Next Article in Journal
Cameroonian Spice Extracts Modulate Molecular Mechanisms Relevant to Cardiometabolic Diseases in SW 872 Human Liposarcoma Cells
Previous Article in Journal
The Relationship between Modern Fad Diets and Kidney Stone Disease: A Systematic Review of Literature
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Acute Inositol-Stabilized Arginine Silicate Improves Cognitive Outcomes in Healthy Adults

1
Department of Health, Human Performance and Recreation, Exercise Science Research Center, University of Arkansas, Fayetteville, AR 72701, USA
2
St. Elizabeth Healthcare, Edgewood, KY 41017, USA
3
Neurotrack Technologies, Inc., Redwood City, CA 94063, USA
4
JDS Therapeutics LLC., Harrison, NY 10528, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2021, 13(12), 4272; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13124272
Submission received: 21 October 2021 / Revised: 22 November 2021 / Accepted: 25 November 2021 / Published: 26 November 2021
(This article belongs to the Section Nutrition and Public Health)

Abstract

:
Inositol-stabilized arginine silicate (ASI) is an ergogenic aid that upregulates nitric oxide. Acute ASI supplementation improves working memory and processing speed in young adults but there is a lack of data examining other cognitive tasks. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to examine acute ASI effects on young healthy adults by assessing multiple cognitive domains. Nineteen young adults (20.9 ± 3.2 years) completed this randomized, double-blind, crossover study consuming ASI (1.5 g ASI + 12 g dextrose) and placebo (12 g dextrose). The participants completed the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS) and two digital cognitive assessments before consuming the supplement and then completed the same battery of tests 60 min post-supplementation. Repeated measures ANOVA demonstrated that ASI consumption significantly improved total RBANS and immediate memory scores compared to the placebo (p < 0.05). However, no significant differences were displayed between trials for other cognitive domains (p > 0.05). Acute ASI ingestion increased overall RBANS scores and immediate memory scores in young adults. More research is needed to examine the acute effects of ASI on other domains of cognition, in older populations, and its long-term effects on cognition.

1. Introduction

The sports nutrition industry is a growing, lucrative market; however, a common pitfall within the industry is a lack of scientific efficacy with many over-the-counter supplements. Inositol-stabilized arginine silicate (ASI: Nitrosigine®, Nutrition 21 LLC, Harrison, NY) is an ergogenic aid that is gaining more scientific backing due to its ability to significantly improve the bioavailability and absorption of arginine and silicon, thus increasing nitric oxide (NO) circulation [1]. Studies have shown that a single dose of ASI significantly enhances plasma arginine levels up to six hours after consumption [2]. Enhanced NO availability, via arginine administration, induces vasodilation, increases blood flow to the working musculature, improves cardiovascular health, lowers blood pressure, improves exercise performance, and enhances memory [3,4]. Moreover, research demonstrates that NO may be a prospective therapeutic approach in managing and mitigating mild cognitive impairment due to its mechanistic action, increasing systemic blood flow and, conceivably, cerebral circulation [5,6]. Among individuals with learning and memory impairments, the beneficial effects of NO in improving these impairments are well-known [7]. Furthermore, enhancing learning and memory function could potentially delay or slow the cognitive decline process. While it has been established that augmented NO levels improve cognitive functioning in impaired individuals, there have also been studies demonstrating beneficial effects in cognitively normal individuals. In healthy adults, arginine supplementation demonstrated decreases in participants’ stress and anxiety levels [8]. In recent investigations, ASI ingestion augmented mental concentration and sharpness potentially through augmented NO levels, which enhanced blood circulation and aided in the distribution of nutrients to the brain [1,3].
Silicon is a trace element, found in plant foods, thought to increase the bioavailability of L-arginine [9], and plays an essential role in the body (e.g., skin, hair, immune function) [10]. Studies have shown that silica potentially helps to maintain vascular integrity in vascular diseases that occur with age [11]. Additionally, evidence demonstrates a link between silica intake and a decreased likelihood of Alzheimer’s disease and other forms of dementia, possibly due to the mechanistic action of silica binding to and eliminating aluminum from the brain, indicating a positive association between silica ingestion and brain health [12]. While slowing cognitive decline is important in aging, it is also imperative to maintain or enhance cognitive capacity in young adults, which may result in better performance in athletic, academic, and occupational settings.
As such, silicon and arginine combined have shown promising results for improving cognitive function in young adults. ASI supplementation has led to increases in processing speed of up to 45% while completing the Trail Making Test (TMT) [3]. Moreover, ASI ingestion has been shown to increase processing speed after a strenuous bout of exercise compared to placebo [1]. In both of these studies, the TMT was used to assess processing speed. While this test is valuable, it only measures a limited scope of cognitive processes. Therefore, the purpose of this investigation was to evaluate the acute effects of ASI supplementation in young healthy adults on multiple domains of cognition. We hypothesized that acute ASI ingestion should upregulate NO, thereby increasing performance on cognitive tasks in healthy adults. We believed that acute ASI consumption would increase (a) cognitive performance from pre-supplementation to post-supplementation and (b) produce better cognitive battery scores than a placebo (PLA) trial.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

This protocol was a double-blind, crossover, placebo-controlled study. The overall sample was 63% female (n = 12 participants) and the mean age of the participants was 21.0 ± 3.2 years (range 18–28 years). Of the 19 participants enrolled in the study, 16 completed both trials. Individuals were excluded from the study if they had disabling vision loss; were unable to complete the calibration procedure for the web camera; had a history of substance abuse, learning disability, neurological illness (i.e., stroke, tumor), or psychiatric illness; or if they consumed a commercial pre-workout product that included arginine or ASI on the ingredients list. All the participants were required to sign a statement of informed consent approved by University of Arkansas’ Institutional Review Board (Protocol #: 1909220945R001).

2.2. Procedures

The participants reported to the Exercise Science Research Center at the University of Arkansas on two separate occasions. The first visit included the completion of an informed consent document, a medical history questionnaire, and biometric assessments upon arrival on the first trial. The participants completed three different cognitive tests (PRE), consumed ASI (1.5 g and 8 g of dextrose) or PLA (8 g of dextrose), and observed a 60 min digestion period; three different versions of the same cognitive tests were administered for both trials (POST) (Figure 1). The second testing session occurred at least 7 days later (Figure 1). The test order was randomized for each visit. The protocol (Protocol #: 1909220945R001) was approved following Full Board Review by the IRB Committee that oversees research with human subjects.

2.3. Biometric Assessments

Then biometric assessments included height, weight, and body composition. Height was assessed using a standing stadiometer (SECA; Hamburg, Deutschland, Germany). During this assessment, the subjects were asked to remove their shoes and stand as straight as possible. Height was recorded to the nearest 0.1 cm. Weight was measured using a balance-beam scale (Sunbeam Products, Inc., McCook, IL, USA); the participants were asked to remove their shoes and any heavy clothing (sweaters, jackets/coats), and empty their pockets. Weight was measured to the nearest 0.1 kg. Body composition was measured using dual-energy X-ray absorptiometry (DXA; General Electric Company, Madison, WI, USA).

2.4. Cognitive Assessments

The cognitive assessments were completed via computer and paper-pencil mediums during both visits.

2.4.1. Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status (RBANS)

Each participant was individually administered the RBANS assessment (RBANS; Forms A, B, C, & D). The RBANS assessment was completed on an iPad along with paper and pencil. The RBANS assessment construction is explained in detail elsewhere [13]. Briefly, the RBANS is made up of 12 subtests that are used to calculate five index scores and a total score. Test catalogues included: Immediate memory (list learning and story memory tasks), visuospatial/constructional (comprised of figure copy and line orientation tasks), language (picture naming and semantic fluency tasks), attention (digit span and coding tasks), and delayed memory (list recall, story recall, figure recall, and list recognition tasks). Each index score falls within an age-adjusted score [13]. The index scores are combined to produce a total score, which is a summary score of the participant’s performance on the RBANS. The RBANS test takes ~30 min to administer and finish. Minimal clinically important differences (MCID) exist for total RBANS scores (≥5.8 for within-group, ≥3.3 between-group), immediate memory scores (≥6.2 for within group, ≥4.1 between group), delayed memory scores (≥5.4 for within group, ≥3.7 between group) and language scores (≥5.7 for within group, ≥4.8 between group). Previous research showed that the RBANS is significantly correlated with more extensive exams, such as the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale III and the Wechsler Memory Scale III; it also offers strong test-retest reliability [14,15].

2.4.2. Digital Image Pairs

Image Pairs is an eye-tracking-based task measuring visual recognition memory and learning [16,17,18]. The visual paired comparison portion of the test measures the participant’s ability to recognize images already viewed during the familiarization phase. The paired recognition portion of the test measures the participant’s ability to learn and identify image pairs they have been tasked with learning.
There are four phases (one familiarization phase, one learning phase, and two testing phases) for this specific exam. During the first (familiarization) phase, the participants were presented with 10 pairs of identical visual images for 5 s. After the first phase of the VPC, a continuous and cumulative delay occurred across each test trial. Before the second (testing) phase began, the participants were instructed to look at the novel or unfamiliar image. During the test phase, the participants were presented with 10 pairs of images, including one from the familiarization phase and one novel image. The amount of time a participant spent viewing the novel image relative to the total viewing time created a novelty preference score, with higher scores indicating better declarative memory and lower scores suggesting impaired cognitive function [16]. During phase three (learning), the participants were shown the same 10 paired images from phase 2 and asked to remember the paired images for phase four. During phase four (testing), the participants were presented with 25 image pairs. Ten of the pairs were from phase three (correct), ten contained familiar images that were paired incorrectly (incorrect), and five contained sham images that had never been viewed before (sham). For each image pair, the participants selected either yes or no for correct pairs, incorrect pairs, or sham pairs. Accuracy scores were produced from phase 4. Eye movements were tracked and scored. Detailed scoring information is published elsewhere [16]. Previous research has found that digital image pairs hhave significant associations with the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) and digit coding symbol test [16,17,18] while offering strong test-retest reliability [17,18].

2.4.3. Digital Symbol Match

Symbol Match is a processing speed and executive functioning task that utilizes a paired verification or rejection paradigm (forced choice). The participants were instructed to determine whether two symbols are equal or unequal utilizing a legend with nine number/symbol pairs over a two minute time span. At the conclusion of the task, a brief implicit learning trial was administered without the legend present. Scoring was determined by speed and the number of correct trials subtracted by the number of incorrect trials, producing a derived accuracy score for the learning trial. Previous investigations identified temporal stability and good test-retest reliability during this task [19].

2.5. Nutritional Data

The participants were asked to keep track of their dietary intake 24 h before each trial. The participants recorded 24 h dietary recall upon arrival of the visit. The macronutrient and total caloric values were calculated based on publicly available nutrient profiles published by the USDA. The nutritional data were assessed using nutritionist pro (Axxya Systems; Redmond, WA, USA).

3. Statistical Analysis

The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS, version 25) (Armonk, NY, USA) was used to conduct all the analyses. The normal distribution of the data was assessed using histograms and boxplots. Descriptive statistics (means and standard deviations) were calculated for all the data. An a priori power analysis based on 0.80 for power, alpha of 0.05, and two-tailed while using a repeat measures ANOVA (RMANOVA) suggested that 12 participants was a sufficient number with which to obtain meaningful results. Partial eta-squared (η2p) was used to demonstrate the effect size. The following criteria for η2p were used to explain the practical significance of the findings: small (0.01), moderate (0.06), and large (0.14).
An RMANOVA was utilized to analyze within-trial (PRE to POST supplement) differences for both supplement trials. The pre-supplement scores for ASI and PLA supplement trials were covariates for between-trial analyses. The percentage changes from the pre-supplement and post-supplement trials were calculated for both supplements. Dependent t-tests were used to examine differences between pre-trial nutritional intakes between both supplement trials. Statistical outliers were determined through box and whisker plots. No statistical outliers were found as no data points were beyond 3three times the interquartile range. Statistical significance was set at α = 0.05. The demographic information is presented as means ± SD and the inferential data are presented as means ± SE.

4. Results

4.1. RBANS

The total RBANS percentile scores were analyzed through RMANOVA for both PLA and ASI trials. The within-trial analysis differences showed no significant differences from pre- to post-supplement for both trials (F(1, 27) = 0.331, p = 0.57; η2p = 0.01). However, a group by time effect was identified between trials (F(1, 27) = 4.379, p = 0.04; η2p = 0.14; Figure 2) with an increase in performance for ASI scores and a decrease in PLA scores from pre- to post-trial (Table 1). The between-trial differences showed a significant increase in total RBANS scores during the ASI trial compared to the PLA trial (F(1, 30) = 5.658, p = 0.02; η2p = 0.18; Table 1). ASI supplementation exhibited an 11% increase in total RBANS score.
The immediate memory percentile scores were examined through RMANOVA for both the PLA and ASI trials. There were no significant differences within-trial (pre-to-post supplement) (F(1, 27) = 1.098, p = 0.30; η2p = 0.04). A group by time effect was noticed between trials (F(1, 27) = 6.213, p = 0.01; η2p = 0.19; Figure 3), with an enhanced performance observed during ASI supplementation and a decreased performance after the PLA trial (Table 1; Table 2). Moreover, ASI showed a significant increase in scores compared to the PLA trial (F(1, 31) = 4.292, p = 0.04; η2p = 0.12; Table 1); the ASI trial exhibited a 27% increase from pre- to post-supplementation scores.
The language percentile scores were evaluated through RMANOVA for both the PLA and ASI trials. There was a significant increase in the scores exhibited within-trial (pre-post supplement scores; F(1, 27) = 7.483, p = 0.01; η2p = 0.22). However, no significant differences were identified between supplement trials (F(1, 32) = 2.195, p = 0.84; η2p = 0.06; Figure 4).
The delayed memory percentile scores were examined through RMANOVA. Significant within-trial differences were found (F(1, 27) = 19.119, p < 0.001; η2p = 0.42; Figure 5). No significant between-trial differences were found between supplement trials (F(1, 30) = 2.109, p = 0.15; η2p = 0.06; Table 1).
No significant differences were identified during the attention tasks within or between supplement trials (p > 0.05; Table 1). Visuospatial/constructional abilities did not differ between trials during the placebo trial (p > 0.05; Table 1).

4.2. Digital Image Pairs and Symbol Match

The image pairs percentile scores displayed within-trial differences (F(1, 27) = 11.002, p = 0.003; η2p = 0.05). No significant differences were found between the ASI trial and the PLA trial (F(1, 29) = 0.513, p > 0.05; Table 2).
Moreover, the digital symbol match percentile scores showed no within-trial or between-trial differences (p > 0.05; Table 2). The ASI trial exhibited a 6% increase from pre- to post-supplementation scores while the PLA trial showed a 1% increase from pre- to post-supplement scores.

4.3. Nutritional Analysis

A dependent t-test was used to analyze the differences in caloric (kcal), protein (g), carbohydrate (g), and total fat (g) intake variables between trials. There were no significant differences in calorie, carbohydrate, protein, or total fat intake in the 24 h prior to each trial (p > 0.05)

5. Discussion

The primary aims of this study were to examine ASI’s ability to improve cognitive battery performance in healthy participants. Our a priori hypothesis predicted that ASI supplementation would augment various cognitive domains compared to PLA. Our hypotheses were partially supported; acute ASI ingestion improved global cognition and immediate memory performance compared to the PLA. In addition, ASI significantly improved immediate memory scores by 27%. However, there were no significant differences between supplement trials during other cognitive tasks. However, MCID improved in terms of RBANS total score and immediate and delayed memory outcomes during the ASI supplementation trials compared to the PLA trials, suggesting acute beneficial acute after ASI ingestion.
Our results align with previous ASI findings, in which mental focus and acuity were obtained through the increase in NO levels [1,3]. Improved NO levels potentially enhanced blood flow to the brain, providing increased glucose uptake, but this mechanism cannot be confirmed through the current study. However, previous studies have demonstrated that acute ASI supplementation increases blood flow in the brachial artery and augments plasma arginine levels for up to 6 h [2,20]. Moreover, immediate memory improved significantly for the ASI trial group compared to the PLA group. Previous investigations have indicated that NO upregulation augments memory and learning tasks [7]. ASI improved RBANS total and immediate memory scores, while preserving delayed memory scores compared to the PLA trial. The delayed memory scores decreased across both trials, but to a greater degree during the PLA trial. The length of the trial (~3-h) may have contributed to the reduction in performance across trials. The length of the total trial in combination with the 2 h fast pre-trial may have caused participant fatigue during the delayed memory portion of post-supplement testing.
Language scores improved during both supplement trials. However, the only significant improvement was shown in the picture naming assessment during the PLA trial. However, the participants were able to improve by one additional point for both trials from pre- to post-supplementation (Table 2). Processing speed (attention domain and digit symbol match) did not change significantly during the ASI trial. Although previous investigations have identified a 45% increase in processing speed after ASI consumption [1,3], our study did not demonstrate this outcome using digit symbol coding tasks on paper or electronically. Kalman et al. (2016) [3] utilized the TMT, which potentially caused the discrepancy among outcomes. The TMT is known to have a significant learning effect when completing multiple trials [21]. However, the current investigation found that total RBANS scores were significantly different between supplement trials. Therefore, global cognitive functioning was improved with the supplementation of ASI in young adults.
The design of the current investigation as a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study is a strength because it removes common sources of bias in less rigorous study designs. The limitations of this investigation include missing data from participants that did not complete both trials and unequal numbers of subjects between sex. Sex differences in cognition are usually more prevalent in early stages of life, but it could be of interest to compare differences between males and females consuming ASI [22]. Next, the study design took three hours per visit; the participants could not eat or drink anything before the trial and were required to wait two hours after the trial began to start the post-supplementation tests. This may have impacted their performance. As seen in the results section, post-supplementation delayed memory scores decreased during the RBANS assessment. Lastly, the small sample size limits the generalizations that can be taken from this investigation.
The data from this investigation demonstrated the clinical relevance of ASI for healthy populations. ASI supplementation increased overall performance on RBANS and significantly improved memory compared to the PLA trial. One converse finding was the lack of difference in processing speed, which was seen in previous ASI supplementation studies [1,3]. Further studies should examine how much ASI supplementation can improve cognition scores in older populations and explore sex differences. Furthermore, future studies should decrease the amount of time participants must spend in the laboratory to account for fatigue as a confounding variable.
In conclusion, the results of this investigation demonstrate that acute ASI supplementation increased overall cognitive scores in healthy participants. Specifically, memory scores significantly increased in ASI vs. PLA. No significant differences were identified between groups for attention, processing speed, recognition memory, language, and visuospatial skills. However, it should be noted that a minimal clinically meaningful difference was observed for immediate memory, delayed memory, and global cognition. Thus, acute ASI supplementation improved overall cognitive function in healthy adults.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, J.L.G. and M.G.; Data curation, J.L.G. and A.C.; Formal analysis, J.R.M.; Funding acquisition, J.L.G. and J.K.; Investigation, J.L.G., A.C. and J.R.M.; Methodology, J.L.G., M.J., E.N.M. and J.K.; Project administration, J.L.G., M.J. and M.G.; Resources, J.R.M., E.N.M., J.M.G., J.K. and M.G.; Software, J.R.M., E.N.M. and J.M.G.; Supervision, J.L.G., S.P., E.N.M., J.M.G. and M.G.; Writing—original draft, J.L.G.; Writing—review & editing, A.C., M.J., S.P., J.R.M., E.N.M., J.M.G., J.K. and M.G. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

Health: Human Performance and Recreation Department at the University of Arkansas. This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board (or Ethics Committee) of University of Arkansas (protocol code 1909220945R001 and 4 August 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data can be found in the Health, Human Performance, and Recreation department at the University of Arkansas in Fayetteville, AR, USA.

Acknowledgments

JDS Therapeutics LLC for providing inositol-stabilized arginine silicate supplement.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Evans, M.; Zakaria, N.; Marzuk, M.; Ojalvo, S.P.; Sylla, S.; Komorowski, J. An Evaluation of the Effects of Inositol-Stabilized Arginine Silicate (ASI; Nitrosigine®) in Preventing the Decline in Cognitive Function Caused by Strenuous. In Proceedings of the 2018 International Society of Sports Nutrition Conference, Clearwater Beach, FL, USA, 7–9 June 2018; Available online: https://blog.priceplow.com/wp-content/uploads/nitrosigine-preventing-cognitive-decline-caused-by-strenuous-exercise.pdf) (accessed on 20 April 2019).
  2. Komorowski, J.; Ojalvo, S.P. A Pharmacokinetic Evaluation of the Duration of Effect of Inositol-Stabilized Arginine Silicate and Arginine Hydrochloride in Healthy Adult Males. FASEB J. 2016, 30, 690.17. [Google Scholar]
  3. Kalman, D.; Harvey, P.D.; Ojalvo, S.P.; Komorowski, J. Randomized Prospective Double-Blind Studies to Evaluate the Cognitive Effects of Inositol-Stabilized Arginine Silicate in Healthy Physically Active Adults. Nutrients 2016, 8, 736. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  4. Sylla, S.; Ojalvo, S.P.; Komorowski, J. An Evaluation of the Effect of Inositol-Stabilized Arginine Silicate on Heart Rate and Blood Pressure. FASEB J. 2018, 32, 724.12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Austin, S.A.; Santhanam, A.V.; Katusic, Z.S. Endothelial Nitric Oxide Modulates Expression and Processing of Amyloid Precursor Protein. Circ. Res. 2010, 107, 1498–1502. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  6. Talarowska, M.; Gałecki, P.; Maes, M.; Orzechowska, A.; Chamielec, M.; Bartosz, G.; Kowalczyk, E. Nitric oxide plasma concentration associated with cognitive impairment in patients with recurrent depressive disorder. Neurosci. Lett. 2012, 510, 127–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Calabrese, V.; Mancuso, C.; Calvani, M.; Rizzarelli, E.; Butterfield, D.A.; Stella, A.M.G. Nitric oxide in the central nervous system: Neuroprotection versus neurotoxicity. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2007, 8, 766–775. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Smriga, M.; Ando, T.; Akutsu, M.; Furukawa, Y.; Miwa, K.; Morinaga, Y. Oral treatment with L-lysine and L-arginine reduces anxiety and basal cortisol levels in healthy humans. Biomed. Res. 2007, 28, 85–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Proctor, S.D.; Kelly, S.E.; Vine, D.F.; Russell, J.C. Metabolic effects of a novel silicate inositol complex of the nitric oxide precursor arginine in the obese insulin-resistant JCR:LA-cp rat. Metabolism 2007, 56, 1318–1325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Jugdaohsingh, R. Silicon and bone health. J. Nutr. Health Aging 2007, 11, 99. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  11. Buffoli, B.; Foglio, E.; Borsani, E.; Exley, C.; Rezzani, R.; Rodella, L.F. Silicic acid in drinking water prevents age-related alterations in the endothelium-dependent vascular relaxation modulating eNOS and AQP1 expression in experimental mice: An immunohistochemical study. Acta Histochem. 2013, 115, 418–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  12. Rondeau, V.; Jacqmin-Gadda, H.; Commenges, D.; Helmer, C.; Dartigues, J.-F. Aluminum and Silica in Drinking Water and the Risk of Alzheimer’s Disease or Cognitive Decline: Findings From 15-Year Follow-up of the PAQUID Cohort. Am. J. Epidemiol. 2008, 169, 489–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Dickerson, F.; Boronow, J.J.; Stallings, C.; Origoni, A.E.; Cole, S.K.; Yolken, R.H. Cognitive functioning in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: Comparison of performance on the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status. Psychiatry Res. 2004, 129, 45–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Hobart, M.P.; Goldberg, R.; Bartko, J.J.; Gold, J.M. Repeatable battery for the assessment of neuropsychological status as a screening test in schizophrenia, II: Convergent/discriminant validity and diagnostic group comparisons. Am. J. Psychiatry 1999, 156, 1951–1957. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Wilk, C.M.; Gold, J.M.; Bartko, J.J.; Dickerson, F.; Fenton, W.S.; Knable, M.; Randolph, C.; Buchanan, R.W. Test-Retest Stability of the Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status in Schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatry 2002, 159, 838–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Bott, N.; Madero, E.N.; Glenn, J.; Lange, A.; Anderson, J.; Newton, D.; Brennan, A.; Buffalo, E.A.; Rentz, D.; Zola, S. Device-Embedded Cameras for Eye Tracking–Based Cognitive Assessment: Validation with Paper-Pencil and Computerized Cognitive Composites. J. Med. Internet Res. 2018, 20, e11143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  17. Gills, J.L.; Glenn, J.M.; Madero, E.N.; Bott, N.T.; Gray, M. Validation of a digitally delivered visual paired comparison task: Reliability and convergent validity with established cognitive tests. GeroScience 2019, 41, 441–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Gills, J.L.; Bott, N.T.; Madero, E.N.; Glenn, J.M.; Gray, M. A short digital eye-tracking assessment predicts cognitive status among adults. GeroScience 2020, 43, 297–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  19. Myers, J.R.; Glenn, J.M.; Madero, E.N.; Anderson, J.; Mak-McCully, R.; Gray, M.; Gills, J.; Harrison, J.E. Rising to the challenge: Asynchronous remote assessment for cognitive impairment. J. Prev. Alzheimer Dis. 2021. Under Review. [Google Scholar]
  20. Rogers, J.M.; Gills, J.; Gray, M. Acute effects of Nitrosigine® and citrulline malate on vasodilation in young adults. J. Int. Soc. Sports Nutr. 2020, 17, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Buck, K.K.; Atkinson, T.M.; Ryan, J.P. Evidence of practice effects in variants of the Trail Making Test during serial assessment. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2008, 30, 312–318. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Pascalis, O.; De Haan, M.; Nelson, C.A.; De Schonen, S. Long-term recognition memory for faces assessed by visual paired comparison in 3-and 6-month-old infants. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn. 1998, 24, 249. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Trial order.
Figure 1. Trial order.
Nutrients 13 04272 g001
Figure 2. Total RBANS percentile score. * indicates differences between supplement (p < 0.05); #—indicates differences within-trial.
Figure 2. Total RBANS percentile score. * indicates differences between supplement (p < 0.05); #—indicates differences within-trial.
Nutrients 13 04272 g002
Figure 3. Immediate memory percentile score. * indicates differences between supplement; # indicates differences within-trial.
Figure 3. Immediate memory percentile score. * indicates differences between supplement; # indicates differences within-trial.
Nutrients 13 04272 g003
Figure 4. Language percentile score. #-indicates differences within-trial.
Figure 4. Language percentile score. #-indicates differences within-trial.
Nutrients 13 04272 g004
Figure 5. Delayed memory percentile score. # indicates differences within-trial.
Figure 5. Delayed memory percentile score. # indicates differences within-trial.
Nutrients 13 04272 g005
Table 1. RBANS data.
Table 1. RBANS data.
VariableSupplementPrePostLower CIUpper CIp-Value
Total Score IndexASI102.7 ± 4.8105.5 ± 4.6−6.671.140.017 *
PLA104.9 ± 3.8100.9 ± 3.5−0.108.22
Total Score PercentileASI51.1 ± 8.356.6 ± 7.6−13.712.700.024 *
PLA59.3 ± 7.951.8 ± 7.50.3514.65
Immediate Memory IndexASI101.8 ± 4.7107.2 ± 3.7−11.440.610.108
PLA105.8 ± 4.1103.5 ± 3.9−4.699.16
Immediate Memory PercentileASI50.1 ± 8.363.5 ± 6.5−23.45−3.40.047 *
PLA59.3 ± 7.951.8 ± 7.5−8.0415.9
Visuospatial/Constructional IndexASI107.5 ± 3.1106.3 ± 4.1−4.797.150.270
PLA110.9 ± 4.3105.0 ± 3.60.9810.79
Visuospatial/Constructional PercentileASI63.9 ± 6.361.9 ± 8.1−10.3914.520.186
PLA75.4 ± 7.461.2 ± 6.75.1523.32
Language IndexASI90.2 ± 4.999.6 ± 3.2−15.71−3.120.689
PLA90.1 ± 3.898.2 ± 3.1−15.88−0.24
Language PercentileASI37.2 ± 7.849.6 ± 6.9−23.14−1.690.148
PLA33.5 ± 6.245.8 ± 6.7−26.241.58
Attention IndexASI109.1 ± 4.1110.5 ± 5.2−8.595.890.521
PLA109.0 ± 3.7107.8 ± 3.6−3.135.60
Attention PercentileASI63.4 ± 6.963.4 ± 7.7−11.2911.190.960
PLA66.6 ± 6.766.6 ± 7.5−7.116.96
Delayed Memory IndexASI98.8 ± 3.192.8 ± 3.50.4211.700.272
PLA100.4 ± 2.389.4 ± 3.64.3217.55
Delayed Memory PercentileASI46.7 ± 6.437.7 ± 6.5−1.2319.110.157
PLA50.8 ± 5.230.3 ± 6.58.4332.57
Note. means ± SE, ASI: Bonded Arginine Silicate, PLA: Placebo, CI = 95% confidence interval of the mean difference, * p-value < 0.05 for between-differences.
Table 2. RBANS subtest raw scores and Digital cognitive test data.
Table 2. RBANS subtest raw scores and Digital cognitive test data.
VariableSupplementPrePostLower CIUpper CIp-Value
List Learning (IM)ASI29.9 ± 1.231.7 ± 1.1−4.410.810.169
PLA31.7 ± 1.030.6 ± 0.9−1.063.18
Story Memory (IM)ASI18.5 ± 1.019.7 ± 0.9−2.990.730.476
PLA19.4 ± 1.019.4 ± 1.0−1.591.38
Figure Copy (VSC)ASI19.7 ± 0.219.6 ± 0.3−0.320.460.308
PLA19.4 ± 1.019.4 ± 0.7−0.130.83
Line Orientation (VSC)ASI17.5 ± 1.016.6 ± 1.1−0.682.420.741
PLA18.3 ± 0.617.6 ± 0.7−0.241.65
Semantic Fluency (Language)ASI17.7 ± 1.318.1 ± 1.22−2.761.880.599
PLA17.3 ± 1.318.7 ± 0.95−4.421.53
Picture Naming (Language)ASI9.5 ± 0.510.5 ± 0.5−2.540.540.236
PLA8.8 ± 0.39.8 ± 0.1−1.48−0.52
Coding (Attention)ASI61.9 ± 2.563.3 ± 3.0−5.242.490.176
PLA63.5 ± 2.561.2 ± 1.9−1.25−5.84
Digit Span (Attention)ASI12.2 ± 0.612.3 ± 0.72−1.551.430.578
PLA11.7 ± 0.412.4 ± 0.5−1.810.25
List Recall (DM)ASI7.3 ± 0.55.7 ± 0.80.462.790.438
PLA7.8 ± 0.46.9 ± 0.8−0.862.64
Story Recall (DM)ASI11.1 ± 0.610.9 ± 0.2−11.2911.190.347
PLA10.2 ± 0.410.3 ± 0.4−1.231.73
Figure Recall (DM)ASI18.3 ± 3.118.8 ± 0.3−1.540.660.337
PLA17.8 ± 0.618.0 ± 0.5−1.501.00
List Recognition (DM)ASI19.6 ± 0.218.5 ± 0.50.032.220.560
PLA19.8 ± 0.118.8 ± 0.30.441.67
Image Pair (Digital)ASI0.86 ± 0.040.91 ± 0.03−0.090.010.480
PLA0.87 ± 0.040.93 ± 0.02−0.10−0.02
Symbol Digit Match (Digital)ASI0.86 ± 0.040.92 ± 0.04−0.140.030.546
PLA0.91 ± 0.040.92 ± 0.05−0.090.07
Note. means ± SE, ASI: Bonded Arginine Silicate, PLA: Placebo, CI = 95% confidence interval of the mean difference; IM = immediate memory, VSC = visuospatial/constructional; DM = delayed memory
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Gills, J.L.; Campitelli, A.; Jones, M.; Paulson, S.; Myers, J.R.; Madero, E.N.; Glenn, J.M.; Komorowski, J.; Gray, M. Acute Inositol-Stabilized Arginine Silicate Improves Cognitive Outcomes in Healthy Adults. Nutrients 2021, 13, 4272. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13124272

AMA Style

Gills JL, Campitelli A, Jones M, Paulson S, Myers JR, Madero EN, Glenn JM, Komorowski J, Gray M. Acute Inositol-Stabilized Arginine Silicate Improves Cognitive Outcomes in Healthy Adults. Nutrients. 2021; 13(12):4272. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13124272

Chicago/Turabian Style

Gills, Joshua L., Anthony Campitelli, Megan Jones, Sally Paulson, Jennifer Rae Myers, Erica N. Madero, Jordan M. Glenn, James Komorowski, and Michelle Gray. 2021. "Acute Inositol-Stabilized Arginine Silicate Improves Cognitive Outcomes in Healthy Adults" Nutrients 13, no. 12: 4272. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13124272

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop