Next Article in Journal
Comparing Performance of NAF and NT-2013 to SGA as Nutritional Assessment Tools in Systemic Sclerosis Patients
Previous Article in Journal
Comorbidities Across Functional Neurological Disorder Subtypes: A Comprehensive Narrative Synthesis
Previous Article in Special Issue
Preclinical Parkinson’s Disease Models for Non-Motor Symptoms: Research Recommendations from a Systematic Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Olfactory Network Functional Connectivity as a Marker for Parkinson’s Disease Severity

1
Department of Radiology, The Pennsylvania State University College of Medicine, Hershey, PA 17033, USA
2
Medical School of Nanjing University, Nanjing University, Nanjing 210993, China
3
Department of Neurology, The Pennsylvania State University College of Medicine, Hershey, PA 17033, USA
4
Department of Neurology, School of Medicine, The University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA 22903, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Life 2025, 15(8), 1324; https://doi.org/10.3390/life15081324
Submission received: 9 July 2025 / Revised: 12 August 2025 / Accepted: 15 August 2025 / Published: 20 August 2025

Abstract

Olfactory impairment was assessed in akinetic-rigid (PDAR) and tremor-predominant (PDT) subtypes of Parkinson’s disease (PD), classified based on motor symptoms. Seventeen PDAR, fifteen PDT, and twenty-four cognitively normal (CN) participants completed the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test (UPSIT). Groups were well-matched for age and demographic variables, with cognitive performance statistically controlled. Resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI) and seed-based functional connectivity (FC) analyses were conducted to characterize olfactory network (ON) connectivity across groups. UPSIT scores were significantly lower in PDAR compared to PDT. Consistently, ON FC values were reduced in PDAR relative to both PDT and CN. FC of the primary olfactory cortex (POC) significantly differed between CN and the PD subtypes. Furthermore, connectivity in the orbitofrontal cortex and insula showed significant differences between PDAR and PDT, as well as between PDAR and CN. Notably, ON FC between the left hippocampus and the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) also differed significantly between PDAR and PDT. These findings reveal distinct ON FC patterns across PDAR and PDT subtypes. Variations in UPSIT scores suggest that motor symptom subtype is associated with olfactory performance. Moreover, ON connectivity closely paralleled the UPSIT scores, reinforcing a neural basis for olfactory deficits in PD. Given the accelerated motor and cognitive decline often observed in the PDAR, these results support the potential of olfactory impairment as a clinical marker for disease severity.

1. Introduction

Parkinson’s disease (PD) is marked by progressive motor symptoms, including tremors, rigidity, and bradykinesia [1,2]. Variability in symptom presentation has led to classification into subtypes, notably akinetic-rigid (PDAR) and tremor-predominant (PDT) [3,4,5]. Among these, PDAR is associated with poorer clinical outcomes and more rapid cognitive decline [6,7,8,9]. Importantly, motor symptoms are often preceded by a range of non-motor symptoms, sometimes emerging years earlier [10,11]. Impaired olfaction is one such early symptom and is considered a predictive marker of cognitive decline and neurodegeneration in PD [12,13,14,15]. Specifically, this impairment has been partly attributed to olfactory bulb (OB) degeneration, alpha-synuclein deposition, and the loss of mitral and tufted cells [12].
Motor symptoms in PD are primarily linked to dopaminergic cell loss in the nigrostriatal pathway [16], whereas olfactory impairment—a common preclinical symptom in de novo PD—is associated with extranigral pathology [17,18]. Despite both being predictive of PD dementia [10,19], the relationship between motor and olfactory symptoms remains unclear. However, behavioral studies suggest more pronounced olfactory deficits in PDAR compared to PDT [20,21]. For instance, Solla. et al. (2021) reported greater odor threshold impairment in PDAR [20].
Previously, Karunanayaka et al. (2016) used resting-state fMRI (rs-fMRI) from this cohort to examine default mode network (DMN) differences in PD [22]. Another rs-fMRI study explored neuro-temporal functional connectivity (FC) patterns associated with olfactory dysfunction in PD, Alzheimer’s disease (AD), and mild cognitive impairment (MCI) [23]. In healthy adults and older individuals, the sense of smell has been shown to correlate with cognitive performance [24,25]. In contrast, the current study focuses specifically on olfactory network (ON) FC differences between PDAR and PDT subtypes—a distinct aim not addressed in our prior work. Any potential overlap with previous studies is acknowledged and clarified in the Section 4.
This study investigated whether the neural substrate underlying olfactory function is differentially affected in the PDAR and PDT subtypes [26], which were well-matched for age, demographic variables, and cognitive performance. Olfactory performance was measured using the UPSIT [27], while rs-fMRI data were used to examine ON FC. We hypothesized that ON FC would differ between PDAR and PDT, and that these differences would correspond with variations in UPSIT performance.

2. Methods

2.1. Study Subjects

In total, 17 PDAR, 15 PDT, and 24 CN subjects were recruited for this study. Based on the G*Power 3.1.9.7 analysis conducted for a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) with fixed effects, the required sample size was calculated to achieve a power of 0.95 (β = 0.05) with an effect size of f = 0.6, a significance level of α = 0.05, and 3 groups. The analysis determined that a total sample size of 48 (16 per group) would yield an actual power of approximately 0.958. However, our study’s sample size of 56 participants exceeds this requirement, ensuring even greater statistical power to detect the hypothesized effect. The results confirm that the sample size is sufficient to reliably test the hypothesized group differences under the specified parameters, reducing the risk of a Type II error and enhancing the robustness of our findings. The samples were matched as closely as possible for age, sex, education, and cognitive status (Table 1). PDAR and PDT subjects were additionally matched for disease severity via standard clinical scales. Both the PD cohort and the CN subjects, including spouses and relatives of PD participants, were recruited from a tertiary care movement disorder clinic and were part of NIH-funded PD biomarker studies at the Penn State Hershey Medical Center. As previously stated, a description of the data used in this study can also be found in Karunanayaka et al. (2016) [22].
The protocols of this study strictly followed the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki [28] and were also approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the Penn State Hershey Medical Center. As stipulated in the IRB approval, informed, written consent was obtained from each subject before taking part in this study between the year 2009 and 2014.
All PD subjects were assessed by a movement disorder specialist following the criteria outlined in Calne et al. (1992) [29]. Two PD subjects had very mild symptoms and were drug-naïve; all others were treated with antiparkinsonian medications. PDAR and PDT subjects had no other health issues including neurological disorders, hypothyroidism, otolaryngological diseases, vitamin B12 and folate deficiencies, and kidney or liver diseases. Only right-handed PD subjects less than 70 years of age with a Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) score of at least 24, and who took neither a centrally acting acetylcholinesterase inhibitor nor memantine, were included in this study. Subjects completed neuropsychological testing (summary provided in Supplementary Table S2) to confirm a normal range of cognition as well as screening for depression. CN subjects had no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders, or previous head injuries. CN and PD subjects were pre-screened for possible MRI compatibility complications such as metal implants and claustrophobia.

2.2. PDAR and PDT Classification

PD subjects were classified as either PDAR or PDT using the modified ratio developed by Schiess et al., which is based on the UPDRS III, with a numerical ratio derived from mean tremor and akinetic-rigidity scores [8,30]. The tremor was assessed using a nine-item scale, which included a history of left or right arm tremors (two items); rest tremors of the face, lips, chin, and each limb (five items); as well as postural tremors of the right and left upper extremities (two items). The 14-item akinetic-rigidity scale assessed the passive range of motion: the rigidity of the neck and at each extremity (five items), rapid opening and closing of the hands (two items), finger tapping (two items), rising from a chair (one item), posture and postural instability (two items), gait (one item), and body bradykinesia (one item). Each item was rated from 0 to 4, with zero representing an absence of symptoms (or normal activity), and 4 indicating the presence of significant symptoms or impairments. The mean of each scale was calculated and then the ratio (tremor/akinetic rigidity score) was determined. Using this method, PDAR subjects had a ratio ≤0.8, whereas PDT subjects had a ratio ≥0.9. In the current study, the average ratio for PDAR subjects was 0.18 ± 0.22 (range 0–0.73), vs. 2.63 ± 1.77 (range 0.9–7.14; Table 1) for PDT subjects.

2.3. MRI Imaging

MRI data were acquired on a Siemens 3T MRI system (Magnetom Trio, Siemens Medical Solutions USA, Inc., Malvern, PA, USA) with an 8-channel phased array head coil. Imaging was carried out while PD subjects were on medication. The rs-fMRI data were acquired with the following parameters: TR/TE/FA = 2000 ms/30 ms/90°; FOV = 240 × 240 mm2; acquisition matrix = 80 × 80; number of slices = 34; slice thickness = 4 mm; and the number of repetitions = 240. A 3D MPRAGE image was also acquired for volumetric analysis and anatomical overlay. To ensure wakefulness during fMRI scanning, subjects were instructed to relax and keep their eyes open. Imaging protocol did not include collecting respiration or heart rate data. A high-resolution, T1-weighted, 3D gradient-echo sequence (MPRAGE sequence) was also acquired using the following parameters: TR = 2300 ms; TE = 2.98 ms; flip angle = 90°; FOV = 256 × 256 mm2; matrix size = 256 × 256; slice thickness = 1 mm (no slice gap); number of slices = 160; and voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3.

2.4. Olfactory Network

The coordinates of brain regions in the ON were selected based on a meta-analysis of fMRI task activation studies, identifying regions most likely to be activated by olfactory stimulation [31]. Figure 1 shows the ON, which includes the piriform cortex (PC) [(−22 0 −14) (22 2 −12)], insula [(−30 18 6), (28 16 8)], and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) [(−24 30 −10) (28 34 −12)] [31]. These regions of interest (ROIs) in the ON have been shown to have the highest likelihood of being activated during olfactory stimulation [31]. Using AFNI’s 3dDeconvole, time courses from these seed regions were used to identify ON FC, including remote brain structures that comprised the extended ON. Functional connectivity proximal to seed regions was used to identify the core ON [32,33].

2.5. Functional Connectivity Analysis

Rs-fMRI data pre-processing and statistical analyses were performed using DPARSFA V4.5, implemented in DPABI (http://rfmri.org/DPARSFA (accessed on 2 June 2024)). In brief, the methods entailed the following: (1) the removal of the first 10 time points; (2) slice time correction; (3) realignment; (4) unified segmentation using 3D-T1 images and spatial normalization using the deformation parameters; and (5) time course de-trending and spatial smoothing.
The same software was used to estimate the motion parameters for all subjects, ensuring that maximum translation did not exceed 2.5 mm in any xyz direction or 3.0° rotation about any axis. One healthy control (HC) and one PDT participant had maximum translations between 2.5 and 3.0 mm; however, since respiration and heart rate data were not collected, these subjects were excluded from the FC analysis. Seed-based FC analysis, as described in Tobia et al. (2016) [32], was employed to identify differences in ON connectivity between PDAR and PDT groups. Seed time courses were extracted from preprocessed rs-fMRI data normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space, averaging signals within a five-voxel radius around the PC, insula, and OFC coordinates. These time courses identified correlated brain regions both proximal (core ON) and distal (extended ON) to the seeds. ON FC maps were corrected using AlphaSim in AFNI, with a voxel-level threshold p < 0.001 and cluster-level p < 0.05.

2.6. Voxel-Based Morphometry

Voxel-based morphometry (VBM) was performed using SPM12 r7771, accessed on 2 June 2024 (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/). We performed the following steps: (1) inspected T1-weighted anatomical images to ensure no gross anatomical abnormalities; (2) segmented images into gray matter (GM), white matter (WM), and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF); (3) spatially normalized the segmented images—the image intensity of each voxel was modulated by Jacobian determinants to ensure that regional differences in the total amount of GM volume was conserved—(4) transformed registered images into the MNI space using the affine spatial normalization—during this normalization step, images were Jacobian-scaled for “modulated” VBM and resampled to 1.5 mm3 isotropic voxels—(5) the normalized and modulated GMV images were smoothed with an 8 mm full-width, at half-maximum (FWHM), isotropic Gaussian kernel; and (6) a one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was performed to determine volumetric group differences.

2.7. Statistical Analysis

Demographic (age and education) and clinical factors (HAM-D, MMSE, LEDD, and UPDRS III) were compared using a simple one-way ANOVA. The sex ratio between groups was compared using Fisher’s Exact Test. We used standardized neuropsychological testing to evaluate the overall cognitive health of PD subjects in five cognitive domains [34], namely in executive function, spatial memory, verbal memory, attention, and working memory: A detailed analysis of cognitive scores for this cohort is presented in Karunanayaka et al. (2016) [22]. Briefly, a one-way analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) was used to examine group differences while controlling for HAM-D scores. Similarly, group differences were examined using one-way ANCOVA and the Tukey–Kramer method was used to correct for multiple comparisons. See Table S2 in the Supplementary Materials for details.

2.8. Multivariate Classification of ON FC Values

We examined whether FC patterns within the ON contain discriminative information that can differentiate between PDAR and PDT subtypes. A multivariate classification approach was implemented by combining a principal components analysis (PCA) of ON FC values across regions with a nearest-neighbor classifier using the Classify function in Mathematica (Wolfram, Champaign, IL, USA). This embedding-based method integrates a neurobiologically informed model of ON function with a machine learning-based classification. Compared to traditional correlation-based FC approaches, such multivariate techniques can better capture subtle, distributed connectivity differences. As demonstrated by Brodersen et al. (2011), these methods exploit discriminative information encoded in latent physiological variables—such as synaptic connectivity strength—to improve network classification accuracy [35].

2.9. Data Availability

The data cannot be made available as no patient approval has been obtained for sharing coded data. However, the anonymized data and output files of the analyses will be made available on request.

