Next Article in Journal
The Effects of One Anastomosis Gastric Bypass Surgery on the Gastrointestinal Tract
Next Article in Special Issue
Food Addiction in Eating Disorders: A Cluster Analysis Approach and Treatment Outcome
Previous Article in Journal
Adherence to the Mediterranean Diet among Children and Youth in the Mediterranean Region in Croatia: A Comparative Study
Previous Article in Special Issue
Impact of COVID-19 Lockdown in Eating Disorders: A Multicentre Collaborative International Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Are Peripheral Biomarkers Determinants of Eating Styles in Childhood and Adolescence Obesity? A Cross-Sectional Study

by
Lorena Desdentado
1,2,3,
Jaime Navarrete
2,
María Folgado-Alufre
2,3,
Ana de Blas
4,
Jéssica Navarro-Siurana
2,
Francisco Ponce
1,4,
Guadalupe Molinari
1,
Andrea Jimeno-Martínez
1,5,
Azahara I. Rupérez
1,5,
Gloria Bueno-Lozano
1,5,6,
Aida Cuenca-Royo
1,7,
Emili Corbella
1,8,
Zaida Agüera
1,9,
Rosa M. Baños
1,2,3 and
Julio Álvarez-Pitti
1,4,*
1
CIBER Fisiopatología Obesidad y Nutrición (CIBEROBN), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Av. Monforte de Lemos, 3-5. Pabellón 11, Planta 0, 28029 Madrid, Spain
2
Polibienestar Research Institute, University of Valencia, Calle Serpis, 29, 46022 Valencia, Spain
3
Department of Personality, Evaluation, and Psychological Treatments, University of Valencia, Avda. Blasco Ibañez, 21, 46010 Valencia, Spain
4
Pediatric Department, Consorcio Hospital General Universitario de Valencia, Avda. Tres Cruces, 2, 46014 Valencia, Spain
5
Growth, Exercise, Nutrition and Development (GENUD) Research Group, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Instituto Agroalimentario de Aragón, Universidad de Zaragoza, Calle Miguel Servet, 177, 50013 Zaragoza, Spain
6
Paediatric Endocrinology Department, Clinical Hospital Lozano Blesa, Zaragoza, Avda. San Juan Bosco, 50009 Zaragoza, Spain
7
Integrative Pharmacology and Systems Neurosciences Research Group, Neurosciences Research Program, Hospital del Mar Medical Research Institute (IMIM), 08003 Barcelona, Spain
8
Cardiovascular Risk Unit, Internal Medicine Department, Bellvitge University Hospital—IDIBELL, Feixa Llarga, s/n, 08907 Barcelona, Spain
9
Department of Public Health, Mental Health and Perinatal Nursing, Health Sciences Campus Bellvitge, School of Nursing, University of Barcelona, Feixa Llarga, s/n, 08907 Barcelona, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2022, 14(2), 305; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14020305
Submission received: 21 October 2021 / Revised: 24 November 2021 / Accepted: 30 November 2021 / Published: 12 January 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Eating Disorders and Obesity: Through the Life Course)

Abstract

:
Disturbances in eating behaviors have been widely related to obesity. However, little is known about the role of obesity-related biomarkers in shaping habitual patterns of eating behaviors (i.e., eating styles) in childhood. The objective of the present study was to explore the relationships between several biomarkers crucially involved in obesity (ghrelin, insulin resistance, and leptin/adiponectin ratio) and eating styles in children and adolescents with obesity. Seventy participants aged between 8 and 16 (56.2% men) fulfilled the Spanish version of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire for Children to measure external, emotional, and restrained eating styles. In addition, concentrations of ghrelin, leptin, adiponectin, insulin, and glucose were obtained through a blood test. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses controlling for age and sex were computed for each eating style. Results indicated that individuals with higher ghrelin concentration levels showed lower scores in restrained eating (β = −0.61, p < 0.001). The total model explained 32% of the variance of the restrained pattern. No other relationships between obesity-related biomarkers and eating behaviors were found. This study highlights that one of the obesity-risk factors, namely lower plasma ghrelin levels, is substantially involved in a well-known maladaptive eating style, restraint eating, in childhood obesity.

1. Introduction

Obesity is a multifactorial disease that is currently considered a public health problem. Specifically, the prevalence of obesity has increased rapidly in childhood and adolescence (5–19 years old), with more than 100 million affected in 2016 [1]. Although recent literature has suggested maintenance, even slight decrease, of childhood obesity prevalence in Spain, it continues to be one of the highest prevalence of obesity and overweight in Europe [2].
These high prevalence figures are partly because there is currently no effective treatment for obesity, which can be generalized to most children and adolescents [3]. Recently, the American Association of Clinical Endocrinologists [4] and the European Association for the Study of Obesity [5] has proposed a new diagnostic term, namely “adiposity-based chronic disease” (ABCD). The phrase “adiposity-based” is justified because the disease is primarily due to abnormalities in the mass, distribution, and/or function of adipose tissue. This fact reinforces the concept of obesity as a chronic, complex, multifactorial, and incurable disease at present. For this reason, it is important to continue exploring the different roots of obesity since this is the only way to develop individualized treatments that ensure a higher success rate.
Eating behaviors stand out as crucial proximal determinants of body weight and the motivation to eat since childhood [6,7]. Eating styles represent dispositional tendencies of food intake and have been closely related to obesity in adults [8] and children and adolescents [9,10]. According to the three main theories about impaired food consumption (i.e., the external, psychosomatic, and restraint theories), three predominant eating styles have been identified [11]. External eating refers to the predominance of external environmental factors, such as smell or sight, as determinants of eating behavior, independently of the internal bodily state (i.e., feeling hungry or satiated) [12,13]. Emotional eating emphasizes the influence of emotional factors so that eating behavior is prompted in response to emotional arousal states, such as fear, anxiety, or anger, again without considering internal physiological signals of hunger and satiety [14]. Restraint eating concerns the attempt to control eating behavior through cognitive control and suppression of internal hunger signals to lose weight or avoid weight gain [13].
Previous research has shown that emotional eating can be considered an “obese” eating pattern, since it has been consistently found that higher emotional eating is related to overweight and long-term weight gain in both adults [8,15,16] and children [17], although the prevalence in childhood (7 to 12 years old) is actually low [11]. Emotional eating seems to emerge in adolescence [18,19] and is more prevalent in females than in males [20].
Restrained eating is also conceptualized as an “obese” eating style [21]. Overall, the distinctive features of restrained eaters are restricting food intake for long periods of time to lose or maintain weight and expressing dissatisfaction towards their body size and/or shape [22]. According to the restraint theory, when the cognitive control of eating is disrupted, restrained eaters tend to show a more disinhibited behavior, increasing their food intake and overeating [23]. Hence, although paradoxical, the involvement of restrained eating in obesity, as well as eating disorders, is not surprising [24].
Unlike emotional and restrained eating, some studies suggest that external eating might not be an “obese eating” pattern (i.e., an eating tendency related to a higher body mass index (BMI)) since levels of external eating seem to be similar between normal-weight and overweight individuals [8,25]. Moreover, it has been argued that external eating could represent an adaptive response that helped survive whenever food is available in periods of food shortage [26]. However, it should be noted that these findings contrast with evidence indicating that overweight and obese individuals show poor interoceptive abilities (i.e., awareness of internal bodily cues accompanying homeostatic states such as satiety) [27], which might predispose them to rely on external cues rather than internal signals of satiety and, therefore, to less adaptive eating behaviors [28]. In this line, Mata and colleagues found that external eating was positively related to the insula activation—which is considered a crucial brain hub for interoceptive processing [29]—in adolescents with excess weight, whereas this relationship was negative among healthy weight adolescents [30]. These results suggest the presence of an altered relationship between insula function and interoceptive/exteroceptive processing in adolescents with excess weight. Moreover, it is known that children with higher reward sensitivity (including sensitivity towards external cues of appetitive stimuli) are more vulnerable to become overweight [31], although this relationship is not straightforward. Specifically, external eating and food responsive behavior mediate the relationship between weight and reward sensitivity in childhood [32].
In the regulation of appetite, not only the central nervous system (CNS), but the adrenal glands, the pancreas, and the gastrointestinal tract are involved, and the adipose tissue also plays a relevant role [33]. Adipose tissue is a metabolically active organ involved in multiple biological processes and communicates through the secretion of peptides and hormones, known as adipokines [34]. As mentioned above, obesity is characterized by a pathological expansion and/or unhealthy distribution of body fat, producing adipose tissue dysfunction. This dysfunction produces a disbalance in the homeostasis of children and adolescents with obesity, favoring a profile characterized by resistance to insulin action (with a secondary elevation of insulin), resistance to leptin action (which favors hyperleptinemia), and low adiponectin and ghrelin values [35]. This profile increases the risk of developing associated cardiometabolic diseases [36] but also could be involved in (maladaptive) eating behaviors in childhood obesity. However, no previous studies have examined this relation.
Ghrelin is a well-known gut hormone involved not only in food intake, but also in energy storage, stimulating adipogenesis [37,38]. Specifically, circulating ghrelin levels increase during intake restriction, leading to increased appetite [39] and then fall quickly after ingestion [39]. Thus, it is postulated that one primary role of ghrelin is to act as a meal initiator. However, to our best knowledge, research on its association with eating styles is scarce in normal-weight [40,41] and obese adults [42] and even absent in obese children and adolescents.
The main metabolic disturbance driven by obesity is insulin resistance (IR) [43]. Insulin levels rise and fall rapidly in response to feeding and starvation. These changing insulin levels orchestrate the metabolic switch between anabolism and catabolism. Glucose is the main regulator of insulin, but also other nutrients, hormones, and the autonomic nervous system, influence its serum levels [44].
As insulin, leptin, which is mostly released by adipose tissue, is well-known to reduce appetite and increase energy expenditure. In obesity, leptin resistance is produced due to the excess adiposity, and, therefore, leptin does not properly reduce food consumption, leading to increased body weight [45]. Circulating leptin levels throughout the day do not undergo large variations, showing a circadian rhythm and oscillatory pattern [46].
Adiponectin is also almost exclusively produced in adipose tissue [47]. The mechanisms of this adipokine in appetite regulation are intricate, and it could be an appetite stimulator or inhibiting factor depending on feeding status, the content of glucose in cerebrospinal fluid, and the degree of fatness [48]. In contrast to other adipokines, the circulating levels of adiponectin are inversely proportional to total fat mass [49], and also with fasting insulin concentration and plasma triglycerides, but positively with the plasma cholesterol contained in HDL [50].
Leptin and adiponectin are regulated in an opposite manner in most cases. Children and adolescents with obesity tend to have higher leptin levels and lower adiponectin levels [51]. This unfavorable leptin/adiponectin ratio has been proposed as a functional biomarker of adipose tissue inflammation and seems to be a good indicator of cardiometabolic risk associated with obesity and metabolic syndrome [52]. However, it remains unknown whether this biomarker acts as an underlying factor of eating styles in obesity.
Thus, eating behavior is modulated by both external and internal signals from the body [53]. External cues include environmental factors such as the hedonic properties of food, as well as social factors such as other people’s behavior [54]. Internal signals usually refer to physiological processes underlying feelings of hunger and satiety, such as the blood concentrations of ghrelin, glucose, and leptin [55]. At this point, it becomes evident that eating behavior and, ultimately, obesity are highly complex. Disentangling the multiple factors involved and the relationships between them can help us better understand obesity and, therefore, develop more effective prevention and treatment methods, especially in the early stages of human development. As pointed out by Verbiest and colleagues [10], interdisciplinary work is needed to do so.
Despite a certain amount of research focused on the regulatory role of these hormonal biomarkers in eating behavior in obese samples, most studies have considered imminent appetite and food intake as outcome variables. However, little is known regarding the contribution of peripheral obesity-related biomarkers to eating styles, that is, to (relatively) well-established patterns of eating behaviors such as emotional, external, and restrained eating.
The aim of this study is to explore the relationship between several peripheral obesity-related biomarkers (i.e., ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio) and eating styles (i.e., external, emotional, and restrained) in a Spanish sample of obese children and adolescents. Specifically, based on preliminary research on this field, it is hypothesized that poorer obesity-related biomarkers in obese children and adolescents, namely lower ghrelin, higher IR, and higher leptin/adiponectin ratio, will be associated with higher scores on external, emotional, and restrained eating.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Participants in the current study were recruited from the Obesity and Cardiovascular Risk Unit at General University Hospital Consortium of Valencia (CHGUV) and Pediatric Endocrinology Department at the University Clinical Hospital “Lozano Blesa” (HCULB) in Zaragoza (Spain). Data were collected between November 2019 and September 2021 as a part of the Eat4HealthyLife research project, a multicentered study that explores potential risk factors of obesity and eating disorders (EDs). Eligible criteria considered were the following: aged between 8 and 16 years old, diagnosis of obesity (BMI Z-score > 2 standard deviations according to the scales of their reference group by age and sex), and not having received or not currently receiving obesity treatment in our outpatient clinic. None of the subjects included suffer from any other disease or eating disorder. The sample consisted of 70 children and adolescents with obesity. The mean age of the sample was 12.36 (SD = 2.19), and 56.2% were boys.

2.2. Measures

2.2.1. Sociodemographic Data and Anthropometrics

Anthropometric measurements were collected under standardized conditions with patients wearing light clothing and no shoes. Bioelectrical impedance analysis, using a TANITA TBF-410 M over a horizontal and hard surface and to the nearest 0.1 kg, was the selected method to measure body weight and composition. Height was measured to the nearest 0.5 cm using a portable height board (SECA 216). BMI z-score (BMIz) was calculated for each patient. BMIz correlates to growth percentile charts and allows children’s relative weight follow-up from childhood up to adolescence. Participants’ obesity was defined with BMIz scores greater than 2 SD following World Health Organization (WHO) growth charts [56].

2.2.2. Metabolic Assessment

Blood samples were collected from fasting participants scheduled early in the morning. Glucose and insulin measurements and a lipid panel were requested for every patient. The homeostatic model assessment (HOMA) index of IR was measured as the product of insulin (μU/mL) and glucose (mmol/L) divided by 22.5. Hyperinsulinism was defined according to pubertal stage normative data [57].
Adipokine laboratory measurements were obtained from the Cardiovascular Risk Unit Laboratory at Hospital General of Valencia (Valencia, Spain). Ghrelin, leptin, and adiponectin were measured using commercially available BMS 2192, BMS 2039, and BMS 2032/2 eBioscience ELISA kits. The leptin assay detection limit was set at 894 ng/L. Adiponectin assay range was set from 764 μg/L to 16,893 μg/L. The ghrelin assay was set from a mean value of 915 pg/mL.

2.2.3. Eating Styles

The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire for Children (DEBQ-C) [11,58] was used to measure eating styles, which is a 20 item self-report questionnaire rated in a 3-point Likert scale from 1 to 3 (1 = “no”, 2 = “sometimes”, 3 = “yes”) that assess three different eating behavior patterns: external (e.g., “Does walking past a candy store make you feel eating? ”), emotional (e.g., “Does worrying make you feel like eating?”), and restrained (e.g., “Do you intentionally eat food that helps you lose weight?”) eating. Higher scores on each subscale are indicative of greater emotional, external, or restrained eating. Emotional eating and restrained eating subscales are composed of 7 items each, whereas external eating is composed of 6 items. Total scores for each subscale are computed with the mean score of their corresponding items. In this sample, the internal consistency of each scale was acceptable (α = 0.87, α = 0.78, and α = 0.65, respectively). EDs were ruled out according to the diagnostic criteria of the DSM-5 [59].

2.3. Procedure

New patients arriving at the Obesity Risk Units were considered potential candidates to participate in this study. At their first appointment at the Unit, participants were weighed and measured to calculate BMIz. Once the patients were diagnosed with obesity, parents were offered the possibility for their children to voluntarily participate in the study. All parents of participants signed the informed consent documents before starting the assessment, in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The study was approved by the Ethics Committee at CHGUV (reference number: 4/2019) and the Research Ethics Committee of the Autonomous Community of Aragon (CEICA) (reference number: PI19/269). Then, patients who agreed to participate were scheduled to come to the hospital in a second session. In this session, participants underwent a blood test to measure biomarkers, and one hour later (after having breakfast), they completed several psychological measures in the context of the Eat4HealthyLife project, including the DEBQ-C, assisted by a trained psychologist. Specifically, the assessment protocol of this project included some neurocognitive measurements, including the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test Second Edition [60] and the Digit span subtest of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children [61]. However, they are not reported herein because they are not relevant for the objective of this study.
It should be noted that recruitment of participants was stopped during the lockdown period (between March and June 2020), as well as during the third wave of COVID-19 in Spain (between January and March 2021) when the incidence of cases was very high. From the beginning of the pandemic, the study sessions took place with appropriate safety measures, such as mask and ventilation in the room. None of the participants presented symptoms compatible with an infectious process either at the time of the evaluation or in the previous week.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

Data analyses were conducted using the SPSS v26 software (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA). First, descriptive statistics (mean, standard deviation) were performed to analyze the sociodemographic data of the sample. Second, Cronbach’s alpha was calculated to establish the internal consistency of the psychometrical measures, with coefficients above 0.70 being considered adequate [62].
Finally, three hierarchical multiple regressions were conducted to determine whether external, emotional, and restrained eating styles (DEBQ-C) were explained by ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio after controlling for sex and age. Thus, the control variables (sex and age) were entered at Block 1, and the rest of the independent variables were entered at Block 2. Preliminary analyses were performed to check normality, linearity, homoscedasticity, and multicollinearity assumptions, with no serious violations noted.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics

The means, standard deviations, ranges, and percentiles 25, 50, and 75 of the different measures for eating styles and the peripheral obesity-related biomarkers, as well as height and weight, are displayed in Table 1.

3.2. Hierarchical Multiple Regressions

3.2.1. External Eating

Regarding the first hierarchical multiple regression model, sex and age explained 5% of the variance of external eating, which did not reach statistical significance: F (2, 50) = 1.32, p = 0.277, R2 = 0.050. After entering ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio at Block 2, the total model did not statistically significantly explain external eating: F (5, 47) = 1.25, p = 0.303, R2 = 0.117. Table 2 shows the regression coefficients of the independent variables on external eating.

3.2.2. Emotional Eating

In the second hierarchical multiple regression model, sex and age explained 13% of the variance of emotional eating: F (2, 50) = 3.63 p = 0.034, R2 = 0.127. However, after entering ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio at Block 2, the total model did not statistically explain emotional eating: F (5, 47) = 2.36, p = 0.054, R2 = 0.201. Table 3 shows regression coefficients of the independent variables on emotional eating.

3.2.3. Restrained Eating

The third hierarchical multiple regression model showed that sex and age (Block 1) did not significantly explain the variance in restrained eating: F (2, 50) = 0.32 p = 0.727, R2 = 0.013. After entry of ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio at Block 2, the total model statistically significantly explained restrained eating: F (5, 47) = 4.37, p = 0.002, R2 = 0.317. Among them, ghrelin was the unique significant predictor of restrained eating (β = –0.61; p < 0.001), indicating that individuals with higher ghrelin levels showed lower scores on restrained eating, explaining 31% of its variance. Table 4 shows regression coefficients of the independent variables on restrained eating.

4. Discussion

The present study was aimed at examining the involvement of different peripheral obesity-related biomarkers (namely, ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio) to explain different eating styles, i.e., external, emotional, and restrained patterns, in childhood and adolescence obesity.
Our findings show that plasma ghrelin concentrations were negatively related to restrained eating after controlling for sex and age (nonsignificant) effects. In other words, a poor obesity-related biomarker (i.e., lower ghrelin concentrations) was related to restrained eating in childhood and adolescence obesity, as expected. These findings extend previous research that supports the restraint theory [21,23] by suggesting that restrained eating is not only an “obese” eating style, but also a “higher-risk-obesity” eating style. It should be noted that our results contrast with some previous studies conducted in normal-weight adults, indicating that restrained eating is positively correlated [41] or not correlated [40] with ghrelin. According to preliminary research, this discrepancy in the direction of the relationship could be due to the obesity status [63]. Moreover, different measures of restrained eating could reflect distinct conceptualizations of what it means, which might also explain inconsistencies in the literature in this regard. Mainly, scales measuring restrained eating differ in the purpose for which authors developed them [22], i.e., to assess chronic dieters who cyclically restrain their intake or over-eat [64], or individuals who success in reducing their intake [11]. Future studies should be cautious in this concern and simultaneously include individuals with normal and excess weight through different life course stages to disentangle the nature of this relationship. Finally, IR and leptin/adiponectin ratio were not significantly related to restrained eating. In the study conducted by Schur and colleagues [41], leptin and insulin were also measured, but they were not related to cognitive restraint, similar to our results.
External eating was hypothesized to be underlaid by high-risk peripheral biomarkers, specifically lower ghrelin levels, higher IR, and higher inflammation indicators (i.e., leptin/adiponectin ratio). However, our results showed no significant contribution from these biochemical indices to explain external eating once sex and age effects were partialized out. Although there are no previous studies examining the role of ghrelin, IR, leptin, and adiponectin in self-report eating styles in obesity, preliminary evidence exists regarding the influence of ghrelin on the brain activity involved in processing external food cues. In this regard, Malik and colleagues found an increased brain activity response to food-related visual cues after ghrelin administration, which was indeed correlated with self-reported hunger ratings [65]. However, our results suggest that this effect is not extended to the habitual external eating style in childhood obesity.
Similarly, contrary to what was hypothesized, emotional eating was not significantly related to any obesity-related biomarkers in our sample. Sex was the only statistically significant predictor of emotional eating, indicating that women showed higher scores on emotional eating than men, which is consistent with previous findings [20]. Noteworthy in this regard is a previous cohort study that found that higher leptin was cross-sectionally related to emotional overeating at age 7 years, but it was not prospectively associated with emotional overeating three years later after controlling for BMIz. However, this study used a parent-reported instead of a self-report measure of eating styles, that is, parents (or main caregivers) responded to the questionnaire instead of the children on their own (e.g., “My child eats more when annoyed”). Therefore, some biases of their parents could have influenced these results.
To our knowledge, this is the first study to comprehensively consider both physiological risk indicators of obesity and eating styles in a young sample. Theoretically, this study suggests that metabolic dysfunction of adipokines and IR might not yet be involved in eating behavior patterns in obesity in the early stages of development (i.e., childhood and adolescence). This fact promisingly implies that these hormonal disruptions could not have impaired the usual eating responses yet, even if they might be affecting current satiety perceptions, as pointed out by previous research [66]. Given the predictive role of eating styles on prospective weight in adults [15], it can be thought that the clinical condition of obese children and adolescents could still be reversed without consequences in their way of relating to food. In particular, this can be applied to eating styles that involve appropriate processing of interoceptive satiety signals, namely the absence of eating as a maladaptive emotion regulation strategy (emotional eating) and the adaptive levels of sensitivity towards food-related rewards (external eating). However, this is not the case with the cognitive suppression of these internal hunger signals, as evidenced by the negative association between restrained eating and ghrelin levels. In other words, it seems that cognitively ignoring the interoceptive signs of hunger is not a good eating strategy in children and adolescents with obesity, since it is related to a poorer ghrelin profile.
Although interoceptive cues might be ignored at a cognitive level in those with lower ghrelin concentrations, it might be plausible that interoceptive abilities are not yet disturbed in young obese individuals, unlike obese adults [27]. This could explain why emotional and external eating styles were not related to metabolic biomarkers in our sample. Taking all into account, and in light of preliminary research on the role of interoceptive processing as an underlying mechanism of eating styles in adults [67], future studies should examine the interaction between peripheral biomarkers involved in satiety, their perception and processing in the central nervous system (i.e., interoception), and eating styles, in childhood obesity. From a clinical point of view, our findings suggest that dietary treatments mainly focused on restricting the amount of food intake (which inevitably involves ignoring the sensations of appetite) rather than on eating healthy food are contraindicated. In this line, previous research has shown the negative effects of dieting for healthy management of obesity during childhood and adolescence in the long term [68].
The current study presents some limitations that should be noted. First, the sample size was not very large, so the statistical power might be compromised, leading to not finding a statistical effect in this sample that could exist in the population. Second, the age range of participants in this study covered two different developmental stages (childhood and adolescence). Although statistical control has been applied over the potential effect of age (and sex) on the relationships of interest, future studies should replicate this research throughout the different development stages. Third, our results refer only to children and adolescents with moderate and severe obesity (BMIz > 2), so they cannot be generalized to other populations such as children and adolescents with normal weight or overweight (BMIz < 2), nor to adults. Fourth, our findings may be biased by the recruitment period, which was characterized by the COVID-19 pandemic for a portion of our sample. In this regard, a recent longitudinal study showed that external, emotional, and restrained eating styles remained stable during the lockdown in college students [69], supporting the notion of eating styles as trait-like rather than state-like patterns. Although these findings might be different in children and adolescents with obesity, to our best knowledge, there is no evidence to think that the associations between eating styles and biomarkers may vary according to circumstantial factors. Finally, given the cross-sectional nature of the design of the current study, causal relationships cannot be established. Future studies should adopt longitudinal designs to determine the directionality between ghrelin and restrained eating in childhood obesity and the mediating mechanisms by which this relationship occurs.
Despite these limitations, this study contributes substantially to a better understanding of the interactions between two different types of crucial factors (biochemical and psychological) involved in childhood and adolescent obesity, namely circulating hormones and eating styles.

5. Conclusions

This is the first study to report the link between the low ghrelin concentrations (which is a well-known biomarker of risk in obesity) and high dispositional tendency to cognitively suppress internal signals of hunger, i.e., restraint eating, which is an eating style considered maladaptive and widely related to obesity across the lifespan. Expanding on previous research, our study indicates that dietary restraint, in addition to being an obesity-related eating style, is also a behavioral pattern of increased risk within the obesity spectrum.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, L.D., J.N., M.F.-A., J.N.-S., G.M., A.d.B., A.C.-R., E.C., Z.A., R.M.B. and J.Á.-P.; methodology, L.D., J.N., M.F.-A., J.N.-S., G.M., A.d.B., R.M.B., A.J.-M., A.I.R., G.B.-L. and J.Á.-P.; validation, L.D., J.N., M.F.-A., J.N.-S., G.M., A.d.B., R.M.B. and J.Á.-P.; formal analysis, L.D., J.N., M.F.-A. and J.N.-S.; investigation, L.D., J.N., M.F.-A., J.N.-S., A.d.B., F.P., A.J.-M., A.I.R., G.B.-L. and J.Á.-P.; resources, G.M., R.M.B. and J.Á.-P.; data curation, L.D., J.N., M.F.-A., J.N.-S., A.d.B., F.P., A.J.-M., A.I.R. and J.Á.-P.; writing—original draft preparation, L.D., J.N., M.F.-A., J.N.-S., A.d.B. and J.Á.-P.; writing—review and editing, L.D., J.N., M.F.-A., J.N.-S., G.M., A.d.B., R.M.B. and J.Á.-P.; supervision, J.Á.-P., G.M. and R.M.B.; project administration, J.Á.-P., G.M., Z.A. and R.M.B.; funding acquisition, J.Á.-P., Z.A. and R.M.B. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Fisiopatología de la Obesidad y Nutrición (CIBEROBN), Instituto de Salud Carlos III, Spain, through the EAT4HEALTHYLIFE project (Obesity and abnormal eating behavior across the lifespan. A cross sectional and longitudinal approach of environmental and neurobiological factors). In addition, L.D. and M.F.-A. are supported by their grants FPU18/01690 and FPU19/03720, respectively, funded by Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación/Agencia Estatal de Investigación/10.13039/501100011033 and by “European Social Fund – Investing in your future”.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of the General University Hospital Consortium of Valencia (CHGUV) (reference number: 4/2019) and the Research Ethics Committee of the Autonomous Community of Aragon (CEICA) (reference number: PI19/269).

Informed Consent Statement

Written informed consent was obtained from all participants involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

The dataset generated and analyzed is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

We thank the families who kindly participated in the Eat4HealthyLife research project.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. NCD Risk Factor Collaboration (NCD-RisC). Worldwide Trends in Body-Mass Index, Underweight, Overweight, and Obesity from 1975 to 2016: A Pooled Analysis of 2416 Population-Based Measurement Studies in 128.9 Million Children, Adolescents, and Adults. Lancet 2017, 390, 2627–2642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Buoncristiano, M.; Spinelli, A.; Williams, J.; Nardone, P.; Rito, A.I.; García-Solano, M.; Grøholt, E.K.; Gutiérrez-González, E.; Klepp, K.I.; Starc, G.; et al. Childhood Overweight and Obesity in Europe: Changes from 2007 to 2017. Obes. Rev. 2021, e13226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Al-Khudairy, L.; Loveman, E.; Colquitt, J.L.; Mead, E.; Johnson, R.E.; Fraser, H.; Olajide, J.; Murphy, M.; Velho, R.M.; O’Malley, C.; et al. Diet, Physical Activity and Behavioural Interventions for the Treatment of Overweight or Obese Adolescents Aged 12 to 17 Years. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2017, 2017, CD012691. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Mechanick, J.I.; Hurley, D.L.; Garvey, W.T. Adiposity-Based Chronic Disease as a New Diagnostic Term: The American Association of Clinical Endocrinologists and American College of Endocrinology Position Statement. Endocr. Pract. 2017, 23, 372–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Frühbeck, G.; Busetto, L.; Dicker, D.; Yumuk, V.; Goossens, G.H.; Hebebrand, J.; Halford, J.G.C.; Farpour-Lambert, N.J.; Blaak, E.E.; Woodward, E.; et al. The ABCD of Obesity: An EASO Position Statement on a Diagnostic Term with Clinical and Scientific Implications. Obes. Facts 2019, 12, 131–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Blundell, J.E. The Contribution of Behavioural Science to Nutrition: Appetite Control. Nutr. Bull. 2017, 42, 236–245. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Llewellyn, C.; Fildes, A. Behavioural Susceptibility Theory: Professor Jane Wardle and the Role of Appetite in Genetic Risk of Obesity. Curr. Obes. Rep. 2017, 6, 38–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Van Strien, T.; Herman, C.; Verheijden, M. Eating Style, Overeating, and Overweight in a Representative Dutch Sample. Does External Eating Play a Role? Appetite 2009, 52, 380–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Obregón, A.M.; Pettinelli, P.P.; Santos, J.L. Childhood Obesity and Eating Behaviour. J. Pediatr. Endocrinol. Metab. 2015, 28, 497–502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  10. Verbiest, I.; Michels, N.; Tanghe, A.; Braet, C. Inflammation in Obese Children and Adolescents: Association with Psychosocial Stress Variables and Effects of a Lifestyle Intervention. Brain Behav. Immun. 2021, 98, 40–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Van Strien, T.; Frijters, J.; Bergers, G.; Defares, P. The Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire (DEBQ) for Assessment of Restrained, Emotional, and External Eating Behavior. Int. J. Eat. Disord. 1986, 5, 295–315. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Schachter, S. The Interaction of Cognitive and Physiological Determinants of Emotional State. Adv. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1964, 1, 49–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Elfhag, K.; Morey, L. Personality Traits and Eating Behavior in the Obese: Poor Self-Control in Emotional and External Eating but Personality Assets in Restrained Eating. Eat. Behav. 2008, 9, 285–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Bruch, H. Eating Disorders. Obesity, Anorexia Nervosa, and the Person within; Routledge & Kegan Paul: Oxfordshire, UK, 1974. [Google Scholar]
  15. Koenders, P.G.; Van Strien, T. Emotional Eating, Rather than Lifestyle Behavior, Drives Weight Gain in a Prospective Study in 1562 Employees. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2011, 53, 1287–1293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Sung, J.; Lee, K.; Song, Y. Relationship of Eating Behavior to Long-Term Weight Change and Body Mass Index: The Healthy Twin Study. Eat. Weight Disord. 2009, 14, e98–e105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Sleddens, E.F.; Kremers, S.P.; Thijs, C. The Children’s Eating Behaviour Questionnaire: Factorial Validity and Association with Body Mass Index in Dutch Children Aged 6–7. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2008, 5, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  18. Van Strien, T.; Snoek, H.; van der Zwaluw, C.; Engels, R. Parental Control and the Dopamine D2 Receptor Gene (DRD2) Interaction on Emotional Eating in Adolescence. Appetite 2010, 54, 255–261. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Van Strien, T.; van der Zwaluw, C.; Engels, R. Emotional Eating in Adolescents: A Gene (SLC6A4/5-HTT)—Depressive Feelings Interaction Analysis. J. Psychiatr. Res. 2010, 44, 1035–1042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Van Strien, T.; Konttinen, H.; Homberg, J.R.; Engels, R.C.M.E.; Winkens, L.H.H. Emotional Eating as a Mediator between Depression and Weight Gain. Appetite 2016, 100, 216–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Varela, C.; Andrés, A.; Saldaña, C. The Behavioral Pathway Model to Overweight and Obesity: Coping Strategies, Eating Behaviors and Body Mass Index. Eat. Weight Disord. 2019, 25, 1277–1283. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Polivy, J.; Herman, C.; Mills, J. What Is Restrained Eating and How Do We Identify It? Appetite 2020, 155, 104820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Herman, C.P.; Polivy, J. A Boundary Model for the Regulation of Eating. Psychiatr. Ann. 1983, 13, 918–927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Baños, R.; Cebolla, A.; Moragrega, I.; Van Strien, T.; Fernández-Aranda, F.; Agüera, Z.; de la Torre, R.; Casanueva, F.; Fernández-Real, J.; Fernández-García, J.; et al. Relationship between Eating Styles and Temperament in an Anorexia Nervosa, Healthy Control, and Morbid Obesity Female Sample. Appetite 2014, 76, 76–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Wardle, J. Eating Style: A Validation Study of the Dutch Eating Behaviour Questionnaire in Normal Subjects and Women with Eating Disorders. J. Psychosom. Res. 1987, 31, 161–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Van Strien, T. Causes of Emotional Eating and Matched Treatment of Obesity. Obesity 2018, 18, 35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Herbert, B.M.; Pollatos, O. Attenuated Interoceptive Sensitivity in Overweight and Obese Individuals. Eat. Behav. 2014, 15, 445–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Herbert, B.M.; Blechert, J.; Hautzinger, M.; Matthias, E.; Herbert, C. Intuitive Eating Is Associated with Interoceptive Sensitivity. Effects on Body Mass Index. Appetite 2013, 70, 22–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Craig, A.D. (Bud) How Do You Feel—Now? The Anterior Insula and Human Awareness. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2009, 10, 59–70. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Mata, F.; Verdejo-Roman, J.; Soriano-Mas, C.; Verdejo-Garcia, A. Insula Tuning towards External Eating versus Interoceptive Input in Adolescents with Overweight and Obesity. Appetite 2015, 93, 24–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Rollins, B.Y.; Loken, E.; Savage, J.S.; Birch, L.L. Measurement of Food Reinforcement in Preschool Children. Associations with Food Intake, BMI, and Reward Sensitivity. Appetite 2014, 72, 21–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Vandeweghe, L.; Verbeken, S.; Vervoort, L.; Moens, E.; Braet, C. Reward Sensitivity and Body Weight: The Intervening Role of Food Responsive Behavior and External Eating. Appetite 2017, 112, 150–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hellström, P.M.; Geliebter, A.; Näslund, E.; Schmidt, P.T.; Yahav, E.K.; Hashim, S.A.; Yeomans, M.R. Peripheral and Central Signals in the Control of Eating in Normal, Obese and Binge-Eating Human Subjects. Br. J. Nutr. 2004, 92, S47–S57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Funcke, J.-B.; Scherer, P.E. Beyond Adiponectin and Leptin: Adipose Tissue-Derived Mediators of Inter-Organ Communication. J. Lipid Res. 2019, 60, 1648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Mechanick, J.I.; Zhao, S.; Garvey, W.T. The Adipokine-Cardiovascular-Lifestyle Network: Translation to Clinical Practice. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 2016, 68, 1785–1803. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Gadde, K.M.; Martin, C.K.; Berthoud, H.R.; Heymsfield, S.B. Obesity: Pathophysiology and Management. J. Am. Coll. Cardiol. 2018, 71, 69–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Wiedmer, P.; Nogueiras, R.; Broglio, F.; D’Alessio, D.; Tschöp, M.H. Ghrelin, Obesity and Diabetes. Nat. Clin. Pract. Endocrinol. Metab. 2007, 3, 705–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Austin, J.; Marks, D. Hormonal Regulators of Appetite. Int. J. Pediatr. Endocrinol. 2009, 2009, 41753. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Cummings, D.; Purnell, J.; Frayo, R.; Schmidova, K.; Wisse, B.; Weigle, D. A Preprandial Rise in Plasma Ghrelin Levels Suggests a Role in Meal Initiation in Humans. Diabetes 2001, 50, 1714–1719. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  40. Langlois, F.; Langlois, M.F.; Carpentier, A.C.; Brown, C.; Lemieux, S.; Hivert, M.F. Ghrelin Levels Are Associated with Hunger as Measured by the Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire in Healthy Young Adults. Physiol. Behav. 2011, 104, 373–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Schur, E.A.; Cummings, D.E.; Callahan, H.S.; Foster-Schubert, K.E. Association of Cognitive Restraint with Ghrelin, Leptin, and Insulin Levels in Subjects Who Are Not Weight-Reduced. Physiol. Behav. 2008, 93, 706–712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Buss, J.; Havel, P.J.; Epel, E.; Lin, J.; Blackburn, E.; Daubenmier, J. Associations of Ghrelin with Eating Behaviors, Stress, Metabolic Factors, and Telomere Length among Overweight and Obese Women: Preliminary Evidence of Attenuated Ghrelin Effects in Obesity? Appetite 2014, 76, 94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Ten, S.; Maclaren, N. Insulin Resistance Syndrome in Children. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2004, 89, 2526–2539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Flier, J.S. Starvation in the Midst of Plenty: Reflections on the History and Biology of Insulin and Leptin. Endocr. Rev. 2019, 40, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Obradovicm, M.; Sudar-Milovanovic, E.; Soskic, S.; Essack, M.; Arya, S.; Stewart, A.; Gojobori, T.; Isenovic, E. Leptin and Obesity: Role and Cinical Implication. Front. Endocrinol. 2021, 12, 585887. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Sinha, M.K.; Sturis, J.; Ohannesian, J.; Magosin, S.; Stephens, T.; Heiman, M.L.; Polonsky, K.S.; Caro, J.F. Ultradian Oscillations of Leptin Secretion in Humans. Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 1996, 228, 733–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Scherer, P.; Williams, S.; Fogliano, M.; Baldini, G.; Lodish, H. A Novel Serum Protein Similar to C1q, Produced Exclusively in Adipocytes. J. Biol. Chem. 1995, 270, 26746–26749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  48. Tang, N.; Zhang, X.; Chen, D.; Li, Z. The Controversial Role of Adiponectin in Appetite Regulation of Animals. Nutrients 2021, 13, 3387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Silha, J.; Krsek, M.; Skrha, J.; Sucharda, P.; Nyomba, B.; Murphy, L. Plasma Resistin, Adiponectin and Leptin Levels in Lean and Obese Subjects: Correlations with Insulin Resistance. Eur. J. Endocrinol. 2003, 149, 331–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Zhao, S.; Kusminski, C.M.; Scherer, P.E. Adiponectin, Leptin and Cardiovascular Disorders. Circ. Res. 2021, 136–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Martos-Moreno, G.Á.; Barrios, V.; Chowen, J.A.; Argente, J. Adipokines in Childhood Obesity; Elsevier: London, UK, 2013; Volume 91, ISBN 9780124077669. [Google Scholar]
  52. Frühbeck, G.; Catalán, V.; Rodríguez, A.; Ramírez, B.; Becerril, S.; Salvador, J.; Colina, I.; Gómez-Ambrosi, J. Adiponectin-Leptin Ratio Is a Functional Biomarker of Adipose Tissue Inflammation. Nutrients 2019, 11, 454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Freitas, A.; Albuquerque, G.; Silva, C.; Oliveira, A. Appetite-Related Eating Behaviours: An Overview of Assessment Methods, Determinants and Effects on Children’s Weight. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2018, 73, 19–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Vartanian, L.R.; Spanos, S.; Peter Herman, C.; Polivy, J. Conflicting Internal and External Eating Cues: Impact on Food Intake and Attributions. Health Psychol. 2017, 36, 365–369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. De Graaf, C.; Blom, W.; Smeets, P.; Stafleu, A.; Hendriks, H. Biomarkers of Satiation and Satiety. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2004, 79, 946–961. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. De Onis, M.; Onyango, A.; Borghi, E.; Siyam, A.; Nishida, C.; Siekmann, J. Development of a WHO Growth Reference for School-Aged Children and Adolescents. Bull. World Health Organ. 2007, 85, 660–667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Peplies, J.; Jiménez-Pavón, D.; Savva, S.; Buck, C.; Günther, K.; Fraterman, A.; Russo, P.; Iacoviello, L.; Veidebaum, T.; Tornaritis, M.; et al. Percentiles of Fasting Serum Insulin, Glucose, HbA1c and HOMA-IR in Pre-Pubertal Normal Weight European Children from the IDEFICS Cohort. Int. J. Obes. 2014, 38, 39–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Baños, R.M.; Cebolla, A.; Etchemendy, E.; Felipe, S.; Rasal, P.; Botella, C. Validation of the Dutch Eating Behavior Questionnaire for Children (DEBQ-C) for Use with Spanish Children. Nutr. Hosp. 2011, 26, 890–898. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  60. Kaufman, A.S.; Kaufman, N.L. K-BIT, Test Breve de Inteligencia de Kaufman; TEA Ediciones: Madrid, Spain, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  61. Wechsler, D. WISC-IV: Escala Wechsler de Inteligencia Para Niños-IV; Pearson: Madrid, Spain, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  62. DeVellis, R. Scale Development: Theory and Applications; SAGE Publications: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  63. Konttinen, H.; Haukkala, A.; Sarlio-Lähteenkorva, S.; Silventoinen, K.; Jousilahti, P. Eating Styles, Self-Control and Obesity Indicators. The Moderating Role of Obesity Status and Dieting History on Restrained Eating. Appetite 2009, 53, 131–134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Herman, C.; Polivy, J. Experimental and clinical aspects of restrained eating. In Obesity: Basic Mechanisms and Treatment; Stunkard, A., Ed.; W.B. Saunders: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 1980; pp. 208–225. [Google Scholar]
  65. Malik, S.; McGlone, F.; Bedrossian, D.; Dagher, A. Ghrelin Modulates Brain Activity in Areas That Control Appetitive Behavior. Cell Metab. 2008, 7, 400–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Kullmann, S.; Heni, M.; Veit, R.; Scheffler, K.; Machann, J.; Häring, H.-U.; Fritsche, A.; Preissl, H. Intranasal Insulin Enhances Brain Functional Connectivity Mediating the Relationship between Adiposity and Subjective Feeling of Hunger. Sci. Rep. 2017, 7, 1627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Young, H.A.; Williams, C.; Pink, A.E.; Freegard, G.; Owens, A.; Benton, D. Getting to the Heart of the Matter: Does Aberrant Interoceptive Processing Contribute towards Emotional Eating? PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0186312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  68. Dulloo, A.; Montani, J. Pathways from Dieting to Weight Regain, to Obesity and to the Metabolic Syndrome: An Overview. Obes. Rev. 2015, 16, 1–6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Escrivá-Martínez, T.; Miragall, M.; Herrero, R.; Rodríguez-Arias, M.; Baños, R.M. Eating Behaviors and Body Mass Index before and during COVID-19 Confinement: A Predictive Model; Polibienestar Research Institute, University of Valencia: Valencia, Spain, 2021; Unpublished. [Google Scholar]
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for study measures (n = 70).
Table 1. Descriptive statistics for study measures (n = 70).
MeasureMSDRangePercentiles
P25P50P75
Height (cm)158.2211.12133.90–181.20151.00158.40166.30
Weight (kg)77.8618.7139.80–115.4064.8076.7091.90
External eating1.990.551–31.502.002.50
Emotional eating1.530.621–31.001.211.86
Restrained eating1.910.451–31.571.862.18
Ghrelin (pg/mL)1497.841109.3690.40–4386.18690.651084.362076.84
HOMA index5.245.730.89–39.242.534.145.86
Leptin/adiponectin ratio1.642.030.04–9.680.480.941.76
Notes. n = number of participants; M = mean; SD = standard deviation.
Table 2. Regression coefficients of sex, age, ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio on external eating.
Table 2. Regression coefficients of sex, age, ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio on external eating.
Model
VariablesBSE BβpR2R2
Block 1 0.050.05
Constant1.380.47 0.005
Sex0.220.150.200.164
Age0.020.040.100.494
Block 2 0.120.07
Constant1.600.59 0.010
Sex0.260.160.240.099
Age−0.000.04−0.010.967
Ghrelin−0.000.00−0.110.508
IR0.020.010.190.213
Leptin/adiponectin ratio0.020.040.070.638
Notes. B = unstandardized beta values; SE B = standard error of B; β = standardized beta values; R2 = coefficient of determination; ΔR2 = coefficient of determination change.
Table 3. Regression coefficients of sex, age, ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio on emotional eating.
Table 3. Regression coefficients of sex, age, ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio on emotional eating.
Model
VariablesBSE BβpR2R2
Block 1 0.130.13 *
Constant0.500.51 0.311
Sex0.400.170.320.020
Age0.040.040.130.318
Block 2 0.200.07
Constant0.230.63 0.713
Sex0.420.170.340.015
Age0.030.040.120.448
Ghrelin0.000.000.120.454
IR0.020.020.190.191
Leptin/adiponectin ratio0.060.040.190.188
Notes. B = unstandardized beta values; SE B = standard error of B; β = standardized beta values; R2 = coefficient of determination; ΔR2 = coefficient of determination change. * p < 0.05.
Table 4. Regression coefficients of sex, age, ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio on restrained eating.
Table 4. Regression coefficients of sex, age, ghrelin, IR, and leptin/adiponectin ratio on restrained eating.
Model
VariablesBSE BβpR2R2
Block 1 0.010.01
Constant1.630.39 0.000
Sex0.080.130.080.558
Age0.010.030.070.622
Block 2 0.320.31 **
Constant2.640.42 0.000
Sex0.140.110.160.198
Age−0.050.03−0.230.102
Ghrelin0.000.00−0.61<0.001
IR0.010.010.090.506
Leptin/adiponectin ratio−0.010.03−0.040.758
Notes. B = unstandardized beta values; SE B = standard error of B; β = standardized beta values; R2 = coefficient of determination; ΔR2 = coefficient of determination change. ** p < 0.01.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Desdentado, L.; Navarrete, J.; Folgado-Alufre, M.; de Blas, A.; Navarro-Siurana, J.; Ponce, F.; Molinari, G.; Jimeno-Martínez, A.; Rupérez, A.I.; Bueno-Lozano, G.; et al. Are Peripheral Biomarkers Determinants of Eating Styles in Childhood and Adolescence Obesity? A Cross-Sectional Study. Nutrients 2022, 14, 305. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14020305

AMA Style

Desdentado L, Navarrete J, Folgado-Alufre M, de Blas A, Navarro-Siurana J, Ponce F, Molinari G, Jimeno-Martínez A, Rupérez AI, Bueno-Lozano G, et al. Are Peripheral Biomarkers Determinants of Eating Styles in Childhood and Adolescence Obesity? A Cross-Sectional Study. Nutrients. 2022; 14(2):305. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14020305

Chicago/Turabian Style

Desdentado, Lorena, Jaime Navarrete, María Folgado-Alufre, Ana de Blas, Jéssica Navarro-Siurana, Francisco Ponce, Guadalupe Molinari, Andrea Jimeno-Martínez, Azahara I. Rupérez, Gloria Bueno-Lozano, and et al. 2022. "Are Peripheral Biomarkers Determinants of Eating Styles in Childhood and Adolescence Obesity? A Cross-Sectional Study" Nutrients 14, no. 2: 305. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14020305

APA Style

Desdentado, L., Navarrete, J., Folgado-Alufre, M., de Blas, A., Navarro-Siurana, J., Ponce, F., Molinari, G., Jimeno-Martínez, A., Rupérez, A. I., Bueno-Lozano, G., Cuenca-Royo, A., Corbella, E., Agüera, Z., Baños, R. M., & Álvarez-Pitti, J. (2022). Are Peripheral Biomarkers Determinants of Eating Styles in Childhood and Adolescence Obesity? A Cross-Sectional Study. Nutrients, 14(2), 305. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14020305

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop