The Role of Experience and Different Sources of Knowledge in Shaping Flood Risk Awareness
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Heterogeneous Definitions of Experience
1.2. Risk Awareness and Risk Perception
1.3. Information and Knowledge
2. Materials and Methods
3. Results
3.1. Flood Experience
3.2. Sources of Flood Risk Knowledge
3.3. Socio-Economic Variables
4. Discussion
- Inertia: this type includes individuals who lack or have a low degree of both experience and knowledge, who are therefore less familiar or not familiar at all with the hazard, in a state of inertia, passivity;
- Tacit/empirical knowledge: this type includes individuals who only experienced the event but who did not gain any information (or gained very limited information) from other sources of knowledge;
- Theoretical knowledge: this type includes individuals who only gained information from other sources of knowledge but who never experienced the hazard; and,
- Wisdom: this type includes individuals who experienced the hazard and who gained information from various sources of knowledge, who therefore reached wisdom by integrating experience and knowledge.
Supplementary Materials
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Guha-Sapir, D.; Below, R.; Hoyois, P. EM-DAT: The CRED/OFDA International Disaster Database; Catholic University of Leuven: Leuven, Belgium, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Lavell, A.; Oppenheimer, M.; Diop, C.; Hess, J.; Lempert, R.; Li, J.; Muir-Wood, R.; Myeong, S.; Moser, S.; Takeuchi, K.; et al. Climate Change: New Dimensions in Disaster Risk, Exposure, Vulnerability, and Resilience; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2012; Volume 9781107025. [Google Scholar]
- Jongman, B.; Winsemius, H.C.; Aerts, J.C.J.H.; Coughlan de Perez, E.; van Aalst, M.K.; Kron, W.; Ward, P.J. Declining vulnerability to river floods and the global benefits of adaptation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2015, 112, E2271–E2280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Brandimarte, L.; Brath, A.; Castellarin, A.; Baldassarre, G. Di Isla Hispaniola: A trans-boundary flood risk mitigation plan. Phys. Chem. Earth 2009, 34, 209–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dawson, R.J.; Ball, T.; Werritty, J.; Werritty, A.; Hall, J.W.; Roche, N. Assessing the effectiveness of non-structural flood management measures in the Thames Estuary under conditions of socio-economic and environmental change. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2011, 21, 628–646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vis, M.; Klijn, F.; Bruijn, K.M.D.; Buuren, M. Van Resilience strategies for flood risk management in the netherlands. Int. J. River Basin Manag. 2003, 1, 33–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kreibich, H.; Di Baldassarre, G.; Vorogushyn, S.; Aerts, J.C.J.H.; Apel, H.; Aronica, G.T.; Arnbjerg-Nielsen, K.; Bouwer, L.M.; Bubeck, P.; Caloiero, T.; et al. Adaptation to flood risk: Results of international paired flood event studies. Earth’s Future 2017, 5, 953–965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Buchecker, M.; Salvini, G.; Di Baldassarre, G.; Semenzin, E.; Maidl, E.; Marcomini, A. The role of risk perception in making flood risk management more effective. Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. 2013, 13, 3013–3030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wachinger, G.; Renn, O.; Begg, C.; Kuhlicke, C. The risk perception paradox-implications for governance and communication of natural hazards. Risk Anal. 2013, 33, 1049–1065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Botzen, W.J.W.; Aerts, J.C.J.H.; Van Den Bergh, J.C.J.M. Dependence of flood risk perceptions on socioeconomic and objective risk factors. Water Resour. Res. 2009, 45, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Fielding, J.L. Inequalities in exposure and awareness of flood risk in England and Wales. Disasters 2012, 36, 477–494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Grothmann, T.; Patt, A. Adaptive capacity and human cognition: The process of individual adaptation to climate change. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2005, 15, 199–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Miceli, R.; Sotgiu, I.; Settanni, M. Disaster preparedness and perception of flood risk: A study in an alpine valley in Italy. J. Environ. Psychol. 2008, 28, 164–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mileti, D.S.; O’Brien, P. Public response to aftershock warnings. In US Geological Survey Professional Paper; United States Government Publishing Office: Washington, DC, USA, 1993. [Google Scholar]
- Whitmarsh, L. Are flood victims more concerned about climate change than other people? The role of direct experience in risk perception and behavioural response. J. Risk Res. 2008, 11, 351–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Comănescu, L.; Nedelea, A. Floods and Public Perception on their Effect. Case Study: Tecuci Plain (Romania), Year 2013. Procedia Environ. Sci. 2016, 32, 190–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bradford, R.A.; O’Sullivan, J.J.; Van Der Craats, I.M.; Krywkow, J.; Rotko, P.; Aaltonen, J.; Bonaiuto, M.; De Dominicis, S.; Waylen, K.; Schelfaut, K. Risk perception—Issues for flood management in Europe. Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. 2012, 12, 2299–2309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Green, C.H.; Tunstall, S.M.; Fordham, M.H. The risks from flooding: Which risks and whose perception? Disasters 1991, 15, 227–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lawrence, J.; Quade, D.; Becker, J. Integrating the effects of flood experience on risk perception with responses to changing climate risk. Nat. Hazards 2014, 74, 1773–1794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Qasim, S.; Nawaz Khan, A.; Prasad Shrestha, R.; Qasim, M. Risk perception of the people in the flood prone Khyber Pukhthunkhwa province of Pakistan. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2015, 14, 373–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wachinger, G.; Renn, O. Risk Perception and Natural Hazards; CapHaz-Net WP3 Report; DIALOGIK Non-Profit Institute for Communication and Cooperative Research: Stuttgart, Germany, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- Bustillos Ardaya, A.; Evers, M.; Ribbe, L. What influences disaster risk perception? Intervention measures, flood and landslide risk perception of the population living in flood risk areas in Rio de Janeiro state, Brazil. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2017, 25, 227–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ho, M.-C.; Shaw, D.; Lin, S.; Chiu, Y.-C. How Do Disaster Characteristics Influence Risk Perception? Risk Anal. 2008, 28, 635–643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Halpern-Felsher, B.L.; Millstein, S.G.; Ellen, J.M.; Adler, N.E.; Tschann, J.M.; Biehl, M. The role of behavioral experience in judging risks. Health Psychol. 2001, 20, 120–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burningham, K.; Fielding, J.; Thrush, D. “It’ll never happen to me”: Understanding public awareness of local flood risk. Disasters 2008, 32, 216–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Knuth, D.; Kehl, D.; Hulse, L.; Schmidt, S. Risk Perception, Experience, and Objective Risk: A Cross-National Study with European Emergency Survivors. Risk Anal. 2014, 34, 1286–1298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Scolobig, A.; De Marchi, B.; Borga, M. The missing link between flood risk awareness and preparedness: Findings from case studies in an Alpine Region. Nat. Hazards 2012, 63, 499–520. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Siegrist, M.; Gutscher, H.; Earle, T.C. Perception of risk: The influence of general trust, and general confidence. J. Risk Res. 2005, 8, 145–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Santoro, S.; Pluchinotta, I.; Pagano, A.; Pengal, P.; Cokan, B.; Giordano, R. Assessing stakeholders’ risk perception to promote Nature Based Solutions as flood protection strategies: The case of the Glinščica river (Slovenia). Sci. Total Environ. 2019, 655, 188–201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bera, M.K.; Daněk, P. The perception of risk in the flood-prone area: A case study from the Czech municipality. Disaster Prev. Manag. Int. J. 2018, 27, 2–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thistlethwaite, J.; Henstra, D.; Brown, C.; Scott, D. How Flood Experience and Risk Perception Influences Protective Actions and Behaviours among Canadian Homeowners. Environ. Manag. 2018, 61, 197–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Becker, G.; Aerts, J.C.J.H.; Huitema, D. Influence of flood risk perception and other factors on risk-reducing behaviour: A survey of municipalities along the Rhine. J. Flood Risk Manag. 2014, 7, 16–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steinführer, A.; Kuhlicke, C. Social Vulnerability and the 2002 Flood. County Report Germany (Mulde River); Helmholz Unweltforschungszentrum (UFZ): Leipzig, Germany, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- De Marchi, B.; Scolobig, A.; Delli Zotti, G.; Del Zotto, M. Risk Construction and Social Vulnerability in an Italian Alpine Region; Report T11-07-12 of FLOODsite Integrated Project; European Commission: Brussels, Belgium, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Grothmann, T.; Reusswig, F. People at risk of flooding: Why some residents take precautionary action while others do not. Nat. Hazards 2006, 38, 101–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slovic, P. Perception of Risk. Adv. Sci. 1987, 236, 280–285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sibthorpe, B. The Social Construction of Sexual Relationships as a Determinant of HIV Risk Perception and Condom Use among Injection Drug Users. Med. Anthropol. Q. 1992, 6, 255–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rothman, S.; Lichter, R. Elite Ideology and Risk Perception in Nuclear Energy Policy. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 1987, 81, 383–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dretske, F. Conscious Experience. Mind Assoc. 1993, 102, 263–283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cattaneo, Z.; Silvanto, J. Mental Imagery: Visual Cognition. In International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Wrigth, J.D., Ed.; Elsevier: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2015; Volume 15, pp. 220–227. ISBN 9780080970875. [Google Scholar]
- Gallese, V. The manifold nature of interpersonal relations: The quest for a common mechanism. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2003, 358, 517–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Park, C.W.; Gardner, M.P.; Thukral, V.K. Self-Perceived Knowledge: Some Effects on Information Processing for a Choice Task. Am. J. Psychol. 1988, 101, 401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stoutenborough, J.W.; Vedlitz, A. The effect of perceived and assessed knowledge of climate change on public policy concerns: An empirical comparison. Environ. Sci. Policy 2014, 37, 23–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Meeds, R. Cognitive and attitudinal effects of technical advertising copy: The roles of gender, self-assessed and objective consumer knowledge. Int. J. Advert. 2004, 23, 309–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fischhoff, B.; Slovic, P.; Lichtenstein, S.; Read, S.; Combs, B. How safe is safe enough? A psychometric study of attitudes towards technological risks and benefits. Policy Sci. 1978, 9, 127–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slovic, P. Trust, emotion, sex, politics and science: Surveying the risk-assessment battlefield. Percept. Risk 1999, 19, 390–412. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kellens, W.; Terpstra, T.; De Maeyer, P. Perception and Communication of Flood Risks: A Systematic Review of Empirical Research. Risk Anal. 2013, 33, 24–49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Weichselgartner, J.; Pigeon, P. The Role of Knowledge in Disaster Risk Reduction. Int. J. Disaster Risk Sci. 2015, 6, 107–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Centro Funzionale Decentrato. Bollettino di Nowcasting; Verona. Regione del Veneto: Venice, Italy, 2018. Available online: https://www.regione.veneto.it/c/document_library/get_file?uuid=5f95c794-6a46-4893-9341-0b74db6edc83&groupId=90748 (accessed on 7 July 2020).
- Borga, M. Piena improvvisa del 1 settembre 2018 sul Progno di Novare—Valutazione del carattere di eccezionalità della precipitazione innescante; University of Padua: Padua, Italy, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Roder, G.; Hudson, P.; Tarolli, P. Flood risk perceptions and the willingness to pay for flood insurance in the Veneto region of Italy. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2019, 37, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sofia, G.; Roder, G.; Dalla Fontana, G.; Tarolli, P. Flood dynamics in urbanised landscapes: 100 years of climate and humans’ interaction. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 1–12. [Google Scholar]
- Amponsah, W.; Ayral, P.A.; Boudevillain, B.; Bouvier, C.; Braud, I.; Brunet, P.; Delrieu, G.; DIdon-Lescot, J.F.; Gaume, E.; Lebouc, L.; et al. Integrated high-resolution dataset of high-intensity European and Mediterranean flash floods. Earth Syst. Sci. Data 2018, 10, 1783–1794. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Borga, M.; Comiti, F.; Ruin, I.; Marra, F. Forensic analysis of flash flood response. Wiley Interdiscip. Rev. Water 2019, 6, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stockemer, D. Quantitative Methods for the Social Sciences—A Practical Introduction with Examples in SPSS and Stata, I ed.; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Weyrich, P.; Mondino, E.; Borga, M.; Di Baldassarre, G.; Patt, A.; Scolobig, A. A flood-risk-oriented, dynamic protection motivation framework to explain risk reduction behaviours. Nat. Hazards Earth Syst. Sci. 2020, 20, 287–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Christensen, R.H.B.C. Ordinal: Regression Models for Ordinal Data. R package version 2019.12-10. 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Liu, D.; Li, Y.; Shen, X.; Xie, Y.; Zhang, Y. Flood risk perception of rural households in western mountainous regions of Henan Province, China. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2018, 27, 155–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Vries, D.H. Temporal Vulnerability and the Post-Disaster ‘Window of Opportunity to Woo’: A Case Study of an African-American Floodplain Neighborhood after Hurricane Floyd in North Carolina. Hum. Ecol. 2017, 45, 437–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Aristotle Book VI. In Nichomachaean Ethics; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2020.
- Polanyi, M. The Tacit Dimension; Doubleday: Chicago, IL, USA, 1966. [Google Scholar]
Variable name | Question | Answer Options * |
---|---|---|
General feeling of safety | In general, how safe do you feel living in this area? | On a scale from 1, “Not at all safe” to 5, “Very safe”, or 0, “I don’t know” |
Experience | ||
Presence during the 2018 event | Were you present during the 2018 event? | 1. Yes; 2. No |
Previous flood experience | Were you ever involved in a similar event in the past, here or elsewhere? | 1. Yes; 2. No |
Damage severity | Can you evaluate the severity of the damage suffered by yourself or your house? | On a scale from 1, “No damage” to 5, “Serious damage”, or 0, “I don’t know” |
Length of residence | For how long have you been living here? | 1. Since birth; 2. Since ____(year) |
Knowledge | ||
Knowledge from direct experience | How much did direct experience with the event contribute to your knowledge of floods? | On a scale from 1, “No contribution” to 5, “Great contribution”, or 0, “I don’t know” |
Information passed on by others | How much did information passed on by others (parents, relatives, friends, etc.) contribute to your knowledge of floods? | On a scale from 1, “No contribution” to 5, “Great contribution”, or 0, “I don’t know” |
Information from official sources | How much did official information contribute to your knowledge of floods? | On a scale from 1, “No contribution” to 5, “Great contribution”, or 0, “I don’t know” |
Personal research of information | How much did your personal research for information contribute to your knowledge of floods? | On a scale from 1, “No contribution” to 5, “Great contribution”, or 0, “I don’t know” |
Threat appraisal before the event | Before the 2018 event, did you think something like this could occur here? | 1. Yes; 2. No; 0. I don’t know |
Variable name | Question | Available Answers * |
---|---|---|
Perceived threat to self | Considering floods, to what extent do you think they represent a threat to yourself personally? | On a scale from 1, “Not at all a threat” to 5, “Serious threat”, or 0, “I don’t know” |
Perceived threat to home | Considering floods, to what extent do you think they represent a threat to your home? | On a scale from 1, “Not at all a threat” to 5, “Serious threat”, or 0, “I don’t know” |
Perceived threat to town as a whole | Considering floods, to what extent do you think they represent a threat to the town as a whole? | On a scale from 1, “Not at all a threat” to 5, “Serious threat”, or 0, “I don’t know” |
Perceived threat from other types of hazard ** | Considering (hazard), to what extent do you think it represents a threat to the town as a whole? | On a scale from 1, “Not at all a threat” to 5, “Serious threat”, or 0, “I don’t know” |
Expected future damage | How much damage do you think a potential future flood could cause to your home? | On a scale from 1, “No damage” to 5, “Serious damage”, or 0, “I don’t know” |
Independent variables | Perceived Threat | Expected Future Damage | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Self | Home | Town | ||
General feeling of safety (–) | 1.79 ** (1.29–2.50) | 1.94 ** (1.38–2.76) | 1.56 (1.11–2.20) | 1.73 ** (1.24–2.42) |
Experience | ||||
Presence during 2018 event | 3.03 * (1.47–6.72) | 0.60 (0.28–1.25) | 0.81 (0.38–1.74) | 1.58 (0.73–3.44) |
Damage severity in 2018 | 1.2 (0.98–1.48) | 2.45 *** (1.87–3.27) | 1.22 (0.98–1.52) | 2.70 *** (2.07–3.60) |
Previous experience | 0.99 (0.50–1.97) | 0.68 (0.34–1.35) | 0.64 (0.31–1.31) | 1.62 (0.78–3.37) |
Length of residence | 1.37 (1.02–1.83) | 1.46 (1.08–2.02) | 1.38 (1.01–1.94) | 1.00 (0.99–1.02) |
Knowledge | ||||
From direct experience | 1.40 * (1.11–1.76) | 1.41 ** (1.14–1.77) | 1.45 ** (1.17–1.80) | 1.39 * (1.13–1.72) |
Passed on by others | 1.36 * (1.12–1.66) | 1.18 (0.98–1.44) | 1.21 (0.99–1.49) | 1.04 (0.86–1.26) |
From official information | 0.99 (0.77–1.27) | 0.98 (0.75–1.25) | 1.05 (0.81–1.35) | 0.90 (0.70–1.16) |
From personal research | 1.10 (0.90–1.33) | 1.11 (0.91–1.35) | 1.10 (0.90–1.39) | 1.07 (0.88–1.30) |
Threat appraisal before the event | 2.99 ** (1.51–6.02) | 0.65 (0.33–1.30) | 0.78 (0.39–1.55) | 1.07 (0.54–2.11) |
Socio-economic | ||||
Gender | 1.85 (1.03–3.35) | 1.69 (0.95–3.04) | 2.61 * (1.43–4.85) | 2.44 * (1.35–4.46) |
Education (–) | 1.30 (0.99–1.71) | 1.06 (0.82–1.38) | 1.23 (0.92–1.62) | 1.04 (0.80–1.34) |
Income (–) | 1.44 (1.10–1.89) | 1.13 (0.87–1.47) | 1.65 ** (1.23–2.22) | 1.07 (0.83–1.40) |
Independent Variables | Knowledge From Direct Experience |
---|---|
Damage severity in 2018 | 1.59 (1.24–2.08) *** |
Presence during 2018 event | 1.35 (0.62–2.95) ** |
Previous experience | 2.60 (1.24–5.77) * |
Length of residence | 1.00 (0.98–1.02) ** |
© 2020 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Mondino, E.; Scolobig, A.; Borga, M.; Di Baldassarre, G. The Role of Experience and Different Sources of Knowledge in Shaping Flood Risk Awareness. Water 2020, 12, 2130. https://doi.org/10.3390/w12082130
Mondino E, Scolobig A, Borga M, Di Baldassarre G. The Role of Experience and Different Sources of Knowledge in Shaping Flood Risk Awareness. Water. 2020; 12(8):2130. https://doi.org/10.3390/w12082130
Chicago/Turabian StyleMondino, Elena, Anna Scolobig, Marco Borga, and Giuliano Di Baldassarre. 2020. "The Role of Experience and Different Sources of Knowledge in Shaping Flood Risk Awareness" Water 12, no. 8: 2130. https://doi.org/10.3390/w12082130
APA StyleMondino, E., Scolobig, A., Borga, M., & Di Baldassarre, G. (2020). The Role of Experience and Different Sources of Knowledge in Shaping Flood Risk Awareness. Water, 12(8), 2130. https://doi.org/10.3390/w12082130