3. Results

3.1. Demographic and Cognitive Comparisons

There were no significant differences in age, MMSE, sex ratio, or education between PDAR, PDT, and CN subjects (p > 0.068; see Table 1 and Table S1 in Supplementary Material). PD subjects had significantly higher UPDRS III scores (p < 0.0001) compared to CN. PDAR subjects had significantly higher HAM-D scores than CN. The five cognitive domains showed no differences among the three groups after FDR correction (p > 0.075). Details are given in Table S2 of the Supplementary Material.

3.2. UPSIT Group Differences

The ANCOVA analysis detected group differences in UPSIT scores (F = 54.21, p < 0.001). Figure 2 provides the results of a post hoc analysis showing group differences in UPSIT scores. CN showed significantly higher UPSIT scores compared to PD subtypes. PDAR, however, had significantly lower UPSIT scores than PDT.

3.3. Volume Differences

VBM differences between groups were not detected after correcting for multiple comparisons with family-wise error (FWE) in SPM12. Differences were detected within the supplementary motor area with a statistical threshold of p < 0.001, uncorrected. A detailed analysis of GM volumes in respective groups is provided in Karunanayaka et al. (2016) [22]. A sample size calculation revealed that our study may be underpowered to detect significant GM group differences between CN and PD.

3.4. ON FC Group Differences

To investigate the neural basis of the UPSIT score differences shown in Figure 2, we conducted a whole-brain one-way ANOVA on ON FC values for each group. The primary olfactory cortex (POC), OFC, insula, cerebellum, hippocampus, and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) showed ON FC group differences (Figure 3).
We extracted ON FC values within the six brain regions identified in Figure 3 and investigated their group differences, which are highlighted in Figure 4 and Figure 5. In the core ON shown in Figure 4, POC connectivity to the ON differed significantly between the CN and both PD subtypes. In contrast, both OFC and insula connectivity to the ON was significantly different between PDAR and CN as well as between PDAR and PDT. These results support differential ON FC patterns between PDT and PDAR within the core ON.
Another pattern of differential ON FC was observed within the extended ON. As shown in Figure 5, ON FC between the left hippocampus and the PCC significantly differed between PDT and PDAR. Note that the left hippocampus FC to the ON differed among all groups.

3.5. Correlations Between ON FC and UPSIT Scores

ON FC values within the six brain regions identified in Figure 3 did not correlate with UPSIT scores within individual groups. However, as shown in Figure 6, ON FC across core and extended ON regions was positively correlated with UPSIT scores in the combined sample. When group effects were considered, the correlations became insignificant. Thus, as shown in Figure 6, the correlations are likely driven by group differences between CN and PD participants. Additionally, Figure 6 shows subtype-dependent clustering: PDAR participants showed the weakest ON FC and lowest UPSIT scores.

3.6. Multivariate Classification of ON FC

Multivariate classification analysis was performed in Mathematica to determine whether ON FC values contained sufficient information, above chance level, to distinguish between PDAR and PDT subtypes. Feature vectors consisted of the ON FC values from each group. Classification was implemented using the versatile Classify function in Mathematica (see Classify—Wolfram Language Documentation for details). The analysis demonstrated that ON FC values provided above-chance discriminative information (classification accuracy > 50%) for separating PDAR from PDT, suggesting that ON FC can serve as a potential neural marker for differentiating Parkinson’s disease subtypes. Before performing the nearest-neighbor classification analysis, we performed PCA on the multivariate ON FC data, as shown in Figure 7. The classification accuracies were 67% for PDT and 83% for PDAR. As such, these results provide convincing evidence to suggest that ON FC in fact contains above-chance discriminatory information for distinguishing between the two PD subtypes.

4. Discussion

Olfactory dysfunction is highly prevalent in neurodegenerative diseases and often emerges in the preclinical stage, persists throughout disease progression, and is easily testable—making it a promising marker for early diagnosis, differential diagnosis, and prognosis. PD-related olfactory dysfunction is believed to be due to alpha-synuclein pathology in the OB and anterior olfactory nucleus, consistent with Braak staging of PD [12,36,37,38]. This suggests that PD pathology may begin in the primary olfactory structures and the lower brainstem before reaching higher-order olfactory structures and the substantia nigra.
This study provides strong evidence for dissociable impairments in ON FC between PDT and PDAR subtypes of PD. Aside from the olfactory performance, the subtypes were well-matched for age, demographic variables, and cognitive performance. Prior clinical studies have documented differences in cortical and subcortical networks affected in these subtypes, which may contribute to variations in disease progression and cognitive decline [8,26].
In line with these findings, PDAR participants—known to experience faster motor and cognitive deterioration—exhibited lower UPSIT scores and reduced ON FC strength compared to PDT, despite being matched on clinical measures such as UPDRS-III, the Hoehn and Yahr scale, levodopa dose, disease duration, and cognitive status [9]. ON FC likely reflects the temporal coordination of activity across anatomically distributed olfactory regions, and such functional interactions appear to underlie olfactory performance [39]. These subtype-specific ON FC patterns highlight central olfactory system deficits in PD and reinforce the presence of distinct neuropathological mechanisms in PDAR versus PDT [8,40].

4.1. Impact of Non-Olfactory Cognitive Scores on UPSIT

Olfaction is a multidimensional sense that can be influenced by other cognitive domains, such as memory, verbal processing, and attention [41]. However, in the present study, the three groups were closely matched in age, demographic characteristics, and cognitive performance. This careful matching minimizes the potential confounding effects of non-olfactory cognitive factors on UPSIT scores. Furthermore, the ON FC group differences shown in Figure 3 involve regions implicated in higher-order olfactory processing—for example, the hippocampus—highlighting the well-established relationship between olfaction and memory function [24,42]. These findings suggest that the observed group differences are likely attributable to genuine differences in olfactory function. Specifically, higher ON FC appears to be associated with a better olfactory performance.

4.2. ON FC Distribution

We observed a positive correlation between ON FC and UPSIT scores across the combined sample, suggesting a meaningful link between ON FC and olfactory performance [43]. While within-group correlations were not specifically quantified, the combined analysis (Figure 6) revealed subtype-dependent clustering: PDAR participants showed the weakest ON FC and lowest UPSIT scores. However, once group effects were accounted for, the bivariate correlations lost statistical significance, suggesting that the observed association was primarily driven by PD subtype differences. This may also reflect limitations due to the relatively small sample sizes. Despite this, the observed trends support the potential for ON FC to reflect pathological changes in key olfactory regions such as the POC and hippocampus, as well as behavioral deficits in PD. Similar associations have been proposed in Alzheimer’s disease using ON FC as a marker of olfactory dysfunction [33]. Our findings offer in vivo evidence for central ON involvement—beyond the OB and tract—in PD, consistent with the view that olfactory impairments in PD arise from disrupted central processing [44].
Olfactory deficits are common in early PD and currently serve as supportive diagnostic indicators. Often emerging as a prodromal symptom, smell loss frequently precedes motor and cognitive decline. For instance, the reduced intrinsic integrity of the substantia nigra in individuals with idiopathic olfactory loss has been used to confirm a PD “at-risk” status [45]. Conversely, patients who are normosmic and cognitively intact at diagnosis tend to show a stable cognitive performance for up to a decade [46]. Additionally, PD-related olfactory impairments have been shown to help differentiate PD from other movement disorders [47,48].
Our results suggest that the preferential involvement of the ON in PD subtypes—PDT and PDAR—may lead to measurable differences in ON FC even before cognitive impairments emerge [22]. This raises the possibility that ON FC could serve as a prognostic marker for identifying individuals at risk for cognitive decline, such as progression to MCI in PD. Moreover, ON FC may reflect the integrity of functional brain communication within the olfactory system, offering a potential indicator of disease severity [39]. These findings reveal a novel brain–behavior relationship, underscoring the close correspondence between motor and olfactory impairments in PD.
Motor symptoms in PD result from the loss of dopaminergic neurons in the nigrostriatal pathway [49]. In contrast, PD-related olfactory deficits do not improve with dopaminergic treatment and are believed to arise from extranigral pathology [50]. Our findings indicate that olfactory and motor impairments may share a common neural substrate at the network level, as evidenced by the rs-fMRI data. We hypothesize that dysfunction in this shared substrate may increase the risk of developing PD dementia, given the strong link between olfaction and cognition.
Unlike Zhou et al. (2019), who estimated FC of individual olfactory brain regions [51], our study focused on the overall connectivity of the ON, combining both common and individual FC patterns. Our network-level approach aimed to demonstrate that group differences in UPSIT scores correspond to connectivity changes within the ON. Nonetheless, we acknowledge that Zhou et al.’s method could serve as an alternative way to investigate the neural basis of UPSIT group differences in our data [51].

4.3. Network Perspective of Olfactory Function in PD

Localized brain functions are thought to arise from distributed global connectivity patterns [52]. Extending this principle to olfaction, our study suggests that ON FC may provide a mechanistic framework for understanding PD-related olfactory impairments. Identifying how PD pathology disrupts brain function is critical for developing early diagnostic tools and interventions to slow disease progression. Notably, olfactory regions are among the first affected by pathological protein aggregates [53,54], making the ON particularly vulnerable to early neurodegeneration. This supports our decision to include cortical brain regions in the ON that are most likely to be activated by olfactory stimulation. However, as mentioned earlier, the pathology in the OB—which has strong bidirectional connections with the POC—is common in early PD [55]. Although the OB is difficult to image with 3T fMRI and was not included in this study, the consistent presence of olfactory deficits across neurodegenerative diseases may reflect the disruption of a shared, evolutionarily conserved neural substrate.

4.4. Resting-State fMRI

RS brain networks are intrinsically organized and modulated by the task performance [52,56], and a strong correspondence between RS functional connectivity (RSFC) and task-evoked connectivity has been demonstrated, including in the olfactory system. RSFC also closely aligns with structural brain connectivity [57]. In this study, we examined whether ON FC differs between the core and extended ON in PD subtypes. While no significant differences in ON FC to the POC were observed, PDAR participants exhibited significantly reduced ON FC to the OFC and insula—key multisensory integration hubs. Additionally, connectivity to the hippocampus and PCC within the extended ON was significantly lower in PDAR. Notably, ON FC to the hippocampus mirrored patterns in the UPSIT scores (Figure 3), aligning with findings by Westermann et al. (2008) suggesting hippocampal involvement in olfactory sensitivity [58]. These results reveal widespread ON FC alterations in PD and support the hypothesis that resting-state connectivity disruptions underlie olfactory deficits in PD [59].

4.5. Gray Matter Comparison

In PD, the relationship between ON atrophy and olfactory impairment remains unclear [60]. Some studies have reported cortical GM loss—such as atrophy in the right piriform cortex—to be associated with olfactory deficits in PD. However, other investigations have failed to identify consistent relationships between GM atrophy and olfactory impairment, highlighting the need for further research to clarify the structural basis of olfactory dysfunction in PD [61,62,63]. The present study examined cortical atrophy within the ON and its relationship with olfactory function in a relatively large sample of early-stage PD patients. We did not observe statistically significant GM volume loss within the ON, suggesting that regional atrophy may be less pronounced in the absence of comorbid cognitive impairment. However, this absence should not be interpreted as evidence that PD has no impact on the GM structure. Rather, our findings suggest that functional connectivity changes in the ON may be more sensitive than structural changes, particularly in early stages of the disease. Given that olfactory deficits are predictive of dementia in PD, ON FC may serve as a promising biomarker for identifying individuals at elevated risk of cognitive decline [64].

4.6. Multivariate ON FC

Our findings demonstrated that multivariate ON FC measurements—specifically the first two principal components—contain sufficient discriminative information to distinguish between PDAR and PDT subtypes. The PCA revealed distinct ON FC patterns across regions, enabling the accurate classification of the two groups and pointing to underlying differences in neural mechanisms. While the classifier accuracy offers insight into discriminative power, it should not be interpreted as a direct measure of the effect size. Accuracy can increase when data are aggregated within groups, but this often comes at the cost of reduced statistical power due to smaller sample sizes. Future work should explore integrated approaches that combine multivariate and univariate analyses to better understand the brain mechanisms underlying PD subtypes.

4.7. Study Limitations and Future Directions

Given the small sample size of our study, larger cross-sectional and longitudinal investigations are needed to validate the integrity of ON FC and clarify its relationship with motor and olfactory impairments in PD. Complex neurodegenerative changes may occur across brain networks even in PD patients with mild cognitive impairment (PD-MCI), and characterizing olfactory dysfunction in this group is crucial for understanding PD pathophysiology. Such insights could inform early intervention strategies to delay or prevent the onset of PD dementia. Additionally, the framework presented here could be extended by integrating resting-state FC with task-based FC measures—such as those derived from odor threshold, discrimination, or identification paradigms—to develop a unified FC metric for better characterization of PD subtypes.
Although we initially defined a simplified ON consisting of six core regions, our functional connectivity analysis revealed the recruitment of additional brain areas, including the hippocampus, cerebellum, and PCC—regions that are well-documented to play key roles in both olfactory and memory-related processing [24,31,42,65]. Therefore, in line with Zhou et al. (2019), future research should focus on connectivity within an expanded ON, which represents a valuable extension of our current approach [32,33]. We expect that incorporating enriched ON FC features will be particularly useful for developing more sensitive classifiers to distinguish PDAR from PDT.
Since PD is characterized by heterogeneous symptoms and progression patterns, subtype identification has become a critical focus of current research. For instance, the side of motor symptom onset (left or right) has been shown to influence disease progression and the manifestation of specific non-motor symptoms. Subtype classification can also be based on prominent non-motor features such as depression, anxiety, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep behavior disorder (RBD), and olfactory dysfunction. Furthermore, growing evidence suggests that the classification of motor subtypes in PD is not static and may shift over time with disease progression duration. Therefore, it is essential to integrate and build upon current findings from studies focused on various PD subtypes to improve disease characterization and targeted interventions.
Although olfactory impairment is one of the earliest symptoms of PD, often preceding the onset of motor signs and cognitive decline, it is also prevalent in other neurodegenerative disorders and, therefore, lacks specificity [47]. As such, it is best utilized in combination with other prodromal markers of PD—such as REM sleep behavior disorder and constipation [66,67]—to enhance early detection strategies.

5. Conclusions

Alterations in olfaction were associated with distinct ON FC patterns in PD subtypes. Compared to CN and PDT patients, the PDAR subtype showed greater ON FC impairments, despite groups being well-matched for age, demographic, and clinical variables, as shown in Table 1. These differences persisted even after adjusting for the gray matter volume and cognitive performance (MMSE). The data revealed a strong association between olfactory and motor symptoms in PD and suggest that olfactory dysfunction may serve as a novel biomarker for early PD severity. Previously, using this cohort, we demonstrated that posterior DMN impairments in PDAR may contribute to an increased risk of cognitive decline [22]. Our current findings similarly show greater ON FC deficits in PDAR, supporting the hypothesis that olfactory impairment may predict cognitive vulnerability in PD [68]. Future studies with larger cohorts and longitudinal designs are needed to validate these findings and explore whether combining ON and DMN metrics enhances the prognostic accuracy [69,70]. Overall, this study highlights olfactory impairment as a promising clinical biomarker of PD severity and progression.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/life15081324/s1, Table S1: Full summary of UPDRS scores for PDAR and PDT. AR indicates akinetic-rigidity subtype. T indicates tremor-predominant subtype, Table S2: Neuropsychological test scores and analyses.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, P.K., X.H., P.E. and S.P.; methodology, P.K., A.E. and S.P.; software, S.P., A.E. and J.L.; writing—original draft preparation, P.K. and X.H.; writing—review and editing, S.P., P.E., A.E., R.C., K.G. and R.E.; supervision, P.K.; funding acquisition, P.K. and X.H. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institute on Aging (NIA) grants R01AG070088 & AG027771, National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS) grant NS060722, the Penn State Hershey General Clinical Research Center (GCRC; NIH M01RR10732), GCRC Construction Grants (C06RR016499), the Leader Family Foundation, and the Department of Radiology, Penn State College of Medicine. This project also received partial funding from a grant with the Pennsylvania Department of Health using Tobacco CURE Funds.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of Institutional Review Board Penn State College of medicine (protocol code PRAMS00028989 and 11 December 2018).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data generated and analyzed during the current study are not publicly available since they were purposively collected by the authors for the present study, but they are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the support received from the Pennsylvania State University College of Medicine MRI core facility, Center for NMR research, and the Translational Brain Research Center.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Martí, M.J.; Tolosa, E. New Guidelines for Diagnosis of Parkinson Disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2013, 9, 190–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Lees, A.J.; Hardy, J.; Revesz, T. Parkinson’s Disease. Lancet 2009, 373, 2055–2066. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Zetusky, W.J.; Jankovic, J.; Pirozzolo, F.J. The Heterogeneity of Parkinson’s Disease: Clinical and Prognostic Implications. Neurology 1985, 35, 522–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Jankovic, J.; McDermott, M.; Carter, J.; Gauthier, S.; Goetz, C.; Golbe, L.; Huber, S.; Koller, W.; Olanow, C.; Shoulson, I.; et al. Variable Expression of Parkinson’s Disease: A Base-Line Analysis of the Datatop Cohort. The Parkinson Study Group. Neurology 1990, 40, 1529–1534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Lewis, S.; Foltynie, T.; Blackwell, A.D.; Robbins, T.W.; Owen, A.M.; Barker, R.A. Heterogeneity of Parkinson’s Disease in the Early Clinical Stages Using a Data Driven Approach. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2005, 76, 343–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Eggers, C.; Pedrosa, D.J.; Kahraman, D.; Maier, F.; Lewis, C.J.; Fink, G.R.; Schmidt, M.; Timmermann, L. Parkinson Subtypes Progress Differently in Clinical Course and Imaging Pattern. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e46813. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Helmich, R.C.; Hallett, M.; Deuschl, G.; Toni, I.; Bloem, B.R. Cerebral Causes and Consequences of Parkinsonian Resting Tremor: A Tale of Two Circuits? Brain 2012, 135, 3206–3226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Lewis, M.M.; Du, G.; Sen, S.; Kawaguchi, A.; Truong, Y.; Lee, S.; Mailman, R.B.; Huang, X. Differential Involvement of Striato- and Cerebello-Thalamo-Cortical Pathways in Tremor- and Akinetic/Rigid-Predominant Parkinson’s Disease. Neuroscience 2011, 177, 230–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Zaidel, A.; Arkadir, D.; Israel, Z.; Bergman, H. Akineto-Rigid Vs. Tremor Syndromes in Parkinsonism. Curr. Opin. Neurol. 2009, 22, 387–393. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Schapira, A.H.; Chaudhuri, K.R.; Jenner, P. Non-Motor Features of Parkinson Disease. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017, 18, 435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Solla, P.; Wang, Q.; Frau, C.; Floris, V.; Loy, F.; Sechi, L.A.; Masala, C. Olfactory Impairment Is the Main Predictor of Higher Scores at Rem Sleep Behavior Disorder (Rbd) Screening Questionnaire in Parkinson’s Disease Patients. Brain Sci. 2023, 13, 599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Doty, R.L. Olfactory Dysfunction in Parkinson Disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2012, 8, 329–339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Siderowf, A.; Jennings, D.; Eberly, S.; Oakes, D.; Hawkins, K.A.; Ascherio, A.; Stern, M.B.; Marek, K.; Investigators, P. Impaired Olfaction and Other Prodromal Features in the Parkinson at-Risk Syndrome Study. Mov. Disord. 2012, 27, 406–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Postuma, R.; Gagnon, J.-F. Cognition and Olfaction in Parkinson’s Disease. Brain 2010, 133, e160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Fullard, M.E.; Tran, B.; Xie, S.X.; Toledo, J.B.; Scordia, C.; Linder, C.; Purri, R.; Weintraub, D.; Duda, J.E.; Chahine, L.M. Olfactory Impairment Predicts Cognitive Decline in Early Parkinson’s Disease. Park. Relat. Disord. 2016, 25, 45–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Helmich, R.C.; Janssen, M.J.; Oyen, W.J.; Bloem, B.R.; Toni, I. Pallidal Dysfunction Drives a Cerebellothalamic Circuit into Parkinson Tremor. Ann. Neurol. 2011, 69, 269–281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Ponsen, M.M.; Stoffers, D.; Booij, J.; van Eck-Smit, B.L.; Wolters, E.C.; Berendse, H.W. Idiopathic Hyposmia as a Preclinical Sign of Parkinson’s Disease. Ann. Neurol. 2004, 56, 173–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Tissingh, G.; Berendse, H.; Bergmans, P.; DeWaard, R.; Drukarch, B.; Stoof, J.; Wolters, E.C. Loss of Olfaction in De Novo and Treated Parkinson’s Disease: Possible Implications for Early Diagnosis. Mov. Disord. 2001, 16, 41–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Poewe, W.; Seppi, K.; Tanner, C.M.; Halliday, G.M.; Brundin, P.; Volkmann, J.; Schrag, A.-E.; Lang, A.E. Parkinson Disease. Nat. Rev. Dis. Primers 2017, 3, 17013. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Solla, P.; Masala, C.; Ercoli, T.; Orofino, G.; Loy, F.; Pinna, I.; Fadda, L.; Defazio, G. Olfactory Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease Patients with Tremor Dominant Subtype Compared to Those with Akinetic Rigid Dominant Subtype: A Pilot Study. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Stern, M.B.; Doty, R.; Dotti, M.; Corcoran, P.; Crawford, D.; McKeown, D.; Adler, C.; Gollomp, S.; Hurtig, H. Olfactory Function in Parkinson’s Disease Subtypes. Neurology 1994, 44, 266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Karunanayaka, P.R.; Lee, E.Y.; Lewis, M.M.; Sen, S.; Eslinger, P.J.; Yang, Q.X.; Huang, X. Default Mode Network Differences between Rigidity- and Tremor-Predominant Parkinson’s Disease. Cortex 2016, 81, 239–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Cieri, F.; Giriprakash, P.; Nandy, R.; Zhuang, X.; Caldwell, J.Z.K.; Cordes, D. Functional Connectivity Differences of the Olfactory Network in Parkinson’s Disease, Mild Cognitive Impairment and Cognitively Normal Individuals: A Resting-State Fmri Study. Neuroscience 2024, 559, 8–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Yahiaoui-Doktor, M.; Luck, T.; Riedel-Heller, S.G.; Loeffler, M.; Wirkner, K.; Engel, C. Olfactory Function Is Associated with Cognitive Performance: Results from the Population-Based Life-Adult-Study. Alzheimers Res. Ther. 2019, 11, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Uchida, S.; Shimada, C.; Sakuma, N.; Kagitani, F.; Kan, A.; Awata, S. The Relationship between Olfaction and Cognitive Function in the Elderly. J. Physiol. Sci. 2020, 70, 48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hummel, T.; Fliessbach, K.; Abele, M.; Okulla, T.; Reden, J.; Reichmann, H.; Wüllner, U.; Haehner, A. Olfactory Fmri in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 2010, 4, 125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Doty, R.L.; Shaman, P.; Dann, M. Development of the University of Pennsylvania Smell Identification Test: A Standardized Microencapsulated Test of Olfactory Function. Physiol. Behav. 1984, 32, 489–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Goodyear, M.D.; Krleza-Jeric, K.; Lemmens, T. The Declaration of Helsinki. BMJ 2007, 335, 624. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Calne, D.; Snow, B.; Lee, C. Criteria for Diagnosing Parkinson’s Disease. Ann. Neurol. 1992, 32, S125–S127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Schiess, M.C.; Zheng, H.; Soukup, V.M.; Bonnen, J.G.; Nauta, H.J. Parkinson’s Disease Subtypes: Clinical Classification and Ventricular Cerebrospinal Fluid Analysis. Park. Relat. Disord. 2000, 6, 69–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Seubert, J.; Freiherr, J.; Djordjevic, J.; Lundström, J.N. Statistical Localization of Human Olfactory Cortex. NeuroImage 2013, 66, 333–342. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Tobia, M.J.; Yang, Q.X.; Karunanayaka, P. Intrinsic Intranasal Chemosensory Brain Networks Shown by Resting-State Functional Mri. Neuroreport 2016, 27, 527–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Lu, J.; Testa, N.; Jordan, R.; Elyan, R.; Kanekar, S.; Wang, J.; Eslinger, P.; Yang, Q.X.; Zhang, B.; Karunanayaka, P.R. Functional Connectivity between the Resting-State Olfactory Network and the Hippocampus in Alzheimer’s Disease. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Lee, E.-Y.; Sen, S.; Eslinger, P.J.; Wagner, D.; Shaffer, M.L.; Kong, L.; Lewis, M.M.; Du, G.; Huang, X. Early Cortical Gray Matter Loss and Cognitive Correlates in Non-Demented Parkinson’s Patients. Park. Relat. Disord. 2013, 19, 1088–1093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Brodersen, K.H.; Schofield, T.M.; Leff, A.P.; Ong, C.S.; Lomakina, E.I.; Buhmann, J.M.; Stephan, K.E. Generative Embedding for Model-Based Classification of Fmri Data. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2011, 7, e1002079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Braak, H.; Del Tredici, K.; Rüb, U.; De Vos, R.A.; Steur, E.N.J.; Braak, E. Staging of Brain Pathology Related to Sporadic Parkinson’s Disease. Neurobiol. Aging 2003, 24, 197–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Attems, J.; Walker, L.; Jellinger, K.A. Olfactory Bulb Involvement in Neurodegenerative Diseases. Acta Neuropathol. 2014, 127, 459–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Beach, T.G.; White, C.L.; Hamilton, R.L.; Duda, J.E.; Iwatsubo, T.; Dickson, D.W.; Leverenz, J.B.; Roncaroli, F.; Buttini, M.; Hladik, C.L. Evaluation of A-Synuclein Immunohistochemical Methods Used by Invited Experts. Acta Neuropathol. 2008, 116, 277–288. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Van Den Heuvel, M.P.; Pol, H.E.H. Exploring the Brain Network: A Review on Resting-State Fmri Functional Connectivity. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010, 20, 519–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Fishman, P.S. Paradoxical Aspects of Parkinsonian Tremor. Mov. Disord. 2008, 23, 168–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gottfried, J.A. Central Mechanisms of Odour Object Perception. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010, 11, 628–641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Kostka, J.K.; Bitzenhofer, S.H. How the Sense of Smell Influences Cognition Throughout Life. Neuroforum 2022, 28, 177–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Ercoli, T.; Masala, C.; Cadeddu, G.; Mascia, M.M.; Orofino, G.; Gigante, A.F.; Solla, P.; Defazio, G.; Rocchi, L. Does Olfactory Dysfunction Correlate with Disease Progression in Parkinson’s Disease? A Systematic Review of the Current Literature. Brain Sci. 2022, 12, 513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Witt, M.; Bormann, K.; Gudziol, V.; Pehlke, K.; Barth, K.; Minovi, A.; Hähner, A.; Reichmann, H.; Hummel, T. Biopsies of Olfactory Epithelium in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. Mov. Disord. 2009, 24, 906–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Haehner, A.; Schöpf, V.; Loureiro, A.; Linn, J.; Reichmann, H.; Hummel, T.; Kitzler, H.H. Substantia Nigra Fractional Anisotropy Changes Confirm the Pd at-Risk Status of Patients with Idiopathic Smell Loss. Park. Relat. Disord. 2018, 50, 113–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Domellöf, M.E.; Lundin, K.-F.; Edström, M.; Forsgren, L. Olfactory Dysfunction and Dementia in Newly Diagnosed Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. Park. Relat. Disord. 2017, 38, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Marin, C.; Vilas, D.; Langdon, C.; Alobid, I.; López-Chacón, M.; Haehner, A.; Hummel, T.; Mullol, J. Olfactory Dysfunction in Neurodegenerative Diseases. Curr. Allergy Asthma Rep. 2018, 18, 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. White, T.L.; Sadikot, A.F.; Djordjevic, J. Metacognitive Knowledge of Olfactory Dysfunction in Parkinson’s Disease. Brain Cogn. 2016, 104, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Dirkx, M.F.; den Ouden, H.; Aarts, E.; Timmer, M.; Bloem, B.R.; Toni, I.; Helmich, R.C. The Cerebral Network of Parkinson’s Tremor: An Effective Connectivity Fmri Study. J. Neurosci. 2016, 36, 5362–5372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Mundinano, I.-C.; Caballero, M.-C.; Ordóñez, C.; Hernandez, M.; DiCaudo, C.; Marcilla, I.; Erro, M.-E.; Tunon, M.-T.; Luquin, M.-R. Increased Dopaminergic Cells and Protein Aggregates in the Olfactory Bulb of Patients with Neurodegenerative Disorders. Acta Neuropathol. 2011, 122, 61. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Zhou, G.; Lane, G.; Cooper, S.L.; Kahnt, T.; Zelano, C. Characterizing Functional Pathways of the Human Olfactory System. eLife 2019, 8, e47177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Cole, M.W.; Bassett, D.S.; Power, J.D.; Braver, T.S.; Petersen, S.E. Intrinsic and Task-Evoked Network Architectures of the Human Brain. Neuron 2014, 83, 238–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Braak, H.; Braak, E. Evolution of Neuronal Changes in the Course of Alzheimer’s Disease. J. Neural. Transm. Suppl. 1998, 53, 127–140. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  54. Braak, H.; Rub, U.; Jansen Steur, E.N.; Del Tredici, K.; de Vos, R.A. Cognitive Status Correlates with Neuropathologic Stage in Parkinson Disease. Neurology 2005, 64, 1404–1410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Wang, J.; You, H.; Liu, J.-F.; Ni, D.-F.; Zhang, Z.-X.; Guan, J. Association of Olfactory Bulb Volume and Olfactory Sulcus Depth with Olfactory Function in Patients with Parkinson Disease. Am. J. Neuroradiol. 2011, 32, 677–681. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Deco, G.; Jirsa, V.K.; McIntosh, A.R. Emerging Concepts for the Dynamical Organization of Resting-State Activity in the Brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2011, 12, 43–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Honey, C.J.; Kötter, R.; Breakspear, M.; Sporns, O. Network Structure of Cerebral Cortex Shapes Functional Connectivity on Multiple Time Scales. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 10240–10245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Westermann, B.; Wattendorf, E.; Schwerdtfeger, U.; Husner, A.; Fuhr, P.; Gratzl, O.; Hummel, T.; Bilecen, D.; Welge-Lüssen, A. Functional Imaging of the Cerebral Olfactory System in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 2008, 79, 19–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Parkkinen, L.; Silveira-Moriyama, L.; Holton, J.L.; Lees, A.J.; Revesz, T. Can Olfactory Bulb Biopsy Be Justified for the Diagnosis of Parkinson’s Disease? Comments on “Olfactory Bulb [Alpha]-Synucleinopathy Has High Specificity and Sensitivity for Lewy Body Disorders”. Acta Neuropathol. 2009, 117, 213–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Brodoehl, S.; Klingner, C.; Volk, G.F.; Bitter, T.; Witte, O.W.; Redecker, C. Decreased Olfactory Bulb Volume in Idiopathic Parkinson’s Disease Detected by 3.0-Tesla Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Mov. Disord. 2012, 27, 1019–1025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Wattendorf, E.; Welge-Lüssen, A.; Fiedler, K.; Bilecen, D.; Wolfensberger, M.; Fuhr, P.; Hummel, T.; Westermann, B. Olfactory Impairment Predicts Brain Atrophy in Parkinson’s Disease. J. Neurosci. 2009, 29, 15410–15413. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Wu, X.; Yu, C.; Fan, F.; Zhang, K.; Zhu, C.; Wu, T.; Li, K.; Chan, P. Correlation between Progressive Changes in Piriform Cortex and Olfactory Performance in Early Parkinson’s Disease. Eur. Neurol. 2011, 66, 98–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  63. Lee, J.E.; Cho, K.H.; Ham, J.H.; Song, S.K.; Sohn, Y.H.; Lee, P.H. Olfactory Performance Acts as a Cognitive Reserve in Non-Demented Patients with Parkinson’s Disease. Park. Relat. Disord. 2014, 20, 186–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Tessitore, A.; Amboni, M.; Esposito, F.; Russo, A.; Picillo, M.; Marcuccio, L.; Pellecchia, M.T.; Vitale, C.; Cirillo, M.; Tedeschi, G.; et al. Resting-State Brain Connectivity in Patients with Parkinson’s Disease and Freezing of Gait. Park. Relat. Disord. 2012, 18, 781–787. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Karunanayaka, P.R.; Wilson, D.A.; Tobia, M.J.; Martinez, B.E.; Meadowcroft, M.D.; Eslinger, P.J.; Yang, Q.X. Default Mode Network Deactivation During Odor–Visual Association. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017, 38, 1125–1139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Doty, R.L. Olfaction in Parkinson’s Disease and Related Disorders. Neurobiol. Dis. 2012, 46, 527–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Barone, D.A.; Henchcliffe, C. Rapid Eye Movement Sleep Behavior Disorder and the Link to Alpha-Synucleinopathies. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2018, 129, 1551–1564. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Kawasaki, I.; Baba, T.; Takeda, A.; Mori, E. Loss of Awareness of Hyposmia Is Associated with Mild Cognitive Impairment in Parkinson’s Disease. Park. Relat. Disord. 2016, 22, 74–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Aarsland, D.; Creese, B.; Politis, M.; Chaudhuri, K.R.; Weintraub, D.; Ballard, C. Cognitive Decline in Parkinson Disease. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2017, 13, 217–231. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. De Cleene, N.; Schwarzová, K.; Labrecque, S.; Cerejo, C.; Djamshidian, A.; Seppi, K.; Heim, B. Olfactory Dysfunction as Potential Biomarker in Neurodegenerative Diseases: A Narrative Review. Front. Neurosci. 2025, 18, 1505029. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Brain regions in the ON in (a) axial slices and (b) in a 3D glass brain. These regions were selected based on published fMRI activation studies [31]. Time courses were extracted in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space [(x y z) coordinates] from these six regions as an average time course within a five voxel radius centered on respective coordinates.
Figure 1. Brain regions in the ON in (a) axial slices and (b) in a 3D glass brain. These regions were selected based on published fMRI activation studies [31]. Time courses were extracted in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space [(x y z) coordinates] from these six regions as an average time course within a five voxel radius centered on respective coordinates.
Life 15 01324 g001
Figure 2. Group differences in UPSIT scores (mean ± SD) between CN, PDT, and PDAR. CN: 36.41 ± 2.15; PDT: 23.58 ± 6.96; PDAR: 19.47 ± 6.65. *, p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Figure 2. Group differences in UPSIT scores (mean ± SD) between CN, PDT, and PDAR. CN: 36.41 ± 2.15; PDT: 23.58 ± 6.96; PDAR: 19.47 ± 6.65. *, p < 0.05; ** p < 0.01.
Life 15 01324 g002
Figure 3. Group differences in ON FC between CN, PDAR, and PDT groups. These brain regions have been implicated in olfactory processing in previous studies [31]. Significant clusters are displayed at p < 0.01, AlphaSim corrected. The heatmap represents t-values reflecting the magnitude of between-group differences in ON FC.
Figure 3. Group differences in ON FC between CN, PDAR, and PDT groups. These brain regions have been implicated in olfactory processing in previous studies [31]. Significant clusters are displayed at p < 0.01, AlphaSim corrected. The heatmap represents t-values reflecting the magnitude of between-group differences in ON FC.
Life 15 01324 g003
Figure 4. ON FC group differences within the core ON. The ON FC to the OFC and insula significantly differs between PDAR and PDT. *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01.
Figure 4. ON FC group differences within the core ON. The ON FC to the OFC and insula significantly differs between PDAR and PDT. *, p < 0.05; **, p < 0.01.
Life 15 01324 g004
Figure 5. ON FC differences within the extended ON. The FC of the left hippocampus and the PCC significantly differs between PDAR and PDT. *, p < 0.05, **: p < 0.01. Cere, Cerebellum; Hipp, Hippocampus.
Figure 5. ON FC differences within the extended ON. The FC of the left hippocampus and the PCC significantly differs between PDAR and PDT. *, p < 0.05, **: p < 0.01. Cere, Cerebellum; Hipp, Hippocampus.
Life 15 01324 g005
Figure 6. The correlations between UPSIT scores and the FC of each brain region within the ON were examined. Among the PD subtypes, PDAR exhibited the lowest ON FC and UPSIT scores. Given that PDAR is associated with a poorer prognosis, these findings suggest that ON FC and UPSIT performance may have potential as indicators of disease severity.
Figure 6. The correlations between UPSIT scores and the FC of each brain region within the ON were examined. Among the PD subtypes, PDAR exhibited the lowest ON FC and UPSIT scores. Given that PDAR is associated with a poorer prognosis, these findings suggest that ON FC and UPSIT performance may have potential as indicators of disease severity.
Life 15 01324 g006
Figure 7. Multivariate ON FC data projected onto the first two principal components. In this reduced plane, a decision boundary can be identified, separating PDAR (17) and PDT (12) at above-chance levels.
Figure 7. Multivariate ON FC data projected onto the first two principal components. In this reduced plane, a decision boundary can be identified, separating PDAR (17) and PDT (12) at above-chance levels.
Life 15 01324 g007
Table 1. Demographic information of the study cohort (mean ± standard deviation). p-values were derived from Fisher’s Exact test for gender. All other comparisons, age, education, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), and Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D Scale), were performed using one-way ANOVA. Statistical significance was evaluated using p < 0.05. Other abbreviations: UPDRS = Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; T/AR = mean tremor/mean akinetic rigidity score; LEDD = Levodopa Equivalent Daily Dose; H&Y stage = Hoehn and Yahr; NA = Not Available, * = p-value < 0.05.
Table 1. Demographic information of the study cohort (mean ± standard deviation). p-values were derived from Fisher’s Exact test for gender. All other comparisons, age, education, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), and Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HAM-D Scale), were performed using one-way ANOVA. Statistical significance was evaluated using p < 0.05. Other abbreviations: UPDRS = Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale; T/AR = mean tremor/mean akinetic rigidity score; LEDD = Levodopa Equivalent Daily Dose; H&Y stage = Hoehn and Yahr; NA = Not Available, * = p-value < 0.05.
CN
(n = 24)
PDAR
(n = 17)
PDT
(n = 15)
p-Value
CN vs. PDARCN vs. PDTPDAR vs. PDT
Age (yrs.)57.8 ± 7.459.1 ± 7.461.7 ± 6.70.8360.2360.569
Gender (M:F)11:139:86:90.8760.4230.724
Education (yrs.)15.8 ± 2.413.9 ± 2.115.2 ± 3.20.0680.7950.329
MMSE29.6 ± 0.929.1 ±1.229.7 ± 0.50.2570.9590.221
HAM-D Scale4.2± 2.77.2 ± 3.36.9 ± 4.00.0003 *0.078 *0.215
Disease duration (yrs.)NA.3.7 ± 4.63.5 ± 3.5NA.NA.0.91
UPDRS III1.2 ± 2.620.8 ± 12.917.1 ± 9.5<0.0001 *<0.0001 *0.48
T/AR NA.0.18 ± 0.222.63 ± 1.77NA.NA.<0.0001 *
H&Y stageNA.1.7 ± 0.61.7 ± 0.6NA.NA.0.06
LEDD (mg/day)NA.366 ± 229354 ± 282NA.NA.0.88
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Peiris, S.; Ekanayake, A.; Lu, J.; Elyan, R.; Geesey, K.; Cottrill, R.; Eslinger, P.; Huang, X.; Karunanayaka, P. Olfactory Network Functional Connectivity as a Marker for Parkinson’s Disease Severity. Life 2025, 15, 1324. https://doi.org/10.3390/life15081324

AMA Style

Peiris S, Ekanayake A, Lu J, Elyan R, Geesey K, Cottrill R, Eslinger P, Huang X, Karunanayaka P. Olfactory Network Functional Connectivity as a Marker for Parkinson’s Disease Severity. Life. 2025; 15(8):1324. https://doi.org/10.3390/life15081324

Chicago/Turabian Style

Peiris, Senal, Anupa Ekanayake, Jiaming Lu, Rommy Elyan, Katie Geesey, Ross Cottrill, Paul Eslinger, Xuemei Huang, and Prasanna Karunanayaka. 2025. "Olfactory Network Functional Connectivity as a Marker for Parkinson’s Disease Severity" Life 15, no. 8: 1324. https://doi.org/10.3390/life15081324

APA Style

Peiris, S., Ekanayake, A., Lu, J., Elyan, R., Geesey, K., Cottrill, R., Eslinger, P., Huang, X., & Karunanayaka, P. (2025). Olfactory Network Functional Connectivity as a Marker for Parkinson’s Disease Severity. Life, 15(8), 1324. https://doi.org/10.3390/life15081324

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop