Next Article in Journal
Positive Association between High Protein Food Intake Frequency and Physical Performance and Higher-Level Functional Capacity in Daily Life
Next Article in Special Issue
Nutritional Status Predicts Fatty Acid Uptake from Fish and Soybean Oil Supplements for Treatment of Cancer-Related Fatigue: Results from a Phase II Nationwide Study
Previous Article in Journal
Regulatory Effects of Quercetin on M1/M2 Macrophage Polarization and Oxidative/Antioxidative Balance
Previous Article in Special Issue
Independent and Joint Impacts of Acid-Producing Diets and Depression on Physical Health among Breast Cancer Survivors
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

‘Food for Thought’—The Relationship between Diet and Cognition in Breast and Colorectal Cancer Survivors: A Feasibility Study

1
Behaviour-Brain-Body (BBB) Research Centre, UniSA Justice & Society, University of South Australia, Adelaide, SA 5000, Australia
2
UniSA Allied Health & Human Performance, Alliance for Research in Exercise, Nutrition and Activity (ARENA), University of South Australia, Adelaide, SA 5000, Australia
3
Flinders Medical Centre, Department of Medical Oncology, Adelaide, SA 5000, Australia
4
College of Medicine & Public Health, Flinders University, Adelaide, SA 5000, Australia
5
Rosemary Bryant AO Research Centre, UniSA Clinical & Health Sciences, University of South Australia, Adelaide, SA 5000, Australia
6
UniSA Clinical & Health Sciences, University of South Australia, Adelaide, SA 5000, Australia
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2022, 14(1), 71; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14010071
Submission received: 10 November 2021 / Revised: 14 December 2021 / Accepted: 20 December 2021 / Published: 24 December 2021
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Nutrition for Cancer Survivors)

Abstract

:
Survivors of cancer frequently experience persistent and troublesome cognitive changes. Little is known about the role diet and nutrition plays in survivors’ cognition. We explored the feasibility of collecting cross-sectional online data from Australian survivors of breast and colorectal cancer to enable preliminary investigations of the relationships between cognition with fruit and vegetable intake, and the Omega-3 Index (a biomarker of long chain omega 3 fatty acid intake). A total of 76 participants completed online (and postal Omega-3 Index biomarker) data collection (62 breast and 14 colorectal cancer survivors): mean age 57.5 (±10.2) years, mean time since diagnosis 32.6 (±15.6) months. Almost all of the feasibility outcomes were met; however, technical difficulties were reported for online cognitive testing. In hierarchical linear regression models, none of the dietary variables of interest were significant predictors of self-reported or objective cognition. Age, BMI, and length of treatment predicted some of the cognitive outcomes. We demonstrated a viable online/postal data collection method, with participants reporting positive levels of engagement and satisfaction. Fruit, vegetable, and omega-3 intake were not significant predictors of cognition in this sample, however the role of BMI in survivors′ cognitive functioning should be further investigated. Future research could adapt this protocol to longitudinally monitor diet and cognition to assess the impact of diet on subsequent cognitive function, and whether cognitive changes impact dietary habits in survivors of cancer.

1. Introduction

Cancer-related cognitive impairment (CRCI) describes changes to cognition associated with cancer or its treatment [1]. These changes can be self-reported or identified through objective neurocognitive assessment. CRCI prevalence varies widely according to demographics, cancer-related variables, and assessment measures [2,3]. However, it is commonly cited that up to 35% of survivors of cancer experience CRCI long-term [4]. Cognitive changes can profoundly impact survivors’ sense of self and functioning [3]. Understanding factors associated with CRCI is important, to identify ways of ameliorating negative cognitive changes.
The cause of CRCI is unclear, but likely multifactorial. Common predictors include age, education, cancer type, treatment, fatigue, depression, and sleep disruption [2,5]. The specific biological mechanisms involved are complex and equivocal, but may involve neurotoxicity, inflammation, and increased oxidation [1].
Multiple dietary elements play a role in cognitive function in non-cancer populations [6]. Compared with other modifiable factors such as physical activity, less research has explored the role of diet in CRCI. Survivors of cancer often look to dietary advice to improve health and manage long-term cancer and treatment effects [7]. Previous research indicates that survivors believe diet can impact their thinking ability, with some making dietary changes to improve cognition [8]. In this way, it is important to identify how dietary aspects are related to survivors’ cognitive functioning to inform evidence-based recommendations. Further, in the same study, survivors also noted that changes in their cognitive functioning following cancer diagnosis had impacted their dietary behaviours, often perceived to be in unhealthy ways. Cognitive function is known to influence dietary habits in non-cancer populations [9] but this association has not yet been explored in cancer populations. Understanding how diet and cognition are related in individuals who commonly experience cognitive difficulties is therefore worth exploring, especially considering the lack of evidence-based dietary guidance for CRCI.
Preliminary evidence suggests two aspects of diet may play a role in CRCI. Fruit and vegetable intake are positively associated with self-reported and objective cognition in cancer survivors in several correlational studies [10]. Intake of long chain (LC) omega-3 (n-3) polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFA; eicosapentanoic acid [EPA] and docosahexanoic acid [DHA]) may also play a role as fish oil supplementation has been found to be related to cognitive improvements post-diagnosis [11]. Since the intake of these dietary components can improve cognition in non-cancer populations, they may also improve cognition in cancer survivors [12]. Possible mechanisms of action for LC n-3 PUFAs include anti-inflammatory and antioxidant effects [13,14]. These dietary mechanisms are implicated in CRCI, thus additional research is warranted to identify how fruit, vegetable, and LC n-3 PUFA intake is associated with cognition in survivors of cancer.
We sought to identify the feasibility of an online data collection research protocol in Australian survivors of breast and colorectal cancer, and identify the preliminary relationships between fruit, vegetable, and LC n-3 PUFA intake and cognition. We hypothesised better self-reported and objectively assessed cognition would both be predicted by greater intake of: (1) fruits, (2) vegetables, and (3) LC n-3 PUFAs and the Omega-3 Index (a biomarker that measures blood levels of LC n-3 PUFA).

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Design

This was a feasibility study using a cross-sectional research design with online data collection in Australian post-treatment survivors of breast and colorectal cancer. Ethics approval was granted by the University of South Australia (UniSA) Human Resources Ethics Committee (approval: 202999), and the procedures adhered to the tenets of the Declaration of Helsinki. This work was guided by the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology (STROBE) guidelines.

2.2. Eligibility

Inclusion criteria: diagnosed with primary adult-onset breast or colorectal cancer between 6-months and 5 years ago; able to complete study requirements including have computer/internet access; fluent in English, with normal or corrected vision/hearing; residing in Australia.
Exclusion criteria: Received primary treatment in last 3 months (chemotherapy, radiotherapy, immunotherapy, cancer surgery); child-onset cancer (due to potential developmental impact); diagnosed with dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, multiple sclerosis, Parkinson’s disease; experienced unconsciousness for five minutes as result of head/brain injury in the last ten years, or stroke or transient ischemic attack (due to potential impact on cognition); currently pregnant/breastfeeding (due to impact on diet).
Breast and colorectal cancers were chosen due to their prevalence in Australia [15] and previous research highlighting potential impact of diet on CRCI in these populations [10]. To reduce the acute effects of diagnosis upon measures of interest, participants were required to be diagnosed at least six months prior. For participant safety, the fingerstick was excluded where participants reported bleeding disorders or anticoagulant use.

2.3. Procedure

2.3.1. Recruitment

Participants were recruited via noticeboards, websites, social media, survivorship groups, and radio interview. Recruitment material contained a weblink to online screening, hosted on ‘Research Electronic Data Capture’ (REDCap) data collection platform [16]. Recruitment occurred between September 2020 and February 2021.

2.3.2. Screening

Interested participants accessed REDCap and completed a brief screening questionnaire assessing primary inclusion/exclusion criteria. Eligible participants completed a longer screening questionnaire requesting contact details and demographics, cancer-related medical history, and diet/lifestyle information. The Participant Information Sheet and consent forms were provided at both screening stages.

2.3.3. Enrolment

Enrolled participants were mailed a dried blood spot omega-3 fingerstick test (OmegaQuant Laboratories), a snack consisting of a muesli bar and dried sultanas, and login instructions to access their online test session. Due to dietary requirements, alternatives were provided for some participants with every effort made to provide a snack with similar energy and carbohydrate content.

2.3.4. Data Collection

Participants were asked to fast from midnight (water and medication allowed) on a morning of their choosing. They were instructed to login between 8 a.m. and 10 a.m., using a unique password, to complete their test session. The session commenced with the bloodspot test, followed by self-report measures. Participants were instructed to take a ten-minute break to consume the provided snacks before completing the final cognitive assessment. They completed a short exit survey and were asked to mail their blood sample to the testing laboratory using a reply-paid envelope. Participants were offered a $50 honorarium upon study completion.

2.4. Instruments

Demographic details included age, sex, ethnicity, education, and cancer-related information (e.g., type, stage, treatment history).

2.4.1. Feasibility Measures

Participation satisfaction: participants completed an exit survey to assess overall study experience. The primary a prioi criterion used to assess participant satisfaction was a 5-point Likert scale question (“Was your overall experience in this study: very bad, bad, acceptable, good, very good). Two additional quantitative questions (“Would you participate in this study again? Yes/No” and “Would you recommend participating in this study to someone you know? Yes/No”) were also asked and are reported in Section 3.2 as supportive data to the primary outcome. Brief open-ended qualitative questions, allowing participants to share additional experiences surrounding study difficulties and study content, were used to provide additional context to study design but not used as feasibility criteria.
Process-related outcomes: additional a prioi criteria relating to study protocol were used to assess rates of eligibility, participation refusal, recruitment, retention, and blood sample return.

2.4.2. Clinical Measures

Self-reported cognition (FACT-Cog): The Functional Assessment of Cancer Treatment–Cognitive Function v3 (FACT-Cog) [17] is a self-report measure of cognition in cancer survivors. It contains 37 items querying cognitive function over the previous seven days, utilising a 5-point Likert-type scale with responses ranging from 0 (‘Never’) to 4 (‘Several times a day’). It contains four subscales: Perceived Cognitive Impairment (CogPCI) is the most used subscale, focusing on poor cognitive performance in the previous week (e.g., “My thinking has been slower than usual”). It has excellent internal consistency (Cronbach α = 0.96) and test-retest reliability (6-week correlation, r = 0.92, p < 0.001) [18]. Higher scores indicate better cognition. In this sample, CogPCI demonstrated high internal consistency (α: 0.94).
Objective cognition (CANTAB test battery): The Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB) is software developed for cognitive testing [19]. Five tasks comprised the cognitive testing: Paired Associates Learning (PAL; visual memory, learning), Spatial Working Memory (SWM; visuospatial memory, executive function), Delayed Matching to Samples (DMS; attention, short-term visual memory), Rapid Visual Processing (RVP; sustained attention), and One Touch Stockings of Cambridge (OTS; executive function, working memory). Further task details are in Appendix A. CANTAB tasks were administered via web-based testing, allowing remote completion. Performance in unsupervised web-based testing has been compared with in-person lab-based assessment, with good overall agreement [20]. Automated task directions were provided to ensure all participants receive standardised instructions. The mean CANTAB duration was 45 min (SD 6.5).
Omega-3 Index: The omega-3 index is the sum of the red blood cell (RBC) levels of the two LC n-3 PUFAs: EPA and DHA [21]. To include a biomarker assessment of LC n-3 PUFA, an Omega-3 Index Blood Test kit (OmegaQuant Analytics) was sent to participants without bleeding disorders (n = 74). The kit allows dried bloodspot self-collection, which was returnable by post to OmegaQuant Asia Pacific (Adelaide, South Australia). Specific collection, processing and gas chromatography analysis details have been previously described [22]. This index provides an objective measurement of total whole blood fatty acid percentage; EPA and DHA are converted to a percent total RBC membrane fatty acid equivalent, reported as ‘RBC equivalent’. This method of measuring whole blood fatty acids has been validated, with equivalence highly correlated with the Omega-3 Index both within laboratory settings, and most importantly using samples sent through the mail (r = 0.98, p < 0.0001) [22], the method of collection used in the current study. The blood sample collection method has been validated to be stable at room temperature for up to 44 days [22]; the average time between sample collection in this study (i.e., participants’ test session date) and laboratory sample analysis was 12.6 days (SD 5.1), with the longest time being 30 days, well within the sample integrity window.
Dietary intake, self-reported (AES): The Australian Eating Survey® (AES) food frequency questionnaire was used to determine participants’ dietary intake [23]. The adult questionnaire samples typical consumption frequency of standard servings of 120 food and drink items over the previous 3–6 months, completed in approximately 20 min. Estimations of daily intake of fruit and vegetable serves were calculated by the AES, which has been validated in Australian adults against dietary-related biomarkers [24].
Psycho-behavioural measures: two additional instruments were used to measure fatigue and mood. These factors may impact cognition and are briefly discussed here as they were included in regression models. Fatigue was measured with the FACIT-Fatigue [25], a 13-item (5-point Likert scale) self-report questionnaire (score range: 0–52). Internal consistency was high in this sample (α = 0.86). The Depression, Anxiety and Stress short form (DASS-21) is a validated 21-item scale measuring depression, anxiety, and stress using a four-point Likert scale [26]. In addition to common mood questionnaires that measure depression and anxiety, the tripartite aspect of the DASS-21 includes assessment of general stress which could impact variables of interest. It has demonstrated sufficient reliability and validity [27] and has been used in previous Australian CRCI research [28]. Internal consistency was high for all subscales (α = 0.83, 0.73, 0.81, respectively) in this sample.
Additional measures of quality of life, fear of cancer recurrence, sleep, and diet were included, but have not been discussed here as they were beyond the scope of the research goals and questions, and not included in analyses.

2.5. Data Analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted in SPSS (v25). Demographic details were summarised with frequencies, means, and standard deviations (SD) as appropriate. Outliers were defined as values ±3 SD from the mean; winsorising was used for values which appeared to be legitimate outliers (e.g., measure misfunction) replacing them with the next highest non-outlier value [29]. One dietary and ten CANTAB data points were replaced using this approach.
As the population of interest are known to have greater prevalence of cognitive difficulties, to preserve unique within-group variance in objective cognitive measures, raw scores of CANTAB outcome measures were used in lieu of normative matched scores. This aligns with the study’s aim of identifying dietary-cognitive relationships in survivors of cancer, rather than identify differences between survivors and other populations which would necessitate alternate research methods. To statistically confirm this decision, raw scores of the 9 CANTAB variables used were compared against their own population normed z-scores (age, gender, educated matched) with bivariate Pearson correlations (details provided in Appendix B, Table A2), which revealed strong significant correlations (Pearson r = 0.77–0.99; all p-values < 0.001).
A Principal Components Analysis was completed to determine how to best factorise these scores: a four-factor model was chosen based on visual inspection of Scree plot (see Appendix C). These four factors were used to inform creation of four objective cognitive function composite scores (SWM, PAL, DMS/OTS, RVP). These were calculated by standardizing raw scores of the nine CANTAB measures, reversing them as needed with higher scores indicating better performance, and using natural weights of the nine outcomes variables identified in Appendix C. Standardized scores (z-scores) were used to create the four component scores in order to meaningfully combine different outcome variables of a test and in the case of the DMS/OTS, allow combination of variables across two tests.
Hierarchical linear regressions (HLR) were used to identify whether fruit intake, vegetable intake, and the omega-3 index explained variance in cognition. To inform the selection of covariates in the HLRs (from amongst those with scientific basis), a correlation matrix was used to determine which predictors most strongly related to the cognitive outcomes and dietary predictors of interest (alpha = 0.05, two-tailed; see Appendix D, Table A4). Appendix E provides information regarding HLR statistical decisions. Family-wise alpha corrections were not applied to HLR models as this study was primarily exploratory in providing direction for future research.
Individual HLR models were constructed with outcomes for: perceived cognitive impairment, and four objective cognition outcomes (SWM, PAL, DMS/OTS, RVP). GPower software (v3.1) was used to conduct an a priori power analysis: 77 participants were necessary to detect a medium effect size (f2 = 0.15) [30] with 0.05 alpha, and 0.80 power.

3. Results

3.1. Participants

A total of 90 participants were enrolled; 76 completed their test session. Figure 1 displays the participant flow from recruitment to completion. Final sample demographics (n = 76) are listed in Table 1.
Participants were predominantly female, Caucasian, highly educated, post-menopausal, breast cancer survivors, approximately three years post-diagnosis. Most had undergone surgery, radiotherapy, and chemotherapy.
Twenty-three participants reported difficulty with the CANTAB test. Out of this, 5 of these could not access CANTAB during their test session due to technological difficulties, 5 stated they did not fully understand test instructions and 14 identified environmental interruptions during their test session using qualitative feedback (independent samples t-tests confirmed no statistically significant differences between scores of the four CANTAB component measures between those reporting interruptions and who did not; all p-values > 0.05; data not reported here). A total of 9 participants (12%) identified completing the CANTAB session on an iPad.

3.2. Feasibility Outcomes

A priori measures were chosen to assess study feasibility (except post hoc ‘screening refusal rate’); see Table 2. Previous feasibility studies and research with cancer populations were used to inform these criteria [32,33,34].
All a priori targets were met, except the 26-week recruitment rate (90% of target). Participant satisfaction was high: only one participant did not report acceptable or better experience. Additionally, 95% of participants identified they would participate in the study again, and 92% of participants stated they would recommend participating to someone they know. We observed a large proportion (30%) of potential participants exit the screening process before completion. Overall, 190 survivors completed the initial screening and 76 completed their test session, translating to a 40% querent-to-completed-participant ‘conversion’ rate.

3.3. Clinical Outcomes

Descriptive data relating to clinical outcomes are displayed in Table 3. The bivariate correlation matrix for predictors and outcome variables are presented in Appendix D, Table A4. HLRs were run to identify preliminary relationships and effect sizes between dietary and cognitive variables of interest. These are reported in Table 4.
In these HLRs, neither fruit, vegetable, or LC n-3 PUFA intake significantly predicted either self-reported or objectively assessed cognition function. Older age significantly predicted worse performance on two of four objective cognitive measures (SWM and DMS/OTS). Longer duration of radiotherapy and chemotherapy were each significantly predictive of better performance on one objective cognitive function measure (PAL and RVP, respectively). Greater BMI was a significant predictor of worse self-reported cognitive function.

4. Discussion

This study explored the feasibility of an online (and postal biomarker) data collection research protocol and sought to preliminarily identify how self-reported and objectively assessed cognition are related to fruit, vegetable, and LC n-3 PUFA intake in survivors of breast and colorectal cancer.
The feasibility targets were successfully achieved in most cases. All except one a priori feasibility target was met. Overall, the participant burden was acceptable, and remote data collection was successful in sampling survivors of breast and colorectal cancer. Specific protocol aspects could be improved relating to screening, dietary requirements, and objective cognitive assessment. One important post hoc feasibility outcome was explored: Almost a third of individuals did not complete the full screening they commenced. Due to the online and unidentifiable nature of this screening process, we were unable to determine individuals’ reasons not to proceed and whether all of these exit cases were unique individuals or if some completed the screening process again at a later and more convenient time. The screening process may have appeared too burdensome or providing personal information online may have evoked privacy concerns. Trust and the context in which an individual provides information can impact willingness to share information [35]. Having an option to provide personal data by phone could be explored to see if this improves screening completion. This ‘screening dropout’ rate may have affected the final sample representation, such as biasing it towards individuals with more cognitive capacity to complete longer questionnaires; though, ability to engage in potentially cognitively demanding tasks was an essential aspect of successful study participation. Incorporating exit prompts investigating why individuals did not wish to continue may be worthwhile in the future; however, this may not be possible given the nature of online screening where individuals can simply close web-browsers.
More than a quarter of participants identified the intended snack would not be suitable due to dietary requirements. We had more gluten-free participants than expected based on previous studies with cancer survivors [36]. Providing gluten-free food as standard could relatively easily and inexpensively prevent unnecessary exclusion when strict nutritional control is required. Survivors of other cancer types may require additional consideration as to the suitability of any provided snacks, due to possible gastrointestinal responses or distress. Participant feedback identified from open-ended qualitative questions on study participation revealed a small number of participants identified challenges with hunger/fasting (5%), length of time of test session (5%), and blood sample/fingerstick difficulties (4%). Despite this, the overall satisfaction rate was high indicating that these difficulties did not outweigh overall positive experience in participation.
Regarding data completeness, participants successfully completed all self-report measures. There was also a 100% postal blood sample return, indicating the mixed form of data collection to be viable. Some participants had difficulties completing CANTAB testing due to technical difficulties; this could be reduced through a brief rehearsal trial prior to the test session. However, most reported CANTAB issues pertained to situational interruptions, despite being instructed to arrange a quiet period for testing. At the end of each individual CANTAB task, participants were prompted to initiate the next one. This may have assisted containing the effects of external interruptions to within specific task (e.g., a participant being interrupted during one task would proceed to the next one only when they were ready). While participants provided qualitative feedback about their testing sessions (e.g., interruptions, distractions, technical problems), these data were not specific enough to meaningfully and consistently identify the magnitude, length, and frequency of distractions, nor, most importantly, which specific test(s) may have been affected. Therefore, findings related to objective cognition must be considered in light of the online data collection methods and the uncontrolled/unmeasured elements potentially affecting performance. While general instructions were provided to participants to improve the similarity of test conditions, the unsupervised nature of the design relies completely on participants understanding and following instructions, as well as accurately reporting difficulties or impactful events. Future studies using remote/online cognitive assessment may consider including quantitative questions to identify specific distractions/events during test sessions, or where possible, consider supervised or partially supervised remote guided testing to improve data quality [37]. Despite potential errors arising from the uncontrolled environment, inclusion of objective neurocognitive testing was important to identify feasibility and preliminary associations.
In exploring clinical outcome aims, neither fruit, vegetable, nor LC n-3 PUFA intake were significantly associated with self-reported cognition. The lack of positive significant relationships between cognition and fruit and vegetable intake was surprising, given a previous systematic review identifying positive associations [10]; however, the three studies in this review reporting significant associations utilised one- or two-item estimates of fruit and vegetable intake (in contrast with full food frequency questionnaire used in the present study), which may have varying reliability and thus account for these differences. Participants in the present study also reported high levels of education: Two-thirds of participants in our study had a university-level degree or higher qualification, compared with a quarter of Australians [38]. Higher education in survivors of cancer is associated with better dietary habits and greater physical activity [39]. Despite efforts to recruit broadly, a selection bias may have influenced participation and may have played a role in these findings.
Though research in the area of diet and CRCI is limited, the Mediterranean diet (characterised by intake of fruits, vegetables, cereals, legumes, olive oil, and fish) is one of the most frequently researched dietary patterns in relation to cognitive function [40]. While the three dietary variables of interest in the current study form significant parts of this eating pattern, other important dietary and biological factors may need to be considered alongside them; the Mediterranean diet also provides rich sources of polyphenols, antioxidants, fibre, mono- and poly-unsaturated fatty acids [41]. These components can beneficially affect cognitive function and mood, and interestingly may do so via the gut microbiota [42]. For example, the beneficial effect of dietary fibre intake upon cognitive function is linked to the fermentation of fibre by the gut microbiota and subsequent production of short-chain fatty acids in the colon; these in turn influence the gut-brain axis likely through mechanisms including improved intestinal barrier integrity, modulated immune and inflammatory responses, and increased brain-derived neurotrophic factor [42,43]. This is of particular relevance to survivors of cancer, as cancer treatment such as chemotherapy and radiotherapy elicit changes in the gut microbiome which can affect cognition and related factors such as mood and fatigue [44,45]. Therefore, it may be useful for future CRCI research to investigate dietary patterns broadly, other dietary components such as fiber, antioxidants, and polyphenols, or biomarker measures of other important physiological systems such as the gut microbiota, which may play a mediating role between diet and cognition.
Consistent with previous research, self-reported cognition was related with fatigue and stress in bivariate associations [46]. However, in regression models, BMI and cancer type were the only significant predictors of self-reported cognition, with survivors of breast cancer and all survivors with higher BMI reporting worse cognition. Previous research inconsistently reports on how BMI and cognitive function are related in cancer survivors. In breast cancer survivors, higher BMI has been associated with better executive functioning (but not working memory) [47], although a second study found higher BMI predicted poorer delayed memory over time, though not immediate memory or verbal fluency [48]. Interestingly, a third study noted a moderating effect of exercise such that BMI was not related to self-reported cognition among survivors who were regularly physically active, whereas sedentary people with higher BMI had poorer cognitive function than sedentary survivors with lower BMI. This effect was more pronounced in survivors who had received chemotherapy [49]. In colorectal cancer survivors, BMI has not been found to significantly predict cognitive dysfunction [50]. While descriptive data for BMI are often reported as an important clinical detail, it is less commonly explored as a predictor of CRCI, highlighting that further exploration of this relationships is needed to clarify the role of body composition on survivorship cognitive outcomes.
In contrast with previous literature, negative bivariate correlations were observed between fruit and vegetable intake with objective cognition. However, regression models in this study revealed fruit and vegetable intake were not significant predictors of objective cognition when accounting for age and treatment duration. Both age and treatment duration were significant in objective cognitive function regressions, which aligns with prior research indicating age and treatment-factors predict CRCI [2]. While fruit and vegetable intake is often positively associated with cognition [51], the inverse has occasionally been reported. Nooyens and colleagues [52] found total vegetable intake was associated with worse baseline cognition but was predictive of smaller cognitive decline after five years. Thus, it is possible that in this sample individuals noticing cognitive decline were intentionally consuming more fruit and vegetables to improve cognition. Most research examines diet as a predictor of cognition. However, cognitive function can also influence and predict health behaviours [9]. Individuals with poorer cognition could therefore make different dietary choices compared to individuals with better cognition. While the association between diet and cognition was not significant in the regression models, the bivariate trend, and the general lack of research of cognition as a predictor of dietary changes, warrants longitudinal research to explore whether CRCI has dietary and lifestyle consequences. Further, research (not in cancer populations) identifies the relationship between LC n-3 PUFA biomarkers and cognitive function can vary in linearity or nonlinearity [53,54]. While we explored the dietary-cognitive relationship from a linear perspective, considering that other biological factors could play a role in dietary profiles or cognitive performance, future research may need to consider the complexity of interplay between these factors and their potential for obscuration of these relationship.
Building on this and moving forward, due to the potentially complex interactions between various biological, psychological, and social/behavioral elements potentially impacting survivors, intervention studies may be useful to explore whether changes in dietary components can result in cognitive improvements. There is a paucity of such interventions, except one weight loss program combining a dietary and exercise regime and reported cognitive measures as a secondary outcome [55]. Diet can positively impact cognition in non-cancer survivors, and it should be assessed whether this is the case for cancer survivors. Notwithstanding the limitations, this study is one of the first to examine objectively measured cognition and objective biomarkers of diet in survivors of cancer, and the first to establish the feasibility of online and postal data collection.
Study limitations: the study was cross-sectional, limiting conclusions to associations and not causation, particularly in the context that this was foremost a feasibility study. The final sample was predominantly Caucasian and highly educated, and not representative of broader and diverse cancer survivor populations; the lack of healthy comparison groups and sample size precluded better controlling of several potentially confounding variables such as education. Self-reported data (particularly related to retrospective dietary recall) may be subject to recall biases, impacting their accuracy. Objective cognition was assessed in an uncontrolled environment, where various factors could impact performance, such as equipment and test setting differences, external interruptions, distractibility, and not reporting behaviors and events inconsistent with the recommended protocol.
Clinical implications: the feasible method of remote data collection of objective measures that were demonstrated in this study will assist future survivorship research, particularly in sampling populations who cannot easily access researchers in person. It also provides preliminary findings to inform future CRCI dietary research.

5. Conclusions

Cognitive changes are common in survivors, and cognition is vital in health behaviours. While this study did not identify significant associations between cognition and fruit, vegetable, or LC n-3 PUFA intake, it demonstrated a practical and effective data collection method, and highlighted the need for future research to understand cognition and dietary habits of survivors of cancer.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.G.C., A.D.H., S.B., B.K., N.C., A.V. and A.M.C.; methodology, D.G.C., A.D.H., S.B., B.K., N.C., A.V. and A.M.C.; formal analysis, D.G.C.; investigation, D.G.C. and K.A.D.; resources, D.G.C., A.D.H., K.A.D., S.B., B.K., N.C., A.V. and A.M.C.; data curation, D.G.C. and K.A.D.; writing—original draft preparation, D.G.C.; writing—review and editing, D.G.C., A.D.H., K.A.D., S.B., B.K., N.C., A.V. and A.M.C.; visualization, D.G.C.; supervision, A.D.H., S.B. and A.M.C.; project administration, A.D.H., S.B. and A.M.C.; funding acquisition, D.G.C., A.D.H., S.B., B.K., N.C., A.V. and A.M.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by the University of South Australia RTIS grant, and supported by the Australian Government Research Training Program fee offset scholarship (Ref #:110127613(236862); funding DGC).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of South Australia (protocol code 202999, approved 13/10/20).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data supporting the findings of this study are available on reasonable request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank Melissa Sherman for her contribution as a research assistant on this project. Participants in this research were recruited from Breast Cancer Network Australia’s (BCNA) Review and Survey Group, a national, online group of Australian women living with breast cancer who are interested in receiving invitations to participate in research. We acknowledge the contribution of the women involved in the Review and Survey Group who participated in this project.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Appendix A

The five tasks selected for the neurocognitive test battery in this study are identified in Table A1, in order of their administrations, with brief explanations of the task, their measures, and the cognitive domains with which they are associated.
Table A1. Summary of CANTAB tasks and outcome measures used in the cognitive test battery.
Table A1. Summary of CANTAB tasks and outcome measures used in the cognitive test battery.
Test NameVersionRelated DomainsTask DescriptionOutcome TypeOutcome Name
Paired Associates Learning
(PAL)
Recommended standardVisual memory & learning
Episodic memory
Six coloured shapes are randomly and temporarily revealed under ‘boxes’ and re-covered. One pattern is presented and the participant must choose the location where it was originally located. Accuracy
Errors
PALFAMS
PALTEA
Spatial Working Memory
(SWM)
Recommended standard 2.0 extendedVisuospatial working memory
Executive function
Hidden tokens must be collected from under various ‘boxes’, requiring the participant to remember which boxes have yielded tokens.Use of strategy
Errors
SWMS
SWMBE468
Delayed Matching to Sample
(DMS)
Recommended standardAttention
Short term visual memory
A complex visual pattern is shown to the participant. Following a delay, they must identify the target pattern from three distractor patterns.Accuracy
Error tendency
DMSPCAD
DMSPEGE
Rapid Visual Processing
(RVP)
3 targetsSustained attentionSingle digits appear rapidly on-screen. Participants are asked to click a button when a specific sequence of numbers is presented.
Accuracy (detection sensitivity)
Error tendency
RVPA
RVPPFA
One Touch Stockings of Cambridge
(OTS)
StandardExecutive function & planning
Working memory
Based on the ‘Tower of Hanoi’ problem. Participant is shown two different configurations of coloured balls and must calculate in their head how many moves are needed to match configurations. AccuracyOTSPSFC

Appendix B

Table A2. Pearson correlations between CANTAB outcome raw scores and their own corresponding normative z-scores.
Table A2. Pearson correlations between CANTAB outcome raw scores and their own corresponding normative z-scores.
Outcome Measurern
SWMS0.99 *72
SWMBE4680.87 *72
PALFAMS0.98 *72
PALTEA0.95 *72
DMSPEGE0.97 *72
DMSPCAD0.95 *72
OTSPSFC0.98 *72
RVPPFA0.77 *71
RVPA0.97 *71
Normative z-scores used for comparisons were age-, gender-, and education-matched. DMSPCAD = Delayed Matching to Samples, percent of trials correct first time (across all delayed trials); DMSPEGE = Delayed Matching to Samples, probability of an error following an incorrect response (across all trials); OTSPSFC = One Touch Stockings of Cambridge, percent of times correct first attempt (across all trials); PALFAMS = Paired Associated Learning, number of trials correct first time (across all trials); PALTEA = Paired Associated Learning, total errors (adjusted to include estimated amount of errors for trials not completed); RVPA = Rapid Visual Processing, sensitivity to detect target sequence (does not account for errors); RVPPFA = Rapid Visual Processing, probability of false alarm; SWMBE468 = Spatial Working Memory, times incorrectly revisiting a box (across trials with 4, 6 and 8 tokens); SWMS = Spatial Working Memory, number of times starting search from same box (across trials with 6 and 8 boxes).* p < 0.001 (2-tailed).

Appendix C

Principal component analysis (PCA) was used to identify the common underlying cognitive domains that were represented in the CANTAB tasks. It has been recommended that a minimum of 5–10 cases per variables are included in the PCA, allowing between 7–14 variables to be included in the analysis for our sample size (n = 71; sample size for this analysis was 71 as one participant’s data were partially missing across some CANTAB tests and could therefore not be included in the PCA).
We took a liberal approach using ±3 SD from the mean to identify potential outliers due to the feasibility nature of this study. Winsorising was used for values appearing to be legitimate outliers, being replaced with the next highest non-outlier value [29]. Ten data points were replaced in this way across these nine variables.
For these 9 variables, the Kaiser-Meyer-Olkin (KMO) measure was 0.675, revealing the sample size to be ‘mediocre’ but adequate to proceed with PCA [56]. Assessing the anti-image correlation matrix (as a measure of sampling adequacy), all variables had a KMO value >0.50, the absolute minimum required [56]. Bartlett’s test of sphericity was significant (p < 0.0005), indicating the data were likely factorizable.
Visual inspection of the point of inflection upon the scree plot (Figure A1) was used to determine how many components were to be included; this was identified as four components, which explained 39.2%, 17.7%, 14.4%, and 9.0% of the variance, respectively. These 4 components explained 80.3% of the total variance.
Figure A1. Scree plot of nine CANTAB components.
Figure A1. Scree plot of nine CANTAB components.
Nutrients 14 00071 g0a1
A Varimax with Kaiser normalisation orthogonal rotation was employed. Each variable predominantly loaded (>0.50) onto 1 of the 4 components only. Component loadings are presented in Table A3; values greater than 0.40 are considered to have loaded onto the factor.
Table A3. Four factor PCA component loadings for CANTAB variables.
Table A3. Four factor PCA component loadings for CANTAB variables.
Component
1234
SWMS0.909
SWMBE4680.865
PALFAMS 0.951
PALTEA 0.917
DMSPEGE 0.876
DMSPCAD 0.847
OTSPSFC 0.513
RVPPFA 0.886
RVPA 0.800
While some tasks theoretically assess similar cognitive domains (i.e., SWM and OTS assess executive function; DMS and PAL assess visual memory), these did not load onto the same factor. In this way, it appears that participants in the present study utilised a unique combination of cognitive domain and/or strategies to complete each task. These loading were used to create four composite factor-based scores, with each measure contributing equally to the score: SWM, PAL, DMS/OTS, and RVP.
The four objective cognitive outcomes represent an average of the z-scores from each task outcome as indicated by the Principal Component Analysis.

Appendix D

Table A4. Bivariate correlation matrix between demographic, diet, and cognitive variables.
Table A4. Bivariate correlation matrix between demographic, diet, and cognitive variables.
Pearson rCogPCI (+)CANTAB SWM (+)CANTAB PAL (+)CANTAB DMS/OTS (+)CANTAB RVP (+)Omega-3 IndexFruitVegetableAgeBMITime since DiagnosisMonths of ChemoMonths of RadioFatigue (+)Depression (−)Anxiety (−)Stress (−)

Kendall’s τ
CogPCI (+)1−0.080.070.05−0.060.03−0.10−0.070.22−0.25 *−0.10−0.06−0.120.27 *−0.14−0.18−0.29 *
CANTAB SWM (+)−0.0610.32 **0.29 *0.52 **−0.10−0.30 **−0.38 **−0.55 **0.17−0.210.200.06−0.16−0.100.030.16
CANTAB PAL (+)0.000.20 *10.32 **0.24 *−0.06−0.25 *−0.26 *−0.27 *−0.08−0.130.180.26*−0.13−0.01−0.050.05
CANTAB DMS/OTS (+)−0.050.23 **0.1510.26 *−0.11−0.09−0.23−0.34 **0.15−0.220.200.04−0.10−0.190.09−0.02
CANTAB RVP (+)−0.020.33 **0.150.23 **1−0.03−0.10−0.26 *−0.190.09−0.120.32 **−0.040.14−0.20−0.10−0.01
Omega-3 Index0.06−0.08−0.03−0.070.0510.110.050.13−0.090.28 *0.080.100.06−0.13−0.20−0.26 *
Fruit−0.06−0.19 *−0.16−0.08−0.040.0810.32 **0.16−0.180.08−0.22−0.050.25 *−0.10−0.14−0.12
Vegetable−0.04−0.22 **−0.15−0.11−0.18 *−0.030.27 **10.32 **−0.100.17−0.23 *−0.160.23 *0.09−0.01−0.04
Age0.15−0.36 **−0.18 *−0.25 **−0.110.060.150.21 **1−0.25 *0.24 *−0.130.010.36 **0.11−0.25 *−0.25 *
BMI−0.17 *0.08−0.050.09−0.03−0.02−0.10−0.11−0.16 *1−0.040.130.03−0.34 **0.140.42 **0.16
Time since diagnosis−0.07−0.15−0.07−0.07−0.060.18 *0.070.100.17*−0.0210.150.31 **−0.090.190.020.03
Months of chemotherapy−0.060.18 *0.130.100.25 **0.08−0.11−0.12−0.090.21 *0.1510.09−0.040.02−0.010.05
Months of radiotherapy−0.090.050.20 *0.070.030.07−0.02−0.09−0.010.050.23 **0.111−0.120.25*0.170.18
Fatigue (+)0.22 **−0.14−0.08−0.100.070.060.150.16 *0.28 **−0.22 **−0.06−0.06−0.081−0.37 **−0.53 **−0.40 **
Depression (−)−0.13−0.070.02−0.08−0.14−0.13−0.06−0.020.070.130.100.060.18−0.32 **10.43 **0.44 **
Anxiety (−)−0.110.02−0.080.02−0.13−0.13−0.13−0.05−0.140.17 *0.03−0.040.13−0.45 **0.32 **10.72 **
Stress (−)−0.21 *0.140.050.000.03−0.20 *−0.11−0.04−0.16 *0.060.010.060.12−0.31 **0.38 **0.48 **1
Note. As some variables were not normally distributed or contained extreme (but likely valid) values, the non-parametric rank correlation measure, Kendall’s Tau (τ), was reported. Pearson’s r on top diagonal, Kendall’s tau (τ) on bottom diagonal. *, p < 0.05, **, p < 0.001 (2-tailed). Significant items in bold. (n = 69–76); (+) indicates higher value = better outcome, (−) indicates lower value = better outcome; CogPCI = Perceived Cognitive Impairment; SWM = Spatial Working Memory; PAL = Paired Associated Learning; RVP=Rapid Visual Processing; DMS/OTS = Delayed Matching to Samples and One Touch Stockings of Cambridge.

Appendix E

The following assumptions were assessed for each model: independence of observations (Durbin-Watson statistic); linear relationship between variables and homoscedasticity of residuals (visual inspection of partial regression plots, studentized residuals versus predicted values); multicollinearity (variance inflation factor [VIF] values <10); outliers, leverage and influential points (studentized deleted residuals <3, leverage values <0.20, and Cook’s distance <1); approximate normal distribution of errors (P-P plot and distribution of standardised residuals).
Data analyses presented in this paper include data points identified as potential outliers and influential points. A description of these points in each model are provided below, including how model significance changed through their removal. Overall, few changes in significance of predictors were observed through the removal of outliers/influential points. Due to this, and as this paper predominantly reports feasibility aspects and is largely exploratory, models used in the statistical section included all data points (i.e., including potential multivariate outliers and influential points).
Outliers and influential points
Model 1—Outcome: CogPCI
The model was rerun after one outlier was removed (studentized deleted residuals > −3). A total of 4 potential leverage cases were identified (value = 0.30, 0.27, 0.26, and 0.21) but not removed as they were not deemed to be influential (Cook’s distance < 0.06). There were no changes to significance between the regression when excluding the outlier.
Model 2—Outcome: SWM
A total of 3 potential leverage cases were identified (values = 0.20, 0.28, 0.37); one was potentially influential (Cook’s distance = 0.08) and identified as a multivariate outlier (Mahalanobis distance 25.7) with a leverage value approaching ‘risky’ (value: 0.37). The model was rerun after this outlier was removed. Moreover, 3 new potential leverage cases were identified (values = 0.22, 0.21, 0.21) but were retained in the final model as they were not deemed to be influential (Cook’s < 0.04). There were no changes to significance between the regression when excluding the outlier.
Model 3—Outcome: PAL
Here, 1 potential leverage case was identified (value = 0.20); it was not deemed to be influential (Cook’s distance < 0.03) and thus an alternate model was not rerun.
Model 4—Outcome: DMS/OTS
A number of 2 outliers were identified (studentized residuals −3.36 and Mahalanobis 25.7) and the model was rerun excluding them. Furthermore, 3 potentially high leverage points were then detected (values: 0.21, 0.21, 0.23), but were retained as they were not influential. In the regression without outliers, age persisted as the only significant factor. These models had some violations of regression assumptions, with skewed normality of distribution and some heterogeneity of residuals, suggesting this model should be interpreted with caution.
Model 5—Outcome: RVP
In Model 5, 4 outliers were identified (studentized deleted residuals −3.84, −3.42, −3.39; and Mahalanobis 27.4). The model was rerun without these outliers and another outlier was identified (studentized deleted residuals −4.11). It was rerun and a sixth outlier identified (studentized deleted residuals: −3.13). Once removed and rerun a final time, three potentially impactful leverage points (values: 0.27, 0.22, 0.20) were identified, but were retained as they were not influential. Without outliers, the model significance changed, with the third block becoming significant and vegetable intake becoming the only significant predictor in this block (duration of chemotherapy losing significance). However, we emphasise this was with a substantial removal of cases (9%) and is more indicative of poor model fit.

References

  1. Wefel, J.S.; Kesler, S.R.; Noll, K.R.; Schagen, S.B. Clinical Characteristics, Pathophysiology, and Management of Noncentral Nervous System Cancer-Related Cognitive Impairment in Adults. CA A Cancer J. Clin. 2015, 65, 123–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  2. Ahles, T.A.; Root, J.C. Cognitive Effects of Cancer and Cancer Treatments. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2018, 14, 425–451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Hutchinson, A.D.; Hosking, J.R.; Kichenadasse, G.; Mattiske, J.K.; Wilson, C. Objective and Subjective Cognitive Impairment Following Chemotherapy for Cancer: A Systematic Review. Cancer Treat. Rev. 2012, 38, 926–934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Janelsins, M.C.; Kesler, S.R.; Ahles, T.A.; Morrow, G.R. Prevalence, Mechanisms, and Management of Cancer-Related Cognitive Impairment. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 2014, 26, 102–113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Janelsins, M.C.; Heckler, C.E.; Peppone, L.J.; Kamen, C.; Mustian, K.M.; Mohile, S.G.; Magnuson, A.; Kleckner, I.R.; Guido, J.J.; Young, K.L.; et al. Cognitive Complaints in Survivors of Breast Cancer After Chemotherapy Compared with Age-Matched Controls: An Analysis from a Nationwide, Multicenter, Prospective Longitudinal Study. J. Clin. Oncol. 2017, 35, 506–514. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Leigh, S.-J.; Morris, M.J. Diet, Inflammation and the Gut Microbiome: Mechanisms for Obesity-Associated Cognitive Impairment. Biochim. Biophys. Acta (BBA)—Mol. Basis Dis. 2020, 1866, 165767. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. James-Martin, G.; Koczwara, B.; Smith, E.L.; Miller, M.D. Information Needs of Cancer Patients and Survivors Regarding Diet, Exercise and Weight Management: A Qualitative Study. Eur. J. Cancer Care 2014, 23, 340–348. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Coro, D.G.; Hutchinson, A.D.; Banks, S.; Coates, A.M. Diet and Cognitive Function in Cancer Survivors with Cancer-Related Cognitive Impairment: A Qualitative Study. Eur. J. Cancer Care 2020, 29, e13303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Spitznagel, M.B.; Garcia, S.; Miller, L.A.; Strain, G.; Devlin, M.; Wing, R.; Cohen, R.; Paul, R.; Crosby, R.; Mitchell, J.E.; et al. Cognitive Function Predicts Weight Loss Following Bariatric Surgery. Surg. Obes. Relat. Dis. 2013, 9, 453–459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Coro, D.G.; Hutchinson, A.; Dahlenburg, S.; Banks, S.; Coates, A. The Relationship between Diet and Cognitive Function in Adult Cancer Survivors: A Systematic Review. J. Cancer Surviv. 2019, 13, 773–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Huang, Z.; Shi, Y.; Bao, P.; Cai, H.; Hong, Z.; Ding, D.; Jackson, J.; Shu, X.-O.; Dai, Q. Associations of Dietary Intake and Supplement Use with Post-Therapy Cognitive Recovery in Breast Cancer Survivors. Breast Cancer Res. Treat. 2018, 171, 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Martí, A.; Fortique, F. Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Cognitive Decline: A Systematic Review. Nutr. Hosp. 2019, 36, 939–949. [Google Scholar]
  13. Dyall, S.C. Long-Chain Omega-3 Fatty Acids and the Brain: A Review of the Independent and Shared Effects of EPA, DPA and DHA. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2015, 7, 1–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Marventano, S.; Kolacz, P.; Castellano, S.; Galvano, F.; Buscemi, S.; Mistretta, A.; Grosso, G. A Review of Recent Evidence in Human Studies of N-3 and n-6 PUFA Intake on Cardiovascular Disease, Cancer, and Depressive Disorders: Does the Ratio Really Matter? Int. J. Food Sci. Nutr. 2015, 66, 611–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Australian Institute of Health and Welfare Cancer in Australia 2017. Available online: https://www.aihw.gov.au/getmedia/3da1f3c2-30f0-4475-8aed-1f19f8e16d48/20066-cancer-2017.pdf.aspx (accessed on 22 October 2021).
  16. Harris, P.A.; Taylor, R.; Thielke, R.; Payne, J.; Gonzalez, N.; Conde, J.G. Research Electronic Data Capture (REDCap)—A Metadata-Driven Methodology and Workflow Process for Providing Translational Research Informatics Support. J. Biomed. Inform. 2009, 42, 377–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  17. Wagner, L.I.; Sweet, J.; Butt, Z.; Lai, J.; Cella, D. Measuring Patient Self-Reported Cognitive Function: Development of the Functional Assessment of Cancer Therapy-Cognitive Function Instrument. J. Support Oncol. 2009, 7, W32–W39. [Google Scholar]
  18. Koch, V.; Wagner, L.I.; Green, H.J. Assessing neurocognitive symptoms in cancer patients and controls: Psychometric properties of the FACT-Cog3. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Cambridge Cognition Limited. Cambridge Neuro-Psychological Test Automated Battery; Cambridge Cognition Limited: Cambridge, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  20. Backx, R.; Skirrow, C.; Dente, P.; Barnett, J.H.; Cormack, F.K. Comparing Web-Based and Lab-Based Cognitive Assessment Using the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery: A Within-Subjects Counterbalanced Study. J. Med. Internet Res. 2020, 22, e16792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Harris, W.S.; von Schacky, C. The Omega-3 Index: A New Risk Factor for Death from Coronary Heart Disease? Prev. Med. 2004, 39, 212–220. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Harris, W.S.; Polreis, J. Measurement of the Omega-3 Index in Dried Blood Spots. Ann. Clin. Lab. Res. 2016, 4, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Collins, C.E.; Boggess, M.M.; Watson, J.F.; Guest, M.; Duncanson, K.; Pezdirc, K.; Rollo, M.; Hutchesson, M.J.; Burrows, T.L. Reproducibility and Comparative Validity of a Food Frequency Questionnaire for Australian Adults. Clin. Nutr. 2014, 33, 906–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Burrows, T.L.; Hutchesson, M.J.; Rollo, M.E.; Boggess, M.M.; Guest, M.; Collins, C.E. Fruit and Vegetable Intake Assessed by Food Frequency Questionnaire and Plasma Carotenoids: A Validation Study in Adults. Nutrients 2015, 7, 3240–3251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Cella, D.; Lai, J.; Chang, C.-H.; Peterman, A.; Slavin, M. Fatigue in Cancer Patients Compared with Fatigue in the General United States Population. Cancer 2002, 94, 528–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Lovibond, P.F.; Lovibond, S.H. The Structure of Negative Emotional States: Comparison of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS) with the Beck Depression and Anxiety Inventories. Behav. Res. Ther. 1995, 33, 335–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Lee, J.; Lee, E.-H.; Moon, S.H. Systematic Review of the Measurement Properties of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales–21 by Applying Updated COSMIN Methodology. Qual. Life Res. 2019, 28, 2325–2339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Hutchinson, A.D.; Thompson, E.; Loft, N.; Lewis, I.; Wilson, C.; Yong, A.S.M. Cognitive Late Effects Following Allogeneic Stem Cell Transplantation in Haematological Cancer Patients. Eur. J. Cancer Care 2021, 30, e13448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Tabachnick, B.G.; Fidell, L.S.; Ullman, J.B. Using Multivariate Statistics, 7th ed.; Pearson: New York, NY, USA, 2019; ISBN 978-0-13-479054-1. [Google Scholar]
  30. Cohen, J. A Power Primer. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 112, 155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Australian Government Weight and Body Mass Index. Available online: https://www.health.gov.au/resources/pregnancy-care-guidelines/part-d-clinical-assessments/weight-and-body-mass-index (accessed on 22 October 2021).
  32. Roberts, S.C.; Seav, S.M.; McDade, T.W.; Dominick, S.A.; Gorman, J.R.; Whitcomb, B.W.; Su, H.I. Self-Collected Dried Blood Spots as a Tool for Measuring Ovarian Reserve in Young Female Cancer Survivors. Hum. Reprod. 2016, 31, 1570–1578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Solk, P.; Gavin, K.; Fanning, J.; Welch, W.; Lloyd, G.; Cottrell, A.; Nielsen, A.; Santa Maria, C.A.; Gradishar, W.; Khan, S.A. Feasibility and Acceptability of Intensive Longitudinal Data Collection of Activity and Patient-Reported Outcomes during Chemotherapy for Breast Cancer. Qual. Life Res. 2019, 28, 3333–3346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Zuniga, K.E.; Mackenzie, M.J.; Roberts, S.A.; Raine, L.B.; Hillman, C.H.; Kramer, A.F.; McAuley, E. Relationship between Fruit and Vegetable Intake and Interference Control in Breast Cancer Survivors. Eur. J. Nutr. 2016, 55, 1555–1562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Bansal, G.; Zahedi, F.M.; Gefen, D. Do Context and Personality Matter? Trust and Privacy Concerns in Disclosing Private Information Online. Inf. Manag. 2016, 53, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Dunberger, G.; Lind, H.; Steineck, G.; Waldenström, A.-C.; Nyberg, T.; al-Abany, M.; Nyberg, U.; Åvall-Lundqvist, E. Self-Reported Symptoms of Faecal Incontinence among Long-Term Gynaecological Cancer Survivors and Population-Based Controls. Eur. J. Cancer 2010, 46, 606–615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Leong, V.; Raheel, K.; Yi, S.J.; Kacker, K.; Karlaftis, V.M.; Vassiliu, C.; Annabel, S.H.; Robbins, T.W.; Sahakian, B.J.; Kourtzi, Z. A New Remote Guided Method for Supervised Web-Based Cognitive Testing to Ensure High Quality Data. PsyArXiv 2021, 1–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Australian Bureau of Statistics Census of Population and Housing: Reflecting Australia—Stories from the Census. 2016. Available online: https://www.abs.gov.au/ausstats/[email protected]/Lookup/by%20Subject/2071.0~2016~Main%20Features~Educational%20Qualifications%20Data%20Summary%20~65 (accessed on 21 May 2021).
  39. Hughes, S.; Egger, S.; Carle, C.; Smith, D.P.; Chambers, S.; Kahn, C.; Caperchione, C.M.; Moxey, A.; O’Connell, D.L. Factors Associated with the Use of Diet and the Use of Exercise for Prostate Cancer by Long-Term Survivors. PLoS ONE 2019, 14, e0223407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  40. Van de Rest, O.; Berendsen, A.A.; Haveman-Nies, A.; de Groot, L.C. Dietary Patterns, Cognitive Decline, and Dementia: A Systematic Review. Adv. Nutr. 2015, 6, 154–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Merra, G.; Noce, A.; Marrone, G.; Cintoni, M.; Tarsitano, M.G.; Capacci, A.; De Lorenzo, A. Influence of Mediterranean Diet on Human Gut Microbiota. Nutrients 2021, 13, 7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. La Torre, D.; Verbeke, K.; Dalile, B. Dietary Fibre and the Gut–Brain Axis: Microbiota-Dependent and Independent Mechanisms of Action. Gut Microbiome 2021, 2, e3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Sandhu, K.V.; Sherwin, E.; Schellekens, H.; Stanton, C.; Dinan, T.G.; Cryan, J.F. Feeding the Microbiota-Gut-Brain Axis: Diet, Microbiome, and Neuropsychiatry. Transl. Res. 2017, 179, 223–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Jordan, K.R.; Loman, B.R.; Bailey, M.T.; Pyter, L.M. Gut Microbiota-Immune-Brain Interactions in Chemotherapy-Associated Behavioral Comorbidities. Cancer 2018, 124, 3990–3999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Song, B.C.; Bai, J. Microbiome-Gut-Brain Axis in Cancer Treatment-Related Psychoneurological Toxicities and Symptoms: A Systematic Review. Support Care Cancer 2021, 29, 605–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Henneghan, A.M.; Stuifbergen, A.; Becker, H.; Kesler, S.; King, E. Modifiable Correlates of Perceived Cognitive Function in Breast Cancer Survivors up to 10 Years after Chemotherapy Completion. J. Cancer Surviv. 2018, 12, 224–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Ehlers, D.K.; Aguiñaga, S.; Cosman, J.; Severson, J.; Kramer, A.F.; McAuley, E. The Effects of Physical Activity and Fatigue on Cognitive Performance in Breast Cancer Survivors. Breast Cancer Res. Treat. 2017, 165, 699–707. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Huang, Z.; Zheng, Y.; Bao, P.; Cai, H.; Hong, Z.; Ding, D.; Jackson, J.; Shu, X.-O.; Dai, Q. Aging, Obesity, and Post-Therapy Cognitive Recovery in Breast Cancer Survivors. Oncotarget 2017, 8, 12364–12373. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  49. Myers, J.S.; Wick, J.A.; Klemp, J. Potential Factors Associated with Perceived Cognitive Impairment in Breast Cancer Survivors. Support Care Cancer 2015, 23, 3219–3228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Zhou, S.-P.; Fei, S.-D.; Han, H.-H.; Li, J.-J.; Yang, S.; Zhao, C.-Y. A Prediction Model for Cognitive Impairment Risk in Colorectal Cancer after Chemotherapy Treatment. BioMed Res. Int. 2021, 2021, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Jiang, X.; Huang, J.; Song, D.; Deng, R.; Wei, J.; Zhang, Z. Increased Consumption of Fruit and Vegetables Is Related to a Reduced Risk of Cognitive Impairment and Dementia: Meta-Analysis. Front. Aging Neurosci. 2017, 9, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Nooyens, A.C.J.; Bueno-de-Mesquita, H.B.; van Boxtel, M.P.J.; van Gelder, B.M.; Verhagen, H.; Verschuren, W.M.M. Fruit and Vegetable Intake and Cognitive Decline in Middle-Aged Men and Women: The Doetinchem Cohort Study. Br. J. Nutr. 2011, 106, 752–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Ammann, E.M.; Pottala, J.V.; Harris, W.S.; Espeland, M.A.; Wallace, R.; Denburg, N.L.; Carnahan, R.M.; Robinson, J.G. Omega-3 Fatty Acids and Domain-Specific Cognitive Aging: Secondary Analyses of Data from WHISCA. Neurology 2013, 81, 1484–1491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Johnston, D.T.; Deuster, P.A.; Harris, W.S.; MacRae, H.; Dretsch, M.N. Red Blood Cell Omega-3 Fatty Acid Levels and Neurocognitive Performance in Deployed US Servicemembers. Nutr. Neurosci. 2013, 16, 30–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Befort, C.A.; Klemp, J.R.; Austin, H.L.; Perri, M.G.; Schmitz, K.H.; Sullivan, D.K.; Fabian, C.J. Outcomes of a Weight Loss Intervention among Rural Breast Cancer Survivors. Breast Cancer Res. Treat. 2012, 132, 631–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Kaiser, H.F. An Index of Factorial Simplicity. Psychometrika 1974, 39, 31–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. CONSORT diagram of participant recruitment. Note: Tx = treatment; Dx = diagnosis.
Figure 1. CONSORT diagram of participant recruitment. Note: Tx = treatment; Dx = diagnosis.
Nutrients 14 00071 g001
Table 1. Demographics details of final sample (n = 76).
Table 1. Demographics details of final sample (n = 76).
Characteristicsn (M)% (SD)
Age, years(57.5)(10.2)
Sex
Female7294.7
Ethnicity
Caucasian6990.8
Education
Did not complete high school22.6
High school79.2
Non-university qualification1925.0
University2431.6
Post-graduate2431.6
Employment
Full time1621.1
Part time2431.6
Retired2127.6
Other1519.7
Marital status
Single1317.1
Defacto911.8
Married4255.3
Divorced810.5
Other45.2
BMI 1, kg/m2(27.6)(5.6)
Underweight (<18.5)11.3
Healthy (18.5–24.9)2735.5
Overweight (25.0–29.9)2431.6
Obese (≥30.0)2431.6
Smoker22.6
Cancer type
Breast6281.6
Colorectal1418.4
Cancer Stage
Unknown1013.2
045.3
11925.0
21621.1
32127.6
467.9
Months since diagnosis(32.6)(15.6)
Menstrual status
Not applicable56.6
Pre-menopausal56.6
Peri-menopausal1013.2
Post-menopausal5673.7
Current hormonal therapy3850
History of
Surgery for cancer7497.4
Radiotherapy4964.5
Chemotherapy5369.7
Immunotherapy22.6
Treatment length, months
Radiotherapy (n = 48)(1.4)(0.6)
Chemotherapy (n = 53)(5.1)(2.3)
Immunotherapy (n = 2)(13.5)(14.8)
1 As classified by Australian Government [31] Department of Health; M = Mean, SD = Standard Deviation; BMI = body mass index.
Table 2. Process-related feasibility outcomes.
Table 2. Process-related feasibility outcomes.
Feasibility CriterionDescriptionPurposeTarget
Goal
Target
Result
Target Met?
Screening
refusal rate
Percent not completing screening processIdentify perceived screening burden/inconvenienceNot
pre-defined
30%Not
applicable
Eligibility
rate
Percent completing full screening who are eligible for studyIdentify clarity of recruitment criteria in study promotion≥80%98.1%Yes
Refusal
rate
Percent eligible participants declining participationIdentify perceived study burden/inconvenience≤15% 14.3%Yes
Recruitment rateNumber of participants enrolled over timeIdentify expected recruitment rate over time for future larger studiesEnrol 40
participants in 12 weeks
49
participants (123%)
Yes
Enrol 100
participants in 26 weeks
90
participants
(90%)
No
Retention
rate
Percent enrolled participants completing studyIdentify study protocol burden and acceptability≥80% 84.4%Yes
Satisfaction rateParticipant satisfaction at exit survey Identify whether participant burden is acceptable for this study design ≥80% reporting positive/
acceptable overall
experience
98.7%Yes
Blood
sample
return rate
Percent of complete dried blood spot results from completed participantsIdentify feasibility of measure use for cost and participant burden≥90% 100%Yes
Table 3. Outcome measures of final sample (n = 76).
Table 3. Outcome measures of final sample (n = 76).
OutcomeM (Range)SD
Dietary Outcomes
Fruit, serves/day (n = 76)2.42 (0.15–6.11)1.30
Vegetables, serves/day (n = 76)4.95 (1.10–9.72)1.99
Omega-3 Index, % total RBC equiv. (n = 74)6.37 (3.99–10.32)1.38
Cognitive Outcomes
Perceived Cognitive Impairment (n = 76) (max. range: 0–72)53.22 (19–72)12.65
SWMS raw score (n = 72)7.39 (2–12)2.80
SWMBE468 raw score (n = 72)11.78 (0–30)8.62
PALFAMS raw score (n = 72)12.78 (4–20)3.75
PALTEA raw score (n = 72)13.29 (0–46)11.11
DMSPCAD raw score (n = 72)86.53 (60–100)10.75
DMSPEGE raw score (n = 72)0.05 (0.00–0.40)0.12
OTSPSFC raw score (n = 72)10.97 (2–15)2.96
RVPA raw score (n = 71)0.92 (0.78–0.99)0.05
RVPPFA raw score (n = 71)0.01 (0.00–0.04)0.01
SWM overall component z-score (n = 72)0.00 (−1.47–1.65)0.94
PAL overall component z-score (n = 72)0.00 (−2.64–1.56)0.96
DMS/OTS overall component z-score (n = 72)0.00 (−2.83–0.89)0.78
RVP overall component z-score (n = 71)0.00 (−2.97–1.15)0.89
Psychological Outcomes
Fatigue 1 (n = 76) (max. range: 0–52)36.74 (6–52)10.74
Depression (n = 76) (max. range: 0–42)5.16 (0–26)5.72
Anxiety (n = 76) (max. range: 0–42)3.39 (0–22)4.68
Stress (n = 76) (max. range: 0–42)7.13 (0–22)6.07
1 Higher score for fatigue indicates less fatigue; RBC = Red blood cell; SWM = Spatial Working Memory; PAL = Paired Associated Learning; DMS/OTS = Delayed Matching to Samples and One Touch Stockings of Cambridge; RVP = Rapid Visual Processing; DMSPCAD = Delayed Matching to Samples, percent of trials correct first time (across all delayed trials); DMSPEGE = Delayed Matching to Samples, probability of an error following an incorrect response (across all trials); OTSPSFC = One Touch Stockings of Cambridge, percent of times correct first attempt (across all trials); PALFAMS = Paired Associated Learning, number of trials correct first time (across all trials); PALTEA = Paired Associated Learning, total errors (adjusted to include estimated amount of errors for trials not completed); RVPA = Rapid Visual Processing, sensitivity to detect target sequence (does not account for errors); RVPPFA = Rapid Visual Processing, probability of false alarm; SWMBE468 = Spatial Working Memory, times incorrectly revisiting a box (across trials with 4, 6 and 8 tokens); SWMS = Spatial Working Memory, number of times starting search from same box (across trials with 6 and 8 boxes). Overall component scores are transformed/standardized raw scores formed by an equal weighing of contributing measures (see Appendix C). Data reported exclude outliers as identified in data analysis section.
Table 4. Hierarchical linear regression models used to explore the dietary-cognitive relationship.
Table 4. Hierarchical linear regression models used to explore the dietary-cognitive relationship.
Outcome (n)FactorBSEβpR2ΔR2Sig. F Change
CogPCI (74)
Model 1 *0.1930.1930.002
Age0.2160.1440.1650.138
BMI *−0.6520.260−0.2800.014
Cancer type (Breast ref) *10.7073.6440.3210.004
Model 2 0.2280.0350.221
Age0.1390.1500.1060.354
BMI−0.5400.268−0.2320.048
Cancer type (Breast ref)9.4983.6910.2850.012
Fatigue0.1250.1590.1000.432
Stress−0.2940.254−0.1410.250
Model 3 0.2770.0490.235
Age0.1870.1530.1430.227
BMI−0.5770.267−0.2480.035
Cancer type (Breast ref)9.2573.6560.2770.014
Fatigue0.1810.1610.1450.263
Stress−0.2970.260−0.1420.258
Fruit−1.7171.115−0.1750.129
Vegetables−0.6530.743−0.1020.382
n-3 index−0.2471.022−0.0270.810
SWM (70)
Model 1 *0.2740.2740.000
Age *−0.0520.010−0.5230.000
Model 2 0.2870.0140.264
Age−0.0500.010−0.5020.000
Months of chemotherapy0.0360.0320.1180.264
Model 3 0.3380.0500.193
Age−0.0430.011−0.4380.000
Months of chemotherapy0.0190.0320.0640.554
Fruit−0.0570.085−0.0770.505
Vegetables−0.0930.055−0.1950.095
n-3 index−0.0210.071−0.0310.765
PAL (69)
Model 10.0550.0550.052
Age−0.0240.012−0.2350.052
Model 2* 0.1230.0680.027
Age−0.0230.012−0.2250.056
Months of radiotherapy *0.3270.1440.2610.027
Model 3 0.1650.0420.376
Age−0.0160.012−0.1580.198
Months of radiotherapy0.3120.1460.2490.037
Fruit−0.1030.098−0.1350.299
Vegetables−0.0530.064−0.1070.410
n-3 index−0.0340.083−0.0480.685
RVP (69)
Model 10.0490.0490.068
Age−0.0220.012−0.2210.068
Model 2 * 0.1290.0800.017
Age−0.0160.012−0.1590.181
Months of chemotherapy *0.0840.0340.2890.017
Model 3 0.1620.0340.476
Age−0.0120.012−0.1190.346
Months of chemotherapy0.0780.0360.2690.033
Fruit0.0570.0930.0810.538
Vegetables−0.0930.060−0.2030.126
n-3 index−0.0290.079−0.0440.715
DMS/OTS (70)
Model 1 *0.1110.1110.005
Age *−0.0280.010−0.3330.005
Model 2 0.1300.0190.226
Age−0.0260.010−0.3070.010
Months of chemotherapy0.0360.0300.1420.226
Model 3 0.1560.0260.586
Age−0.0240.010−0.2800.025
Months of chemotherapy0.0350.0310.1390.258
Fruit0.0570.0810.0910.485
Vegetables−0.0590.053−0.1460.266
n-3 index−0.0550.068−0.0950.423
* Significant models (p < 0.05) in bold; significant factors (p < 0.05) in bold for models with significant F change; n-3 = omega-3; CogPCI = Perceived Cognitive Impairment; SWM = Spatial Working Memory; PAL = Paired Associated Learning; RVP = Rapid Visual Processing; DMS/OTS = Delayed Matching to Samples and One Touch Stockings of Cambridge.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Coro, D.G.; Hutchinson, A.D.; Dyer, K.A.; Banks, S.; Koczwara, B.; Corsini, N.; Vitry, A.; Coates, A.M. ‘Food for Thought’—The Relationship between Diet and Cognition in Breast and Colorectal Cancer Survivors: A Feasibility Study. Nutrients 2022, 14, 71. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14010071

AMA Style

Coro DG, Hutchinson AD, Dyer KA, Banks S, Koczwara B, Corsini N, Vitry A, Coates AM. ‘Food for Thought’—The Relationship between Diet and Cognition in Breast and Colorectal Cancer Survivors: A Feasibility Study. Nutrients. 2022; 14(1):71. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14010071

Chicago/Turabian Style

Coro, Daniel G., Amanda D. Hutchinson, Kathryn A. Dyer, Siobhan Banks, Bogda Koczwara, Nadia Corsini, Agnes Vitry, and Alison M. Coates. 2022. "‘Food for Thought’—The Relationship between Diet and Cognition in Breast and Colorectal Cancer Survivors: A Feasibility Study" Nutrients 14, no. 1: 71. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu14010071

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop