Next Article in Journal
Visceral Fat, Metabolic Health, and Lifestyle Factors in Obstructive Bronchial Diseases: Insights from Bioelectrical Impedance Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Body Composition and Its Outcomes and Management in Multiple Sclerosis: Narrative Review
Previous Article in Special Issue
Beyond Borders: Investigating the Impact of COVID-19 Anxiety and Eating Attitudes on Psychological Well-Being and Physical Activity Objectives in Poland and China
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Relationships Between Body Image and Body Parameters in Men Under Long-Term Fasting Conditions

1
Institute of Health Sciences, University of Opole, 45-061 Opole, Poland
2
Collegium Medicum, Jan Dlugosz University in Czestochowa, 42-200 Czestochowa, Poland
3
Department of Economy in Opole, WSB Merito University in Wroclaw, 53-609 Wrocław, Poland
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2025, 17(6), 1023; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17061023
Submission received: 18 February 2025 / Revised: 11 March 2025 / Accepted: 12 March 2025 / Published: 14 March 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Train, Eat and Think with Scientific Precision)

Abstract

:
Background/Objectives: Fasting and physical exertion positively affect body mass and composition. This study aimed to examine the effects of physical changes induced by eight days of water-only fasting and a physical exertion test and their influence on body satisfaction and body image parameters in middle-aged men. Methods: Fifteen participants were assessed for physical (height, body mass, and body composition) and psychological (body satisfaction, evaluation of body parts, and body image determinants) aspects before and after the fasting intervention, both at rest and post-exercise. Correlation and concordance coefficients were calculated for the analyzed variables. Results: It was found that the fasting intervention led to a reduction in body mass and a favorable shift in body composition, while also increasing satisfaction with one’s body and its specific parts. This positively impacted volunteers’ self-assessment of their health status. Selected body image parameters remained at an average level for the male population and did not change following the fasting. Attitudes toward body weight control methods and their perceived effectiveness did not change either. Participants favored reducing food intake and eliminating high-calorie products from their diet, rejecting the use pathological methods typically associated with eating disorders. A few associations between physical and psychological aspects of corporeality were observed, whereas significant correlations between satisfaction with body parts and parameters and a positive body image were confirmed. Conclusions: The above suggest that physical conditions and their changes resulting from fasting and physical exertion lead to a positive influence on body satisfaction and its components but do not alter body image.

1. Introduction

Body image is a multidimensional structure that can be defined [1,2] as an attitude toward the body that involves emotional, cognitive, and behavioral components, with an emphasis on its external appearance [3]. It can also be considered as the mental image of one’s body, which is formed as a result of an individual’s experiences and interactions with one’s social environment [4]. Therefore, body image can be viewed as a broad category encompassing various aspects of physical appearance, such as satisfaction with one’s body, evaluation of physical attractiveness, and/or body image distortion [5]. As an important structure of the Self, it remains a relatively permanent construct that can be comparatively independent of objective changes in body appearance—both caused by weight gain or loss and those resulting from aging. Given that body image has both state and trait properties, it can exhibit constancy, but it can also change under different circumstances over relatively short periods of time [6].
To reconcile the different ways of understanding, body image can be defined as a system of beliefs and self-evaluations about one’s appearance (cognitive aspect), shaped by internalized attractiveness norms and social information (social aspect) and accompanied by certain emotions (affective aspect) and behaviors (behavioral aspect) [7].

1.1. Specificity of Body Image in Men

The factors that are significant in creating body image can be divided into physical, interpersonal, and cultural factors. Physical factors are reflected in personal opinions and feelings about body shape and weight. Interpersonal factors relate to the opinions of those around, especially close ones, and is derived from personal observations and experiences. Cultural factors are linked to ideals of physical attractiveness in specific communities. For instance, in Western culture, slim and muscular figures are promoted as the most desirable, while also conducive to health [8,9]. As a result, depending on the accumulated experience, it is possible to produce a positive or negative form of it. Negative body image is influenced by negative mood and lack of self-confidence [3]. Self-esteem and subjective perceptions of health have been identified as psychological factors influencing body evaluation [10]. The way of building this structure of the Self proceeds differently in men and women. Women are more susceptible to the factors that shape a negative body image, and they remain particularly sensitive to the critical opinions of those around them about their appearance. However, men also respond with a decrease in satisfaction with their body image in response to unfavorable comments from those around them or comparisons with beauty ideals. An important mediating variable between criticism and appearance dissatisfaction is low global self-esteem, which determines how critical messages are interpreted [11,12]. Differences regarding men and women’s body image are seen in the importance of being thin and the level of acceptance of one’s body—both variables are more important for women [13]. In contrast, for male body image, tall height and a muscular physique play an important role. The bodily self also does not play as important a role in the structure of the Self in men as it does in women. Men are less likely than women to form critical opinions about their own bodies and experience fewer negative emotions about their own appearances [11]. They also do not try as hard to correct beauty deficiencies and shortcomings and internalize the prevailing beauty standards to a lesser extent [12].

1.2. Body Image and the Use of Fasting

One of the biggest problems facing rich societies today is obesity. The number of overweight people is steadily increasing, contributing to the emergence of psychological, social, and medical problems [14]. People who try unsuccessfully to reduce their body weight often opt for more radical measures. Intermittent fasting is one such weight-loss strategy that can have image and health benefits [15,16,17]. Successful weight reduction can affect body image [11]. On the other hand, erroneous self-perceptions of body weight can limit or nullify the effectiveness of efforts to prevent or reduce obesity [13,17]. Inadequate body image and its role in sustaining well-being are in many cases inextricably linked to obesity [18]. Short-term fasting can exacerbate negative emotions such as anxiety, irritability, or fatigue, and men who highly value the muscularity of the body may feel weaker and less muscular after fasting [19]. For some people, there are positive consequences of fasting—improved mood and an enhanced sense of control [20]. Fasting improves physical, mental, and social quality of life; increases overall vitality; and reduces feelings of fatigue [21] Some studies do not confirm differences between fasting and non-fasting days or between fasting and non-fasting people in terms of experiencing positive and negative emotions [22,23]. Above all, however, intermittent fasting, as an effective strategy for short-term weight loss, contributes to increased satisfaction with body shape and facial appearance and increases self-esteem in terms of the bodily Self [10]. Previous scientific publications confirm the effects of short-term periods of fasting on body image, but there are no reports of studies exploring the importance of long-term fasting.

1.3. Somatic Body Image

The somatic image of a person’s body is determined by his height (BH), the size of his mass (BM), fat-free mass (FFM), fat content (BF), total body water (TBW), the sizes of his individual circumferences, and the proportions of the various parts expressed by various weight and height indices. The most popular of these is the body mass index (BMI). The cited data represent the attractiveness of the human body, its degree of self-acceptance, or social acceptance. In addition, this image is supplemented by body posture, the way of moving, facial appearance, and other features. In modern social perception, a slim figure is more accepted and is synonymous with success in life.
The body of an adult men differs significantly from that of a female and is characterized by, among other things, a larger BH, BM, FFM, and TBW and a smaller BF value. In addition, the individual body circumferences of men are larger (excluding relative hip circumference) than those of women. In addition, the male posture is more characterized by strength and decisiveness, and the psychological profile is more firm and rational than that of women.
Body weight can increase significantly or undergo a significant reduction, where its composition changes fundamentally. Increased body weight expressed by a BMI of more than 25 kg/m2 is referred to as overweight or obesity and can, in extreme cases, reach up to several hundred kilograms. This is the result of a positive energy balance [24] and/or pathological conditions [25] as well as excessive accumulation of adipose tissue. Such a picture of the human body is not universally accepted. In people who participate in strength sports, body weight is often elevated, and BMI can significantly exceed the limit of 25 kg/m2, which is due to increased muscle mass without increased fat. In some members of the public, this finds acceptance or even arouses admiration. At the same time, in the average person, excessive body weight is considered a cause of many diseases; therefore, it is recommended to reduce it.
Weight reduction and a change in body composition can be achieved by eating a different type of diet [26,27,28]. This includes limiting sugars [29] and/or fats [30], changing the frequency of food intake [31], using dietary supplements [32], drinking optimal amounts of water [33], or opting for bariatric surgery [34]. In extreme cases, weight reduction is accomplished using appetite suppressants [35] and laxatives [36] or provoking vomiting [37]. Professional athletes use other, specific methods of reducing body weight [36]. An important way to reduce body weight and changes in body composition is the use of long-term fasting. In our earlier study, 8 days of water-only fasting resulted in reductions of BM by 6.92%, BF by 12.49%, FFM by 5.80%, TBW by 5.64%, and BMI by 7.01% [38]. More drastic changes were registered in a 34-year-old obese man who underwent a 50-day fasting. His body weight during this time was reduced from 96.8 kg to 76.4 kg (21.07%), and his BMI decreased from 30.2 kg/m2 to 23.5 kg/m2 (22.19%), depicting a normal body composition [39]. A classic experiment conducted in the state of Minnesota (USA) also showed that a person’s malnourished state can affect his cognitive and social functions in addition to somatic changes [40].
The prevalence of overweight and obesity among highly developed societies forces people to reduce excessive body weight, and the use of long-term fasting for this purpose is hardly tolerated by them. Therefore, attempts are being made to reduce body weight by practicing less restrictive forms of fasting [41,42,43,44].
An important role in the reduction of body weight and positive modification of its composition is played by increased physical activity used mainly in the form of endurance and strength training, both by healthy and sick people [45]. Combining exercise and dietary modifications can, in addition to other health-promoting aspects, enhance weight reduction and modification of body composition [46,47,48].
We decided to use an 8-day fasting because different populations of people need to quickly reduce body weight, e.g., some groups of athletes or business people, and such an intervention is effective in this respect. Fasting of this duration is not harmful to health and is relatively easily tolerated. However, it should be noted that people undergoing fasting intervention should rest and drink plenty of water to minimize the degree of dehydration, which would have a negative impact on the appearance of the body.
It should be mentioned that significant weight reduction and change in body composition occurs under conditions such as malnutrition, cachexia, metabolic diseases, mental anorexia, mental bulimia, or many other conditions [40,49]. The extreme low BMI in one of these conditions was 12 kg/m2 [50].
With the above data in mind, in the present study, an attempt was made to evaluate selected components of corporeality based on physical and psychological aspects—body image and factors determining it in middle-aged men participating in 8 days of water-only fasting and additionally loaded with maximum intensity exercise.
The following research hypotheses were adopted:
(1)
The consequences of long-term fasting will be somatic changes that include the following:
  • weight loss;
  • decrease in BMI;
  • decrease in the proportion of fat in total body weight.
(2)
Long-term fasting will be followed by positive changes in the cognitive, emotional, behavioral, and social components of body image.
(3)
Positive changes in the physical aspects of corporeality will positively correlate with a positive body image in psychological terms.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Eighteen candidates were enrolled in the study, including 16 men and two women. During the study, one man and two women chose not to continue fasting.
Eligible for the study were subjects who had previously participated in fasting sessions of at least 7 days’ duration, during which they consumed only mineral water. The subjects were in good psychophysical condition before and during the fasting intervention and tolerated the lack of food intake well. The subjects thought positively and were characterized by strong motivation to participate in the next fasting session in their lives. They expected this fasting intervention to improve their health, just as they had done previously. The volunteers remained in their place of residence during the 8-day of water-only fasting and fulfilled their daily work and family obligations. The group consisted of government officials, businessmen, and academics and passed medical examinations. Three days before the start of the study, during the study, and 14 days after the end of the experiment, the volunteers had telephone contact with the study organizer, doctor, and psychologist. During these periods, no cognitive, emotional, behavioral, or somatic disorders were found in the subjects. Volunteers were familiarized with the methodology and conditions of the experiment. They were also reminded of the safety conditions during and after the fasting intervention. Participation in the study was determined by volunteers meeting the following criteria: (1) previous experience in conducting fasting practices; (2) no use of restrictive diets within the past 6 months; (3) no use of cigarettes, alcohol, or drugs; (4) no acute or chronic mental or somatic illness of any kind; and (5) the absence of eating disorders and mental dysfunctions. The participants were also informed of the possible negative health effects of the experiment and especially the fasting intervention, after which the men gave their written consent to participate in this study.
The research protocol was also approved by the Research Ethics Committee of the Jan Długosz University in Częstochowa (Poland; KE-0/1/2019; 5 March 2019) and was conducted in accordance with the requirements of the Declaration of Helsinki.

2.2. Procedure

Male volunteers reported to the laboratory in the morning in the fasting state. At the beginning, the examined men, under the supervision of a psychologist, completed the following tests: the Body Parts and Parameters Satisfaction Scale (BPPSS), the Scale of Weight Control Methods SWCM, the Weight Control Effectiveness Rating Scale (WCERS), and the Body Image Questionnaire (BIQ), all of which are methods reported by A. Glebocka. In addition, the study used the Body Esteem Scale (BES) by S. Franzoi and S. Shields in the form of the Polish adaptation by M. Lipowska and M. Lipowski; the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) in the form of the Polish adaptation by M. Laguna, K. Lachowicz-Tabaczek, and I. Dzwonkowska; and the Lying Scale (LS) from H. Eysenck’s EPQ-R test in the form of the Polish adaptation by P. Brzozowski and R. Drwal [12,51,52].
In the next step, venous blood was collected from the volunteers for determination of β-hydroxybutyrate (β-HB) and cortisol (C) levels. Then, anthropometric and somatic measurements were taken, and the volunteers were interviewed. In the next stage, the men performed an exercise test on a bicycle ergometer with gradually increasing intensity up to maximum power. The body load of the exercise was recorded based on heart rate. At 3 min after the end of the exercise, venous blood was again drawn, and body weight and body composition were again measured. After these activities, fasting was continued for 8 days, during which only arbitrary amounts of moderately mineralized water were consumed.
After the fasting ended, all measurements taken before its start were repeated.

2.3. Measurements

2.3.1. Demographic and Lifestyle Measurements

Finally, 15 men with an average age of 59.8 ± 12.37 years were included in the analyses. Detailed somatic data along with age, diet presentation, and social conditions (obtained during the interview) of the studied men are presented in Table 1.

2.3.2. Anthropometric and Somatic Measurements

Body height (BH) was measured with an anthropometer (A-226, Tristom, Olomouc, Czech Republic). Body weight (BM) and its components, i.e., fat content (BF), fat-free mass (FFM), total water content (TBW), and body mass index (BMI), were determined using a Tanita TBF 300A body composition analyzer (Amsterdam, The Netherlands).

2.3.3. Analysis of Blood Samples

Blood collected from the elbow vein was initially stored at room temperature for no longer than 10 min. In the next step, the blood was centrifuged at 1500× g for 15 min, and the obtained serum samples were frozen at −80 °C and stored for further analysis. MAGLUMI (Cortisol-CLIA, Shenzhen New Industries Biomedical Engineering Co., Ltd., Shenzhen, China) assays were used to determine serum cortisol (C) concentrations; reference values for this variable are 52–350 ng/mL. The concentration of β-hydroxybutyrate (β-HB) was determined using the RANBUD diagnostic kit from Randox, Laboratories Ltd., Crumling, UK. Reference values for serum are—0.03–0.3 mmol/L.

2.3.4. Assessment of Psychological Variables

Dimensions related to body image were examined using the following: (a) The Body Parts and Parameters Satisfaction Scale (BPPSS) by A. Głębocka measures on a nine-point Likert scale (1 means very dissatisfied, 9 means very satisfied) satisfaction with nine body-related dimensions, including seven body parts and weight and height (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.86). (b) The Scale of Weight Control Methods (SWCM) by A. Głębocka refers to various weight control methods, such as eliminating sugar/sweets from the diet or taking an appetite suppressant, rated by respondents on a nine-point scale with 1 indicating never used and 9 indicating use regularly (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.81). (c) The Weight Control Effectiveness Rating Scale (WCERS) by A. Głębocka is used by respondents to rate the effectiveness of each method (1 means totally ineffective, 9 means very effective) (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.81). (d) The Body Image Questionnaire (BIQ) consists of 40 statements rated by respondents on a five-point scale, where 1 means completely disagree and 5 means completely agree. The questionnaire contains four subscales: (i) the cognition-emotions subscale is used to measure opinions about one’s own appearance, such as I feel pressure from my immediate environment to lose weight, and the affective component, such as feelings (positive/negative) about one’s body, attitude toward one’s appearance, general mood, and feelings of guilt or anxiety, e.g., I am ashamed that I look like this, or I am worried about my physical appearance (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.93); (ii) the behavior subscale contains statements that relate to a healthy lifestyle such as I like to relax actively (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.83); (iii) the external criticism subscale of the social environment allows us to determine the subjective level of acceptance of the respondent by the environment, which is expressed in critical remarks about his or her appearance (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.76); and (iv) the pretty-ugly stereotype subscale, measures the degree of internalization of contemporary beauty standards, e.g., a slim body is preferred in our culture, and awareness of negative stereotypes of obese people, e.g., obese people have trouble finding a partner (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.88). The higher the score on the scale is, the more negative the body image. The method underwent a validation procedure, which showed its high reliability (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.93) and accuracy [12]. (e) The Body Esteem Scale (BES) by S. Franzoi and S. Shields in the form of the Polish adaptation by M. Lipowska and M. Lipowski examines people’s feelings toward their own corporeality. In the version for men, the scale has 3 subscales: Physical Attractiveness (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.85), Body Strength (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.85), and Physical Conditioning (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.88). The higher the score on the scale is, the more positive feelings toward one’s own body the respondent has [53]. (f) The Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) in the Polish adaptation by M. Laguna, K. Lachowicz-Tabaczek, and I. Dzwonkowska measures global self-esteem treated as a fixed trait (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.81) [51]. The EPQ-R Lie Scale by H. Eysenck in the form of the Polish adaptation by P. Brzozowski and R. Drwal measures the need for social approval and, thus, the tendency to present oneself in a more favorable light (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.78) [52].

2.4. Statistical Analyses

The data obtained in the study were presented as arithmetic means (Ms) and standard deviations (±SDs). Normality was measured using the Shapiro–Wilk test. Differences between pre- and post-fasting measurements were analyzed using the t-test for paired data and the Wilcoxon paired t-test. Cohen’s d was used to calculate effect size. Standardized effects were defined as small (>0.2), moderate (>0.5), and large (0.8). Differences between dependent variables in the first measurement (before fasting) and variables in the second measurement (after fasting) were examined using the Friedman ANOVA test and Kendall’s concordance coefficient. To determine the effect size in this procedure, partial eta-squared (ηp2) was calculated. The effect size was defined as follows: η2 = 0.01—small (S); η2 = 0.06—medium (M); and η2 = 0.014—large (L).
In addition, Spearman’s rank order correlation analyses were performed to determine the relationships between body image indices and physiological factors. Values at p < 0.05 were considered statistically significant. Calculations were made using Statistica, version 13.

3. Results

The study conducted focused on two areas related to human corporeality—the physical and mental spheres. Of interest were not only the changes in these spheres caused by fasting but also the interrelationships between them.

3.1. Comparisons of Pre- and Post-Fasting Measurements

3.1.1. Comparisons of Physiological Indicators

BM and its components described by absolute values were significantly reduced by the fasting intervention. In contrast, serum β-HB and C concentrations significantly increased (p < 0.001) both at rest and after exercise during 8 days of water-only fasting. As a percentage of BM, its components, i.e., BF (at rest only), FFM, and TBW, did not change after the fasting intervention. The exercise test used did not change BM and its components or serum β-HB and C concentrations (Table 2). During the study’s exercise test, the men achieved a similar maximum heart rate of 162.2 ± 8.24 bpm before the fasting intervention and 161.8 ± 9.17 bpm after.
The respective values of η2 and test power for physiological variables were as follows: BM [kg]—0.076, 0.590; BF [%]—0.004, 0.079; BF [kg]—0.013, 0.134; FFM [%]—0.005, 0.083; FFM [kg]—0.093, 0.663; TBW [%]—0.004, 0.076; TBW [kg]—0.097, 0.691; BMI [kg/m2]—0.060, 0.474; β-HB [mmol/L]—0.787, 1.000; and C [ng/mL]—0.466, 0.999.

3.1.2. Comparisons of Body Image Dimensions

Presentations of dimensions related to psychological aspects of corporeality begin with a summary of data on current and desired body weight. In Table 3, the data show that the weight of the subjects decreased statistically significantly, but the preference for the most desirable weight did not change. On average, the weight of the subjects decreased by 6.273 kg and approached the desired weight. The difference between the post-fasting weight and the most desirable weight was not statistically significant. In addition, with the decrease in weight, a significant increase in the subjects’ evaluation of their own health was observed. Detailed data are included in Table 3.
Subsequent measurements referred to the level of satisfaction with the appearance of selected parts and body parameters. Comparisons between data collected before and after fasting show statistically significant differences. They mainly concern those body parts that are most associated with body weight and are most deformed by excessive weight gain, namely the waist and abdomen. In addition, after fasting, the subjects were more satisfied with their chest and arms. Satisfaction with other body parts and parameters did not change after fasting, including satisfaction with body weight. Detailed data are shown in Table 4. The respectively values of Cohen’s d and test power for the variables above were as follows: body weight—4.221, 1.000; desired weight −4.907, 1.000; and health assessment—0.775, 0.535.
The analyses on body satisfaction are worth supplementing with comparisons between variables in measure I and measure II. They show that the respondents were least satisfied with their abdomens and waistlines. This pattern did not change after the fasting. Before the fasting, the subjects were most satisfied with their faces, followed by their arms and height (see Table 5). After the fasting, the source of greatest satisfaction was the shoulders, height, and face. However, it should be noted that after the fasting, basically all indications exceeded the average for the scale, which is five points. Before the fasting, only satisfaction with the waist and abdomen was lower than the average for the scale. This means that respondents generally had high levels of satisfaction with body parts and parameters (see Table 4). The respective values of Cohen’s d and test power for psychological variables were as follows: face—5.422, 1.000; chest—1.549, 0.984; arms—2.840, 1.000; waist—0.516, 0.277; belly—0.645, 0.400; thighs—4.410, 1.000; legs—6.584, 1.000; height—5.809, 1.000; and mass—2.711, 1.000.
Another element that makes up the psychological aspect of corporeality is body image. In this study, it was measured using the BIQ and BES. Comparisons between measurements taken before and after fasting were not statistically significantly different for any variable. This means that the use of fasting neither modifies beliefs about one’s own body and physical attractiveness, nor the emotions felt toward the body. Neither does affect the subjects’ beliefs about how their physical attractiveness is judged by those around them and the degree to which they internalize the stereotypes according to what is physically attractive, i.e., thin, people have more positive attributes and lead better lives than unattractive, i.e., obese people. In addition, the study included the SES, which measures global self-esteem. Here, too, there were no significant differences between measurements before and after fasting. A comparison of the SES results to norms for the Polish male population shows that the subjects had slightly elevated self-esteem (7 sten). Measurements with the Lie Scale (L Scale)-EPQ-R prove that in giving their answers, the subjects were not guided by the need for self-presentation toward better adjustment, so the results obtained in all questionnaire measurements should be considered reliable (5 sten). Detailed data are presented in Table 6.
Interestingly, global self-esteem as measured using SES correlated significantly only with the pretty-ugly stereotype subscale (−0.679) and the body strength subscale (0.60). This means that the higher self-esteem the respondents had, the less they shared stereotypical opinions about obese people, and the higher they rated body strength in themselves, understood as having typically masculine attributes of physique and fitness.
In order to provide a full account of the relationships between the various dimensions of body image, analyses were performed on the differences between the various variables in measure I and measure II. They show that for the BIQ, the pattern of relationships between subscales was identical before and after fasting. The relatively most positive aspect of the subjects’ body image was their belief that those around them evaluate their physical attractiveness positively and that they do not experience criticism from other people in this area. The most negative element of body image was the relatively strong internalization of pretty-ugly stereotypes. However, it should be noted that the internal differences in the averages obtained in the various subscales of the BIQ cannot be taken as an objective indicator of body image of the subjects. This is because when we compare the results obtained by them to the norms for the adult male population, we see that they obtained results in all subscales oscillating within the limits of average results, i.e., from 4 to 6 sten. A similar situation occurred in the BES measurements. In this study, subjects obtained results typical of the normalization population. The differences between the subscales in measurements I and II boiled down to the fact that before fasting, the mean for the Body Strength subscale differed significantly from the mean of the Physical Attractiveness subscale. In measure II, there were no statistically significant differences between the BES subscales. The respective values of Cohen’s d and test power for psychological variables were as follows: cognition-emotions—11.361, 1.000; behaviors—4.131, 1.000; external criticism—4.518, 1.000; pretty-ugly stereotype—9.553, 1.000; physical attractiveness—9.682, 1.000; body strength—8.391, 1.000; physical condition—12.006, 1.000; Self-Esteem Scale—7.617, 1.000; and Lie Scale (L Scale)-EPQ-R—10.070, 1.000. Detailed data are provided in Table 6 and Table 7.
The final aspect of body image that was more extensively covered in the study was that of behavior toward one’s own body relating to eating habits. The respondents answered two questions with regard to the same specific methods of weight control: whether they use a specific method and how they evaluate its effectiveness. In addition to fairly typical and commonly used methods like limiting the amount of food consumed and eliminating certain products, there were also methods that in science and clinical practice are considered pathological forms of weight control like provoking vomiting or taking laxatives. A comparison of the results before and after fasting shows that it did not change the methods of weight control used by the subjects, which seems obvious. Moreover, it did not affect the evaluation of the effectiveness of weight control methods included in the scale (see Table 8).
Analyses of the weight control methods used and the evaluation of their effectiveness yield interesting results when comparing the differences between the various variables that occurred in study I and study II. All of the measurements included in Table 8 differ statistically significantly. Thus, for example, the subjects use food restriction significantly more often than appetite suppressants, laxatives, or provoking vomiting. In the use of specific methods of weight control, a clear line can be seen between those involving limiting the amount of food consumed or eliminating products considered harmful to health and those that are pathological and involve eating disorders. In assessing the effectiveness of weight control methods, respondents considered fasting and limiting the amount of food consumed and eliminating sugar/sweets from the diet to be the most effective. They rated significantly lower the use of restrictive diets and the elimination of meat and fatty foods from the diet. They rated the lowest the effectiveness of pathological forms of weight control, that is, taking appetite suppressants and laxatives and provoking vomiting (see Table 8 and Table 9). The respective values of Cohen’s d and test power for variables presented in Table 8 were as follows: i. using different ways to control body weight: limiting the amount of food—5.746, 1.000; elimination of sugar—3.357, 1.000; eliminating fatty foods—5.293, 1.000; eliminating meat—3.227, 1.000; restrictive diet—3.486, 1.000; fasting—6.584, 1.000; appetite suppressants—0.258, 0.105; use of laxatives—3.098, 1.000; and provoking vomiting—0.000, 0.050; ii. evaluation of the effectiveness of different weight control methods: limiting the amount of food—8.133, 1.000; elimination of sugar—8.018, 1.000; eliminating fatty foods—8.521, 1.000; eliminating meat—7.746, 1.000; restrictive diet—8.004, 1.000; fasting—1.549, 0.984; appetite suppressants—1.291, 0.927; use of laxatives—2.711, 1.000; and provoking vomiting—0.516, 0.277.

3.2. Correlation Analyses

Table 10 contains correlations between body composition indices and satisfaction with appearance and body parameters. It turned out that statistically significant correlations occurred here incidentally and only in measurements after fasting. Thus, in resting and post-exercise measurements, satisfaction with body weight correlated negatively with FFM [kg], TBW [kg], and BMI.
Correlations were even less frequent between body composition indices and body image dimensions. In this case, in contrast, correlations appeared in the pre-fasting measurements and were only for BMI, which positively correlated with the cognition-emotions subscale and the BIQ ambient criticism subscale, both at rest and after exercise. The remaining correlations did not reach the level of statistical significance (see Table 11).
Finally, to fully illustrate the relationship between psychological aspects of corporeality, correlations between satisfaction with appearance and body parameters and body image indices are presented. In many cases, there were not only statistically significant, but very strong correlations. Based on the results obtained, it can be generally concluded that body satisfaction in the subjects has a clear relationship with their positive body image. In principle, significant correlations with body image indicators appeared for all body parts and parameters. Given the nature of the research presented here, it is noteworthy that in measurements taken before the fasting, satisfaction with the abdomen significantly correlated with six of the seven body image indicators included in the study and satisfaction with the waist with five. In measurements taken after the fasting, significant correlations appeared for the waist in one case and the abdomen in three.
In addition, it can be said that beliefs about one’s own body and the emotions that this body evokes had a strong relationship with satisfaction with face, chest, waist, abdomen, and height before fasting and face, chest, abdomen, height, and weight after fasting. Behaviors toward one’s own body were related to satisfaction with the chest, waist, and abdomen before fasting and with the chest and thighs after fasting. Feelings of being accepted or criticized by those around them were related to satisfaction with face and height in both measurements and with abdomen before fasting. The degree of internalization of the pretty-ugly stereotype was related to satisfaction with height in both measurements and additionally to satisfaction with face, waist, and abdomen before fasting and abdomen and thighs after fasting.
In contrast, feelings of physical attractiveness correlated significantly before fasting with satisfaction with the chest, arms, and thighs and after fasting with satisfaction with the face and legs. Body strength had a strong relationship before the fasting with satisfaction with the chest, arms, waist, abdomen, and thighs and after with satisfaction with the chest and legs. Finally, before the fasting, the physical condition score was associated with satisfaction with the chest, arms, waist, abdomen, and thighs and after the fasting with satisfaction with the face, waist, abdomen, and legs. Detailed data are shown in Table 12.

4. Discussion

The experiment conducted took place under conditions of comfort (study I) and increased stress caused by the fasting intervention (study II). The measure of the intensified stress response was elevated C levels [54], and the expression of a significant metabolic change related to the brain, muscles, and other organs was increased ketosis manifested by elevated serum β-HB levels [55] indicative of the volunteers’ compliance with the fasting conditions.

4.1. Somatic Conditions and Body Perception During Fasting and Physical Exercise

Under the conditions of the fasting intervention and physical exercise, as well as the changing BM and its composition, the volunteers subjectively assessed improvements in their health. Perhaps it was these positive somatic changes that caused their good psychological well-being, which we observed in earlier studies [56], and this influenced them to express a positive health opinion [21]. It should be added that the men surveyed struggled with overweight (BMI~27.8 kg/m2) caused mainly by excess body fat (~23.5%). At the same time, the volunteers had the knowledge that being overweight or obese is detrimental to the body and can cause many dangerous diseases [14,57,58]. Thus, taking health-promoting measures probably compounded their well-being. Another reason for the study men’s good mental attitude, occurring after the fasting intervention, which may have influenced their opinion of good health, was the fact that they achieved a similar maximum heart rate during the exercise ergometric test that was not statistically different before and after 8 days of water-only fasting. Although the exercise test was performed only twice, lasted a relatively short period, and did not cause significant somatic changes, it should be assumed that it was a source of satisfaction with maintaining a relatively high exercise capacity despite the end of the fasting intervention.
Male respondents expressed a significant level of satisfaction with the changes in the chest, arms, waist, and abdomen caused by 8 days of water-only fasting [19]. These indications seem reasonable especially with regard to the chest, as its appearance indicates a normal posture and satisfactory cardiorespiratory capacity. A deformed chest of the funnel-shaped or chicken-shaped type with humps, depressions, or scoliosis decreases the functional efficiency of the lungs and heart and affects a low opinion of this part of the body as well as a lack of satisfaction with it. It should be added that conservative or surgical treatment of such defects is lengthy, sometimes dangerous to health, and often ineffective [59]. Volunteers’ expressions of satisfaction with the appearance of the waist and abdomen are quite common, since it is excessive waist circumference that most distorts the figure, affects the development of cardiovascular disease, and almost always accompanies obesity [60]. Among the many even technologically very advanced diagnostic tools indicating future or currently developing pathologies of this system is an extremely simple measurement, which is the waist circumference. This circumference above 94 cm in men indicates a significant risk of cardiovascular diseases or certain metabolic diseases [61]. It should also be noted that increased waist circumference is usually associated with the accumulation of excess visceral fat in this place, which is a significant factor in the occurrence of cardiovascular diseases, especially in men [62]. In addition, increased waist circumference is accompanied by a thickened skin and fat fold, which lowers the aesthetic image of this part of the body, mainly in people with excessive weight. Such people often resort to reducing waist circumference and thus fat content using various methods even including life-threatening and health-threatening bariatric surgery [63]. On the contrary, in thin people and mainly in those who are physically active or in professional athletes, the waist is proportionally narrower, the folds of skin and fat are significantly thinner, the amount of abdominal fat tissue is smaller, and the straight and oblique muscles of the abdomen are clearly outlined. This creates an impeccable image of this part of the body with great aesthetic value, ensuring a positive perception of one’s own body [64,65]. In these people, the risk of cardiovascular disease is low.
Invariably, however, BH, face, and arms were the greatest source of satisfaction among the different body parts in the men surveyed both before and after fasting [10]. These facts seem to confirm general observations that tall people enjoy more popularity and are more often presented in the media, especially men. Unfortunately, BH is significantly determined genetically, and its correction using hormonal or surgical therapies can be ineffective or detrimental to health. Significant BH also determines its favorable proportions, which can also be a source of satisfaction and account for a person’s positive social perception and image. A large BH usually entails the presence of a significant length of the lower limbs, which has a positive effect on body proportions and body image in both women and men [65]. Unfortunately, in our study, respondents did not pay significant attention to the appearance of the legs. Facial appearance played a similar important role as BH in public perception and body image and is more important in women than in men [10]. Also, the face largely or very largely determines satisfaction with body acceptance and projects body image, which is related to a person’s media presence or popularity. Therefore, facial appearance is a significant focus of cosmetology and plastic surgery, mainly among women. The indication of the arms as a part of the body that gives satisfaction to the surveyed volunteers may be related to the identification of men as people characterized by considerable strength, which translates into their large circumference and their considerable strength [19]. This is manifested especially by people who train strength sports [12,66] and, together with BH and facial appearance, affects a person’s body image. Our study, however, showed that body image did not change after the applied fasting intervention, although its determinants included such factors as physical attractiveness, body strength, and physical condition [22,23]. These results can be taken as further evidence that body image, as a structure of the Self, is a permanent construct and is unlikely to change under the influence of short-term current experiences [3,4,5,6]. Certainly, the age of the volunteers was important for the stability of body image because people around the age of 50 achieve relative stability of features [67]. Another result of the conducted research, which turned out to be inconsistent with expectations, was the highly selective relationship between somatic and mental parameters of the body and its mental representation. It appears that this mental representation is formed primarily on the basis of experiences other than somatic ones, mainly observations, beliefs, emotions related to the body, and opinions of other people and references to standards of attractiveness [7,12].

4.2. Other Methods of Controlling Body Structure and Appearance

In our study, among the most common methods used to control BM both before and after the fasting intervention, volunteers mentioned “physiological” ways, including food restriction, elimination of sugar and fat from the diet, restrictive diets, or fasting interventions. The volunteers considered food restriction and elimination of sugar from the diet and fasting interventions to be the most effective for weight control in both conditions: before and after 8 days of water-only fasting. The aforementioned ways of reducing BM [16] and their effectiveness have been confirmed in the scientific literature [30,36]. It seems that the fasting intervention, although it cannot be maintained for too long, exerts the most spectacular and immediate effects not only in the form of BM reduction and changes in its composition, but also improves urogenital function, economizes endocrine function [38], improves carbohydrate control, activates fat metabolism [68], and leads to better well-being [56]. A positive choice of the subjects was the rejection of so-called “pathological” methods of BM control and their effectiveness in this regard both before and after the fasting intervention. These methods can cause and/or accompany numerous diseases associated with eating disorders [69]. The discussed methods of controlling BM and its composition are used in medical treatment, in intentional BM reduction, or in competitive sports [36]. In addition to the methods of BM control mentioned in our study, there are many others; among them, exercise plays a serious role. It is well known that physical activity plays an important role in the regulation of BM and its composition, when it is present in daily life and characterized by a longer duration and a correspondingly high intensity [70].
In cases of significant obesity or morbid obesity, however, it is necessary to simultaneously include pharmacological therapy in the form of drugs: modifying absorption in the gastrointestinal tract (orlistat), acting on the central nervous system by regulating appetite (phentermine-topiramate and naltrexone-bupropion), or hormonal drugs stimulated by nutrients in the gastrointestinal tract (liraglutide, and tirzepatide). However, these drugs cause a small loss of BM and have such undesirable effects, such as constipation, urgency, paresthesia, nausea, and diarrhea [35]. Semaglutide and its analogues have recently appeared on the market, causing significant weight loss (up to 15%). Setmelanotide has also recently been registered for the treatment of severe genetic obesity [71].
The described somatic and mental states of the human body, also determined by body image, may be the reason for its acceptance [72] and can also be the expression of disapproval and deep impairments [69,73].

5. Limitations of the Presented Manuscript and Future Research Prospects

Among the factors limiting the current study is the relatively small group of people participating in it. At this point, we would like to point out that recruiting a larger number of participants for such a demanding experiment as 8 days of complete fasting and parallel physical exercise of maximum intensity is extremely difficult. Evidence of the above-mentioned difficulties is the lack of articles in the literature involving a larger number of people with such an extremely difficult research protocol. Various research protocols used by other authors, most often taking into account lighter forms of fasting and less intense physical exercise than those used in the presented study, make it difficult to compare the results obtained. We also believe that lifestyle, including the level of physical activity and the type of diet used, reflecting current somatic data and the degree of satisfaction with one’s body, may be factors limiting the current study. Another factor that limits drawing far-reaching conclusions based on this study is the participation of only men in it. However, it is known that women pay much more attention to the appearance of their own body than men. The age of the participants also seems to be a factor limiting this study because the body structure and its perception in different phases of ontogenesis are not the same. Various methods of correcting the structure and appearance of the body previously used may also affect the degree of its acceptance.
It was impossible to explain the above-mentioned limitations in the current study, but these doubts give impetus to conduct future multi-threaded experiments with various forms of fasting and physical exercise and with the participation of a larger number of volunteers of both sexes. It also seems that future research should be conducted by dividing participants into groups according to age, type of diet, or previous body corrections.

6. Conclusions

In conclusion, it should be said that the somatic aspects of the attractiveness of the human body are monitored not only by the interested party itself but also by the fields of cosmetology, aesthetic medicine, physical culture sciences, dietetics, and health sciences. An important, less objective but also more difficult to measure mental image of the human body described by psychology or sociology completes the image of the described object, and only then can one properly determine the degree of acceptance and image. The study we carried out, using simple almost cost-free tools, can have a wide public reception stimulating the initiation of changes in the unfavorable somatic conditions of the human body and also have improving mental effects on its imperfections.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, K.P. and A.G.; methodology, K.P., A.G. and W.P.; software, A.G., K.P. and A.P.; validation, W.P., K.P., A.P. and A.G.; formal analysis, A.G., K.P. and W.P.; investigation, W.P., A.G., A.Ż. and K.P.; resources, A.G., A.Ż. and K.P.; data curation, A.P., A.G. and A.Ż.; writing-original draft preparation, A.G. and K.P.; writing-review and editing, A.Ż. and A.P.; visualization, A.Ż. and A.P.; supervision, W.P., A.G. and K.P.; project administration, A.Ż., K.P. and A.P.; funding acquisition. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

The work was financed with own funds.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Bioethics Research Committee of Jan Dlugosz University in Czestochowa, Poland (KE-0/1/2019, 5 March 2019).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent to participate in the study was obtained from all participants.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated and analyzed during the study are not publicly available due to data protection reasons and proprietary rights, but are available from the correspondence author upon reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all study participants.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Body Image: A Handbook of Theory, Research, and Clinical Practice, Paperback ed.; Cash, T.F., Ed.; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2004; ISBN 978-1-59385-015-9. [Google Scholar]
  2. Rodgers, R.F.; Laveway, K.; Campos, P.; De Carvalho, P.H.B. Body Image as a Global Mental Health Concern. Camb. Prisms Glob. Ment. Health 2023, 10, e9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Cash, T.F.; Pruzinsky, T. Body Image: A Handbook of Theory, Research, and Clinical Practice; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2002; ISBN 978-1-57230-777-3. [Google Scholar]
  4. Schilder, P. The Image and Appearance of the Human Body; Routledge: Abingdon, UK, 2013; ISBN 978-1-315-01041-0. [Google Scholar]
  5. Altabe, M.; Thompson, J.K. Body Image: A Cognitive Self-Schema Construct? Cogn. Ther. Res. 1996, 20, 171–193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Cash, T.; Fleming, E.; Alindogan, J.; Steadman, L.; Whitehead, A. Beyond Body Image as a Trait: The Development and Validation of the Body Image States Scale. Eat. Disord. 2002, 10, 103–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Tort-Nasarre, G.; Pollina Pocallet, M.; Artigues-Barberà, E. The Meaning and Factors That Influence the Concept of Body Image: Systematic Review and Meta-Ethnography from the Perspectives of Adolescents. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 1140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Aimé, A.; Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M.; Dion, J.; Markey, C.H.; Strodl, E.; McCabe, M.; Mellor, D.; Granero Gallegos, A.; Pietrabissa, G.; Alcaraz-Ibánez, M.; et al. Assessing Positive Body Image, Body Satisfaction, Weight Bias, and Appearance Comparison in Emerging Adults: A Cross-Validation Study across Eight Countries. Body Image 2020, 35, 320–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Garner, D.M. The 1997 Body Image Survey Results. Psychol. Today 1997, 30, 30–44. [Google Scholar]
  10. Dairi, G.; Alafghani, R.; Ghafouri, K.; Noorwali, E. Effect of Intermittent Fasting on Body Image Satisfaction and Appreciation Among Saudi Adults. Cureus 2023, 15, e33468. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Caltabiano, M.L. Translational Aspects of Body Image Research for Obesity-Related Quality of Life and Weight Loss Maintenance Post-Bariatric Surgery. Ann. Transl. Med. 2020, 8, S2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Głębocka, A. Niezadowolenie z Wyglądu a Rozpaczliwa Kontrola Wagi; Impuls: Kraków, Poland, 2009; ISBN 978-83-7850-358-3. [Google Scholar]
  13. Bouzas, C.; Bibiloni, M.D.M.; Tur, J.A. Relationship between Body Image and Body Weight Control in Overweight ≥55-Year-Old Adults: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 1622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Janić, M.; Janež, A.; El-Tanani, M.; Rizzo, M. Obesity: Recent Advances and Future Perspectives. Biomedicines 2025, 13, 368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Wang, Y.; Wu, R. The Effect of Fasting on Human Metabolism and Psychological Health. Dis. Markers 2022, 2022, 5653739. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Santos, H.O.; Macedo, R.C.O. Impact of Intermittent Fasting on the Lipid Profile: Assessment Associated with Diet and Weight Loss. Clin. Nutr. ESPEN 2018, 24, 14–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Rynders, C.A.; Thomas, E.A.; Zaman, A.; Pan, Z.; Catenacci, V.A.; Melanson, E.L. Effectiveness of Intermittent Fasting and Time-Restricted Feeding Compared to Continuous Energy Restriction for Weight Loss. Nutrients 2019, 11, 2442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Godoy-Izquierdo, D.; González-Hernández, J.; Lara, R.; Rodríguez-Tadeo, A.; Ramírez, M.J.; Navarrón, E.; Ogallar-Blanco, A.; López-Mora, C.; Arbinaga, F. Considering BMI, Body Image and Desired Weight Change for Suitable Obesity Management Options. Span. J. Psychol. 2020, 23, e35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Chao, H.-L. Body Image Change in Obese and Overweight Persons Enrolled in Weight Loss Intervention Programs: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0124036. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Schaumberg, K.; Anderson, D.A. Does Short-Term Fasting Promote Changes in State Body Image? Body Image 2014, 11, 167–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Anic, K.; Schmidt, M.W.; Furtado, L.; Weidenbach, L.; Battista, M.J.; Schmidt, M.; Schwab, R.; Brenner, W.; Ruckes, C.; Lotz, J.; et al. Intermittent Fasting-Short- and Long-Term Quality of Life, Fatigue, and Safety in Healthy Volunteers: A Prospective, Clinical Trial. Nutrients 2022, 14, 4216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Moreno-Domínguez, S.; Rodríguez-Ruiz, S.; Fernández-Santaella, M.C.; Ortega-Roldán, B.; Cepeda-Benito, A. Impact of Fasting on Food Craving, Mood and Consumption in Bulimia Nervosa and Healthy Women Participants. Eur. Eat. Disord. Rev. 2012, 20, 461–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Solianik, R.; Sujeta, A. Two-Day Fasting Evokes Stress, but Does Not Affect Mood, Brain Activity, Cognitive, Psychomotor, and Motor Performance in Overweight Women. Behav. Brain Res. 2018, 338, 166–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Ludwig, D.S.; Aronne, L.J.; Astrup, A.; De Cabo, R.; Cantley, L.C.; Friedman, M.I.; Heymsfield, S.B.; Johnson, J.D.; King, J.C.; Krauss, R.M.; et al. The Carbohydrate-Insulin Model: A Physiological Perspective on the Obesity Pandemic. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2021, 114, 1873–1885. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Chen, C.; Ye, Y.; Zhang, Y.; Pan, X.-F.; Pan, A. Weight Change across Adulthood in Relation to All Cause and Cause Specific Mortality: Prospective Cohort Study. BMJ 2019, 367, l5584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Sanders, L.M.; Slavin, J. Impact of Pasta Intake on Body Weight and Body Composition: A Technical Review. Nutrients 2023, 15, 2689. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Crosby, L.; Rembert, E.; Levin, S.; Green, A.; Ali, Z.; Jardine, M.; Nguyen, M.; Elliott, P.; Goldstein, D.; Freeman, A.; et al. Changes in Food and Nutrient Intake and Diet Quality on a Low-Fat Vegan Diet Are Associated with Changes in Body Weight, Body Composition, and Insulin Sensitivity in Overweight Adults: A Randomized Clinical Trial. J. Acad. Nutr. Diet. 2022, 122, 1922–1939.e0. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Di Rosa, C.; Lattanzi, G.; Spiezia, C.; Imperia, E.; Piccirilli, S.; Beato, I.; Gaspa, G.; Micheli, V.; De Joannon, F.; Vallecorsa, N.; et al. Mediterranean Diet versus Very Low-Calorie Ketogenic Diet: Effects of Reaching 5% Body Weight Loss on Body Composition in Subjects with Overweight and with Obesity-A Cohort Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 13040. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Paoli, A.; Cenci, L.; Pompei, P.; Sahin, N.; Bianco, A.; Neri, M.; Caprio, M.; Moro, T. Effects of Two Months of Very Low Carbohydrate Ketogenic Diet on Body Composition, Muscle Strength, Muscle Area, and Blood Parameters in Competitive Natural Body Builders. Nutrients 2021, 13, 374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Kahleova, H.; Petersen, K.F.; Shulman, G.I.; Alwarith, J.; Rembert, E.; Tura, A.; Hill, M.; Holubkov, R.; Barnard, N.D. Effect of a Low-Fat Vegan Diet on Body Weight, Insulin Sensitivity, Postprandial Metabolism, and Intramyocellular and Hepatocellular Lipid Levels in Overweight Adults: A Randomized Clinical Trial. JAMA Netw. Open 2020, 3, e2025454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Jon Schoenfeld, B.; Albert Aragon, A.; Krieger, J.W. Effects of Meal Frequency on Weight Loss and Body Composition: A Meta-Analysis. Nutr. Rev. 2015, 73, 69–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Talenezhad, N.; Mohammadi, M.; Ramezani-Jolfaie, N.; Mozaffari-Khosravi, H.; Salehi-Abargouei, A. Effects of L-Carnitine Supplementation on Weight Loss and Body Composition: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of 37 Randomized Controlled Clinical Trials with Dose-Response Analysis. Clin. Nutr. ESPEN 2020, 37, 9–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Çıtar Dazıroğlu, M.E.; Acar Tek, N. Water Consumption: Effect on Energy Expenditure and Body Weight Management. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2023, 12, 99–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Fogarasi, A.; Gonzalez, K.; Dalamaga, M.; Magkos, F. The Impact of the Rate of Weight Loss on Body Composition and Metabolism. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2022, 11, 33–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Gudzune, K.A.; Kushner, R.F. Medications for Obesity: A Review. JAMA 2024, 332, 571–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Kwan, K.; Helms, E. Prevalence, Magnitude, and Methods of Weight Cutting Used by World Class Powerlifters. J. Strength Cond. Res. 2022, 36, 998–1002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Gravina, G.; Milano, W.; Nebbiai, G.; Piccione, C.; Capasso, A. Medical Complications in Anorexia and Bulimia Nervosa. Endocr. Metab. Immune Disord. Drug Targets 2018, 18, 477–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Letkiewicz, S.; Pilis, K.; Ślęzak, A.; Pilis, A.; Pilis, W.; Żychowska, M.; Langfort, J. Eight Days of Water-Only Fasting Promotes Favorable Changes in the Functioning of the Urogenital System of Middle-Aged Healthy Men. Nutrients 2020, 13, 113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Elliott, B.; Mina, M.; Ferrier, C. Complete and Voluntary Starvation of 50 Days. Clin. Med. Insights Case Rep. 2016, 9, 67–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Dulloo, A.G. Physiology of Weight Regain: Lessons from the Classic Minnesota Starvation Experiment on Human Body Composition Regulation. Obes. Rev. 2021, 22, e13189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Aragon, A.A.; Schoenfeld, B.J. Does Timing Matter? A Narrative Review of Intermittent Fasting Variants and Their Effects on Bodyweight and Body Composition. Nutrients 2022, 14, 5022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Tinsley, G.M.; La Bounty, P.M. Effects of Intermittent Fasting on Body Composition and Clinical Health Markers in Humans. Nutr. Rev. 2015, 73, 661–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Tsitsou, S.; Zacharodimos, N.; Poulia, K.-A.; Karatzi, K.; Dimitriadis, G.; Papakonstantinou, E. Effects of Time-Restricted Feeding and Ramadan Fasting on Body Weight, Body Composition, Glucose Responses, and Insulin Resistance: A Systematic Review of Randomized Controlled Trials. Nutrients 2022, 14, 4778. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Domaszewski, P.; Konieczny, M.; Dybek, T.; Łukaniszyn-Domaszewska, K.; Anton, S.; Sadowska-Krępa, E.; Skorupska, E. Comparison of the Effects of Six-Week Time-Restricted Eating on Weight Loss, Body Composition, and Visceral Fat in Overweight Older Men and Women. Exp. Gerontol. 2023, 174, 112116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Ferland, P.-M.; Charron, J.; Brisebois-Boies, M.; St-Jean Miron, F. Body Composition (DXA) and Maximal Strength of Powerlifters: A Descriptive Quantitative and Longitudinal Study. Int. J. Exerc. Sci. 2023, 16, 828–845. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Memelink, R.G.; Hummel, M.; Hijlkema, A.; Streppel, M.T.; Bautmans, I.; Weijs, P.J.M.; Berk, K.A.; Tieland, M. Additional Effects of Exercise to Hypocaloric Diet on Body Weight, Body Composition, Glycaemic Control and Cardio-respiratory Fitness in Adults with Overweight or Obesity and Type 2 Diabetes: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Diabet. Med. 2023, 40, e15096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Bellicha, A.; Van Baak, M.A.; Battista, F.; Beaulieu, K.; Blundell, J.E.; Busetto, L.; Carraça, E.V.; Dicker, D.; Encantado, J.; Ermolao, A.; et al. Effect of Exercise Training on Weight Loss, Body Composition Changes, and Weight Maintenance in Adults with Overweight or Obesity: An Overview of 12 Systematic Reviews and 149 Studies. Obes. Rev. 2021, 22, e13256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Lopez, P.; Taaffe, D.R.; Galvão, D.A.; Newton, R.U.; Nonemacher, E.R.; Wendt, V.M.; Bassanesi, R.N.; Turella, D.J.P.; Rech, A. Resistance Training Effectiveness on Body Composition and Body Weight Outcomes in Individuals with Overweight and Obesity across the Lifespan: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Obes. Rev. 2022, 23, e13428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Brown, L.R.; Sousa, M.S.; Yule, M.S.; Baracos, V.E.; McMillan, D.C.; Arends, J.; Balstad, T.R.; Bye, A.; Dajani, O.; Dolan, R.D.; et al. Body Weight and Composition Endpoints in Cancer Cachexia Clinical Trials: Systematic Review 4 of the Cachexia Endpoints Series. J. Cachexia Sarcopenia Muscle 2024, 15, 816–852. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. US Department of Health and Human Services. Interpretive Guidelines for Long-Term Care Facilities; Department of Health & Human Services Centers for Medicare & Medicaid Services: Woodlawn, MA, USA, 2008.
  51. Łaguna, M.; Lachowicz-Tabaczek, K.; Dzwonkowska, I. Skala Samooceny SES Morrisa Rosenberga—Polska Adaptacja Metod. Psychol. Społeczna 2007, 2, 164–176. [Google Scholar]
  52. Brzozowski, P.; Drwal, R. Kwestionariusz Osobowości Eysencka Polska Adaptacja EPQ-R; Pracownia Testów Psychologicznych Polskiego Towarzystwa Psychologicznego: Warszawa, Poland, 1995; ISBN 83-85512-36-5. [Google Scholar]
  53. Lipowska, M.; Lipowski, M. Polish Normalization of the Body Esteem Scale. Health Psychol. Rep. 2014, 1, 72–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Tomiyama, A.J.; Mann, T.; Vinas, D.; Hunger, J.M.; DeJager, J.; Taylor, S.E. Low Calorie Dieting Increases Cortisol. Psychosom. Med. 2010, 72, 357–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Palmer, B.F.; Clegg, D.J. Starvation Ketosis and the Kidney. Am. J. Nephrol. 2021, 52, 467–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Stec, K.; Pilis, K.; Pilis, W.; Dolibog, P.; Letkiewicz, S.; Głębocka, A. Effects of Fasting on the Physiological and Psychological Responses in Middle-Aged Men. Nutrients 2023, 15, 3444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Fukumoto, Y. Nutrition and Cardiovascular Diseases. Nutrients 2021, 14, 94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Caballero, B. Humans against Obesity: Who Will Win? Adv. Nutr. 2019, 10, S4–S9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Sonaglioni, A.; Nicolosi, G.L.; Trevisan, R.; Lombardo, M.; Grasso, E.; Gensini, G.F.; Ambrosio, G. The Influence of Pectus Excavatum on Cardiac Kinetics and Function in Otherwise Healthy Individuals: A Systematic Review. Int. J. Cardiol. 2023, 381, 135–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Hruby, A.; Manson, J.E.; Qi, L.; Malik, V.S.; Rimm, E.B.; Sun, Q.; Willett, W.C.; Hu, F.B. Determinants and Consequences of Obesity. Am. J. Public Health 2016, 106, 1656–1662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Obsa, M.S.; Ataro, G.; Awoke, N.; Jemal, B.; Tilahun, T.; Ayalew, N.; Woldegeorgis, B.Z.; Azeze, G.A.; Haji, Y. Determinants of Dyslipidemia in Africa: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Front. Cardiovasc. Med. 2022, 8, 778891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Christen, T.; Trompet, S.; Rensen, P.C.N.; Willems Van Dijk, K.; Lamb, H.J.; Jukema, J.W.; Rosendaal, F.R.; Le Cessie, S.; De Mutsert, R. The Role of Inflammation in the Association between Overall and Visceral Adiposity and Subclinical Atherosclerosis. Nutr. Metab. Cardiovasc. Dis. 2019, 29, 728–735. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Chalopin, S.; Betry, C.; Coumes, S.; Wion, N.; Reche, F.; Arvieux, C.; Borel, A.-L. Benefits and Risks of Bariatric Surgery in Patients with Bipolar Disorders. Surg. Obes. Relat. Dis. 2020, 16, 798–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Macuh, M.; Levec, J.; Kojić, N.; Knap, B. Dietary Intake, Body Composition and Performance of Professional Football Athletes in Slovenia. Nutrients 2022, 15, 82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Lijewski, M.; Burdukiewicz, A.; Stachoń, A.; Pietraszewska, J. Differences in Anthropometric Variables and Muscle Strength in Relation to Competitive Level in Male Handball Players. PLoS ONE 2021, 16, e0261141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Marcos-Pardo, P.J.; Orquin-Castrillón, F.J.; Gea-García, G.M.; Menayo-Antúnez, R.; González-Gálvez, N.; Vale, R.G.D.S.; Martínez-Rodríguez, A. Effects of a Moderate-to-High Intensity Resistance Circuit Training on Fat Mass, Functional Capacity, Muscular Strength, and Quality of Life in Elderly: A Randomized Controlled Trial. Sci. Rep. 2019, 9, 7830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Cervone, D.; Pervin, L.A. Personality: Theory and Research, 15th ed.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2022; ISBN 978-1-119-89167-3. [Google Scholar]
  68. Pilis, K.; Pilis, A.; Stec, K.; Antosiewicz, J.; Slezak, A.; Zalejska-Fiolka, J.; Dolibog, P.; Pilis, W.; Letkiewicz, S. Effect of 8 Days of Water-Only Fasting and Vigorous Exercise on Anthropometric Parameters, Lipid Profile and HOMA-IR in Middle-Aged Men. Biomed. Hum. Kinet. 2023, 15, 289–297. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Gibson, D.; Workman, C.; Mehler, P.S. Medical Complications of Anorexia Nervosa and Bulimia Nervosa. Psychiatr. Clin. N. Am. 2019, 42, 263–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  70. Gerber, M.; Lang, C.; Beckmann, J.; Du Randt, R.; Long, K.Z.; Müller, I.; Nienaber, M.; Probst-Hensch, N.; Steinmann, P.; Pühse, U.; et al. Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour, Weight Status, and Body Composition among South African Primary Schoolchildren. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public. Health 2022, 19, 11836. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Ryan, D.H. Drugs for Treating Obesity. In Handbook of Experimental Pharmacology; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2022; Volume 274, pp. 387–414. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Baceviciene, M.; Bliujute, K.; Jankauskiene, R. Analysis of Factors Contributing to State Body Appreciation during Exercise. Behav. Sci. 2024, 14, 690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Scheffers, M.; Kalisvaart, H.; Van Busschbach, J.T.; Bosscher, R.J.; Van Duijn, M.A.J.; Van Broeckhuysen-Kloth, S.A.M.; Schoevers, R.A.; Geenen, R. Body Image in Patients with Somatoform Disorder. BMC Psychiatry 2018, 18, 346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Table 1. Demographic and somatic variables (M ± SD; %) of volunteers before fasting.
Table 1. Demographic and somatic variables (M ± SD; %) of volunteers before fasting.
M ± SDMinimumMaximum HigherSecondary
Age59.8 ± 12.373777Education14 (93.33)1 (6.66)
Height178.133 ± 4.75167.00184.00Marital status, number of people and %YES 9 (60)NO 6 (40)
Weight88.240 ± 11.9569.60108.00ResidenceNumber of People%
BMI27.800 ± 3.4322.1034.10City > 200,000960
Desired weight78.923 ± 8.4868.00091.000City of 50–200
thousand
426.67
Village213.33
Taking Dietary Supplements and Vitamins
D3CB2B6B12E
Number of
people and %
9 (60)7 (46.66)2 (20)5 (33.33)2 (20)1 (6.66)
Nutrition Rules Used
I Eat Everything I Feel Like EatingI Strictly Plan My DietI Eat Only Healthy FoodI Follow a Vegetarian DietI Follow a
Vegan Diet
I’m on a
Keto Diet
Number of
people and %
7 (46.55)05 (33.33)4 (26.66)5 (33.33)0
Table 2. Comparisons of physiological indices of body composition before and after fasting.
Table 2. Comparisons of physiological indices of body composition before and after fasting.
Physiological ParametesBefore FastingRankTotal RanksAfter FastingRankTotal RanksTZp
Rest
BM [kg]88.240 ± 11.9582.967 ± 11.640.003.4070.0001
BF [%]23.560 ± 4.682.83342.5022.927 ± 4.782.96744.5033.001.5330.125
BF [kg]21.200 ± 6.782.33335.0019.273 ± 6.482.13332.005.003.1240.002
FFM [%]76.465 ± 4.697.800117.0077.077 ± 4.787.933119.0033.001.5330.125
FFM [kg]67.047 ± 6.257.133107.0062.727 ± 5.836.733101.000.003.4080.001
TBW [%]55.976 ± 3.445.86788.0056.425 ± 3.506.26794.0033.001.5330.125
TBW [kg]49.080 ± 4.565.20078.0045.920 ± 4.275.06776.000.003.4080.001
BMI [kg/m2]27.800 ± 3.433.83357.5025.807 ± 3.293.90058.500.003.4080.001
β-HB [mmol/L]0.142 ± 0.131.53323.004.537 ± 1.633.73356.000.003.4080.001
C [ng/mL]295.580 ± 92.101.53323.00468.633 ± 111.202.73341.007.0003.0100.003
Exercise
BM [kg]88.227 ± 11.9881.487 ± 11.2314.002.6130.009
BF [%]22.757 ± 4.562.78639.0022.447 ± 4.689.000126.000.003.2960.001
BF [kg]20.336 ± 6.682.35733.0018.820 ± 6.3210.000140.000.003.2960.001
FFM [kg]67.086 ± 6.258.000112.0063.047 ± 5.871.14316.000.003.2960.001
FFM [%]76.734 ± 4.753.78653.0077.140 ± 4.669.600144.0033.001.5330.125
TBW [kg]49.100 ± 4.587.00098.0046.153 ± 4.296.00084.000.003.2960.001
TBW [%]56.175 ± 3.482.07129.0056.355 ± 3.261.80027.0037.001.3060.191
BMI [kg/m2]27.543 ± 3.454.92969.0025.773 ± 3.293.78653.000.003.2960.001
β-HB [mmol/L]0.114 ± 0.081.46722.004.132 ± 1.563.26749.000.003.4070.001
C [ng/mL]332.520 ± 111.622.06731.00529.620 ± 108.853.66755.001.0003.3510.001
Table 3. Indications of body weight and subjective health assessment before and after fasting.
Table 3. Indications of body weight and subjective health assessment before and after fasting.
VariablesBefore Fasting
M ± SD
After Fasting
M ± SD
MinimumMaximumTZp
Body weight88.240 ± 11.9581.967 ± 11.64569.600108.000.002.9340.003
Desired weight78.923 ± 8.4876.250 ± 7.8164.000101.801.001.0690.285
Weight loss 6.273 ± 1.343.007.90
Health assessment7.533 ± 1.408.267 ± 0.964.0010.003.002.3100.020
Table 4. Level of satisfaction with selected body parts and parameters.
Table 4. Level of satisfaction with selected body parts and parameters.
Before FastingAfter Fasting
SatisfactionM ± SDRankTotal RanksM ± SDRankTotal RanksDifferenceTZp
Face7.133 ± 1.686.20093.007.200 ± 1.935.85782.00−0.067 ± 1.4821.000.1780.859
Chest6.467 ± 2.415.56783.507.200 ± 2.075.64379.00−0.733 ± 1.276.001.9550.050
Arms6.933 ± 1.985.96789.507.467 ± 1.996.21487.00−0.533 ± 0.8311.001.9560.050
Waist4.600 ± 2.583.10046.506.000 ± 2.363.75052.50−1.400 ± 1.762.002.4290.015
Belly4.533 ± 2.822.86743.005.467 ± 2.583.07143.00−0.933 ± 1.662.501.9440.047
Thighs6.714 ± 2.235.46782.007.000 ± 2.114.92969.00−0.286 ± 0.8216.501.1210.262
Legs6.133 ± 2.564.96774.506.933 ± 2.015.39375.50−0.800 ± 2.6725.501.3980.162
Height6.933 ± 2.376.46797.007.267 ± 1.945.75080.50−0.333 ± 1.9122.500.5100.610
Weight5.733 ± 2.844.40066.006.533 ± 2.504.39361.50−0.800 ± 1.8910.501.4220.155
Table 5. Level of satisfaction with selected body parts and parameters—comparisons between variables measured before and after fasting.
Table 5. Level of satisfaction with selected body parts and parameters—comparisons between variables measured before and after fasting.
Body Satisfaction Before FastingBody Satisfaction After Fasting
Chi-Square and ANOVA (N = 15, df = 8) = 35.3410;
p < 0.001; Coefficient of Concordance = 0.9451;
r Mean Rank = 0.2441
Chi-Square and ANOVA (N = 15, df = 8) = 24.7873;
p < 0.001; Coefficient of Compliance = 0.221;
r Mean Rank = 0.1614
FaceWaist, abdomenFaceWaist, abdomen, mass
ChestWaist *, belly *, heightChestWaist, abdomen, mass
ArmsWaist *, abdomenArmsWaist, abdomen, thighs
WaistFace, abdomen, arms, thighs, legs, heightWaistFace, chest, shoulders, abdomen, thighs, legs, height
BellyFace, chest, arms, thighs, legs, height, weightBellyFace, chest, arms, waist, thighs, legs, height *, weight
ThighsWaist *, abdomenThighsArms, waist, abdomen
LegsWaist *, abdomenLegsWaist, abdomen
HeightChest, waist *, abdomenHeightWaist, abdomen, mass
WeightAbdomen, heightWeightFace, chest, abdomen, height
Note: Differences statistically significant for p < 0.05. * indicates p < 0.001.
Table 6. Body image dimensions and self-esteem and need for social approval.
Table 6. Body image dimensions and self-esteem and need for social approval.
Before FastingAfter Fasting
BIQM ± SDTotalRankTotal RanksM ± SDTotalRankTotal RanksSten *TZp
Cognition-emotions2.233 ± 12.8235.733 ± 0.802.53338.002.204 ± 0.7235.267 ± 11.582.46737.00644.000.5330.593
Behaviors2.053 ± 3.8610.266 ± 0.772.33335.001.933 ± 0.819.667 ± 4.052.40036.00415.001.2740.202
External criticism1.711 ± 4.1510.785 ± 0.641.13317.001.611 ± 0.6710.333 ± 3.991.13317.00528.000.8620.388
Pretty-ugly stereotype3.579 ± 7.7046.533 ± 0.594.00060.003.692 ± 0.6848.000 ± 8.894.00060.00637.001.3060.191
BES
Physical attractiveness3.697 ± 0.8240.667 ± 9.061.66725.003.764 ± 0.7741.400 ± 8.461.93329.00636.500.6290.529
Body strength3.859 ± 0.8734.733 ± 7.792.43336.503.844 ± 0.8234.600 ± 7.382.13332.00632.500.5090.610
Physical condition3.697 ± 0.8348.06 ± 710.761.90028.503.719 ± 0.8348.353 ± 10.761.93329.00647.000.3450.729
Self-Esteem Scale3.213 ± 7.2932.133 ± 0.73 3.433 ± 4.3534.333 ± 0.44 729.501.4440.149
Lie Scale (L Scale)-EPQ-R 7.667 ± 2.89 7.333 ± 3.92 545.000.4710.638
Note: * The sten scale is used for the normalization of psychological tests, which makes it possible to compare specific scores of individuals or averages for groups to those of a normalization group. Thus, 1 sten is a very low score, 2–3 sten is a low score, 4 is a slightly reduced score, 5–6 is a normal score, 7 is a slightly increased score, 8–9 is a high score, and 10 is a very high score.
Table 7. Body image indices—comparisons between variables measured before and after fasting using Friedman’s ANOVA and Kendall’s Chi squared analysis.
Table 7. Body image indices—comparisons between variables measured before and after fasting using Friedman’s ANOVA and Kendall’s Chi squared analysis.
BIQ—Before FastingBIQ—After Fasting
ANOVA (N = 15, df = 3) = 7.8243, p = 0.00001;
Coefficient of Concordance = 0.8405;
r Mean Rank = 0.8291
ANOVA (N = 15, df = 2) = 0.4285, p = 0.807;
Coefficient of Concordance = 0.01429; r Mean Rank = −0.0561
Cognition-emotions (CE)EC/PUS *Cognition-emotions (CE)EC/PUS *
Behaviors (B)EC */PUS *Behaviors (B)EC */PUS *
External criticism (EC)CE/B */PUS *External criticism (EC)CE/B */PUS *
Pretty-ugly stereotype (PUS)CE */B */EC *Pretty-ugly stereotype (PUS)CE */B */EC *
BES—Before FastingBES—After Fasting
ANOVA (N = 15, df = 2) = 5.1481, p = 0.0762;
Coefficient of Compliance = 0.1716,
r Mean Rank = 0.1124
ANOVA (N = 15, df = 3) = 37.1600, p = 0.00001;
Coefficient of Concordance = 0.8257, r Mean Rank = 0.8133
Physical attractiveness (PA)BSPhysical attractiveness (PA)Lack of differences
Body strength (BS)Lack of differencesBody strength (BS)Lack of differences
Physical condition (PC)Lack of differencesPhysical condition (PC)Lack of differences
Note: Differences statistically significant for p < 0.05. * indicates p < 0.001. The symbols used in the table correspond to the subsequent scales of BIQ and BES, respectively: CE—Cognition-emotions, B—Behaviors, EC—External criticism, PUS—Pretty-ugly stereotype, PA—Physical attractiveness, BS—Body strength, PC—Physical condition.
Table 8. Use and evaluation of the effectiveness of the weight control.
Table 8. Use and evaluation of the effectiveness of the weight control.
Using Different Ways to Control Body WeightBefore
Fasting
RankTotal RanksAfter
Fasting
RankTotal RanksTZp
a. Limiting the amount of food5.571 ± 2.246.214387.006.067 ± 2.436.366795.5016.501.1210.262
b. Elimination of sugar6.800 ± 2.576.892996.507.400 ± 1.686.9667104.5013.001.4780.139
c. Eliminating fatty foods6.267 ± 2.606.428690.006.333 ± 2.356.333395.0020.500.2370.813
d. Eliminating meat6.267 ± 3.066.500091.006.533 ± 2.616.266794.0012.500.7700.441
e. Restrictive diet4.000 ± 2.655.214373.004.867 ± 2.884.933374.0013.501.4270.154
f. Fasting6.000 ± 2.986.428690.005.867 ± 2.646.200093.0025.500.2040.838
g. Appetite suppressants1.333 ± 1.292.321432.502.000 ± 2.482.833342.501.001.0690.285
h. Laxatives1.400 ± 0.002.107129.501.667 ± 1.552.433336.5012.000.3380.735
i. Provoking vomiting1.000 ± 0.632.892940.501.400 ± 1.402.666740.00---
Evaluation of the Effectiveness of
Different Weight Control Methods
Before
Fasting
RankTotal RanksAfter FastingRankTotal RanksTZp
a. Limiting the amount of food8.000 ± 1.136.9667104.507.533 ± 2.007.035798.5031.5000.5880.556
b. Elimination of sugar7.929 ± 1.007.1333107.007.643 ± 1.457.035798.5011.5000.9100.362
c. Eliminating fatty foods6.500 ± 1.915.600084.007.643 ± 1.455.285774.0033.0000.8730.382
d. Eliminating meat6.067 ± 2.375.100076.505.933 ± 2.344.964369.5030.0000.2660.789
e. Restrictive diet5.800 ± 2.734.933374.005.800 ± 2.144.785767.0031.0000.1770.858
f. Fasting8.200 ± 1.427.2667109.007.600 ± 2.037.071499.006.0000.9430.345
g. Appetite suppressants3.733 ± 1.712.800042.003.733 ± 1.873.071443.005.0000.0001.000
h. Use of laxatives3.800 ± 2.272.200033.003.800 ± 1.782.714338.0010.5000.0001.000
i. Provoking vomiting2.867 ± 2.073.000045.003.533 ± 1.883.035742.502.0001.4830.138
Table 9. Use of different weight control methods and evaluation of their effectiveness-comparisons between variables measured before and after fasting with Friedman’s ANOVA and Kendall’s Chi squared analysis.
Table 9. Use of different weight control methods and evaluation of their effectiveness-comparisons between variables measured before and after fasting with Friedman’s ANOVA and Kendall’s Chi squared analysis.
Use ofEvaluation of Effectiveness
Weight Control MethodsBefore FastingAfter FastingBefore FastingAfter Fasting
Limiting the amount of food (LAF)AS/UL/PVRD/AS/UL/PVEM/AS */UL */PV *EFF/EM/RD/AS */PV */UL *
Elimination of sugar (ES)RD/F/AS/hRD/AS/UL/PVEFF/EM/RD/gRD/AS */UL */PV *
Eliminating fatty foods (EFF)AS/UL/PVRD/ASUL/PVES/AS/UL/PVAS/UL/PV
Eliminating meat (EM)EM/AS/UL/PVAS/UL/PV *LAF/ES/F/AS/UL/PVLAF/PV
Restrictive diet (RD)AS/UL/PVF/AS/UL/PVES/F/PVF/UL/PV
Fasting (F)ES/AS/UL/PVRD/AS/UL/PVF/AS */UL */PV *AS/UL */PV *
Appetite suppressants (AS)LAF/ES/EFF/EM/RD/FLAF/ES/EFF/EM/RD/FLAF/ES/EFF/EM /F */PVLAF/ES */EFF/F
Use of laxatives (UL)LAF/ES/EFF/EM/RD/FLAF/ES/EFF/EM/RD/FLAF/EFF/EM/FLAF */ES */EFF/RD/F
Provoking vomiting (PV)LAF/EFF/EM/RD/FLAF/ES/EFF/EM/RD/FLAF/EFF/EM/RD/F */ASLAF */ES */EFF/EM/RD/F
ANOVA (N = 15. df = 8) = 65.16402, p = 0.00001
Coefficient of compliance = 0.5818, r mean rank = 0.5496
ANOVA (N = 15. df = 8) = 60.9051, p =0.00001; concordance coefficient = 0.5075, r mean rank = 0.4723ANOVA (N = 15, df = 8) = 69.8352, p = 0.00001;
coefficient of compliance = 0.5819, r mean rank = 0.5521
ANOVA (N = 15, df = 8) = 5.4217, p =0.00001;
coefficient of compliance = 0.4859, r mean rank = 0.4463
Note: Differences statistically significant for p < 0.05. * indicates p < 0.001. The symbols used in the table correspond to the subsequent weight control methods as follows: LAF—limiting the amount of food; ES—elimination of sugar; EFF—eliminating fatty foods; ED—eliminating meat; RD—restrictive diet; F—fasting; AS—appetite suppressants; UL—use of laxatives; PV—provoking vomiting.
Table 10. Correlations between satisfaction with appearance and body parameters and selected physiological parameters measured at rest and post-exercise.
Table 10. Correlations between satisfaction with appearance and body parameters and selected physiological parameters measured at rest and post-exercise.
RestBefore FastingAfter Fasting
BF [%]BF [kg]FFM [%]FFM [kg]TBW [%]TBW [kg]BMI [kg/m2]BF [%]BF [kg]FFM [%]FFM [kg]TBW [%]TBW [kg]BMI [kg/m2]
Face0.0390.024−0.042−0.103−0.042−0.103−0.143−0.305−0.3290.305−0.3100.30−0.310−0.363
Chest0.2130.136−0.190−0.297−0.190−0.297−0.116−0.087−0.1990.087−0.4010.09−0.401−0.325
Arms0.0720.062−0.060−0.236−0.060−0.236−0.007−0.224−0.3400.224−0.4940.22−0.494−0.322
Waist0.1760.099−0.155−0.321−0.155−0.321−0.200−0.126−0.1460.126−0.1500.13−0.150−0.322
Belly0.2020.136−0.182−0.236−0.182−0.236−0.173−0.153−0.1950.153−0.2090.15−0.209−0.355
Thighs−0.114−0.0930.125−0.2050.125−0.205−0.1340.051−0.007−0.051−0.124−0.05−0.124−0.136
Legs0.3780.383−0.3720.116−0.3720.1160.258−0.087−0.0570.087−0.0090.09−0.009−0.066
Height0.3090.325−0.3070.070−0.3070.0700.079−0.295−0.3310.295−0.3460.30−0.346−0.471
Weight0.2370.132−0.214−0.373−0.214−0.373−0.243−0.430−0.4860.430−0.5700.43−0.570−0.652
ExerciseBefore FastingAfter Fasting
BF [%]BF [kg]FFM [%]FFM [kg]TBW [%]TBW [kg]BMI [kg/m2]BF [%]BF [kg]FFM [%]FFM [kg]TBW [%]TBW [kg]BMI [kg/m2]
Face0.0290.0560.129−0.0700.129−0.070−0.117−0.335−0.3330.274−0.3350.183−0.335−0.354
Chest0.1010.0870.095−0.3270.095−0.327−0.143−0.117−0.2180.195−0.4280.202−0.428−0.316
Arms−0.0200.0430.307−0.2510.307−0.251−0.005−0.252−0.3400.381−0.5010.409−0.501−0.312
Waist0.1320.075−0.051−0.291−0.051−0.291−0.188−0.155−0.139−0.072−0.177−0.090−0.172−0.311
Belly0.1730.132−0.014−0.208−0.014−0.208−0.163−0.186−0.1970.019−0.242−0.005−0.237−0.347
Thighs−0.190−0.087−0.046−0.205−0.046−0.205−0.1360.032−0.0180.163−0.1450.161−0.135−0.116
Legs0.2690.337−0.0620.080−0.0620.0800.212−0.104−0.0810.197−0.0340.147−0.026−0.049
Height0.3010.3690.0320.1360.0320.1360.111−0.325−0.2930.173−0.3730.132−0.373−0.462
Weight0.1480.0220.182−0.4070.182−0.407−0.278−0.452−0.4500.274−0.5970.281−0.591−0.646
Correlations statistically significant at p < 0.05.
Table 11. Correlations between body image dimensions and selected physiological parameters measured at rest and after exercise.
Table 11. Correlations between body image dimensions and selected physiological parameters measured at rest and after exercise.
Before FastingAfter Fasting
Cognition-EmotionBehaviorsExternal CriticismPretty-Ugly StereotypePhysical AttractivenessBody StrengthPhysical ConditionCognition-EmotionBehaviorsExternal CriticismPretty-Ugly StereotypePhysical AttractivenessBody StrengthPhysical Condition
Rest
BF [%]0.261−0.1370.284−0.146−0.315−0.096−0.1030.2150.0000.133−0.084−0.0210.241−0.132
BF [kg]0.304−0.0790.320−0.197−0.343−0.100−0.1350.262−0.0540.191−0.027−0.0110.189−0.082
FFM [%]−0.2740.115−0.2870.1480.3340.1180.128−0.2150.000−0.1330.0840.021−0.2410.132
FFM [kg]0.4440.2440.403−0.207−0.472−0.172−0.2340.3300.0220.250−0.122−0.0860.036−0.114
TBW [%]−0.2740.115−0.2870.1480.3340.1180.128−0.2150.000−0.1330.0840.021−0.2410.132
TBW [kg]0.4440.2440.403−0.207−0.472−0.172−0.2340.3300.0220.250−0.122−0.0860.036−0.114
BMI [kg/m2]0.6390.1890.6430.016−0.492−0.305−0.2550.3760.0290.2110.118−0.1020.075−0.143
Exercise
BF [%]0.289−0.1470.302−0.128−0.433−0.234−0.2410.2540.0110.179−0.070−0.0430.205−0.154
BF [kg]0.295−0.1420.326−0.179−0.401−0.176−0.2230.2440.0040.202−0.081−0.0160.169−0.141
FFM [%]−0.825−0.1590.004−0.2310.138−0.084−0.0160.057−0.022−0.0110.145−0.313−0.4560.047
FFM [kg]0.4200.1680.418−0.231−0.523−0.270−0.3330.3330.0650.251−0.061−0.0970.041−0.143
TBW [%]−0.1590.004−0.2310.3260.138−0.084−0.0160.057−0.022−0.0110.145−0.313−0.4560.047
TBW [kg]0.4200.1680.418−0.231−0.523−0.270−0.3330.3440.0580.273−0.077−0.1060.025−0.141
BMI [kg/m2]0.6150.1150.6250.027−0.514−0.366−0.3250.3710.0110.2120.104−0.0880.083−0.130
Correlations statistically significant at p < 0.05.
Table 12. Correlations between body image indices and satisfaction with appearance and body parameters.
Table 12. Correlations between body image indices and satisfaction with appearance and body parameters.
Before FastingAfter Fasting
BIQBESBIQBES
Body Parts and ParametersCognition-EmotionBehaviorsExternal Criticism Pretty-Ugly StereotypePhysical AttractivenessBody StrengthPhysical ConditionCognition-EmotionBehaviorsExternal Criticism Pretty-Ugly StereotypePhysical AttractivenessBody StrengthPhysical Condition
Face−0.661−0.441−0.747−0.6800.4060.3330.301−0.554−0.268−0.553−0.5060.6380.2440.550
Chest−0.558−0.563−0.342−0.3330.6330.7490.679−0.571−0.643−0.417−0.4560.4690.5940.472
Arms−0.186−0.143−0.108−0.2090.7030.5290.527−0.298−0.316−0.288−0.3590.4330.2860.384
Waist−0.645−0.595−0.483−0.5720.5080.5950.641−0.403−0.314−0.318−0.4940.3840.4180.714
Belly−0.704−0.691−0.559−0.5890.4710.6350.638−0.516−0.450−0.419−0.5240.3750.4840.673
Thighs−0.385−0.309−0.347−0.4190.7900.6860.684−0.410−0.684−0.207−0.5660.4830.5790.403
Legs−0.270−0.219−0.155−0.4400.4150.4570.336−0.152−0.494−0.197−0.2660.5370.5490.568
Height−0.569−0.505−0.545−0.6550.3180.3960.248−0.856−0.484−0.657−0.5150.4240.4060.308
Weight−0.485−0.373−0.330−0.3780.4080.4740.498−0.601−0.301−0.346−0.4170.3580.2840.355
Correlations statistically significant at p < 0.05.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Głębocka, A.; Pilis, W.; Żak, A.; Pilis, A.; Pilis, K. Relationships Between Body Image and Body Parameters in Men Under Long-Term Fasting Conditions. Nutrients 2025, 17, 1023. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17061023

AMA Style

Głębocka A, Pilis W, Żak A, Pilis A, Pilis K. Relationships Between Body Image and Body Parameters in Men Under Long-Term Fasting Conditions. Nutrients. 2025; 17(6):1023. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17061023

Chicago/Turabian Style

Głębocka, Alicja, Wiesław Pilis, Alicja Żak, Anna Pilis, and Karol Pilis. 2025. "Relationships Between Body Image and Body Parameters in Men Under Long-Term Fasting Conditions" Nutrients 17, no. 6: 1023. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17061023

APA Style

Głębocka, A., Pilis, W., Żak, A., Pilis, A., & Pilis, K. (2025). Relationships Between Body Image and Body Parameters in Men Under Long-Term Fasting Conditions. Nutrients, 17(6), 1023. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu17061023

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop