Eye Tracking in the Wild: Piloting a Real-Life Assessment Paradigm for Older Adults
Abstract
:Introduction
Laboratory-Based Eye Tracking Studies
Eye Tracking in the Wild
Innovation: Testing a Real-Life Paradigm
Methods
Participants
Procedure
Legal and Ethical Considerations
Measures
Coding of the Video Material
Statistical Analyses
Results
Feasibility
Proportions
Discussion
Practical Recommendations
- Using the wireless Dikablis Professional Eye Tracking Glasses, it was no problem to record eye movements if participants wore glasses (reading glasses or varifocals).
- As participants move freely during grocery shopping, the connection cable from the eye tracker glasses to the tablet should be tapped down so that it cannot be displayed and thus interrupt the re-cording.
- The WLAN connection might disrupt if the distance from the participant to the router is too large (what easily can happen if the supermarket is big). In this case, the eye tracker data are stored on the tablet (offline) and can be downloaded later. However, it is not possible to monitor the cameras on the laptop in real-time if there no WLAN connection.
- The video coding is very time-intensive if no markers are used. Depending on the area of interest that is coded, it may last up to 24 hours and more to code a video of 10 minutes. For a sample with enough power (i.e., >84 participants), data coding might require approximately 2,016 hours. Videos should be double-coded to provide an inter-rater reliability.
- The illumination in the supermarket may change in different sections. Furthermore, some packing colors (e.g., red) may interact with the illumination. Both of these factors may influence the quality of the video material. In turn, this may cause difficulties to recognize specific products during the video coding process and lead to a weaker inter-rater reliability.
- Participants should pay in cash rather than by credit card (if it is a chip card) as the scene camera will record where they look at, that is the numeric keypad (PIN code).
- Based on our power analysis (see “Statistical Analyses”), we recommend that future studies sample at least 84 participants. This size is required to detect correlation coefficients around r = .30 (Cohen’s d = 0.63). However, to detect weaker associations (i.e., r = .05, Cohen’s d = 0.1), a sample of 3,136 participants is needed.
- Lastly, the articles of Cognolato, Atzori, and Müller (2018) as well as Santini and colleagues (2018) may be helpful guidelines when planning an eye tracker study in a realistic setting.
Future Directions
Ethics and Conflict of Interest
Acknowledgements
References
- Allemand, M., and M. R. Mehl. 2017. Personality assessment in daily life: a roadmap for future personality development research. In Personality development across the lifespan. Edited by J. Specht. San Diego, CA: Elsevier, pp. 437–454. [Google Scholar]
- Aschwanden, D., M. Allemand, and P. L. Hill. in press. Cognitive methods in personality research. In The Wiley-Blackwell encyclopedia of personality and individual differences: Vol. II. Research methods and assessment techniques. B. J. Carducci (Editor-in-Chief & Vol Ed.). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. [Google Scholar]
- Baltes, P. B., U. Lindenberger, and U. M. Staudinger. 2006. Life span theory in developmental psychology. In Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development, 6th ed. Edited by W. Damon and R. M. Lerner. New York, NY: Wiley, pp. 569–664. [Google Scholar]
- Champely, S. 2017. pwr: Basic functions for power analysis. R package version 1.2–1. Available online: https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=pwr.
- Cognolato, M., M. Atzori, and H. Müller. 2018. Head-mounted eye gaze tracking devices: An overview of modern devices and recent advances. Journal of Rehabilitation and Assistive Technologies Engineering 5: 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Duchowski, A. T. 2002. A breadth-first survey of eye-tracking applications. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers: A Journal of the Psychonomic Society, Inc 34: 455–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ergoneers. 2014. D-Lab Manual. Valid for Version 3.0. Available online: https://de1.hostedftp.com/COLnSlQhNsMCRpY1Ko9eLkM6N.
- Folstein, M. F., S. E. Folstein, and P. R. McHugh. 1975. Mini-mental state: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research 12: 189–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gidlöf, K., R. Dewhurst, K. Holmqvist, and A. Wallin. 2013. Using eye tracking to trace a cognitive process: gaze behaviour during decision making in a natural environment. Journal of Eye Movement Research 6: 1–4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grewe, P., A. Kohsik, D. Flentge, E. Dyck, M. Botsch, Y. Winter, and M. Piefke. 2013. Learning real-life cognitive abilities in a novel 360°-virtual reality supermarket: a neuropsychological study of healthy participants and patients with epilepsy. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation 10: 42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hayes, A. F., and K. Krippendorff. 2007. Answering the call for a standard reliability measure for coding data. Communication Methods and Measures 1: 77–89. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoppe, S., T. Loetscher, S. A. Morey, and A. Bulling. 2015. Recognition of curiosity using eye movement analysis. ACM Press; pp. 185–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoppe, S., T. Loetscher, S. A. Morey, and A. Bulling. 2018. Eye movements during everyday behavior predict personality traits. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 12: 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jacob, R. J. K., and K. S. Karn. 2003. Eye tracking in human-computer interaction and usability research: ready to deliver the promises. In The mind’s eye: cognitive and applied aspects of eye movement research. Edited by J. Hyönä, R. Radach and H. Deubel. Amsterdam, Netherlands: Elsevier, pp. 573–605. [Google Scholar]
- John, O. P., L. P. Naumann, and C. J. Soto. 2008. Paradigm shift to the integrative Big-Five trait taxonomy: history, measurement, and conceptual issues. In Handbook of personality: theory and research. Edited by O. P. John, R. W. Robins and L. A. Pervin. New York, NY: Guilford Press, pp. 114–158. [Google Scholar]
- Kashdan, T. B., M. W. Gallagher, P. J. Silvia, B. P. Winterstein, W. E. Breen, D. Terhar, and M. F. Steger. 2009. The Curiosity and Exploration Inventory-II: development, factor structure, and psychometrics. Journal of Research in Personality 43: 987–998. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kaspar, K., and P. König. 2012. Emotions and personality traits as high-level factors in visual attention: a review. Frontiers in human neuroscience 6: 321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lappi, O. 2015. Eye tracking in the wild: the good, the bad and the ugly. Journal Of Eye Movement Research 8: 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Matsumoto, K., S. Shibata, S. Seiji, C. Mori, and K. Shioe. 2010. Factors influencing the processing of visual information from non-verbal communications. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 64: 299–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mehl, M. R., M. L. Robbins, and g. F. Deters. 2012. Naturalistic observation of health-relevant social processes: The Electronically Activated Recorder (EAR) methodology in psychosomatics. Psychosomatic Medicine 74: 410–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Menz, C., and R. Groner. 1985. The effects of stimulus characteristics, task requirements and individual differences on scanning patterns. In Eye movements and human information processing. Edited by R. Groner, G. W. McConkie and Ch. Menz. Amsterdam: North Holland. [Google Scholar]
- Mõttus, R., W. Johnson, and I. J. Deary. 2012. Personality traits in old age: Measurement and rank-order stability and some mean-level change. Psychology and Aging 27: 243–249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Nitzschner, M. M., U. K. J. Nagler, J. F. Rauthmann, A. Steger, and M. R. Furtner. 2015. The role of personality in advertising perception: An eye tracking study. Psychologie des Alltagshandelns 8: 10–17. [Google Scholar]
- Paulitzki, J. R., E. F. Risko, J. M. Oakman, and J. A. Stolz. 2008. Doing the unpleasant: How the emotional nature of a threat-relevant task affects task-switching. Personality and Individual Differences 45: 350–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rauthmann, J. F., C. T. Seubert, P. Sachse, and M. R. Furtner. 2012. Eyes as windows to the soul: Gazing behavior is related to personality. Journal of Research in Personality 46: 147–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K. 1998. Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychological Bulletin 124: 372–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rayner, K. 2009. The thirty-fifth Sir Frederick Bartlett Lecture: eye movements and attention in reading, scene perception, and visual search. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 62: 1457–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Risko, E. F., N. C. Anderson, S. Lanthier, and A. Kingstone. 2012. Curious eyes: Individual differences in personality predict eye movement behavior in scene-viewing. Cognition 122: 86–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- R Core Team. 2016. R: A language and environment for statistical computing (Version 1.1.383). [Computer software]. Available online: https://www.rproject.org/.
- Redmon, J., S. Divvala, R. Girshick, and A. Farhadi. 2015. You only look once: unified, real-time object detection. In 2016 IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (CVPR). Las Vegas, NV, USA: IEEE, pp. 779–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Robbins, M. L. 2017. Practical suggestions for legal and ethical concerns with social environment sampling methods. Social Psychological and Personality Science 8: 573–580. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Salvucci, D. D., and J. H. Goldberg. 2000. Identifying fixations and saccades in eye-tracking protocols. ACM Press: pp. 71–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Santini, T., H. Brinkmann, L. Reitstätter, H. Leder, R. Rosenberg, W. Rosenstiel, and E. Kasneci. 2018. The art of pervasive eye tracking: unconstrained eye tracking in the Austrian Gallery Belvedere. Proceedings of the 7th Workshop on Pervasive Eye Tracking and Mobile Eye-Based Interaction-PETMEI ’18; pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Treue, S. 2003. Visual attention: the where, what, how and why of saliency. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 13: 428–432. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wilkowski, B. M., M. D. Robinson, and C. K. Friesen. 2009. Gaze-triggered orienting as a tool of the belongingness self-regulation system. Psychological Science 20: 495–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wrzus, C., and M. R. Mehl. 2015. Lab and/or field? Measuring personality processes and their social consequences. European Journal of Personality 29: 250–271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- World Economic and Social Survey. 2007. Development in an ageing world. New York: United Nations Department of Social and Economic Affairs. Available online: https://www.un.org/en/development/desa/policy/wess/wess_archive/2007wess.pdf.
Copyright © 2019. This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License.
Share and Cite
Aschwanden, D.; Langer, N.; Allemand, M. Eye Tracking in the Wild: Piloting a Real-Life Assessment Paradigm for Older Adults. J. Eye Mov. Res. 2019, 12, 1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.1.4
Aschwanden D, Langer N, Allemand M. Eye Tracking in the Wild: Piloting a Real-Life Assessment Paradigm for Older Adults. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2019; 12(1):1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.1.4
Chicago/Turabian StyleAschwanden, Damaris, Nicolas Langer, and Mathias Allemand. 2019. "Eye Tracking in the Wild: Piloting a Real-Life Assessment Paradigm for Older Adults" Journal of Eye Movement Research 12, no. 1: 1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.1.4
APA StyleAschwanden, D., Langer, N., & Allemand, M. (2019). Eye Tracking in the Wild: Piloting a Real-Life Assessment Paradigm for Older Adults. Journal of Eye Movement Research, 12(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.16910/jemr.12.1.4