Implicit Prosody and Contextual Bias in Silent Reading
Abstract
:Introduction
Stress clash in German: The case of ‘nicht mehr’
- (1)
- Der Polizist sagte, dass man nicht mehr ermitteln kann, wer der Täter war.‘The policeman said that one couldn’t determine anymore who the culprit was.’
- (2)
- ...dass man nicht mehr ermitteln kann, als die Tatzeit.‘...that one couldn’t determine more than the date of the crime.’
- (3)
- ...dass man nicht mehr nachweisen kann, wer der Täter war.‘...that one couldn’t prove anymore who the culprit was.’
- (4)
- ...dass man nicht mehr nachweisen kann, als die Tatzeit.‘...that one couldn’t prove more than the date of the crime.’
Motivation for the present study
Experiment
Design & Materials
- (5)
- Context
- (6)
- Example Test Item
- (7)
- Example Filler ItemContext
Predictions
Results
Discussion
A post-hoc explanation in terms of the unrestricted race model
Acknowledgments
References
- Altmann, G., K. van Nice, A. Garnham, and J. Henstra. 1998. Late closure in context. Journal of Memory and Language 38, 4: 459–484. [Google Scholar]
- Altmann, G., and M. Steedman. 1988. Interaction with context during human sentence processing. Cognition 30, 3: 191–238. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Ashby, J., and C. Clifton. 2005. The prosodic property of lexical stress affects eye movements during silent reading. Cognition 96, 3: B89–B100. [Google Scholar]
- Ashby, J., and A. Martin. 2008. Prosodic phonological representations early in visual word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human perception and performance 34, 1: 224. [Google Scholar]
- Augurzky, P. 2006. Attaching relative clauses in German: The role of implicit and explicit prosody in sentence processing. MPI for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences. [Google Scholar]
- Bader, M. 1996. Prosodic effects and the distinction between primary and non-primary phrases. In Poster presented at the Conference on Architectures and Mechanisms of Language Processing (AMLaP), Torino: University of Torino. [Google Scholar]
- Bader, M. 1998. Prosodic influences on reading syntactically ambiguous sentences. Reanalysis in sentence processing, 1–46. [Google Scholar]
- Bates, D., and D. Sarkar. 2007. lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes.
- Binder, K., S. Duffy, and K. Rayner. 2001. The effects of thematic fit and discourse context on syntactic ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 44, 2: 297–324. [Google Scholar]
- Boland, J. 2004. Edited by C. Clifton and M. Carreiras. Linking eye movements to sentence comprehension in reading and listening. In The on-line study of sentence comprehension: Eyetracking, erp, and beyond. Psychology Press: New York, NY: pp. 51–76. [Google Scholar]
- Boland, J., and A. Blodgett. 2001. Understanding the constraints on syntactic generation: Lexical bias and discourse congruency effects on eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language 45, 3: 391–411. [Google Scholar]
- Boland, J., and A. Cutler. 1996. Interaction with autonomy: Multiple output models and the inadequacy of the great divide. Cognition 58, 3: 309–320. [Google Scholar]
- Breen, M., and C. Clifton. 2011. Stress matters: Effects of anticipated lexical stress on silent reading. Journal of Memory and Language 64, 2: 153–170. [Google Scholar]
- Britt, M. 1994. The interaction of referential ambiguity and argument structure in the parsing of prepositional phrases. Journal of Memory and Language. [Google Scholar]
- Carver, R. 1992. Reading rate: Theory, research, and practical implications. Journal of Reading, 84–95. [Google Scholar]
- Chafe, W. 1988. Punctuation and the prosody of written language. Written communication 5, 4: 395–426. [Google Scholar]
- Clifton, C., A. Staub, and K. Rayner. 2007. Eye Movements in Reading Words and Sentences. In Eye movements: A window on mind and brain. Edited by R. V. Gompel, M. Fisher, W. Murray and R. L. Hill. Elsevier: chap. 15. [Google Scholar]
- Crain, S., and M. Steedman. 1985. On not being led up the garden path: The use of context by the psychological parser. Natural Language Parsing, 320–358. [Google Scholar]
- Dilley, L., and J. McAuley. 2008. Distal prosodic context affects word segmentation and lexical processing. Journal of Memory and Language 59, 3: 294–311. [Google Scholar]
- Drieghe, D., K. Rayner, and A. Pollatsek. 2008. Mislocated fixations can account for parafoveal-on-foveal effects in eye movements during reading. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 61, 8: 1239–1249. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Fodor, J. 1998. Learning to parse? Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 27, 2: 285–319. [Google Scholar]
- Frazier, L., and C. Clifton. 1995. Construal. MIT Press. [Google Scholar]
- Frazier, L., and K. Rayner. 1982. Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences. Cognitive Psychology 14, 2: 178–210. [Google Scholar]
- Hirose, Y. 2003. Recycling prosodic boundaries. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 32, 2: 167–195. [Google Scholar]
- Hoeks, J., W. Vonk, and H. Schriefers. 2002. Processing coordinated structures in context: The effect of topic-structure on ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 46, 1: 99–119. [Google Scholar]
- Kaiser, E., and J. Trueswell. 2004. The role of discourse context in the processing of a flexible word-order language. Cognition 94, 2: 113–147. [Google Scholar]
- Keller, F., S. Gunasekharan, N. Mayo, and M. Corley. 2009. Timing accuracy of web experiments: A case study using the WebExp software package. Behavior Research Methods 41, 1: 1–12. [Google Scholar]
- Kelly, M., and J. Bock. 1988. Stress in time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 14, 3: 389. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- Kentner, G. 2012. Linguistic rhythm guides parsing decisions in written sentence comprehension. Cognition 123: 1–20. [Google Scholar]
- Kjelgaard, M., and S. Speer. 1999. Prosodic facilitation and interference in the resolution of temporary syntactic closure ambiguity. Journal of Memory and Language 40, 2: 153–194. [Google Scholar]
- Liberman, M., and A. Prince. 1977. On stress and linguistic rhythm. Linguistic inquiry, 249–336. [Google Scholar]
- Logačev, P., and S. Vasishth. 2006. The em package for computing eyetracking measures. Potsdam, Germany: University of Potsdam. [Google Scholar]
- Logačev, P., and S. Vasishth. 2013. A multiple-channel model of task-dependent ambiguity resolution in sentence comprehension, Submitted.
- MacDonald, M., N. Pearlmutter, and M. Seidenberg. 1994. The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution. Psychological Review 101, 4: 676. [Google Scholar]
- McDonald, J., K. Bock, M. Kelly, and et al. 1993. Word and world order: Semantic, phonological, and metrical determinants of serial position. Cognitive Psychology 25: 188–188. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
- McRae, K., M. Spivey-Knowlton, and M. Tanenhaus. 1998. Modeling the influence of thematic fit (and other constraints) in on-line sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language 38, 3: 283–312. [Google Scholar]
- Nespor, M., I. Vogel, and et al. 1989. On clashes and lapses. Phonology 6, 1: 69–116. [Google Scholar]
- Ni, W., S. Crain, and D. Shankweiler. 1996. Sidestepping garden paths: Assessing the contributions of syntax, semantics and plausibility in resolving ambiguities. Language and Cognitive Processes 11, 3: 283–334. [Google Scholar]
- Niebuhr, O. 2009. F0-based rhythm effects on the perception of local syllable prominence. Phonetica 66, 1-2: 95–112. [Google Scholar]
- R Development Core Team. 2006. R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software manual]. Vienna, Austria. Available from http://www.R-project.orgISBN 3-900051-07-0.
- R Development Core Team. 2012. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- Rayner, K. 1975. The perceptual span and peripheral cues in reading. Cognitive Psychology 7, 1: 65–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K., M. Carlson, and L. Frazier. 1983. The interaction of syntax and semantics during sentence processing: Eye movements in the analysis of semantically biased sentences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 22, 3: 358–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K., J. Pacht, and S. Duffy. 1994. Effects of prior encounter and global discourse bias on the processing of lexically ambiguous words: Evidence from eye fixations. Journal of Memory and Language; Journal of Memory and Language 33: 527–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shih, S., J. Grafmiller, R. Futrell, and J. Bresnan. 2009. Rhythms role in genitive and dative construction choice in spoken English. In 31st Annual Meeting of the Linguistics Association of Germany (DGfS). Germany: University of Osnabrück. [Google Scholar]
- Snedeker, J., and J. Trueswell. 2004. The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical-biases and referential scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology 49, 3: 238–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spivey, M., S. Fitneva, W. Tabor, and S. Ajmani. 2002. The time course of information integration in sentence processing. In Sentence processing and the lexicon: Formal, computational, and experimental perspectives. Edited by P. Merlo and S. Stevenson. Amsterdam: J. Benjamins Publishing Company, pp. 207–232. [Google Scholar]
- Spivey, M., and M. Tanenhaus. 1998. Syntactic ambiguity resolution in discourse: Modeling the effects of referential context and lexical frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 24, 6: 1521–1543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Steinhauer, K., K. Alter, A. Friederici, and et al. 1999. Brain potentials indicate immediate use of prosodic cues in natural speech processing. Nature Neuroscience 2: 191–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sturt, P. 2003. The time-course of the application of binding constraints in reference resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 48: 542–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Traxler, M., M. Pickering, and C. Clifton. 1998. Adjunct attachment is not a form of lexical ambiguity resolution. Journal of Memory and Language 39, 4: 558–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Gompel, R., and M. Pickering. 2007. Syntactic parsing. In The Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics, 289–307. [Google Scholar]
- Van Gompel, R., M. Pickering, and M. Traxler. 2001. Reanalysis in sentence processing: Evidence against current constraint-based and two-stage models. Journal of Memory and Language 45, 2: 225–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vasishth, S., T. von der Malsburg, and F. Engelmann. 2012. What eye movements can tell us about sentence comprehension. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wickham, H. 2011. ggplot2: Elegant Graphics for Data Analysis. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
- Yuan, J., M. Liberman, and C. Cieri. 2006. Towards an integrated understanding of speaking rate in conversation. In In Proceedings of the Conference on Spoken Language Processing. [Google Scholar]
Copyright © 2013. This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Share and Cite
McCurdy, K.; Kentner, G.; Vasishth, S. Implicit Prosody and Contextual Bias in Silent Reading. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2013, 6, 1-17. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.6.2.4
McCurdy K, Kentner G, Vasishth S. Implicit Prosody and Contextual Bias in Silent Reading. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2013; 6(2):1-17. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.6.2.4
Chicago/Turabian StyleMcCurdy, Kate, Gerrit Kentner, and Shravan Vasishth. 2013. "Implicit Prosody and Contextual Bias in Silent Reading" Journal of Eye Movement Research 6, no. 2: 1-17. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.6.2.4
APA StyleMcCurdy, K., Kentner, G., & Vasishth, S. (2013). Implicit Prosody and Contextual Bias in Silent Reading. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 6(2), 1-17. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.6.2.4