsci 13 00528 g003
Table 1. Mean and standard deviation of straightness ratio, smoothness, and average speed of hand movement by task type and phase.
Table 1. Mean and standard deviation of straightness ratio, smoothness, and average speed of hand movement by task type and phase.
Task TypePhaseVariableMeanStd. Deviation
BilateralPrehensionStraightness Ratio1.180.109
Smoothness1.703.59
Speed (mm/s)465125
Transport 1Straightness Ratio1.060.049
Smoothness1.752.68
Speed (mm/s)467113
Transport 2Straightness Ratio1.100.0528
Smoothness1.973.44
Speed (mm/s)45099.9
WithdrawalStraightness Ratio1.250.150
Smoothness1.431.31
Speed (mm/s)407114
Reach-to-eatPrehensionStraightness Ratio1.110.0792
Smoothness1.100.357
Speed (mm/s)460104
Transport 1Straightness Ratio1.040.0294
Smoothness1.140.415
Speed (mm/s)514116
Transport 2Straightness Ratio1.050.0799
Smoothness1.110.345
Speed (mm/s)514105
WithdrawalStraightness Ratio1.280.171
Smoothness1.180.435
Speed (mm/s)423121
Reach-to-drinkPrehensionStraightness Ratio1.100.0692
Smoothness1.190.852
Speed (mm/s)42993.6
Transport 1Straightness Ratio1.020.0174
Smoothness1.341.70
Speed (mm/s)42087.4
Transport 2Straightness Ratio1.030.0185
Smoothness1.482.07
Speed (mm/s)38976.3
WithdrawalStraightness Ratio1.210.175
Smoothness1.330.807
Speed (mm/s)399128
Table 2. Main effects for least squares means.
Table 2. Main effects for least squares means.
Straightness RatioSmoothnessSpeed
EffectNum DFDen DFFp-ValueDen DFFp-ValueDen DFFp-Value
Task23124107.26<0.0001 *312221.06<0.0001 *3112176.31<0.0001 *
Phase33113801.98<0.0001 *31142.260.0798310964.87<0.0001 *
Task x Phase6311112.59<0.0001 *31131.680.1230310925.38<0.0001 *
Hand1312413.050.000331221.030.310731120.180.6674
* Significant effect.
Table 3. Differences of task x phase least squares means.
Table 3. Differences of task x phase least squares means.
Straightness RatioSmoothnessSpeed
TaskPhaseTaskPhaset Valuep-Valuet Valuep-Valuet Valuep-Value
Ball1Ball214.88<0.0001 *−0.310.7596−0.330.7386
Ball1Ball310.06<0.0001 *−2.020.04382.110.0352
Ball1Ball4−9.02<0.0001 *1.550.12088.13<0.0001 *
Ball2Ball3−4.75<0.0001 *−1.690.09022.420.0158
Ball2Ball4−23.44<0.0001 *1.840.06648.39<0.0001 *
Ball3Ball4−18.73<0.0001 *3.500.0005 *6.00<0.0001 *
Cracker1Cracker29.71<0.0001 *−0.250.8026−7.60<0.0001 *
Cracker1Cracker37.84<0.0001 *−0.040.9664−7.63<0.0001 *
Cracker1Cracker4−19.94<0.0001 *−0.760.44764.82<0.0001 *
Cracker2Cracker3−1.860.06260.210.8365−0.030.9773
Cracker2Cracker4−29.31<0.0001 *−0.510.610212.22<0.0001 *
Cracker3Cracker4−27.47<0.0001 *−0.710.475712.25<0.0001 *
Cup1Cup210.72<0.0001 *−1.040.29921.170.2426
Cup1Cup39.78<0.0001 *−1.960.04995.52<0.0001 *
Cup1Cup4−13.65<0.0001 *−0.920.35893.740.0002 *
Cup2Cup3−0.930.3546−0.920.35854.33<0.0001 *
Cup2Cup4−24.13<0.0001 *0.110.91292.570.0102
Cup3Cup4−23.21<0.0001 *1.020.3085−1.710.0868
Ball1Cracker18.41<0.0001 *3.400.00070.680.4961
Ball1Cup19.75<0.0001 *2.780.00545.06<0.0001 *
Cracker1Cup11.290.1987−0.620.53314.33<0.0001 *
Ball2Cracker23.410.00073.390.0007−6.60<0.0001 *
Ball2Cup25.62<0.0001 *2.000.04526.47<0.0001 *
Cracker2Cup22.170.0301−1.390.164113.00<0.0001 *
Ball3Cracker36.22<0.0001 *5.26<0.0001 *−9.01<0.0001 *
Ball3Cup39.40<0.0001 *2.770.00578.39<0.0001 *
Cracker3Cup33.120.0018−2.510.012117.33<0.0001 *
Ball4Cracker4−2.910.00370.990.3226−2.460.0139
Ball4Cup44.76<0.0001 *0.250.80190.580.5595
Cracker4Cup47.65<0.0001 *−0.740.45693.050.0023
* Significant effect.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Mazzarella, J.; Richie, D.; Chaudhari, A.M.W.; Tudella, E.; Spees, C.K.; Heathcock, J.C. Task-Related Differences in End-Point Kinematics in School-Age Children with Typical Development. Behav. Sci. 2023, 13, 528. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070528

AMA Style

Mazzarella J, Richie D, Chaudhari AMW, Tudella E, Spees CK, Heathcock JC. Task-Related Differences in End-Point Kinematics in School-Age Children with Typical Development. Behavioral Sciences. 2023; 13(7):528. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070528

Chicago/Turabian Style

Mazzarella, Julia, Daniel Richie, Ajit M. W. Chaudhari, Eloisa Tudella, Colleen K. Spees, and Jill C. Heathcock. 2023. "Task-Related Differences in End-Point Kinematics in School-Age Children with Typical Development" Behavioral Sciences 13, no. 7: 528. https://doi.org/10.3390/bs13070528

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop