Next Article in Journal
The Effects of Prolonged Water-Only Fasting and Refeeding on Markers of Cardiometabolic Risk
Previous Article in Journal
Exploratory Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Panax Genus Plant Ingestion Evaluation in Exercise Endurance
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Curcumin-Rich Curry Consumption and Neurocognitive Function from 4.5-Year Follow-Up of Community-Dwelling Older Adults (Singapore Longitudinal Ageing Study)

1
Department of Psychological Medicine, Yong Loo Lin School of Medicine, National University of Singapore, Singapore 119228, Singapore
2
Geriatric Education and Research Institute, Singapore 768024, Singapore
3
Office of the Senior Deputy President and Provost, National University of Singapore, Singapore 119077, Singapore
4
National Public Health and Epidemiology Unit, National Centre for Infectious Diseases, Singapore 308442, Singapore
5
Department of Medicine, Ng Teng Fong General Hospital, Singapore 609606, Singapore
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2022, 14(6), 1189; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14061189
Submission received: 11 January 2022 / Revised: 4 March 2022 / Accepted: 4 March 2022 / Published: 11 March 2022
(This article belongs to the Section Phytochemicals and Human Health)

Abstract

:
The potential neurocognition protective effects of dietary curcumin in curry consumed with food was investigated in this study of 2734 community-dwelling adults (aged ≥ 55, mean ± SD: 65.9 ± 7.4). We analyzed longitudinal data of baseline curry consumption (“never or rarely”, “occasionally”: <once a month, “often”: >once a month and <once a week, “very often”: >once a week or daily) and baseline and 4.5-year follow-up cognitive function in mixed model analyses controlling for confounding risk factors. Significant between-exposure differences were found for Digit Span-Backward (DS-B), Verbal Fluency-Animals (VF-A) and Block Design (BD). Compared to “never or rarely” consumption, “very often” and “often” consumptions were associated with higher DS-B performance; “very often”—with higher VF-A, and “occasional”, “often” and “very often” consumptions—with higher BD: Cohen’s d: from 0.130 to 0.186. Among participants with cardiometabolic and cardiac diseases (CMVD), curry consumption was associated with significantly higher DS-B and VF-A. Among CMVD-free participants, curry consumption was associated with significantly higher DS-B, VF-A and BD: Cohen’s d: from 0.098 to 0.305. The consumption of dietary curcumin was associated with the maintenance over time of higher functioning on attention, short-term working memory, visual spatial constructional ability, language and executive function among community-dwelling older Asian adults.

1. Introduction

Curcumin is a polyphenolic compound isolated from the roots of Curcuma longa, from which turmeric is prepared and used widely as a coloring agent, food additive and traditional Indian and Chinese medicine. For decades, curcumin and related bioactive curcuminoids (demethoxycurcumin and bisdemethoxycurcumin) have been the center of interest of scientific studies worldwide into their therapeutic potentials. Numerous laboratory studies have documented its antioxidant, anti-inflammatory, antimicrobial, antiviral, antineoplastic and antiaging activities [1,2,3]. Curcumin has low bioavailability due to its poor aqueous solubility, low intestinal absorption, low tissue distribution, rapid metabolism and systemic clearance [4]. Hence, various formulations of curcumin, including nanoparticles and liposomal encapsulation, are used in over 100 completed and ongoing clinical trials to investigate its therapeutic potential for chronic diseases, including cancers, diabetes, obesity, cardiovascular, pulmonary, neurological and autoimmune diseases [1].
Curcumin is actively being investigated for its potential neuroprotective benefits. Accelerated cognitive decline and dementia are principally the result of brain neurodegenerative disease due to underlying β amyloid (Aβ) and tau pathology, cerebral microvascular disease and neuroinflammation. In vitro and in vivo studies show that curcumin crosses the blood–brain barrier and can bind to β amyloid (Aβ) and tau-inhibiting Aβ aggregation and modulate tau processing [5,6,7,8,9]. Animal studies show that curcumin decreases Aβ deposition and plaque load in the brain of transgenic mouse models of AD [10,11]. Experimental studies have also demonstrated that curcumin possesses beneficial antiplatelet, antidiabetic and cardioprotective activities ameliorating cardiac hypertrophy and chronic heart failure [12,13,14,15,16,17,18,19]. On top of the known anti-inflammatory activities of curcumin attenuating neuroinflammation, this experimental evidence strongly suggests that curcumin has the potential to prevent accelerated cognitive decline in humans. Several animal studies have shown that curcumin improves cognitive function [20,21]. Randomized controlled trials in humans are limited but suggest that curcumin could be safely and effectively used to slow cognitive decline [22,23,24,25,26,27].
We have previously reported preliminary findings from an observational cross-sectional study that older individuals who consumed greater amounts of curry in their meals showed a higher global cognitive performance score on the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) compared to their counterparts who never or rarely consumed curry [28]. To our knowledge, there has not been any other naturalistic study that has investigated the potential cognitive benefits of dietary food source(s) of curcumin in humans. The main source of curcumin in the diet is almost exclusively from turmeric, the Indian curry spice, which is most commonly used in curry meals consumed by Asians in subcontinental India, Southeast and other parts of Asia. It is therefore pertinent to investigate whether the consumption of curcumin naturally in the diet is associated with enhanced cognitive functioning in older people who are vulnerable to the risk of developing dementia.
In this report, we describe the results of a larger prospective study in a separate cohort of community-dwelling middle-aged and older adults in the Singapore Longitudinal Ageing Study (SLAS) with longitudinal analysis of cognitive function assessed at baseline and 4.5-year follow-up.

2. Materials and Methods

Study design and participants. We analyzed data in two combined population cohorts in the Singapore Longitudinal Ageing Study (SLAS-1 and SLAS-2). SLAS-1 recruited 2804 participants in Southeast Singapore from Sep 2003 to Dec 2004, and SLAS-2 recruited 3270 participants in Southwest Singapore from Mar 2009 to Jun 2013. First wave follow-up visits and assessments were conducted at approximately 3–5 years after the baseline assessment, from Mar 2005 to Sep 2007 for SLAS-1, and Jan 2013–Aug 2018 for SLAS-2. Details of methodology of SLAS-1 and SLAS-2 cohorts have been described in earlier reports [29,30].
Inclusion and exclusion criteria: Singapore citizens or permanent residents in study locations who were aged ≥55 years at recruitment. We excluded individuals who were unable to participate due to severe physical or mental disability, those with stroke, traumatic brain injury and Parkinson’s disease or other brain neurodegenerative disorders.
Ethics approval. This study was approved by the National University of Singapore Institutional Review Board (Protocol Code: 04–140), in accordance with the relevant guidelines and regulations by the Declaration of Helsinki and the ethical principles in the Belmont Report. All participants gave written informed consent to participate in the study.
Measurements. Trained nurses visited participants’ homes to perform face-to-face questionnaire interviews, and clinical measurements were performed in a local study site center to collect an extensive range of sociodemographic, lifestyle, behavior, psychological, neurobehavioral, medical and other data at baseline. Among 6074 participants in the combined cohort, curry consumption data was available for 5226 participants. Neurocognitive performance tests were performed on a random 1-in-3 sample of SLAS participants, and 1-in-2 sample of SLAS2 participants. This resulted in 2846 participants with complete curry consumption data and data for at least one neurocognitive test. Among them, 95 participants with stroke and Parkinson’s disease were excluded from analysis in this study. The final analytical sample was N = 2751, including participants with baseline neurocognitive data varying in numbers between N = 1674 and N = 2734 for different tests. Follow-up neurocognitive test data were variously available for between 1202 and 1747 participants.
Curry consumption. Participants were asked at baseline and follow-up interviews how frequently they usually consumed curry in their meals: Never or rarely (less than once a year), occasionally (at least once/year to less than once/month), often (at least once/month to less than once/week, very often (at least once a week) or daily. Because of the small number of participants who reported daily consumption in this study, they were included in a combined category of “very often or daily”. Participants were also asked whether their curry consumption had remained mostly unchanged or have changed. Only strictly consistent responses at baseline and follow-up interviews were used to assign them unequivocally to the highest and the lowest consumption category. Participants who consistently reported “occasional” or “often” consumption frequencies at baseline and follow-up were also assigned as such to the appropriate category. For modestly inconsistent responses (differing by one response category at baseline and follow-up), the higher consumption response was used to assign to the nearest “occasional” or “often” categories. For more widely inconsistent responses, we used the average response score to assign them to the nearest response category, either “occasional” or “often”.
Neurocognitive assessments. Global cognition was assessed using a locally translated version of the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) which was previously validated for use among Chinese and non-Chinese (Malay, Indian and others) Singaporean older adults [31]. A comprehensive battery of neurocognitive tests was used for assessment of cognitive domains, including verbal learning and memory: Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test-Delayed Recall (RAVLT-DR), visual memory: Visual Reproduction-Delayed Recall (VR-DR), attention and working memory: Digit Span Forward (DS-F) and Backward (DS-B), language: Categorical Verbal Fluency-Animals (VF-A), executive function, visual, spatial and constructional ability: Block Design (BD), visual-motor tracking and mental flexibility: Trail Making Test A (TMT-A) and B (TMT-B). The assessment was administered by trained psychology research assistants in the participants’ habitual language: English, Mandarin, or dialects. The validation of the neuropsychological tests for use in Singaporean older adults and their normative values have been described previously [32]. The test performance results were standardized by transforming their raw scores into z-scores.
Covariates. Other measurements were made for potential confounding risk factors that included sex, age (single years), ethnicity (Chinese versus non-Chinese (Malay, Indian Other)), education (none, 1–6 years, >6 years), smoking (never, past smoker, current smoker), alcohol (≥once/week) and body mass index (BMI, kg/m2). Physical activity score, social activity score and productive activity score were calculated from the number and frequencies of usual participation in 18 different categories of physical, social and productive activities [29]. Central obesity was defined as waist circumference ≥80 cm in women and ≥90 cm in men [33]. Hypertension was assessed using the criteria of systolic blood pressure ≥140 mmHg and/or diastolic blood pressure ≥90 mmHg [34], or a self-report history of hypertension diagnosis and treatment, verified by recorded names of drugs shown on medication packages. Diabetes was assessed using fasting blood glucose concentrations of ≥5.6 mmol/L [35] or a self-report history of diabetes diagnosis and treatment. Dyslipidemia was defined as either triglyceride levels of ≥1.7 mmol/L or high-density lipoprotein cholesterol <1 mmol/L in men and <1.3 mmol/L in women [36]. The presence of cardiovascular disease was defined by a self-report history of stroke, myocardial infarct, or heart failure. Depressive symptoms were assessed by the locally translated and validated 15-item Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS) [37].
Statistical analysis. Categorical variables are presented as numbers (percentages), and continuous variables are presented as means (SD). Differences between curry exposure groups were examined using ANOVA for continuous variables, and chi-square test for categorical variables. Between-group differences over time for neurocognitive performances were analyzed using mixed-effect model with curry group (≥1 x/week vs. <1 x/week), time (baseline and follow-up) and their interactions as fixed effects and subject as random effect. Maximum likelihood estimation was used to analyze missing data for neurocognitive performance. To control for confounding by risk factors, the mixed models included as additional covariates of age, sex, ethnicity (Chinese versus non-Chinese), educational level, physical, social and productive activity scores, smoking (nonsmoker, ex-smoker, current smoker), alcohol (never, <once/week, ≥once/week), central obesity, hypertension, high fasting blood glucose or diabetes, high triglyceride, low HDL-cholesterol, cardiac diseases and GDS score. Effect size was estimated using Cohen’s d statistic. All statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics version 27, and a value of p < 0.05 was considered significant.

3. Results

The mean (SD) age of the study participants was 65.9 (7.4), and 63.6% were women. Participants who reported higher levels of curry consumption were more likely to be younger, men, of non-Chinese (Malay, Indian) ethnicity, better educated, had higher levels of productive (mentally stimulating) activity, more central obesity, and lower GDS depression scores (Table 1).
As shown in Supplementary Table S1, these known or potential risk factors were significantly associated with various neurocognitive performance. In particular, Chinese ethnicity was significantly associated with all neurocognitive test performance; age was significantly associated with MMSE, VR-DR, TMT-A, TMT-B and BD; education was significantly associated with MMSE, DS-F, DS-B, RAVLT-DR, VR-DR and VF-A; productive activity was associated with MMSE, RAVLT, VR-DR, TMT-A and BD; central obesity was associated with MMSE, VR-DR; and GDS with MMSE, RAVLT, VR-DR and BD.
Increasing levels of curry consumption were significantly associated with better crude estimates of mean neurocognitive performance scores at both baseline and follow-up (Table 2) for MMSE, Digit Span-Backward, Verbal Fluency-Animals, Trail-Making Tests A and B and Block Design. In mixed model analyses, (Table 3 and Figure 1) controlling for known or potential confounding risk factors, significant between-group differences were shown for Digit Span (Backward), Verbal Fluency (Animals) and Block Design. Pairwise comparisons showed that compared to “never or rarely” consumption, “very often” and “often” consumptions were associated with significantly higher levels of Digit Span-Backward; “very often” was associated with significantly higher performance on Verbal Fluency-Animals, and “occasional”, “often” and “very often” consumptions were associated with significantly higher performance on Block Design. The estimated Cohen’s d for significantly enhanced neurocognitive functions ranged from 0.130 (Digit Span-Backward) to 0.186 (Block Design).
We further performed stratified analyses by subgroups of participants with or without cardiometabolic or cardiac diseases. (Table 4 and Figure 2) Among participants with cardiometabolic and cardiac diseases, curry consumption was associated with significantly higher performance on Digit Span (Backward) and Verbal Fluency (Animals). Among participants free of cardiometabolic and cardiac diseases, curry consumption was associated with significantly higher performance on Digit Span-Backward, Verbal Fluency-Animals and Block Design. The estimated Cohen’s d of effect size for curry consumption ranged from 0.098 (Digit Span-Backward) among those with cardiometabolic and cardiac disease to 0.305 (Block Design) among those without cardiometabolic and cardiac disease.

4. Discussion

We found in this observational cohort study that the consumption of curcumin-rich curry in food was associated with the maintenance of higher levels of cognitive functioning over time among community-dwelling middle-aged and older Asian adults in Singapore. To our knowledge, this is the first longitudinal study demonstrating the cognitive benefits of curcumin from natural dietary food sources in humans. There are a number of notable observations from these findings for discussion.
Cognitive domains. Curcumin exposure from curry consumption appeared in this study to be associated with enhanced cognitive functioning on attention, short-term working memory, visual spatial constructional ability, language and executive function and evidently not with long-term memory ability. Non-amnestic related and executive function deficits are hallmarks of subcortical vascular cognitive impairment, a clinical continuum of vascular-related cognitive impairment, including vascular mild cognitive impairment and vascular dementia [38]. This suggests that, notwithstanding its known anti-amyloid, anti-tau, antioxidant and anti-inflammatory activities, curcumin exposure from dietary ingestion appears to offer neuroprotection especially through its beneficial metabolic, antiplatelet and cardioprotective properties. The results from the stratified analysis support this by showing that among participants with pre-existing metabolic and cardiovascular disease, curry consumption was associated with enhanced performance on attention, working memory and language executive function. In vitro and high-dose animal studies show that curcumin modulates molecular pathways that restore glucose homeostasis, lipid metabolism, endothelial function and insulin signaling and may thus offer potential cardiovascular protective effect. Clinical studies to date, however, remain rather unconvincing [2]. There was no suggestion from observing the cross-sectional relationship at baseline in this study that there was a meaningful association between curry consumption and cardiometabolic health, although the prevalence bias in this observation should be noted.
Curcumin bioavailability. The low bioavailability from oral ingestion of curcumin as a chemical compound with its poor aqueous solubility is well-known. This begs the question how the dietary ingestion of curcumin from natural food sources could actually make a difference in achieving desired physiological effects in various biological tissues, including the brain. Unlike pharmatherapeutic formulations, dietary curcumin is ingested not as curcumin per se, but as turmeric, which contains essential oils (turmeric oil) such as zingiberene, α-Turmerone and β-Turmerone [39]. These structural analogues of curcumin, together with spices and spice oils such as chilli, pepper, cumin, coriander and plant-based cooking oils, which are commonly blended with turmeric in the preparation of curry meals, as well as with intestinal bile salts, may act as its own lipid-based oral delivery systems, forming microemulsions, polymeric micelles or phospholipid complexes of curcumin, for enhanced solubility and bioavailability. There is evidence that the combination of piperine in black pepper with curcumin in turmeric increases the bioavailability of curcumin by 2000% and inhibits the intestinal and hepatic glucuronidation of curcumin [40].
Microbiota–gut–brain axis. High concentrations of curcumin remain in the intestines after oral ingestion. Bacterial enzymatic actions on curcumin may form metabolites pharmacologically more active than curcumin. A current working hypothesis is that curcumin could act primarily in the gastrointestinal tract, exerting direct regulatory effects on the brain in a bidirectional “gut–brain” communication axis, thereby influencing core neurological processes, including neurogenesis, synaptic plasticity, neurotransmitter signaling, neurodevelopment and neuroinflammation [41]. Studies show that by enhancing the diversity of the gut microbiota, curcumin and its metabolites restore gut microbial dysbiosis, decreases gut permeability and inflammation and metabolic endotoxemia associated with a high fat diet, obesity, metabolic syndrome, diabetes, CVD and neurodegenerative diseases [42,43]. These potential leads connecting dietary curcumin to the gut–brain axis development of cognitive disorders needs to be more intensively investigated in further studies.
Dose–response relationship. It is striking to note that better cognitive performance was evident with curry consumption frequencies that are relatively low in absolute terms. In this study, significantly better performance on the Digit Span (Backward) and Verbal Fluency (Animals) tasks were observed with curry consumption more frequent than only once a month (defined as “often” in this study). Performance on Block Design was even observed to be significantly better with curry consumption “occasionally” at less than once a month. Laboratory evidence suggests that curcumin is a hormetic agent (hormetin), exhibiting biphasic dose–responses, with low doses having stronger effects than high doses for some effects [44]. The hermetic effect of curcumin on cognitive functioning in humans is thus supported by the results in this study. The estimated Cohen’s d (between 0.10 and 0.30) indicated a small to medium effect. However, we estimated that the between-group differences in cognitive function were the equivalents of between 4 and 14 years of decrements in age.
Strengths and limitations. In this study, real-world observations of the potential cognitive benefits of curcumin in humans were provided in a large population-based cohort of community-dwelling older adults with follow-up over three to five years. This is relatively short compared to longer-term follow observation from, say, 10 years or more, and the changes in neurocognitive function were indeed very small. However, they reflect the actual observed amount of change associated with the exposure groups, as well as the survival bias from selective follow-up of participants with a relatively healthy risk profile and high cognitive performance status. The participants who were assessed at follow-up showed uniformly a more favorable baseline risk factor, curry consumption and neurocognitive profiles than those who were not assessed. The effect sizes are therefore probably underestimated, and this population selection bias thus warrants caution in generalizing the results from this study. The risk of neurocognitive disorders (mild cognitive impairment and dementia) was not analyzed as outcomes in this study and should be investigated in further studies.
Health implications. These observations suggest that population health in terms of healthy cognitive ageing and potential reduction of risks of dementia could be enhanced by increased consumption of curcumin in a turmeric-rich diet. The observed effect sizes were small, but nontrivial from a population health perspective. Further studies are desired to replicate these findings in other populations with measurable frequencies of turmeric consumption.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/nu14061189/s1, Table S1: Association of known or potential risk factors with neurocognitive test performance.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.P.N.; methodology, T.P.N.; formal analysis, T.P.N.; investigation, M.S.Z.N., Q.G., X.G., D.Q.L.C. and K.B.Y.; data curation, T.P.N.; writing—original draft preparation, T.P.N.; writing—review and editing, T.P.N., M.S.Z.N., Q.G., X.G., D.Q.L.C. and K.B.Y.; visualization, T.P.N.; project administration, M.S.Z.N., Q.G., X.G., D.Q.L.C. and K.B.Y.; funding acquisition, T.P.N. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the Agency for Science, Technology and Research (A*STAR) Biomedical Research Council, grant number BMRC/08/1/21/19/567 and the National Medical Research Council, grant numbers NMRC/1108/2007 and NMRC/CIRG/1409/2014.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of National University of Singapore (protocol code 04−140 and date of approval 17 June 2005).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets used and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We thank the following voluntary welfare organizations for their support: Geylang East Home for the Aged, Presbyterian Community Services, St Luke’s Eldercare Services, Thye Hua Kwan Moral Society (Moral Neighbourhood Links), Yuhua Neighbourhood Link, Henderson Senior Citizens’ Home, NTUC Eldercare Co-op Ltd., Thong Kheng Seniors Activity Centre (Queenstown Centre) and Redhill Moral Seniors Activity Centre.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Kunnumakkara, A.B.; Bordloi, D.; Padmavathi, G.; Monisha, J.; Roy, N.K.; Prasad, S.; Aggarwal, B.B. Curcumin, the golden nutraceutical: Multitargeting for multiple chronic diseases. Br. J. Pharmacol. 2017, 174, 1325–1348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Xu, X.Y.; Meng, X.; Li, S.; Gan, R.Y.; Li, Y.; Li, H.B. Bioactivity, Health Benefits, and Related Molecular Mechanisms of Curcumin: Current Progress, Challenges, and Perspectives. Nutrients 2018, 10, 1553. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. Ahsan, R.; Arshad, M.; Khushtar, M.; Ahmad, M.A.; Muazzam, M.; Akhter, M.S.; Gupta, G.; Muzahid, M. A Comprehensive Review on Physiological Effects of Curcumin. Drug Res. 2020, 70, 441–447. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Stohs, S.J.; Chen, O.; Ray, S.D.; Ji, J.; Bucci, L.R.; Preuss, H.G. Highly Bioavailable Forms of Curcumin and Promising Avenues for Curcumin-Based Research Application: A Review. Molecules 2020, 25, 1397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  5. Voulgaropoulou, S.D.; van Amelsvoort, T.A.M.J.; Prickaerts, J.; Vingerhoets, C. The effect of curcumin on cognition in Alzheimer’s disease and healthy aging: A systematic review of pre-clinical and clinical studies. Brain Res. 2019, 1725, 146476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Mutsuga, M.; Chambers, J.K.; Uchida, K.; Tei, M.; Makibuchi, T.; Mizorogi, T.; Takashima, A.; Nakayama, H. Binding of curcumin to senile plaques and cerebral amyloid angiopathy in the aged brain of various animals and to neurofibrillary tangles in Alzheimer’s brain. J. Vet. Med. Sci. 2012, 74, 51–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  7. Mithu, V.S.; Sarkar, B.; Bhowmik, D.; Das, A.K.; Chandrakesan, M.; Maiti, S.; Madhu, P.K. Curcumin alters the salt bridge-containing turn region in amyloid beta(1-42) aggregates. J. Biol. Chem. 2014, 289, 11122–11131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Ma, Q.L.; Zuo, X.; Yang, F.; Ubeda, O.J.; Gant, D.J.; Alaverdyan, M.; Teng, E.; Hu, S.; Chen, P.P.; Maiti, P.; et al. Curcumin suppresses soluble tau dimers and corrects molecular chaperone, synaptic, and behavioral deficits in aged human tau transgenic mice. J. Biol. Chem. 2013, 288, 4056–4065. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Garcia-Alloza, M.; Borrelli, L.A.; Rozkalne, A.; Hyman, B.T.; Backskai, B.J. Curcumin labels amyloid pathology in vivo, disrupts existing plaques, and partially restores distorted neurites in an Alzheimer mouse model. J. Neurochem. 2007, 102, 1095–1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Lim, G.P.; Chu, T.; Yang, F.; Frautschy, S.A.; Cole, G.M. The curry spice curcumin reduces oxidative damage and amyloid pathology in an Alzheimer transgenic mouse. J. Neurosci. 2001, 21, 8370–8377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  11. Yang, F.; Lim, G.P.; Begum, A.N.; Ubeda, O.J.; Simmons, M.R.; Ambegaokar, S.S.; Chen, P.P.; Kayed, R.; Glabe, C.G.; Frautschy, S.A.; et al. Curcumin inhibits formation of amyloid beta oligomers and fibrils, binds plaques, and reduces amyloid in vivo. J. Biol. Chem. 2005, 280, 5892–5901. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Yu, W.; Zha, W.L.; Ke, Z.Q.; Min, Q.; Li, C.R.; Sun, H.R.; Liu, C. Curcumin protects neonatal rat cardiomyocytes against high glucose-induced apoptosis via PI3K/Akt signalling pathway. J. Diabetes Res. 2016, 2016, 4158591. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Bai, X.J.; Hao, J.T.; Wang, J.; Zhang, W.F.; Yan, C.P.; Zhao, J.H. Curcumin inhibits cardiac hypertrophy and improves cardiovascular function via enhanced Na+/Ca2+ exchanger expression after transverse abdominal aortic constriction in rats. Pharmacol. Rep. 2018, 70, 60–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Chen, R.C.; Peng, X.F.; Du, W.M.; Wu, Y.; Huang, B.; Xue, L.; Wu, Q.; Qiu, H.M.; Jiang, Q.S. Curcumin attenuates cardiomyocyte hypertrophy induced by high glucose and insulin via the PPAR gamma/Akt/NO signaling pathway. Diabetes Res. Clin. Pract. 2015, 108, 235–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Katanasaka, Y.; Sunagawa, Y.; Hasegawa, K.; Morimoto, T. Application of curcumin to heart failure therapy by targeting transcriptional pathway in cardiomyocytes. Biol. Pharm. Bull. 2013, 36, 13–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Meng, B.; Li, J.; Cao, H. Antioxidant and antiinflammatory activities of curcumin on diabetes mellitus and its complications. Curr. Pharm. Des. 2013, 19, 2101–2113. [Google Scholar]
  17. Chuengsamarn, S.; Rattanamongkolgul, S.; Luechapudiporn, R.; Phisalaphong, C.; Jirawatnotai, S. Curcumin extract for prevention of type 2 diabetes. Diabetes Care 2012, 35, 2121–2127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. Tabeshpour, J.; Hashemzaei, M.; Sahebkar, A. The regulatory role of curcumin on platelet functions. J. Cell. Biochem. 2018, 119, 8713–8722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Kim, Y.; Clifton, P. Curcumin, Cardiometabolic Health and Dementia. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 2093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  20. Dong, S.; Zeng, Q.; Mitchell, E.S.; Xiu, J.; Duan, Y.; Li, C.; Tiwari, J.K.; Hu, Y.; Cao, X.; Zhao, Z. Curcumin enhances neurogenesis and cognition in aged rats: Implications for transcriptional interactions related to growth and synaptic plasticity. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e31211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Yu, S.Y.; Zhang, M.; Luo, J.; Zhang, L.; Shao, Y.; Li, G. Curcumin ameliorates memory deficits via neuronal nitric oxide synthase in aged mice. Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry 2013, 45, 47–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Kuszewski, J.C.; Wong, R.H.X.; Howe, P.R.C. Can Curcumin Counteract Cognitive Decline? Clinical Trial Evidence and Rationale for Combining ω-3 Fatty Acids with Curcumin. Adv. Nutr. 2018, 9, 105–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Baum, L.; Lam, C.W.; Cheung, S.K.; Kwok, T.; Lui, V.; Tsoh, J.; Lam, L.; Leung, V.; Hui, E.; Ng, C.; et al. Six-month randomized, placebo-controlled, doubleblind, pilot clinical trial of curcumin in patients with Alzheimer disease. J. Clin. Psychopharmacol. 2008, 28, 110–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Ringman, J.M.; Frautschy, S.A.; Teng, E.; Begum, A.N.; Bardens, J.; Beigi, M.; Gylys, K.H.; Badmaev, V.; Heath, D.D.; Apostolova, L.G.; et al. Oral curcumin for Alzheimer’s disease: Tolerability and efficacy in a 24-week randomized, double blind, placebo-controlled study. Alzheimer’s Res. Ther. 2012, 4, 43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  25. Lee, M.S.; Wahlqvist, M.L.; Chou, Y.C.; Fang, W.H.; Lee, J.T.; Kuan, J.C.; Liu, H.Y.; Lu, T.M.; Xiu, L.; Hsu, C.C.; et al. Turmeric improves post-prandial working memory in pre-diabetes independent of insulin. Asia Pac. J. Clin. Nutr. 2014, 23, 581–591. [Google Scholar]
  26. Cox, K.H.; Pipingas, A.; Scholey, A.B. Investigation of the effects of solid lipid curcumin on cognition and mood in a healthy older population. J. Psychopharmacol. 2015, 29, 642–651. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Rainey-Smith, S.R.; Brown, B.M.; Sohrabi, H.R.; Shah, T.; Goozee, K.G.; Gupta, V.B.; Martins, R.N. Curcumin and cognition: A randomised, placebo-controlled, double-blind study of community-dwelling older adults. Br. J. Nutr. 2016, 115, 2106–2113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Ng, T.P.; Chiam, P.C.; Lee, T.; Chua, H.C.; Lim, L.; Kua, E.H. Curry consumption and cognitive function in the elderly. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2006, 164, 898–906. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Niti, M.; Yap, K.B.; Kua, E.H.; Tan, C.H.; Ng, T.P. Physical, social and productive leisure activities, cognitive decline and interaction with APOE-epsilon 4 genotype in Chinese older adults. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2008, 20, 237–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ng, T.P.; Feng, L.; Nyunt, M.S.; Feng, L.; Gao, Q.; Lim, M.L.; Collinson, S.L.; Chong, M.S.; Lim, W.S.; Lee, T.S.; et al. Metabolic Syndrome and the Risk of Mild Cognitive Impairment and Progression to Dementia: Follow-up of the Singapore Longitudinal Ageing Study Cohort. JAMA Neurol. 2016, 73, 456–463. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Ng, T.P.; Niti, M.; Chiam, P.C.; Kua, E.H. Ethnic differences in cognitive performance on Mini-Mental State Examination in Asians. Am. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry 2007, 15, 130–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Lee, C.K.; Collinson, S.L.; Feng, L.; Ng, T.P. Preliminary normative neuropsychological data for an elderly chinese population. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2012, 26, 321–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. HPB-MOH Clinical Practice Guidelines on Obesity. Available online: http://www.moh.gov.sg/cpg (accessed on 3 January 2022).
  34. Tay, J.C.; Sule, A.A.; Chew, E.K.; Tey, J.S.; Lau, T.; Lee, S.; Lee, S.H.; Leong, C.K.; Lim, S.T.; Low, L.P.; et al. Ministry of Health Clinical Practice Guidelines: Hypertension. Singap. Med. J. 2018, 59, 17–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  35. Goh, S.Y.; Ang, S.B.; Bee, Y.M.; Chen, Y.T.; Gardner, D.S.; Ho, E.T.; Adaikan, K.; Lee, Y.C.; Lee, C.H.; Lim, F.S.; et al. Ministry of Health Clinical Practice Guidelines: Diabetes Mellitus. Singap. Med. J. 2014, 55, 334–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Tai, E.S.; Chia, B.L.; Bastian, A.C.; Chua, T.; Ho, S.C.; Koh, T.S.; Low, L.P.; Tey, J.S.; Poh, K.K.; Tan, C.E.; et al. Ministry of Health Clinical Practice Guidelines: Lipids. Singap. Med. J. 2017, 58, 155–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Nyunt, M.S.Z.; Jin, A.Z.; Fones, C.S.L.; Ng, T.P. Criterion-based validity and reliability of the Geriatric Depression Screening Scale (GDS-15) in a large validation sample of community-living Asian older adults. Aging Ment. Health 2009, 13, 376–382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Sudo, F.K.; Amado, P.; Alves, G.S.; Laks, J.; Engelhardt, E. A continuum of executive function deficits in early subcortical vascular cognitive impairment: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Dement. Neuropsychol. 2017, 11, 371–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  39. Qin, N.Y.; Yang, F.Q.; Wang, Y.T.; Li, S.P. Quantitative determination of eight components in rhizome (Jianghuang) and tuberous root (Yujin) of Curcuma longa using pressurized liquid extraction and gas chromatography-mass spectrometry. J. Pharm. Biomed. Anal. 2007, 43, 486–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Shoba, G.; Joy, D.; Joseph, T.; Majeed, M.; Rajendran, R.; Srinivas, P.S. Influence of piperine on the pharmacokinetics of curcumin in animals and human volunteers. Planta Med. 1998, 64, 353–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Rogers, G.B.; Keating, D.J.; Young, R.L.; Wong, M.L.; Licinio, J.; Wesselingh, S. From gut dysbiosis to altered brain function and mental illness: Mechanisms and pathways. Mol. Psychiatry 2016, 21, 738–748. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Lopresti, A.L. The Problem of Curcumin and Its Bioavailability: Could Its Gastrointestinal Influence Contribute to Its Overall Health-Enhancing Effects? Adv. Nutr. 2018, 9, 41–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  43. Di Meo, F.; Margarucci, S.; Galderisi, U.; Crispi, S.; Peluso, G. Curcumin, Gut Microbiota, and Neuroprotection. Nutrients 2019, 11, 2426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Moghaddam, N.S.A.; Oskouie, M.N.; Butler, A.E.; Petit, P.X.; Barreto, G.E.; Sahebkar, A. Hormetic effects of curcumin: What is the evidence? J. Cell. Physiol. 2019, 234, 10060–10071. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Significant associations between curry consumption and Digit Span (Backward), Verbal Fluency (Animals) and Block Design.
Figure 1. Significant associations between curry consumption and Digit Span (Backward), Verbal Fluency (Animals) and Block Design.
Nutrients 14 01189 g001
Figure 2. Significant associations between curry consumption and Digit Span (Backward), Verbal Fluency (Animals) and Block Design in subgroups of participants with and without cardiometabolic and vascular disease.
Figure 2. Significant associations between curry consumption and Digit Span (Backward), Verbal Fluency (Animals) and Block Design in subgroups of participants with and without cardiometabolic and vascular disease.
Nutrients 14 01189 g002
Table 1. Baseline characteristics of SLAS participants (N = 2751) by curry consumption categories.
Table 1. Baseline characteristics of SLAS participants (N = 2751) by curry consumption categories.
Never/Rarely<Once/Month≥Once/Month to <Once/Week≥Once/Week to Dailyp
N of participants 519 1306 559 367
Sex: Male31.2(162)33.8(442)39.4(220)48.2(177)<0.001
Age, years67.9±8.265.7±7.165.2±7.264.9±7.3<0.001
Ethnicity: Chinese98.3(510)97.6(1275)95.5(534)84.5(310)<0.001
Non-Chinese1.7(9)2.4(31)4.5(25)15.5(57)
Education: None28.3(147)15.9(208)13.4(75)15.5(57)<0.001
1–6 years42.4(220)39.4(514)38.3(214)33.2(122)
>6 years29.3(152)44.7(584)48.3(270)51.2(188)
Smoking: Never81.1(421)82.5(1077)81.9(458)77.4(284)0.216
Past smoker11.8(61)9.7(127)10.7(60)11.2(41)
Current smoker7.1(37)7.8(102)7.3(41)11.4(42)
Alcohol ≥ once/week4.6(24)1.8(24)3.2(18)4.1(15)0.006
Physical activity score2.311.732.421.612.371.782.441.850.618
Social activity score3.072.653.192.583.432.663.202.590.146
Productive activity score3.831.853.961.844.211.834.031.880.005
BMI, kg/m223.93.723.83.724.13.523.42.90.392
Central obesity46.6(242)54.2(708)54.2 (303)53.7(197)0.023
Hypertension60.3(313)54.1(706)57.8(323)57.8(212)0.077
Diabetes or FBG > 5.6 mmol/L25.0(130)26.3(343)28.1(157)26.2(96)0.723
High triglyceride41.2(214)44.6(583)45.8(256)44.1(162)0.468
Low HDL-Cholesterol44.9(233)49.4(645)49.7(278)47.1(173)0.297
Cardiac diseases9.8(51)7.9(103)6.3(35)10.1(37)0.673
GDS depression score1.452.250.881.641.032.081.292.40<0.001
Continuous variables are normally distributed and expressed as mean ± SD. Categorical variables are expressed as % (N). p-values are based on ANOVA for continuous variables and chi-squared tests for categorical variables.
Table 2. Crude mean estimates of neurocognitive performance at baseline and follow-up by curry consumption levels.
Table 2. Crude mean estimates of neurocognitive performance at baseline and follow-up by curry consumption levels.
Neurocognitive PerformanceCurry ConsumptionBaselineBetween Group ANOVA, pFollow-UpBetween-Group ANOVA, p
NMean±SELinearVs Never/RarelynMean±SELinearVs Never/Rarely
MMSENever or rarely51627.1±3.27<0.001Reference27227.73.18<0.001Reference
Occasionally (<once a month)129728.1±2.40 <0.00186228.82.41 <0.001
Often (>once a month, <once a week)55528.4±1.86 <0.00138228.92.04 <0.001
Very often (>once a week to daily36628.0±2.70 <0.00123128.92.26 <0.001
Digit span (forward)Never or rarely4780.07±1.000.414Reference167−0.031.020.316Reference
Occasionally (<once a month)11080.05±1.05 0.723678−0.011.05 0.847
Often (>once a month, <once a week)4850.11±1.07 0.551293−0.051.07 0.847
Very often (>once a week to daily324−0.03±1.06 0.159169−0.121.08 0.434
Digit span (backward)Never or rarely470−0.24±1.020.004Reference166−0.110.930.007Reference
Occasionally (<once a month)1098−0.18±1.00 0.2896740.050.98 0.062
Often (>once a month, <once a week)4800.02±1.05 <0.0012920.141.10 0.011
Very often (>once a week to daily317−0.12±1.06 0.1171690.171.05 0.011
RAVLT, delayed recallNever or rarely436−0.34±1.130.084Reference162−0.141.090.985Reference
Occasionally (<once a month)1060−0.15±1.02 0.002676−0.021.07 0.221
Often (>once a month, <once a week)465−0.15±1.01 0.006288−0.061.07 0.451
Very often (>once a week to daily299−0.20±1.10 0.084169−0.091.02 0.703
Visual Reproduction, delayed recallNever or rarely421−0.25±1.100.181Reference1600.041.000.405Reference
Occasionally (<once a month)1043−0.15±1.11 0.1386680.061.01 0.806
Often (>once a month, <once a week)465−0.07±1.05 0.0192890.131.09 0.367
Very often (>once a week to daily291−0.18±1.17 0.4231660.100.91 0.633
Verbal Fluency-AnimalsNever or rarely445−0.27±1.07<0.001Reference167−0.061.070.005Reference
Occasionally (<once a month)1066−0.16±0.99 0.0446750.001.00 0.498
Often (>once a month, <once a week)4580.00±1.03 <0.0012920.111.03 0.086
Very often (>once a week to daily308−0.05±1.01 0.0031670.190.86 0.021
Trail-Making ANever or rarely281−0.39±1.440.005Reference1590.101.380.013Reference
Occasionally (<once a month)899−0.09±1.18 <0.0016630.011.16 0.389
Often (>once a month, <once a week)397−0.12±1.02 0.005291−0.061.32 0.180
Very often (>once a week to daily238−0.17±1.58 0.046164−0.210.82 0.019
Trail Making-BNever or rarely239−0.32±1.250.090Reference1410.492.150.021Reference
Occasionally (<once a month)825−0.14±1.20 0.0566300.151.46 0.014
Often (>once a month, <once a week)362−0.11±1.38 0.0482740.171.25 0.035
Very often (>once a week to daily218−0.11±1.24 0.0771570.001.13 0.004
Block DesignNever or rarely324−0.38±1.130.002Reference162−0.181.000.082Reference
Occasionally (<once a month)886−0.18±1.03 0.004671−0.050.99 0.120
Often (>once a month, <once a week)343−0.06±1.03 <0.0012880.020.99 0.035
Very often (>once a week to daily232−0.14±1.11 0.010166−0.010.92 0.113
Table 3. Mixed linear model analyses: covariate-adjusted mean neurocognitive z-scores at baseline and follow-up by curry consumption.
Table 3. Mixed linear model analyses: covariate-adjusted mean neurocognitive z-scores at baseline and follow-up by curry consumption.
Neurocognitive PerformanceCurry Consumption Baseline Mean ± SEFollow-Up Mean ± SEGrouppδ95%CICohen’s d
MMSENever or rarelyA26.9±0.19825.6±0.207Overall0.284
Occasionally (<once a month)B27.1±0.19325.7±0.200B vs. A0.1410.124−0.041,0.2890.048
Often (>once a month, <once a week)C27.2±0.20425.7±0.213C vs. A0.0620.186−0.010,0.3820.072
Very often (>once a week to dailyD27.0±0.20325.8±0.217D vs. A0.3140.115−0.109,0.3390.045
Digit span (forward)Never or rarelyA−0.23±0.108−0.38±0.119Overall0.713
Occasionally (<once a month)B−0.30±0.106−0.40±0.111B vs. A0.2990.048−0.139,0.0430.046
Often (>once a month, <once a week)C−0.25±0.111−0.37±0.120C vs. A0.9060.006−0.114,0.1010.006
Very often (>once a week to daily)D−0.28±0.110−0.38±0.124D vs. A0.7030.024−0.147,0.0990.023
Digit span (backward)Never or rarelyA−0.23±0.105−0.06±0.114Overall0.012
Occasionally (<once a month)B−0.23±0.103−0.03±0.108B vs. A0.7000.017−0.070,0.1050.017
Often (>once a month, <once a week)C−0.17±0.108−0.05±0.115C vs. A0.0100.1350.032,0.2390.132
Very often (>once a week to daily)D−0.12±0.1060.09±0.119D vs. A0.0280.1330.014,0.2520.130
RAVLT, delayed recallNever or rarelyA−0.44±0.113−0.30±0.123Overall0.156
Occasionally (<once a month)B−0.32±0.111−0.23±0.115B vs. A0.0410.0960.004,0.1890.091
Often (>once a month, <once a week)C−0.34±0.115−0.22±0.124C vs. A0.1180.087−0.022,0.1960.083
Very often (>once a week to daily)D−0.27±0.114−0.23±0.127D vs. A0.0690.117−0.009,0.2420.111
VR, delayed recallNever or rarelyA−0.42±0.116−0.16±0.123Overall0.303
Occasionally (<once a month)B−0.42±0.113−0.25±0.117B vs. A0.335−0.045−0.137,0.0470.041
Often (>once a month, <once a week)C−0.35±0.118−0.14±0.124C vs. A0.4220.044−0.064,0.153−0.040
Very often (>once a week to daily)D−0.37±0.118−0.20±0.126D vs. A0.8590.011−0.114,0.137−0.010
Verbal Fluency-AnimalsNever or rarelyA−0.57±0.108−0.43±0.117Overall0.017
Occasionally (<once a month)B−0.59±0.106−0.45±0.110B vs. A0.705−0.0170.103,0.0700.016
Often (>once a month, <once a week)C−0.47±0.111−0.37±0.118C vs. A0.112−0.0830.019,0.1850.081
Very often (>once a week to daily)D−0.46±0.109−0.28±0.121D vs. A0.0260.1600.134,0.2510.131
Trail-Making ANever or rarelyA−0.61±0.178−0.53±0.181Overall0.375
Occasionally (<once a month)B−0.41±0.172−0.31±0.174B vs. A0.3150.060−0.057,0.1760.047
Often (>once a month, <once a week)C−0.42±0.176−0.43±0.180C vs. A0914−0.007−0.143,0.1280.006
Very often (>once a week to daily)D−0.46±0.176−0.49±0.182D vs. A0.496−0.055−0.214,0.1040.044
Trail Making-BNever or rarelyA−0.64±0.198−0.20±0.209Overall0.975
Occasionally (<once a month)B−0.52±0.191−0.19±0.198B vs. A0.0710.008−0.131,0.1480.007
Often (>once a month, <once a week)C−0.49±0.195−0.29±0.208C vs. A0.083−0.023−0.186,0.1390.019
Very often (>once a week to daily)D−0.45±0.194−0.31±0.212D vs. A0.097−0.013−0.203,0.1760.011
Block DesignNever or rarelyA−0.48±0.142−0.42±0.146Overall0.003
Occasionally (<once a month)B−0.42±0.140−0.29±0.141B vs. A0.0390.0960.005,0.1880.090
Often (>once a month, <once a week)C−0.28±0.146−0.22±0.146C vs. A<0.0010.1980.089,0.3080.186
Very often (>once a week to daily)D−0.30±0.145−0.27±0.147D vs. A0.0100.1660.040,0.2920.156
Covariables in model: age, sex, ethnicity (Chinese versus non-Chinese), educational level, leisure time activity score, smoking (nonsmoker, ex-smoker, current smoker), alcohol (never, <once/week, ≥once/week), central obesity, hypertension, high fasting blood glucose or diabetes, high triglyceride, low HDL-cholesterol, cardiac diseases and GDS. MMSE: Time, p ≤ 0.001, Group × time, p = 0.458; Digit span (forward): time, p = 0.001, group × time, p = 0.950; Digit span (backward): time, p ≤ 0.001, group × time, p = 0.849 RAVLT, delayed recall: time, p = 0.012, group × time, p = 0.835; Visual Reproduction, delayed recall: time, p = 0.001, group × time, p = 0.759; Verbal Fluency-Animals: time, p = 0.001, group × time, p = 0.937; Trail-Making A: time, p = 0.027, group × time, p = 0.130; Trail Making-B: time, p = 0.001, group × time, p = 0.107; Block Design: time, p = 0.087, group × time, p = 0.803.
Table 4. Mixed linear model analyses: covariate-adjusted mean neurocognitive z-scores at baseline and follow-up by cardiometabolic morbidity and curry consumption groups.
Table 4. Mixed linear model analyses: covariate-adjusted mean neurocognitive z-scores at baseline and follow-up by cardiometabolic morbidity and curry consumption groups.
Neurocognitive TestCurry > Once/Month to Daily BaselineFollow-UpGrouppδ95%CICohen’s d
Mean95% CIMean95% CI
Cardiometabolic or cardiac diseases
MMSENoA26.926.6, 27.327.527.2, 27.9
YesB27.127.0, 27.427.627.2, 28.0A vs. B0.2130.095−0.054,0.2440.037
Digit span (forward)NoA−0.24−0.44, −0.05−0.37−0.58, −0.17
YesB−0.25−0.45, −0.05−0.43−0.64, −0.21A vs. B0.450−0.031−0.110,0.0490.029
Digit span (backward)NoA−0.23−0.41, −0.04−0.06−0.26, 0.14
YesB−0.13−0.32, 0.06−0.05−0.16, 0.25A vs. B0.0100.1010.024,0.1780.098
RAVLT, delayed recallNoA−0.35−0.56, −0.14−0.22−0.44, −0.01
YesB−0.29−0.49, −0.98−0.22−0.44, 0.00A vs. B0.3870.036−0.045,0.1170.034
VR, delayed recallNoA−0.47−0.68, −0.26−0.23−0.45, −0.02
YesB−0.38−0.59, −0.17−0.21−0.43, 0.01A vs. B0.2020.052−0.028,0.1320.047
Verbal Fluency-AnimalsNoA−0.540.73, −0.34−0.37−0.57, −0.16
YesB−0.40−0.60, −0.20−0.28−0.49, −0.07A vs. B0.005 0.1080.032,0.1840.106
Trail-Making ANoA−0.54−0.87, −0.22−0.32−0.65, 0.00
YesB−0.49−0.81, −0.17−0.44−0.76, −0.11A vs. B0.558−0.030−0.129,0.0700.024
Trail Making-BNoA−0.49−0.85, −0.12−0.05−0.42, 0.32
YesB−0.41−0.77, −0.05−0.07−0.45, 0.30A vs. B0.6610.027−0.093,0.1460.021
Block DesignNoA−0.40−0.66, −0.14−0.28−0.54, −0.02
YesB−0.29−0.55, −0.02−0.27−0.53, −0.01A vs. B0.1190.063−0.016,0.1430.060
No cardiometabolic or cardiac diseases
MMSENoC27.026.2, 27.127.326.8, 27.8
YesD26.526.0, 27.027.226.6, 27.7 C vs. D0.396−0.158−0.522,0.2070.061
Digit span (forward)NoC−0.30−0.53, −0.06−0.19−0.46, 0.07
YesD−0.20−0.46, 0.07−0.28−0.59, 0.04C vs. D0.9190.010−0.185,0.2050.010
Digit span (backward)NoC−0.28−0.50, −0.050.05−0.20, 0.30
YesD−0.10−0.16, 0.360.26−0.05, 0.56C vs. D0.0030.2910.101,0.4800.284
RAVLT, delayed recallNoC−0.22−0.47, 0.03−0.11−0.38, 0.16
YesD−0.33−0.461, 0.05−0.14−0.47, 0.18C vs. D0.508−0.068−0.269,0.1330.064
VR, delayed recallNoC−0.39−0.64, −0.13−0.34−0.61, −0.07
YesD−0.48−0.77, −0.19−0.06−0.38, 0.25C vs. D0.3460.095−0.103,0.2940.086
Verbal Fluency-AnimalsNoC−0.46−0.69, −0.22−0.46−0.71, −0.20
YesD−0.34−0.61, −0.07−0.15−0.45, −0.16C vs. D0.0240.2150.028,0.4020.211
Trail-Making ANoC−0.49−0.87, −0.11−0.35−0.73, 0.02
YesD−0.41−0.84, 0.02−0.67−1.10, 0.24C vs. D0.372−0.118−0.376,0.1410.093
Trail Making-BNoC−0.33−0.74, 0.08−0.14−0.58, 0.31
YesD−0.30−0.76, 0.16−0.52−1.03, 0.00C vs. D0.255−0.176−0.478,0.1270.139
Block DesignNoC−0.47−0.78, −0.16−0.40−0.70, −0.10
YesD−0.16−0.53, 0.22−0.06−0.40, 0.27C vs. D0.0030.3250.110,0.5410.305
Covariables in the model: age, sex, ethnicity (Chinese versus non-Chinese), educational level, leisure time activity score, smoking (nonsmoker, ex-smoker, current smoker), alcohol (never, <once/week, ≥once/week), central obesity, hypertension, high fasting blood glucose or diabetes, high triglyceride, low HDL-cholesterol, cardiac diseases and GDS score. Hypothesis-driven pairwise comparisons of interest are shown for A vs. B and C vs. D. MMSE: Group p = 0.005, Tim, p ≤ 0.001, Group × time, p = 0.949; Digit span (forward): group, p = 0.432, time, p = 0.191, group × time, p = 0.193; Digit span (backward): group, p ≤ 0.001, time, p ≤ 0.001, group × time, p = 0.581; RAVLT, delayed recall: group, p = 0.278, time, p = 0.028, group × time, p = 0.869; Visual Reproduction, delayed recall: group, p = 0.474, time, p = 0.001, group × time, p = 0.235; Verbal Fluency-Animals: group, p = 0.004, time, p = 0.018, group × time, p = 0.517; Trail-Making A: group, p = 0.756, time, p = 0.604, group × time, p = 0.090; Trail Making-B: group, p = 0.649, time, p = 0.023, group × time, p = 0.064 Block Design: group, p = 0.011, time, p = 0.198, group × time, p = 0.610.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Ng, T.P.; Nyunt, M.S.Z.; Gao, Q.; Gwee, X.; Chua, D.Q.L.; Yap, K.B. Curcumin-Rich Curry Consumption and Neurocognitive Function from 4.5-Year Follow-Up of Community-Dwelling Older Adults (Singapore Longitudinal Ageing Study). Nutrients 2022, 14, 1189. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14061189

AMA Style

Ng TP, Nyunt MSZ, Gao Q, Gwee X, Chua DQL, Yap KB. Curcumin-Rich Curry Consumption and Neurocognitive Function from 4.5-Year Follow-Up of Community-Dwelling Older Adults (Singapore Longitudinal Ageing Study). Nutrients. 2022; 14(6):1189. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14061189

Chicago/Turabian Style

Ng, Tze Pin, Ma Shwe Zin Nyunt, Qi Gao, Xinyi Gwee, Denise Qian Ling Chua, and Keng Bee Yap. 2022. "Curcumin-Rich Curry Consumption and Neurocognitive Function from 4.5-Year Follow-Up of Community-Dwelling Older Adults (Singapore Longitudinal Ageing Study)" Nutrients 14, no. 6: 1189. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14061189

APA Style

Ng, T. P., Nyunt, M. S. Z., Gao, Q., Gwee, X., Chua, D. Q. L., & Yap, K. B. (2022). Curcumin-Rich Curry Consumption and Neurocognitive Function from 4.5-Year Follow-Up of Community-Dwelling Older Adults (Singapore Longitudinal Ageing Study). Nutrients, 14(6), 1189. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14061189

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop