Next Article in Journal
A Lombard Variety of Sweet Pepper Regulating Senescence and Proliferation: The Voghera Pepper
Previous Article in Journal
Simplification of Dietary Treatment in Pharmacoresistant Epilepsy: Impact of C8 and C10 Fatty Acids on Sirtuins of Neuronal Cells In Vitro
Previous Article in Special Issue
The Combined Effects of High-Intensity Interval Exercise Training and Dietary Supplementation on Reduction of Body Fat in Adults with Overweight and Obesity: A Systematic Review
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Effects of High-Intensity Interval Training and Moderate Alcohol Consumption on Cognitive Performance—A Multidisciplinary Intervention in Young Healthy Adults

by
Cristina Molina-Hidalgo
1,2,*,
Francisco J. Amaro-Gahete
1,3,4,
Jamie C. Peven
5,
Kirk I. Erickson
2,6,
Andres Catena
7 and
Manuel J. Castillo
1
1
Department of Medical Physiology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Granada, 18001 Granada, Spain
2
AdventHealth Research Institute, Neuroscience Institute, Orlando, FL 32804, USA
3
CIBER de Fisiopatología de la Obesidad y Nutrición (CIBEROBN), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, 28029 Granada, Spain
4
Instituto de Investigación Biosanitaria, Ibs.Granada, 18012 Granada, Spain
5
Behavioral Health Service Line, VA Pittsburgh Healthcare System, Pittsburgh, PA 15240, USA
6
Department of Psychology, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA 15260, USA
7
Department of Experimental Psychology, Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center (CIMCYC), University of Granada, 18011 Granada, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2024, 16(11), 1680; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16111680
Submission received: 2 May 2024 / Revised: 25 May 2024 / Accepted: 28 May 2024 / Published: 29 May 2024

Abstract

:
Background. The main purpose of this study was to determine the effects of a high-intensity interval training (HIIT) intervention in the context of moderate alcohol consumption on cognitive performance in healthy young adults. Methods. We conducted a 10-week HIIT program along with four types of beverages with/without alcohol content. A total of 75 healthy adults (18–40 years old; 46% female) were allocated to either a control Non-Training group or an HIIT program group (2 days/week). Using block randomization, participants in the HIIT group were further allocated to an HIIT-Alcohol group (alcohol beer or sparkling water with vodka added, 5.4%) or an HIIT-NonAlcohol group (sparkling water or non-alcohol beer, 0.0%). The control group was instructed to maintain an active lifestyle but did not undergo any regular training. A comprehensive neuropsychological battery was used to evaluate cognitive performance (i.e., memory, working memory, processing speed, inhibitory control, and verbal fluency). Changes from baseline to week 10 were included in the main analyses. Results. All groups improved in all neuropsychological measures (all p ≤ 0.001), independent of sex and alcohol consumption, with no statistical differences between groups (all p > 0.05). Furthermore, larger increases in maximal oxygen uptake were associated with greater improvements in processing speed, inhibitory control, and verbal fluency (all p < 0.050). Conclusions. Although the improvements found in cognitive performance cannot be attributed to the HIIT intervention, no significant impairments in cognitive functions were noted due to moderate alcohol intake. Furthermore, our results confirmed that exercise-induced physical fitness improvements were associated with cognitive performance enhancements in young healthy adults.

1. Introduction

Exercise training is a powerful approach for improving cognitive function in both children and older adults [1,2,3]. In fact, it has been reported that cognitive function in the elderly is maintained or even improved following moderate-intensity exercise for at least 6 months [4]. Similarly, enhancements of visual–spatial and short-term memory have been reported after 6 to 12 weeks of programmed exercise in young adults [5,6]. A recent study has suggested that repeated high-intensity interval exercise (i.e., high-intensity interval training (HIIT)) could provide additional physiological and psychological adaptations in higher-order cognitive functions such as cognitive flexibility compared with lower-intensity exercise in older adults [7]. Similar improvements in cognitive flexibility have been found after a 7-week interval training intervention in young active individuals [8]. Complementary research indicates that an acute bout of interval exercise training can also positively impact cognitive function in healthy middle-aged individuals [9]. Nevertheless, there is still controversy regarding the optimal intensity of exercise training to improve cognitive performance in young adults [10]. One obvious reason for this absence of scientific literature is that cognitive health peaks during young adulthood, suggesting there is limited space for exercise-related cognitive improvement during this period [2].
Lifestyle habits, such as alcohol consumption, have been considered to play an important role in the development of dementia and other neurodegenerative diseases [11]. Alcohol intake, especially beer, is a common practice for many physically active people [12,13]. In addition to the deleterious effects of excessive consumption of alcohol on general health [11], there have been mixed results regarding the effects of low-to-moderate alcohol consumption on cognitive performance [14]. While Mehlig et al. suggested that low-to-moderate alcohol consumption increases the risk of developing cognitive impairments [15], other studies have found negligible effects associated with alcohol consumption [16,17]. Additionally, several studies have found significant improvements in cognitive function associated with low-to-moderate alcohol intake [18,19]. Due to the high heterogeneity of the above-mentioned results, investigating whether moderate alcohol consumption could influence the potential positive effects of exercise training on cognitive function is of scientific and clinical interest. To the best of our knowledge, no study has explored the combination of an HIIT intervention and moderate alcohol consumption in healthy individuals, which is common in a social context for active people.
The primary purpose of the present study was to investigate whether a highly demanding training intervention would improve cognitive function (i.e., memory, working memory, processing speed, inhibitory control, and verbal fluency) in healthy young adults, and whether those potential positive effects may be influenced by moderate alcohol consumption or by exercise-induced changes in other health-related parameters (cardiorespiratory fitness assessed by maximal oxygen uptake (VO2max), muscular strength determined by the handgrip test, and body composition). We predicted that concurrent regular alcohol intake, even in moderate amounts, could blunt any of the positive effects of training. Furthermore, we hypothesized that higher fitness levels would be associated with greater cognitive performance in young healthy adults.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Trial Design

The BEER-HIIT study is a registered controlled trial (ClinicalTrials.gov ID: NCT03660579) with a follow-up of 10 weeks. The procedures were designed in accordance with the last revised Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee on Human Research of the University of Granada (321/CEIH/2017).

2.2. Participants

Eligible participants of the BEER-HIIT project were healthy young adults who lived in the province of Granada, Spain. The study was announced via social networks, local media, and posters. Prior to enrolment, all individuals provided written informed consent, completed a medical examination, and were fully informed about the study objectives, design, inclusion criteria, assessments to be undertaken, exercise program intervention, and types of beverages to be ingested. Subjects who met the inclusion criteria (i.e., (i) having a body mass index (BMI) from 18.5 to 30 kg/m2, (ii) not being engaged in a previous structured training program or a weight-loss program (in the last 5 months), (iii) having a stable body weight during the last 5 months (body weight changes < 3 kg), (iv) being free of disease, (v) not being pregnant or lactating, (vi) not taking any medication for chronic diseases, and (vii) not suffering pain, recent injuries, or other problems preventing strenuous physical activity) were invited to an information meeting in which the research staff gave specific information about healthy dietary patterns and the physical activity recommendations provided by the World Health Organization [20].

2.3. Randomization and Follow-Up

After the baseline measurements, a total of 83 individuals were allocated to a training (i.e., HIIT) or a non-training control group based on personal preferences. Participants in the control group were instructed to maintain their usual physical activity levels and not engage in a structured exercise program. Those participants included in the training group were subsequently allocated to an ethanol-containing beverage (i.e., 5.4% alcohol) group or an alcohol-free beverage group. The participants willing to consume ethanol were randomly allocated to either a group consuming alcohol beer (HIIT-Beer) or to a group consuming sparkling water with added vodka ethanol (HIIT-Alcohol). The participants choosing non-alcoholic beverages were randomly allocated to either an alcohol-free beer group (HIIT-0.0Beer) or a sparkling water group (HIIT-Water). This type of non-random (i.e., based on individual preference) and random allocation of the participants was conducted following ethical considerations and advice provided by the ethical committee (321-CEIH-2017), since drinking alcohol or participating in a highly demanding training program should be a personal choice.

2.4. Intervention

The HIIT intervention consisted of 2 sessions/week performed from Monday to Friday over 10 consecutive weeks with at least 48 h of recovery between sessions. The training intervention was divided into two different phases, starting with a familiarization phase to learn the main movement patterns, aiming to avoid injuries or potential dropouts. The volume and intensity of the sessions in the familiarization phase were fixed at 40 min/week and 8–9 Rating of Perceived Exertion (0–10 RPE), respectively [21,22]. Subsequent increments in both volume and intensity were established in Phase I (50 min/week and 10 RPE) and in Phase II (65 min/week and 10 RPE). Eight self-loading exercises were performed in a circuit form twice per set (i.e., frontal plank, high knees up, TRX horizontal row, squat, deadlift, side plank, push up, and burpees) with a passive rest between exercises and an active rest between sets (i.e., 6 RPE intensity, which corresponds to 60% VO2max) [22,23]. A dynamic standardized warm-up and an active global-stretching cooling-down protocol were completed at the beginning and the end of each training session, respectively. A detailed description of each exercise of the training program can be found elsewhere [24].
During the intervention, the alcohol consumption allowed was 330 mL of the respective beverage at lunch and 330 mL at dinner for men, and 330 mL at dinner for women, from Monday to Friday: (i) the HIIT-Alcohol group ingested randomly assigned alcohol beer (5.4% alcohol—Alhambra Especial®, Granada, Spain) or sparkling water with the exact equivalent amount of distilled alcohol added (vodka, 37.5% ethanol and 62.5% water), and (ii) the HIIT-NonAlcohol group was randomly assigned to ingest alcohol-free beer (0.0% alcohol—Cruzcampo®, Sevilla, Spain) or sparkling water (Eliqua 2®, Font Salem, Valencia, Spain). The amount of alcohol selected to ingest was based on scientific evidence (i.e., 2–3 drinks/day or 24–36 g of ethanol/day for men and 1–2 drinks/day or 12–24 g of ethanol/day for women) [25,26]. During weekends, participants were requested to respect the beverage intake condition (i.e., moderate alcohol consumption and non-alcohol consumption). All beverages were coded and provided by a blinded staff member of our research laboratory at the beginning of each week. Additionally, they were asked to report, before and after the 10-week intervention program, their usual frequency of alcohol intake through seven possible response categories using the Beverage Intake Questionnaire (BEVQ) [27,28]. Similarly, dietary habits were assessed using the MEDAS questionnaire [29].

2.5. Cognitive Function

Cognitive variables were taken at baseline and after 10 weeks of the supervised HIIT program. We used the Spanish Complutense Verbal Learning Test (TAVEC) to evaluate episodic memory, as well as its codification processes, data storage, and retrieval [30]. The main outcomes of this test were as follows: (i) learning process (defined as the sum of correctly recalled words across all five learning trials); (ii) short-term memory (free recall of list A after an interference list was presented); (iii) delay memory (free recall of list A after a 20 min rest period); and (iv) recognition (defined as total correct score (tc)) and discriminability index (calculated as the difference between correctly detected words from list A minus false alarms to new words (id)). Working memory was assessed using the Letter–Number Sequencing test, according to the instructions of the WAIS-IV manual [31], where the span score ranges from 0 to 7 and total scores range from 0 to 21. We measured processing speed and inhibitory control using the one-page paper-and-pencil cancelation test version of the D2 test [32]. In addition, we used a standard verbal fluency test to measure phonemic, semantic, and total verbal fluency [33]. Higher scores indicate better cognitive performance on these tasks. Comprehensive information on the cognitive measurements can be found in the study protocol [24].
Age, sex, and occupational activity were also registered by a self-report demographic questionnaire before the intervention.

2.6. Statistical Analyses

These results presented in the current study were not the main outcomes of the BEER-HIIT project [34,35].
Our sample size calculations revealed that 13 participants per group were needed to detect an effect size of 0.25 in memory scores with an α error of 0.05 and a power of 0.85 [36]. However, a minimum of 16 participants per group (a total of 80) were recruited, allowing us to consider a maximum loss of 20% at the follow-up [37].
Standard statistical methods were used for the calculation of means and standard deviations. Normal Gaussian distribution of the data was verified by the Shapiro–Wilk test and visual check of histograms, Q–Q, and box plots. Homoscedasticity was verified by the modified Levene test. The compound symmetry, or sphericity, was checked by the Mauchly test. When the assumption of sphericity was not met, the significance of F-ratios was adjusted according to the Greenhouse–Geisser procedure when the epsilon correction factor was <0.75, or according to the Huyn–Feld procedure when the epsilon correction factor was >0.75.
Composite scores for memory (immediate, short-term, delayed recall, and recognition scores; α = 0.900), working memory (direct and processing scores; α = 0.922), processing speed (total productivity, correct work, and concentration index scores; α = 0.986), inhibitory control (total effectiveness score), and verbal fluency (total score) were calculated by averaging z scores for their individual components. Recategorization of the occupational activity measure was performed into three levels, showing a relatively high internal consistency (α = 0.705).
Age, sex, and occupational activity were used as potential confounders in the analyses. We combined the responses of computed and classified occupational activities and categorized them as level 1 (unemployment, homemaker, and student), level 2 (primary sector services, retail or catering services, and administrative clerk), and level 3 (support and scientific/intellectual technicians and professionals, and business administration and management). Given that we did not observe a sex interaction, we conducted the analysis including males and females together. Similarly, since no beverage interaction was observed between groups in any outcome, we analyzed both alcohol beer and sparkling water with ethanol HIIT groups in the same group (i.e., HIIT-Alcohol group) and both 0.0% alcohol beer and sparkling water HIIT groups in the same group (i.e., HIIT-NonAlcohol group).
A repeated-measures analysis of variance (2 × 2 ANOVA; time × training group; and time × beverage) tested group differences over time. Post hoc Bonferroni corrections were conducted for multiple comparisons. The significance level was set at p < 0.05 for all analyses. Finally, simple and multiple linear regression analyses were performed to examine the associations between changes in physical fitness (i.e., maximum oxygen uptake, hand grip strength, and body composition, such as BMI, FMI, and LMI) with changes in cognitive outcomes (i.e., memory, working memory, processing speed, inhibitory control, and verbal fluency) after the intervention. Age, sex, and occupational activity were included as potential confounders, which were selected based on statistical procedures (i.e., hierarchical regressions) and theoretical bases.
All analyses were conducted using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS, v. 25.0, IBM SPSS Statistics, IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA). Graphical presentations were prepared using GraphPad Prism 8 (GraphPad Software, San Diego, CA, USA).

3. Results

A total of 74 participants (34 women) were included in the final analyses after a loss to follow-up of 10% (see Figure 1). The characteristics of the participants are presented in Table 1. There were no significant differences between groups at baseline (all p > 0.05), except for age and occupational activity (p = 0.006 and p = 0.005, respectively; see Table 1). Our HIIT intervention has previously been shown to successfully improve physical fitness parameters, including absolute and relative VO2max [35], as well as body composition parameters [34].

3.1. Intervention Effects of an HIIT Program on Cognitive Function

Figure 2 shows changes in learning (number of recalled words across all five trials) tested at baseline and post-intervention. A significant increase in the number of recalled words across trials at baseline and post-intervention measures was observed independently of the intervention group (all p ≤ 0.001; see Figure 2). Significant differences in learning were also noted between baseline and post-intervention measures in the Non-Training group, HIIT-Alcohol group, and HIIT-NonAlcohol group (all p ≤ 0.001; see Figure 2).
Table 2 presents changes in the raw scores and z-transformed cognitive outcomes before and after the intervention. The z-scores are interpreted as the change from baseline in standard deviations. The Non-Training, HIIT-Alcohol, and HIIT-NonAlcohol groups all showed significant improvements in memory (i.e., immediate, short-term, and delayed recall scores) and recognition (all p ≤ 0.001; see Table 2) after the intervention. Similarly, all groups showed significant enhancements in processing speed, inhibitory control, and verbal fluency (all p ≤ 0.01; see Table 2). There were no within-group differences in memory, working memory, processing speed, inhibitory control, or verbal fluency z-scores (all p > 0.05; see Table 2). The ANCOVA of raw scores and z-transformed cognitive outcomes, adjusting for baseline values (Model 0), showed no significant between-group differences in any cognitive function outcomes (all p > 0.05; see Table 2). These results persisted after controlling for potential confounders (i.e., age, sex, and occupational activity; see Table A1).

3.2. Are Exercise-Induced Changes in Cognitive Function Explained by Those Obtained in Physical Fitness and Body Composition after the Intervention?

Higher increases in relative VO2max (mL/kg/min) over the course of the intervention were associated with greater improvements in processing speed, inhibitory control, and verbal fluency (all β > 0.249, all p values < 0.034). These statistically significant relationships persisted after adjusting for sex, age, and occupational activity (see Figure 3). Finally, changes in verbal fluency were positively related to changes in absolute VO2max (mL/min) (β = 0.313, R2 = 0.085, p = 0.007; see Table A3), which remained after adjusting for sex, age, and occupational activity (all p ≤ 0.013; see Table A3). Additionally, higher BMI levels were associated with poorer working memory (composite score) (β = −0.282, R2 = 0.066, p = 0.016; see Figure 3D), which also persisted after adjusting for sex, age, and occupational activity (all p ≤ 0.019; see Figure 3D). No significant associations were observed between changes in cognitive outcomes and changes in hand grip strength, fat mass index, or lean mass index (see Table A2 and Table A3).

4. Discussion

The main aim of this study was to investigate whether a high-intensity training intervention would improve cognitive function (i.e., memory, working memory, processing speed, inhibitory control, and verbal fluency) and whether those potential positive effects would be influenced by regular moderate alcohol consumption in healthy young adults. Additionally, we examined whether improvements in cognitive performance were related to other positive health-related parameters (e.g., VO2max and body composition). Our results did not completely support the hypothesis that the HIIT intervention improved learning, memory, processing speed, inhibitory control, and verbal fluency, since the Non-Training group exhibited similar enhancements in these parameters. However, interestingly, our findings suggest that regular moderate alcohol consumption for this duration does not negatively influence cognitive function. Additionally, in line with our hypothesis, the current findings showed that physical fitness improvements after our 10-week HIIT intervention were associated with better cognitive performance in young healthy adults.

4.1. Intervention Effects of an HIIT Program on Cognitive Function

Previous research has demonstrated that there is moderate evidence supporting the notion that physical activity, in the form of aerobic exercise, benefits cognitive functioning during early and late periods of life [1,3,38]. Regarding a high-intensity exercise program, prior studies have found that short-term HIIT resulted in meaningful improvements in reaction time and cognitive flexibility in older adults [7], and in learning performance in healthy, active male students [39]. These enhancements have been attributed to physiological processes that occur during exercise, including an increase in catecholamines (i.e., dopamine, epinephrine, and norepinephrine) and release of neurotrophic factors [39]. Although these results are encouraging, there are still major gaps in our understanding of the effects of an HIIT intervention on cognitive performance in young to middle-aged adults (aged 18–50 years) [1,3]. In this regard, although cognitive improvements were found in all cognitive domains measured (i.e., learning, memory, processing speed, inhibitory control, and verbal fluency), we cannot conclude that those benefits were a consequence of our HIIT intervention since the Non-Training control group showed similar performance improvements. These findings are somewhat consistent with those obtained by similar investigations applying HIIT interventions that reported no exercise-induced changes in working memory after 7–12 weeks of an HIIT program in healthy middle-aged individuals [8,9,40]. Hence, an important question remains unanswered regarding the inconsistent findings in cognitive performance improvements following exercise interventions among young and middle-aged adults. One reason for this heterogeneity could be that cognitive health peaks during young adulthood, which would suggest a ceiling effect for exercise-related improvements to cognitive function during this period of life [2]. Other reasons that should be considered may be variability in methodological and study design factors, such as (i) the general health and fitness level of participants being enrolled, (ii) the neuropsychological tests used to measure aspects of cognition, (iii) the lack of consistent reporting of blinding and adherence/compliance, or (iv) the nature of control groups. Indeed, many of the instruments used to assess executive functioning are traditional neuropsychological tools primarily developed to aid in clinical diagnosis rather than to assess individual variation in normative cognitive functioning; thus, their sensitivity to detect changes in response to an intervention (especially in the context of a normative sample) remains questionable [1].
In addition to its role in physical health, low-to-moderate alcohol consumption has been suggested as a potential risk factor in the development of cognitive impairment, which is also highly associated with cardiovascular diseases [18]. However, some inconsistencies can be observed across studies. Zhang et al. (2020) [18] hypothesized that the impact of alcohol drinking on cognitive function may be dependent on a balance of its beneficial and harmful effects on the cardiovascular system [18]. However, other studies have reported that moderate drinkers are more likely to have hippocampal atrophy [41] suggesting that, in line with other studies [14], there is no safe level of drinking when referring to health. These variable results could arise from the complicated and plural mechanisms through which alcohol consumption affects health [14], as well as the influence of an individual’s consumption volume and pattern of drinking [42]. Our findings suggest that moderate alcohol consumption did not impair cognitive function after 10 weeks in healthy young adults who engaged in vigorous exercise training.
In summary, there is still controversy regarding the effects of moderate-to-vigorous intensity exercise on cognition in young and middle-aged adults. Therefore, future studies are needed to better elucidate the potential benefits of exercise on cognition during young and middle adulthood [1]. Furthermore, the association of low-to-moderate drinking with cognitive function in the younger ages, as well as the mechanisms underlying this potential association, warrants additional investigation.

4.2. Are Exercise-Induced Changes in Cognitive Function Explained by Those Obtained in Physical Fitness and Body Composition after the Intervention?

Previous studies have confirmed that an enhancement of cardiovascular fitness is associated with changes in cognitive performance in children and older adults [1,2,3,43,44]. However, it is unclear precisely what function cardiovascular fitness might play in instantiating these cognitive changes [45]. Regarding the effects of physical activity on cognition and brain outcomes in young middle-aged adults (18–59 years old), there is a dearth of high-quality data available. Our results partially align with previously demonstrated evidence of a positive relationship between changes in cardiorespiratory fitness and improvements in cognitive functions (e.g., processing speed, inhibitory control, and verbal fluency) being found in a healthy young adult cohort. Although our effect sizes were relatively small, these findings suggest that cardiorespiratory fitness may impact challenging and complex cognitive processes, which has previously been demonstrated in other samples and age ranges [46]. However, future research is needed to bring to light whether exercise training (i.e., HIIT) can optimize cognitive function in young and middle-aged adults.
We found a negative association between BMI levels and working memory, which is consistent with some previously reported evidence from childhood [47] to adulthood [48]. Although specific cognitive deficits have been demonstrated in younger adults and older adults, the findings are inconsistent across age groups. Overall, the results found in the current study could extend our understanding of the possible neuropathological processes underlying obesity-related cognitive dysfunction, demonstrating a specific impact on predominant prefrontal cognitive processes.
In summary, our study has demonstrated a notable advantage to implementing a supervised high-intensity exercise intervention, as evidenced by an 80% adherence rate among participants, which resulted in significant improvements in both physical fitness and body composition [34,35].

4.3. Limitations

Although this study had several strengths, it also had several limitations that should be considered. Firstly, the control group was not purely sedentary since participants were instructed to maintain an active lifestyle. Although physical activity levels were monitored, they were determined using self-reported approaches. Objective methods (i.e., accelerometer systems) might result in different outcomes. Second, the BEER-HIIT project was designed from a quantitative approach that looks forward to modifying physical fitness and/or additional health-related outcomes. Thus, changes in cognitive functions are only expected as a result of physical fitness enhancements [10]. The third limitation was our sample size. Finally, participants were not fully randomized because of ethical considerations regarding alcohol consumption. Thus, a truly double-blind design, placebo-controlled for alcohol, was not possible. Subsequently, future studies are needed to determine the effects of the same training intervention on participants with different biological characteristics, using reliable, objective measurements to identify effective public health strategies that promote a healthy lifestyle where fermented beverages in moderate amounts co-ingested with meals could be included if wished and considered acceptable.

5. Conclusions

Although our 10-week HIIT exercise intervention failed to moderate the impact on cognitive performance, moderate alcohol intake at this level and for this duration did not seem to alter cognitive performance in young healthy adults. Furthermore, our results confirmed that exercise-induced changes in physical fitness were associated with improvements in cognitive performance in young healthy adults.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, M.J.C., C.M.-H. and F.J.A.-G.; methodology, C.M.-H., F.J.A.-G. and M.J.C.; formal analysis, C.M.-H., J.C.P. and K.I.E.; writing—original draft preparation, C.M.-H. and F.J.A.-G.; writing—review and editing, K.I.E., J.C.P., A.C. and M.J.C.; supervision, M.J.C., C.M.-H. and F.J.A.-G.; project administration, C.M.-H. and F.J.A.-G.; funding acquisition, M.J.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was partially funded by an unrestricted grant (EUR 29,870 of the Centro de Información Cerveza y Salud (CICS), Madrid, Spain.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) on Human Research of the University of Granada (321/CEIH/04.06.2017).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors on request.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all study participants for their time and effort. This study is part of Cristina Molina-Hidalgo’s Doctoral Thesis conducted in the Psychology Doctoral Studies of the University of Granada, Spain. We acknowledge Ana Yara Postigo-Fuentes for her assistance with the English language.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

Appendix A

Table A1. Changes in Cognitive measures adjusted by baseline values and age (Model 1), by baseline values and sex (Model 2), and by baseline values and occupational activity (Model 3).
Table A1. Changes in Cognitive measures adjusted by baseline values and age (Model 1), by baseline values and sex (Model 2), and by baseline values and occupational activity (Model 3).
ModelsModel 1Model 2Model 3
Analysis of Covariance
Fp Valueη2Fp Valueη2Fp Valueη2
Memory
Immediate memory0.7670.4680.0210.7900.4580.0220.7180.4910.020
Short-term memory0.3500.7060.0100.7680.4680.0210.6330.5340.018
Delay memory1.1080.3360.0311.8160.1700.0491.6890.1920.046
Recognition (tc)2.4380.0950.0651.8870.1600.0511.7240.1860.047
Recognition (id)0.8370.4370.0231.6530.1990.0451.3350.2700.037
Composite score0.7490.4770.0211.2920.2810.0361.0510.3550.029
Working Memory
Direct Score0.0840.9190.0020.1050.9010.0030.0640.9380.002
Processing Score0.4980.6100.0140.5010.6080.0140.4300.6520.012
Composite score0.1400.8700.0040.1620.8510.0050.1110.8950.003
Processing Speed
Total productivity0.5020.6080.0140.6450.5280.0180.7240.4890.020
Correct work0.3040.7390.0090.5700.5690.0160.6300.5360.018
Concentration Index0.3710.6910.0110.4800.6210.0140.7630.4700.021
Composite score0.4130.6630.0120.6370.5320.0180.7830.4610.022
Inhibitory Control
Omissions1.1090.3350.0311.3970.2540.0381.3300.2710.037
Total Effectiveness0.4060.6680.0110.6430.5290.0180.6030.5500.017
Verbal Fluency
Total Phonologic0.7710.4670.0221.0200.3660.0280.6910.5040.019
Total Semantic2.0980.1300.0572.4500.0940.0652.4590.0930.066
Total Score1.0670.3500.0301.2370.2960.0340.8860.4170.025
Abbreviations: tc, total number of correct responses in the recognition test; id, index of discriminability from learning list in the recognition test.

Appendix B

Table A2. Association of changes in VO2max and hand grip strength/weight changes with changes in Cognitive Composite.
Table A2. Association of changes in VO2max and hand grip strength/weight changes with changes in Cognitive Composite.
VO2max (mL/min)VO2max (mL/kg/min)Hand Grip StrengthHand Grip Strength/Weight
βR2pβR2pβR2pβR2p
Memory
Model 00.1350.0040.2560.1020.0040.3900.018−0.0140.880−0.010−0.0140.932
Model 10.133−0.0070.2630.100−0.0150.4050.016−0.0230.892−0.015−0.0240.899
Model 20.0630.0180.6120.0390.0150.7050.0000.0230.999−0.0110.0220.924
Model 30.0550.0150.6620.0310.0130.708−0.0150.0210.897−0.0340.0210.777
Working Memory
Model 0−0.1220.0010.302−0.021−0.0140.863−0.1740.0170.138−0.066−0.0100.579
Model 1−0.123−0.0120.303−0.022−0.0270.854−0.1750.0040.139−0.068−0.0240.571
Model 2−0.146−0.0230.254−0.029−0.0410.817−0.176−0.0100.140−0.069−0.0390.573
Model 3−0.149−0.0360.249−0.031−0.0560.806−0.185−0.0200.127−0.081−0.0500.515
Processing Speed
Model 00.1980.0250.0940.2730.0610.020−0.034−0.0130.775−0.047−0.0120.693
Model 10.1950.0230.0980.2680.0580.022−0.037−0.0120.755−0.056−0.0120.641
Model 20.2330.0200.0650.3000.0560.014−0.037−0.0270.758−0.056−0.0260.642
Model 30.2440.0260.0540.3010.0630.012−0.020−0.0250.871−0.033−0.0280.790
Inhibitory Control
Model 00.1760.0170.1370.2490.0490.034−0.006−0.0140.960−0.004−0.0140.971
Model 10.1740.0100.1410.2450.0400.037−0.008−0.0190.944−0.011−0.0210.927
Model 20.182−0.0040.1530.2540.0270.040−0.013−0.0300.916−0.010−0.0330.933
Model 30.190−0.0070.1370.2620.0260.0350.001−0.0340.9910.010−0.0380.936
Verbal Fluency
Model 00.3130.0850.0070.3560.1150.0020.077−0.0080.5140.072−0.0090.547
Model 10.3110.0870.0070.3510.1140.0020.074−0.0080.5320.064−0.0110.595
Model 20.3080.0740.0130.3460.1020.0040.069−0.0160.5630.065−0.0240.593
Model 30.3190.0800.0100.3560.1090.0030.084−0.0180.4840.084−0.0300.499
Analyses were controlled for age (Model 1); both age and sex (Model 2); and age, sex, and occupational activity (Model 3). Boldface values indicate significant differences (p < 0.05). β, standardized linear regression coefficient; R2, coefficient of determination, and p value were obtained from the linear regression analyses.

Appendix C

Table A3. Association of BMI, FMI, and LMI changes with Cognitive Composite changes.
Table A3. Association of BMI, FMI, and LMI changes with Cognitive Composite changes.
BMIFMILMI
βR2pβR2pβR2p
Memory
Model 00.004−0.0140.9700.1360.0050.251−0.1250.0020.293
Model 10.008−0.0240.9450.133−0.0060.263−0.118−0.0100.327
Model 20.0020.0260.9880.1360.0460.241−0.1300.0440.266
Model 30.0030.0230.9780.1390.0430.234−0.1380.0420.243
Working Memory
Model 0−0.2820.0660.016−0.2130.0320.070−0.059−0.0110.619
Model 1−0.2810.0530.017−0.2150.0190.070−0.057−0.0250.637
Model 2−0.2810.0390.018−0.2150.0050.072−0.057−0.0400.639
Model 3−0.2810.0260.019−0.214−0.0080.075−0.060−0.0530.626
Processing Speed
Model 00.112−0.0010.3440.058−0.0110.6260.038−0.0130.749
Model 10.1190.0000.3160.053−0.0120.6570.054−0.0120.652
Model 20.120−0.0140.3180.053−0.0260.6600.055−0.0260.648
Model 30.118−0.0140.3240.050−0.0260.6770.063−0.0250.602
Inhibitory Control
Model 00.065−0.0100.5870.050−0.0120.6720.031−0.0130.791
Model 10.070−0.0150.5590.047−0.0180.6970.044−0.0180.715
Model 20.069−0.0280.5690.047−0.0300.6950.042−0.0310.732
Model 30.067−0.0340.5770.045−0.0360.7100.048−0.0360.694
Verbal Fluency
Model 00.1850.0210.1180.1900.0220.108−0.090−0.0060.450
Model 10.1920.0230.1050.1850.0210.118−0.076−0.0090.527
Model 20.1900.0120.1090.1860.0110.118−0.079−0.0190.512
Model 30.1890.0060.1130.1830.0040.124−0.073−0.0260.545
Analyses were adjusted for age (Model 1); both age and sex (Model 2); and age, sex, and occupational activity (Model 3). Boldface values indicate significant differences (p < 0.05). β, standardized linear regression coefficient; R2, coefficient of determination, and p value were obtained from the linear regression analyses. Abbreviations: BMI, body mass index; FMI, fat mass index; LMI, lean mass index.

References

  1. Erickson, K.I.; Hillman, C.; Stillman, C.M.; Ballard, R.M.; Bloodgood, B.; Conroy, D.E.; Macko, R.; Marquez, D.X.; Petruzzello, S.J.; Powell, K.E. Physical Activity, Cognition, and Brain Outcomes: A Review of the 2018 Physical Activity Guidelines. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 2019, 51, 1242–1251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Hillman, C.H.; Erickson, K.I.; Kramer, A.F. Be Smart, Exercise Your Heart: Exercise Effects on Brain and Cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2008, 9, 58–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Stillman, C.M.; Esteban-Cornejo, I.; Brown, B.; Bender, C.M.; Erickson, K.I. Effects of Exercise on Brain and Cognition Across Age Groups and Health States. Trends Neurosci. 2020, 43, 533–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Erickson, K.I.; Gildengers, A.G.; Butters, M.A. Physical Activity and Brain Plasticity in Late Adulthood. Dialogues Clin. Neurosci. 2013, 15, 99–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Stroth, S.; Hille, K.; Spitzer, M.; Reinhardt, R. Aerobic Endurance Exercise Benefits Memory and Affect in Young Adults. Neuropsychol. Rehabil. 2009, 19, 223–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Pereira, A.C.; Huddleston, D.E.; Brickman, A.M.; Sosunov, A.A.; Hen, R.; McKhann, G.M.; Sloan, R.; Gage, F.H.; Brown, T.R.; Small, S.A. An in Vivo Correlate of Exercise-Induced Neurogenesis in the Adult Dentate Gyrus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2007, 104, 5638–5643. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Mekari, S.; Neyedli, H.F.; Fraser, S.; O’brien, M.W.; Martins, R.; Evans, K.; Earle, M.; Aucoin, R.; Chiekwe, J.; Hollohan, Q.; et al. High-Intensity Interval Training Improves Cognitive Flexibility in Older Adults. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 796. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Venckunas, T.; Snieckus, A.; Trinkunas, E.; Baranauskiene, N.; Solianik, R.; Juodsnukis, A.; Streckis, V.; Kamandulis, S. Interval Running Training Improves Cognitive Flexibility and Aerobic Power of Young Healthy Adults. J. Strength. Cond. Res. 2016, 30, 2114–2121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Alves, C.R.R.; Tessaro, V.H.; Teixeira, L.A.C.; Murakava, K.; Roschel, H.; Gualano, B.; Takito, M.Y. Influence of Acute High-Intensity Aerobic Interval Exercise Bout on Selective Attention and Short-Term Memory Tasks. Percept. Mot. Skills 2014, 118, 63–72. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Tomporowski, P.D.; Pesce, C. Exercise, Sports, and Performance Arts Benefit Cognition via a Common Process. Psychol. Bull. 2019, 145, 929–951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. De Gaetano, G.; Costanzo, S.; Di Castelnuovo, A.; Badimon, L.; Bejko, D.; Alkerwi, A.; Chiva-Blanch, G.; Estruch, R.; La Vecchia, C.; Panico, S.; et al. Effects of Moderate Beer Consumption on Health and Disease: A Consensus Document. Nutr. Metab. Cardiovasc. Dis. 2016, 26, 443–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Maughan, R.J. Alcohol and Football. In Nutrition and Football: The FIFA/FMARC Consensus on Sports Nutrition; Routledge: London, UK, 2006; pp. 121–133. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Zhou, J.; Heim, D. Sports and Spirits: A Systematic Qualitative Review of Emergent Theories for Student-Athlete Drinking. Alcohol Alcohol. 2014, 49, 604–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Bill, F.; Foundation, M.G. Articles Alcohol Use and Burden for 195 Countries and Territories, 1990–2016: A Systematic Analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet 2018, 392, 1015–1035. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mehlig, K.; Skoog, I.; Guo, X.; Schutze, M.; Gustafson, D.; Waern, M.; Ostling, S.; Bjorkelund, C.; Lissner, L. Alcoholic Beverages and Incidence of Dementia: 34-Year Follow-up of the Prospective Population Study of Women in Goteborg. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2007, 167, 684–691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Hebert, L.E.; Scherr, P.A.; Beckett, L.A.; Albert, M.S.; Rosner, B.; Talor, J.O.; Evans, D.A. Relation of Smoking and Low-to-Moderate Alcohol Consumption to Change in Cognitive Function: A Longitudinal Study in a Defined Community of Older Persons. Am. J. Epidemiol. 1993, 137, 881–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Elwood, P.C.; Gallacher, J.E.J.; Hopkinson, C.A.; Pickering, J.; Rabbitt, P.; Stollery, B.; Brayne, C.; Huppert, F.A.; Bayer, A. Smelting, Drinking, and Other Life Style Factors and Cognitive Function in Men in the Caerphilly Cohort. J. Epidemiol. Community Health (1978) 1999, 53, 9–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Zhang, R.; Shen, L.; Miles, T.; Shen, Y.; Cordero, J.; Qi, Y.; Liang, L.; Li, C. Association of Low to Moderate Alcohol Drinking with Cognitive Functions from Middle to Older Age Among US Adults. JAMA Netw. Open 2020, 3, e207922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Ganguli, M.; Vander Bilt, J.; Saxton, J.A.; Shen, C.; Dodge, H.H. Alcohol Consumption and Cognitive Function in Late Life: A Longitudinal Community Study. Neurology 2005, 65, 1210–1217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Samuelson, G. Global Strategy on Diet, Physical Activity and Health. Scand. J. Nutr. 2004, 48, 57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Gunnar, A.V. Borg Psychophysical Bases of Perceived Exertion. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc. 1982, 14, 377–381. [Google Scholar]
  22. Gillach, M.C.; Sallis, J.F.; Buono, M.J.; Patterson, P.; Nader, P.R. The Relationship Between Perceived Exertion and Heart Rate in Children and Adults. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 1989, 1, 360–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Lamb, K.L. Children’s Ratings of Effort during Cycle Ergometry: An Examination of the Validity of Two Effort Rating Scales. Pediatr. Exerc. Sci. 1995, 7, 407–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Molina-Hidalgo, C.; De-la-O, A.; Jurado-Fasoli LAmaro-Gahete, F.J.; Catena, A.; Castillo, M. Investigating the Alcohol Effects on the Response to Strenuous Exercise Training: Protocol for a Multidisciplinary Intervention Study in Young Healthy Adults—The BEER-HIIT Study. Subst. Use Addict. J. 2024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Jurado-Fasoli, L.; Amaro-Gahete, F.J.; De-la-O, A.; Gutiérrez, Á.; Castillo, M.J. Alcohol Consumption and S-Klotho Plasma Levels in Sedentary Healthy Middle-Aged Adults: A Cross Sectional Study. Drug Alcohol. Depend. 2019, 194, 107–111. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Poli, A.; Marangoni, F.; Avogaro, A.; Barba, G.; Bellentani, S.; Bucci, M.; Cambieri, R.; Catapano, A.L.; Costanzo, S.; Cricelli, C.; et al. Moderate Alcohol Use and Health: A Consensus Document. Nutr. Metab. Cardiovasc. Dis. 2013, 23, 487–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Hedrick, V.E.; Comber, D.L.; Estabrooks, P.A.; Savla, J.; Davy, B.M. The Beverage Intake Questionnaire: Determining Initial Validity and Reliability. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 2010, 110, 1227–1232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Nissensohn, M.; López-Ufano, M.; Castro-Quezada, I.; et Serra-Majem, L. Valoración de La Ingesta de Bebidas y Del Estado de Hidratación. Span. J. Community Nutr. 2002, 21, 58–65. [Google Scholar]
  29. Schröder, H.; Fitó, M.; Estruch, R.; Martínez-González, M.A.; Corella, D.; Salas-Salvadó, J.; Lamuela-Raventós, R.; Ros, E.; Salaverría, I.; Fiol, M.; et al. A Short Screener Is Valid for Assessing Mediterranean Diet Adherence among Older Spanish Men and Women. J. Nutr. 2011, 141, 1140–1145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  30. Benedet, M.; Alejandre, M. TAVEC: Test de Aprendizaje Verbal España-Complutense; Tea: Madrid, Spain, 1998; ISBN 8471745291. [Google Scholar]
  31. Weschler, D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, 4th ed.; Statistics Solutions: Clearwater, FL, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Bates, M.E.; Lemay, E.P. The D2 Test of Attention: Construct Validity and Extensions in Scoring Techniques. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2004, 10, 392–400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Olabarrieta-Landa, L.; Torre, E.L.; López-Mugartza, J.C.; Bialystok, E.; Arango-Lasprilla, J.C. Verbal Fluency Tests: Developing a New Model of Administration and Scoring for Spanish Language. NeuroRehabilitation 2017, 41, 539–565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Molina-Hidalgo, C.; De-Lao, A.; Jurado-Fasoli, L.; Amaro-Gahete, F.J.; Castillo, M.J. Beer or Ethanol Effects on the Body Composition Response to High-Intensity Interval Training. The BEER-HIIT Study. Nutrients 2019, 11, 909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Molina-Hidalgo, C.; De-La-O, A.; Dote-Montero, M.; Amaro-Gahete, F.J.; Castillo, M.J. Influence of Daily Beer or Ethanol Consumption on Physical Fitness in Response to a High-Intensity Interval Training Program. The BEER-HIIT Study. J. Int. Soc. Sports Nutr. 2020, 17, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Shaffer, M.L.; Chinchilli, V.M. Including Multiple Imputation in a Sensitivity Analysis for Clinical Trials with Treatment Failures. Contemp. Clin. Trials 2007, 28, 130–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Reljic, D.; Lampe, D.; Wolf, F.; Zopf, Y.; Herrmann, H.J.; Fischer, J. Prevalence and Predictors of Dropout from High-intensity Interval Training in Sedentary Individuals: A Meta-analysis. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2019, 29, 1288–1304. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Erickson, K.I.; Donofry, S.D.; Sewell, K.R.; Brown, B.M.; Stillman, C.M. Cognitive Aging and the Promise of Physical Activity. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2022, 18, 417–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Winter, B.; Breitenstein, C.; Mooren, F.C.; Voelker, K.; Fobker, M.; Lechtermann, A.; Krueger, K.; Fromme, A.; Korsukewitz, C.; Floel, A.; et al. High Impact Running Improves Learning. Neurobiol. Learn. Mem. 2007, 87, 597–609. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Coetsee, C.; Terblanche, E. The Effect of Three Different Exercise Training Modalities on Cognitive and Physical Function in a Healthy Older Population. Eur. Rev. Aging Phys. Act. 2017, 14, 13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Topiwala, A.; Allan, C.L.; Valkanova, V.; Zsoldos, E.; Filippini, N.; Sexton, C.; Mahmood, A.; Fooks, P.; Singh-Manoux, A.; Mackay, C.E.; et al. Moderate Alcohol Consumption as Risk Factor for Adverse Brain Outcomes and Cognitive Decline: Longitudinal Cohort Study. BMJ 2017, 357, j2353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Rehm, J.; Room, R.; Graham, K.; Monteiro, M.; Gmel, G.; Sempos, C.T. The Relationship of Average Volume of Alcohol Consumption and Patterns of Drinking to Burden of Disease: An Overview. Addiction 2003, 98, 1209–1228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Cox, E.P.; O’Dwyer, N.; Cook, R.; Vetter, M.; Cheng, H.L.; Rooney, K.; O’Connor, H. Relationship between Physical Activity and Cognitive Function in Apparently Healthy Young to Middle-Aged Adults: A Systematic Review. J. Sci. Med. Sport. 2016, 19, 616–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Cherup, N.; Roberson, K.; Potiaumpai, M.; Widdowson, K.; Jaghab, A.M.; Chowdhari, S.; Armitage, C.; Seeley, A.; Signorile, J. Improvements in Cognition and Associations with Measures of Aerobic Fitness and Muscular Power Following Structured Exercise. Exp. Gerontol. 2018, 112, 76–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Colcombe, S.; Kramer, A.F. Fitness Effects on the Cognitive Function of Older Adults: A Meta-Analytic Study. Psychol. Sci. 2003, 14, 125–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Mekari, S.; Dupuy, O.; Martins, R.; Evans, K.; Kimmerly, D.S.; Fraser, S.; Neyedli, H.F. The Effects of Cardiorespiratory Fitness on Executive Function and Prefrontal Oxygenation in Older Adults. Geroscience 2019, 41, 681–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Prickett, C.; Brennan, L.; Stolwyk, R. Examining the Relationship between Obesity and Cognitive Function: A Systematic Literature Review. Obes. Res. Clin. Pract. 2015, 9, 93–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Stanek, K.M.; Strain, G.; Devlin, M.; Cohen, R.; Paul, R.; Crosby, R.D.; Mitchell, J.E.; Gunstad, J. Body Mass Index and Neurocognitive Functioning Across the Adult Lifespan. Neuropsychology 2013, 27, 141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Flow of participants in the BEER-HIIT study. Abbreviations: HIIT, high-intensity interval training; BMI, body mass index; ECG, electrocardiogram.
Figure 1. Flow of participants in the BEER-HIIT study. Abbreviations: HIIT, high-intensity interval training; BMI, body mass index; ECG, electrocardiogram.
Nutrients 16 01680 g001
Figure 2. Changes in learning process (Trial 1 and 5) were measured by the Spanish Complutense Verbal Learning Test (TAVEC), tested at baseline and post study intervention. F, p, and η of repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) for time, group, and trial. Intragroup changes obtained by repeated-measures ANOVA (group × time × trial, adjusting by trial), with a post hoc Bonferroni-corrected test, are indicated as Trial 1 vs. Trial 5 (a1 and a2; all p < 0.05) for the Non-Training group; Trial 1 vs. Trial 5 (b1 and b2; all p < 0.05) for the HIIT-Alcohol group; the Trial 1 vs. Trial 5 (c1 and c2; all p < 0.05) for the HIIT-NonAlcohol group. Intragroup trials’ changes before and post-intervention obtained by repeated-measures ANOVA (group × time × trial, adjusting by time), with a post hoc Bonferroni-corrected test, are indicated as p < 0.05 for the Non-Training group, b p < 0.05 for the HIIT-Alcohol group, and c p < 0.05 for the HIIT-NonAlcohol group. Raw data (total correct responses) are presented as the mean and standard deviation. Abbreviations: HIIT-Alcohol, group that performed high-intensity interval training and consumed alcoholic beverages; HIIT-NonAlcohol, group that performed high-intensity interval training and consumed non-alcoholic beverages.
Figure 2. Changes in learning process (Trial 1 and 5) were measured by the Spanish Complutense Verbal Learning Test (TAVEC), tested at baseline and post study intervention. F, p, and η of repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) for time, group, and trial. Intragroup changes obtained by repeated-measures ANOVA (group × time × trial, adjusting by trial), with a post hoc Bonferroni-corrected test, are indicated as Trial 1 vs. Trial 5 (a1 and a2; all p < 0.05) for the Non-Training group; Trial 1 vs. Trial 5 (b1 and b2; all p < 0.05) for the HIIT-Alcohol group; the Trial 1 vs. Trial 5 (c1 and c2; all p < 0.05) for the HIIT-NonAlcohol group. Intragroup trials’ changes before and post-intervention obtained by repeated-measures ANOVA (group × time × trial, adjusting by time), with a post hoc Bonferroni-corrected test, are indicated as p < 0.05 for the Non-Training group, b p < 0.05 for the HIIT-Alcohol group, and c p < 0.05 for the HIIT-NonAlcohol group. Raw data (total correct responses) are presented as the mean and standard deviation. Abbreviations: HIIT-Alcohol, group that performed high-intensity interval training and consumed alcoholic beverages; HIIT-NonAlcohol, group that performed high-intensity interval training and consumed non-alcoholic beverages.
Nutrients 16 01680 g002
Figure 3. Associations between maximal oxygen uptake (VO2max) (A) and processing speed (composite score); VO2max (B) and inhibitory control (z-score); VO2max (C) and verbal fluency (total z-score), and body mass index (BMI) adjusted for age, sex, and occupational activity (D) and working memory (composite score) in young healthy adults. The analyses were controlled for: age (Model 1); both age and sex (Model 2); and age, sex, and occupational activity (Model 3). The β (standardized linear regression coefficient), R2 (coefficient of determination), and p value were obtained from the linear regression analyses.
Figure 3. Associations between maximal oxygen uptake (VO2max) (A) and processing speed (composite score); VO2max (B) and inhibitory control (z-score); VO2max (C) and verbal fluency (total z-score), and body mass index (BMI) adjusted for age, sex, and occupational activity (D) and working memory (composite score) in young healthy adults. The analyses were controlled for: age (Model 1); both age and sex (Model 2); and age, sex, and occupational activity (Model 3). The β (standardized linear regression coefficient), R2 (coefficient of determination), and p value were obtained from the linear regression analyses.
Nutrients 16 01680 g003
Table 1. Baseline characteristics of participants.
Table 1. Baseline characteristics of participants.
Non-Training
(n = 17)
HIIT-Alcohol
(n = 27)
HIIT-NonAlcohol
(n = 31)
Mean ± SD p Value
Sex (men%/women%)64.7%/35.3%48.1%/51.8%54.8%/45.2%0.572
Age20.2 ± 1.324.2 ± 1.524.5 ± 1.50.006 *
Alcohol Ingested (mL/week)1144.1 ± 831.9696.7 ± 857.41204.5 ± 1167.30.131
Educational level (%) 0.097
Primary Education --3.7%--
Secondary Education47.1%40.7%22.5%
Vocational Education and Training23.5%18.5%22.5%
University Degree or Certificate of Higher Education29.5%37.3%54.8%
Occupational activity (%) 0.005 *
Level 1 100% † 55.6% 54.8%
Level 2 -- 18.5% 16.1%
Level 3 -- 25.9% 29.1%
Learning Process
Trial 18.8 ± 2.38.3 ± 2.08.3 ± 1.80.666
Trial 211.2 ± 2.712.2 ± 2.111.4 ± 2.30.269
Trial 312.9 ± 2.313.1 ± 2.212.4 ± 2.70.530
Trial 414.4 ± 1.313.4 ± 2.113.4 ± 3.10.329
Trail 514.5 ± 1.313.8 ± 1.714.1 ± 1.90.474
Memory
Immediate memory61.8 ± 7.460.9 ± 7.759.6 ± 9.30.638
Short-term memory13.6 ± 1.213.9 ± 1.812.9 ± 2.50.170
Delay memory13.9 ± 1.414.2 ± 1.813.3 ± 2.20.224
Recognition (tc)15.3 ± 0.915.6 ± 0.615.0 ± 1.40.115
Recognition (id)97.7 ± 2.798.2 ± 1.996.9 ± 4.10.284
Working Memory
Direct Score13.0 ± 1.611.8 ± 2.111.7 ± 2.50.125
Processing Score5.8 ± 0.85.6 ± 0.95.5 ± 0.90.507
Processing Speed
Total productivity516.6 ± 88.0514.5 ± 76.5510.9 ± 67.30.965
Correct work208.7 ± 43.4203.9 ± 39.0200.7 ± 36.50.793
Concentration Index208.4 ± 43.5203.4 ± 39.4200.0 ± 36.60.776
Inhibitory Control
Total Effectiveness502.9 ± 86.7498.3 ± 77.5492.7 ± 68.10.900
Verbal Fluency
Phonologic86.1 ± 23.288.4 ± 20.577.1 ± 15.00.069
Semantic40.5 ± 8.040.1 ± 6.537.5 ± 8.50.315
Total Score126.6 ± 28.0128.4 ± 23.8114.6 ± 19.30.057
Data expressed the as mean ± standard deviation. Differences in baseline characteristics between the different groups were evaluated with analysis of variance (ANOVA). † Occupational activity: 53% unemployment, 47% student. * Boldface values indicate significant differences (p < 0.05). Abbreviations: HIIT-Alcohol group that performed high-intensity interval training and consumed alcoholic beverages; HIIT-NonAlcohol group that performed high-intensity interval training and consumed non-alcoholic beverages; Level 1, unemployment, homemaker, and student; Level 2, agriculture, livestock and fishery services, retail or catering services, and administrative clerk; Level 3, support technicians and professionals, scientific and intellectual technicians and professionals, business administration, and management.
Table 2. Changes before and after the intervention study and changes over time in cognitive scores.
Table 2. Changes before and after the intervention study and changes over time in cognitive scores.
Non-Training HIIT-AlcoholHIIT-NonAlcohol
BaselinePost-Intervention BaselinePost-Intervention BaselinePost-Intervention Model 0
Mean ± SD
Memory p Value p Value p ValueFp Valueη2
Immediate Memory61.82 ± 7.3870.06 ± 5.85≤0.00160.93 ± 7.6870.15 ± 5.56≤0.00159.55 ± 9.2667.94 ± 6.87≤0.0010.7880.4590.022
Short-Term Memory13.59 ± 1.2315.29 ± 0.85≤0.00113.85 ± 1.8114.89 ± 1.580.00412.87 ± 2.4714.77 ± 1.45≤0.0010.8500.4320.023
Delay Memory13.88 ± 1.4115.47 ± 0.62≤0.00114.15 ± 1.8314.93 ± 1.620.001413.29 ± 2.1815.00 ± 1.13≤0.0012.0430.1370.054
Recognition (tc)15.29 ± 0.9216.00 ± 0.000.00515.56 ± 0.6415.89 ± 0.320.10514.97 ± 1.3815.68 ± 0.70≤0.0011.9560.1490.052
Recognition (id)97.73 ± 2.6799.60 ± 0.890.01498.23 ± 1.9398.82 ± 1.930.32196.92 ± 4.0999.12 ± 1.73≤0.0011.6320.2030.044
Composite score0.082 ± 0.2040.277 ± 0.1740.3150.195 ± 0.162−0.002 ± 0.1620.201−0.219 ± 0.151−0.125 ± 0.1290.5131.4620.2390.040
Working Memory
Direct Score13.00 ± 1.6213.06 ± 3.470.92811.78 ± 2.0811.96 ± 3.110.72211.74 ± 2.4911.77 ± 2.970.9470.1160.8910.003
Processing Score5.82 ± 0.815.88 ± 1.270.8455.63 ± 0.885.41 ± 1.120.3545.52 ± 0.895.32 ± 1.490.3870.5740.5660.016
Composite score0.349 ± 0.8200.307 ± 1.0200.852−0.042 ± 0.186−0.050 ± 0.1830.966−0.117 ± 0.173−0.113 ± 0.1710.9820.1870.8300.005
Processing Speed
Total productivity516.59 ± 87.93576.59 ± 72.84≤0.001514.52 ± 76.49566.07 ± 67.83≤0.001510.87 ± 67.316571.87 ± 52.41≤0.0010.6910.5040.019
Correct work208.71 ± 43.44244.82 ± 39.72≤0.001203.85 ± 39.04235.15 ± 36.43≤0.001200.68 ± 36.54237.29 ± 39.76≤0.0010.5300.5910.015
Concentration Index208.41 ± 43.46242.94 ± 41.62≤0.001203.44 ± 39.37234.85 ± 36.50≤0.001200.00 ± 36.58236.94 ± 39.72≤0.0010.4420.6440.012
Composite score0.102 ± 0.2450.136 ± 0.2400.7830.007 ± 0.194−0.075 ± 0.1800.399−0.067 ± 0.181−0.007 ± 0.1780.5160.6180.5420.017
Inhibitory Control
Total Effectiveness502.82 ± 86.61565.18 ± 74.533≤0.001498.26 ± 77.45552.22 ± 66.00≤0.001492.61 ± 68.01554.87 ± 55.62≤0.0010.7130.4940.020
Verbal Fluency
Phonologic86.12 ± 23.1595.71 ± 24.510.00388.37 ± 20.5596.89 ± 16.70≤0.00177.10 ± 14.9384.42 ± 17.160.0020.9640.3860.026
Semantic40.47 ± 8.0239.88 ± 10.780.70540.11 ± 6.4742.37 ± 6.030.07037.48 ± 8.5537.48 ± 6.741.0002.3640.1010.062
Total Verbal Fluency126.59 ± 27.97135.59 ± 33.550.016128.41 ± 23.76139.26 ± 21.09≤0.001114.58 ± 19.25121.90 ± 20.060.0081.1030.3380.030
p value of intragroup changes before and post-intervention obtained by repeated-measures ANOVA. F, p value, and η2 of analysis of covariance adjusting by baseline values. Boldface values indicate significance differences (p < 0.05). Abbreviations: HIIT-Alcohol, group that performed high-intensity interval training and consumed alcoholic beverages; HIIT-NonAlcohol, group that performed high-intensity interval training and consumed non-alcoholic beverages; tc, total number of correct responses in the recognition test; id, index of discriminability from learning list in the recognition test.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Molina-Hidalgo, C.; Amaro-Gahete, F.J.; Peven, J.C.; Erickson, K.I.; Catena, A.; Castillo, M.J. The Effects of High-Intensity Interval Training and Moderate Alcohol Consumption on Cognitive Performance—A Multidisciplinary Intervention in Young Healthy Adults. Nutrients 2024, 16, 1680. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16111680

AMA Style

Molina-Hidalgo C, Amaro-Gahete FJ, Peven JC, Erickson KI, Catena A, Castillo MJ. The Effects of High-Intensity Interval Training and Moderate Alcohol Consumption on Cognitive Performance—A Multidisciplinary Intervention in Young Healthy Adults. Nutrients. 2024; 16(11):1680. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16111680

Chicago/Turabian Style

Molina-Hidalgo, Cristina, Francisco J. Amaro-Gahete, Jamie C. Peven, Kirk I. Erickson, Andres Catena, and Manuel J. Castillo. 2024. "The Effects of High-Intensity Interval Training and Moderate Alcohol Consumption on Cognitive Performance—A Multidisciplinary Intervention in Young Healthy Adults" Nutrients 16, no. 11: 1680. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16111680

APA Style

Molina-Hidalgo, C., Amaro-Gahete, F. J., Peven, J. C., Erickson, K. I., Catena, A., & Castillo, M. J. (2024). The Effects of High-Intensity Interval Training and Moderate Alcohol Consumption on Cognitive Performance—A Multidisciplinary Intervention in Young Healthy Adults. Nutrients, 16(11), 1680. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu16111680

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop