Next Article in Journal
The Effects of Compression Pants on Oxygen Consumption and Heart Rate during Long-Distance Running
Previous Article in Journal
Biomechanical Analysis of Elite Ice-Climbing Performance
Previous Article in Special Issue
Evaluation of Voluntary Dynamic Balance through Standardized Squat-Lift Movements: A Comparison between Gymnasts and Athletes from Other Sports
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

People with Parkinson’s Disease Are Able to Couple Eye Movements and Postural Sway to Improve Stability

by
Fabio Augusto Barbieri
1,*,
Paula Favaro Polastri
2,
José Angelo Barela
3,
Cédrick T. Bonnet
4,
Matheus Belizario Brito
2 and
Sergio Tosi Rodrigues
2
1
Human Movement Research Laboratory (MOVI-LAB), Department of Physical Education, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Bauru 17033-360, SP, Brazil
2
Laboratory of Information, Vision and Action (LIVIA), Department of Physical Education, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Bauru 17033-360, SP, Brazil
3
Institute of Biosciences, Department of Physical Education, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Rio Claro 13506-900, SP, Brazil
4
UMR 9193-SCALab-Sciences Cognitives et Sciences Affectives, National Center for the Scientific Research (CNRS), Univ. Lille, F-59000 Lille, France
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Biomechanics 2024, 4(3), 460-472; https://doi.org/10.3390/biomechanics4030032
Submission received: 23 April 2024 / Revised: 7 July 2024 / Accepted: 17 July 2024 / Published: 1 August 2024
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Gait and Balance Control in Typical and Special Individuals)

Abstract

:
Considering that people with Parkinson’s disease (PD) experience challenges in the control of both balance and eye movements, this study investigated the effects of saccadic eye movements on body sway in people with PD in two bases of support positions (side-by-side and tandem stances). Ten people with PD and 11 healthy individuals performed (a) fixation; (b) horizontal saccadic eye movements to the right and left; and (c) vertical saccadic eye movements up and down. The protocol for each postural task consisted of one block of six trials, making a total of 12 trials. Body sway and gaze parameters were measured during the trials. In both people with PD and healthy individuals, anterior–posterior body sway was significantly reduced in horizontal saccadic eye movements in contrast to fixation, regardless of the body position (side-by-side and tandem stances). Furthermore, vertical saccadic eye movements increased the area of sway in contrast to horizontal ones (and not to fixation) in people with PD. In addition, people with PD showed a higher number of fixations in all experimental conditions, without changes in the mean duration of fixations in both body positions. In conclusion, individuals with PD can improve body sway by coupling eye movements and postural sway when performing horizontal saccadic eye movements but not when performing vertical saccadic eye movements.

1. Introduction

The coordination between postural control and visual system is essential for successfully performing multiple tasks and achieving environmental goals [1]. Young adults are able to control body sway to facilitate eye movements during visual search tasks [1] and to use reliable visual information to enhance body sway control [2]. People with Parkinson’s disease (PD) experience challenges in the control of both balance [3] and eye movements [4].
People with PD display larger body sway magnitude [5,6,7], higher body sway acceleration and velocity, and lower complexity and adaptability [8] in postural control function while standing upright compared to age-matched neurologically healthy controls [9]. In addition, their limits of stability are reduced [10] due to a disruption of the precisely coordinated execution of agonist and antagonist muscles [11]. These postural impairments are problematic because they can lead to falls [12], which are related to asymmetric control of posture in people with PD [13]. The postural sway of people with PD is higher during challenging upright postural tasks, such as tandem position [14,15]. Poor postural control performance in people with PD is not surprising, considering the many changes in sensory systems [16].
It is known that individuals with PD particularly rely on visual information to compensate for deficits in postural sway control [17]. However, eye movement abnormalities in people with PD are extensively discussed in the literature, including a decreased range of eye movements and prolonged saccadic latency [4,18]. In addition, this population shows hypometria during the execution of voluntary saccades (remembered, predictive, and antisaccades) [19] and impairment of antisaccade latencies, which is an indirect marker of impaired anticipatory postural adjustments [20]. The deficits in voluntary saccade movements in PD are attributed to pathological involvement at the brainstem and basal ganglia levels [20]. While the basal ganglia mediate saccade amplitude and latency [18], the cerebellum is involved in saccade accuracy [21]. However, it is not clear yet whether and how aspects of saccade and postural control are conjointly impaired in PD. A better understanding of how eye movements and postural sway are linked in PD should help to develop more effective strategies to minimize balance impairments in this population.
In both young adults [22,23] and healthy older adults [24,25], it has been shown that body sway is reduced when performing saccadic eye movements. This reduction in postural sway allows the brain to shift gaze accurately, indicating a functional connection between posture and gaze control [1]. However, the impact of saccadic eye movements on body sway depends on the direction of the saccadic eye movements (horizontal vs. vertical) and the level of difficulty of the standing posture in both young and older adults. Vertical saccadic eye movements are more challenging for the eye movement system than horizontal saccade ones, leading to a delayed gaze response [26]. Greater postural instability seems to impair eye movements in the vertical direction, particularly in older adults [25]. When exposed to a challenging standing position (e.g., tandem stance), older adults exhibited greater variability in gaze during vertical saccadic eye movements, which is associated with larger head movements compared to young adults [25]. Furthermore, older adults experience an increase in the effects of saccadic eye movements on body sway when performing a more challenging postural task, adopting a more rigid postural control strategy [24].
This stabilization of postural control could be attributed to both afferent and efferent mechanisms for eye movements. In fact, the afferent motion perception mechanism occurs when the central nervous system uses optic flow information to minimize retinal slip and stabilize the distance between the eye and the visual scene; while the efferent motion perception mechanism occurs when the central nervous system uses the copy of motor commands (i.e., “efference copy”) or extraocular muscle afferents that follow eye movements to stabilize posture [2]. Another explanation for the reduction in body sway during saccadic eye movements in healthy adults may be associated with the achievement of a goal in a supra-postural task [27]. This type of activity includes tasks or behavioral goals that are subordinated to the control of posture [27]. In this case, postural coordination and supra-postural performance have a hierarchical relationship, where posture coordination serves as a means to succeed in supra-postural tasks. The main viewpoint is that the neurologically healthy individual improves postural stability to facilitate, at least in part, the performance of this type of task [28]. Both explanations consider additional resources available, such as sensory cues, attentional efforts, and cognitive engagement, as essential for decreasing postural sway and favoring the spatial accuracy of the saccade in terms of target location [23,29].
Considering that postural sway is decreased in performing saccadic eye movements in healthy young and older adults, testing the impact of such saccadic eye movements is important in people with PD who often experience larger body sway, especially in challenging stances (i.e., semi-tandem position) [14,15], and deficits in voluntary saccade movements [4,18], which increase the risk of falls [12]. In this study, we investigated the effects of horizontal and vertical saccadic eye movements on body sway in people with PD (under dopaminergic medication) and neurologically healthy individuals (control group) during quiet stance tasks in two bases of support positions (side-by-side and tandem stances).
We hypothesized that (i) the brain would decrease body sway to perform horizontal saccadic eye movement compared to the eye fixation condition in neurologically healthy individuals, with a greater reduction during tandem stance than side-by-side stance [24], but we did not expect changes in body sway in people with PD during horizontal saccadic eye movement due to their postural [14,15] and visual deficits [4,18]; (ii) the brain would not reduce body sway during vertical saccadic eye movement compared to the eye fixation condition in both neurologically healthy individuals and people with PD because of the weaker functional connection between posture and gaze control in this scenario [25]; (iii) people with PD would show poorer gaze performance, with an increased number of fixations and decreased mean duration of fixations during both horizontal and vertical saccadic eye movements compared to neurologically healthy individuals, with a greater impact during tandem stance than side-by-side stance; and iv) both groups would show worse gaze performance during vertical saccadic eye movements compared to horizontal saccadic eye movements, considering that the first type of eye movement is more challenging for the eye movement system [26].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

Ten individuals diagnosed with PD (PD group) (8 men/2 women) and 11 neurologically healthy individuals (7 men/4 women) participated in this study. The number of participants was determined using a power analysis that used an alpha level of 0.05, effect size of 0.93 and a power of 90% (G-power®). The analysis was based on the mean velocity of the center of pressure (CoP) from Polastri and colleagues’ study [25], which determined that a minimum of nine participants in each group was needed for the study.
All participants included in this study were over the age of 60 and were able to stand independently during the postural task. The PD group included only those who had received a confirmed diagnosis of idiopathic PD from a specialist according to the criteria of the UK Parkinson’s Disease Brain Bank Field [30], classified between stages 1 and 3 on the Hoehn and Yahr (H&Y) scale [31] and treated with dopaminergic medication. Participants with cognitive deficits (defined as a score below 24 on the Mini-Mental State Examination [32]) (for both groups), rheumatic or orthopedic diseases that impaired the performance of the postural task (for both groups), or any neurological diseases (for the control group) or other neurological diseases (for the PD group) were excluded from the study. To ensure consistent visual acuity among participants, we conducted the Snellen test and only selected individuals with visual acuity scores between 20/20 and 20/30 to participate in the study. Five individuals with PD and three neurological healthy individuals did not meet the inclusion/exclusion criteria and were therefore excluded from the study.
All participants signed an informed consent form and heard a thorough explanation of the research procedures before signing the document. The study received approval from the University Ethics and Research Committee (#11322/46/01/12).

2.2. Clinical Evaluation

A specialist in movement disorders conducted a cognitive evaluation of both the control group and the PD group. The control group was evaluated using the Mini-Mental State Examination and an anamnesis. The PD group was evaluated using a procedure similar to that for the control group, in addition to the H&Y scale and the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale—UPDRS-III [33]. The PD group was assessed under dopaminergic medication (“ON” state) [34].

2.3. Postural Task and Eye Movements

During the experiment, each participant was asked to stand barefoot on a force plate (AMTI-AccuGait). An LCD monitor (37.5 cm × 30 cm, LG, Faltron L1952H, 50/60 Hz, 0.8 A) was placed at eye level, and a red dot with a 2 cm diameter was displayed on the monitor, which was 1 m away from the participant’s eyes. The target was presented on a white background, and its subtended visual angle was 1.15° (relative to looking straight ahead). The stimulus was generated using Flash Mx 7.2 software (Macromedia, Portland, OR, USA).
The participants completed the trials of the two postural tasks in a sequential order, and the three visual conditions in a randomized order. The protocol for each postural task consisted of one block of six trials, making a total of 12 trials. After every three trials, the participant was given a one-minute rest period to avoid fatigue or tiredness that could potentially deteriorate their performance. Each trial lasted 70 s.
The participants performed two types of postural tasks under eye movement conditions: (1) to stand with the feet in a side-by-side stance, with the feet parallel and aligned with the shoulders; (2) to stand with the feet in a tandem stance, with the least affected or dominant foot aligned in front of the most affected or non-dominant foot (Figure 1d,e). The three visual tasks were (a) fixation, where the participant fixed their gaze on a single target displayed at the center of the screen (Figure 1b); (b) horizontal saccadic eye movements to the right and left, with the subject tracking the targets in those directions on the screen (Figure 1a); (c) vertical saccadic eye movements up and down, with the individual keeping their eye on the target positioned above or below the screen (Figure 1c). During horizontal and vertical saccadic eye movements, (i) the angle between targets was 11° to avoid head movements, and (ii) the target appeared first on the left side of the monitor, 9.75 cm away from the center, and then disappeared and reappeared immediately on the opposite side (i.e., the right side), also 9.75 cm away from the center.
To determine the lower-limb preference of the control group, the participants were asked to kick a ball. The limb used to kick the ball was considered the preferred limb [35]. For the people with PD, the most affected limb was determined using items 20–23 and 25–26 of UPDRS-III. The value of the right limb was subtracted from the value of the left limb in each item. If the result of this calculation was positive or negative, the most affected limb was the right or left limb, respectively [35].

2.4. Data Analysis

The force plate measures the forces (Fx, Fy and Fz) and moment components (Mx, My and Mz) to calculate the displacement of the CoP in the anterior–posterior (AP) and medial–lateral (ML) axes. The CoP displacement was filtered using a second low-pass Butterworth filter with a 5 Hz cutoff frequency and a fourth-order zero-lag. A head-mounted eye tracking system (model H6, Applied Science Laboratory, Billerica, MA, USA) was used to measure eye movements with a precision of 1° of visual angle. The system calibration was performed from the fixation of nine points displayed in a 3 by 3 grid and checked in each trial. The sampling frequency was 60 Hz. Although each trial lasted 70 s, the first 10 s were not considered in the analyses.
The following CoP variables were calculated for both AP and ML directions: displacement of sway—the total path length of the CoP along the support base in each direction; mean velocity of sway—the division of the total sway in each direction by the duration of the trial; and root mean square (RMS)—the mean variability of the displacement along the trial. In addition, the area was calculated as 95% of the ellipse area that the CoP covered. To analyze the data from the force platform, a group of specific programs written in MATLAB R_2022a (MathWorks, Inc®, Natick, MA, USA) was used.
Eye movement analysis was conducted using Applied Sciences Laboratories Results Plus software® (Billerica, MA, USA) and MATLAB (MathWorks, Inc®). The following gaze parameters were calculated for both conditions: number of fixations (the total number of fixations during the trials), and mean duration of fixations (the average of all fixation durations in each trial). To calculate these gaze parameters, an eye fixation occurred when the value of two times point of gaze standard deviation was less than one degree of visual angle in the horizontal axis and one degree of visual angle in the vertical axis over 100 ms.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

CoP and gaze variables were pre-tested with the Shapiro–Wilk test to verify the normality of the data and with Levene’s test to verify the equality of variance. Independent t-tests were performed to compare anthropometric and cognitive status between the PD group and the control group. Three-way ANOVAs were conducted to test differences between groups, and within-group conditions, for each CoP- and gaze-dependent variable. The study examined the effects of groups (PD vs. control), visual tasks (fixation, horizontal, and vertical saccadic eye movements) and base of support (side-by-side vs. tandem). The last two factors were measured repeatedly. When the ANOVA showed significant differences between variables, post hoc tests with Bonferroni adjustments were performed. For gaze-dependent variables, visual tasks had only two levels (horizontal and vertical saccadic eye movements). The effect size (η2, partial eta-squared) was also calculated for each statistical analysis and was interpreted as a small effect if it was <0.06, moderate if it was within >0.06 and <0.14, and large if it was >0.14 [36]. The significance level was p < 0.05 (SPSS, version 26.0).

3. Results

Table 1 depicts the anthropometric and clinical status of the PD group and control group. There were no significant differences in age, body mass, height and cognitive status between the two groups (all p > 0.05).

3.1. CoP Parameters

Figure 2 and Figure 3 depict the CoP variables for side-by-side and tandem support positions during fixation and eye movements in horizontal and vertical directions. ANOVAs did not reveal a significant three-factor interaction for the group and base of support and visual task (p > 0.05).
ANOVAs indicated a significant group effect for AP displacement (F1,18 = 24.23, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.57), mean velocity of sway (F1,18 = 18.39, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.50) and RMS (F1,18 = 24.96, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.58), and area of sway (F1,18 = 9.80, p < 0.006, η2 = 0.35). The PD group showed higher levels of AP displacement, mean velocity of sway and RMS, and area of sway compared to the control group.
ANOVAs indicated a significant visual task effect for AP displacement (F1,18 = 6.29, p < 0.005, η2 = 0.25) and RMS (F1,18 = 6.76, p < 0.004, η2 = 0.24). During the horizontal saccadic movements, the participants in both groups reduced AP displacement and RMS compared to the fixation condition.
ANOVAs also indicated a significant base of support effect for ML displacement (F1,18 = 64.49, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.78), mean velocity of sway (F1,18 = 47.61, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.72) and RMS (F1,18 = 61.21, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.77), AP mean velocity of sway (F1,18 = 17.13, p < 0.01, η2 = 0.48), and area of sway (F1,18 = 18.30, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.50). In both groups, the tandem stance resulted in higher levels of ML displacement and RMS, AP and ML mean velocity of sway, and area of sway than the side-by-side stance.
A group by base of support interaction was indicated in ANOVAs for AP mean velocity of sway (F1,18 = 9.21, p < 0.007, η2 = 0.33). The PD group showed greater AP mean velocity of sway during the tandem stance compared to the side-by-side stance (p < 0.001), which was not found for the control group.
A group by visual task interaction was indicated in ANOVAs for the area of sway (F2,36 = 3.59, p < 0.04, η2 = 0.16). Only for the PD group, the area of sway was higher when performing vertical saccadic eye movements compared to horizontal saccadic eye movements (p < 0.005), but there was no significant difference in comparison to fixation condition (p = 0.124).

3.2. Gaze Parameters

Figure 4 shows the gaze parameters for side-by-side and tandem stances during saccadic eye movements in horizontal and vertical directions. ANOVA did not reveal a significant interaction for (i) group and base of support, (ii) group and visual task, (iii) base of support and visual task, and (iv) group and base of support and visual task (p > 0.05).
ANOVAs indicated a significant group effect for the number of fixations (F1,17 = 24.60, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.59) and mean duration of fixations (F1,18 = 5.19, p < 0.03, η2 = 0.23). In all tasks, the PD group exhibited a higher number of fixations and shorter mean duration of fixations compared to the control group.
ANOVAs indicated a significant visual task effect for the number of fixations (F1,17 = 4.37, p < 0.05, η2 = 0.21). The participants in both groups exhibited a lower number of fixations in the vertical saccadic eye movements compared to horizontal saccadic conditions.
ANOVAs also indicated a significant base of support effect for the number of fixations (F1,17 = 6.02, p < 0.02, η2 = 0.26). For both groups, the tandem stance resulted in a higher number of fixations than the side-by-side stance.

4. Discussion

We conducted a study to test whether people with PD are able to reduce their body sway when performing horizontal and vertical saccadic eye movements while standing with feet in a side-by-side or tandem stance. Our findings partially supported our first hypothesis, showing a reduction in body sway for both groups when performing horizontal saccadic eye movement compared to the eye fixation condition (we expected reduction only for neurologically healthy individuals). However, we did not find significant effects of the base of support on body sway during horizontal saccadic eye movements as we proposed in the first hypothesis. In addition, our second, third, and fourth hypotheses were fully supported by our findings: vertical saccadic eye movements did not reduce body sway for both people with PD and neurologically healthy individuals, gaze performance was poorer in people with PD compared to neurologically healthy individuals, especially during tandem stance, and vertical saccadic eye movements led to poorer gaze performance compared to horizontal saccadic eye movements, respectively. It is important to note that all significant effects had a large effect size, indicating that eye movements had a noteworthy impact on postural stability.
During horizontal saccadic eye movements, people with PD were able to reduce body sway during both side-by-side and tandem postural tasks by coupling eye movements and postural sway (Figure 2 and Figure 3). This finding was unexpected and invalidated our main hypothesis. A posteriori, the literature also showed that neurologically affected populations are able to reduce their sway to perform gaze shift tasks as well as older healthy and younger populations [22,23,24]. The afferent and efferent mechanisms of visual stabilization of posture can explain these results [2]. On the one hand, the afferent mechanism involves minimizing changes in the projected image on the retina to maintain a relationship between visual information and body posture during fixation [23,29]. On the other hand, the efferent mechanism, particularly efference copy, attenuates body sway in an attempt to connect pre-saccadic and post-saccadic views of the scene, thus enhancing the spatial accuracy of the saccade concerning the target location [23,29]. Since the eye saccade condition requires greater postural stability to allow spatially more accurate gaze shifts, there seems to be a functional integration of postural and gaze control [1]. Also, similarly to neurologically healthy individuals, the achievement of a goal in a supra-postural task may explain the reduction in body sway during saccadic eye movements [27]. Therefore, there is an improvement in postural stability to facilitate, at least in part, the performance of this type of task [28].
One possible reason for the similar findings in people with PD, under dopaminergic medication, and healthy controls in our study (or other populations in other studies) could be related to subcortical control of posture. During horizontal saccadic eye movements, it is believed that the control of posture shifts to a more subcortical level [27]. It means that the brain uses the brainstem/cerebellum to control the body/head position, while other lower structures take control of eye movements. This hypothesis puts postural control on a “second goal”, making it more “automatic” and reducing body sway during horizontal saccadic eye movement. Consistent with this argument, Bonnet et al. [37], Cruz et al. [7], and Feller et al. [38] showed that PD may not affect a person’s ability to control their posture automatically in simple environments (e.g., when looking at a blank target). It means that people with PD are able to control their posture as well as healthy people in quiet stance. Furthermore, and based on our results, PD individuals are also able to improve their postural control, and thus show functional gaze and posture connection when performing horizontal saccadic eye movements. In other words, performing horizontal saccadic eye movements might be a useful strategy to help the central nervous system in people with PD to counter impairments in basal ganglia related to both gaze shift and postural control.
As expected, vertical saccadic eye movements did not reduce body sway and worsened gaze performance in both groups in comparison to the control fixation task. Therefore, people with PD did not show any functional coupling between gaze and posture but also did not show any impairment when performing vertical saccadic eye movements. As complementary information, vertical saccadic eye movements increased the area of sway in people with PD but only when contrasting the horizontal and vertical saccadic eye movements. Hence, this finding only shows that the coupling between gaze shift and postural control is significantly better when performing horizontal saccadic eye movements than vertical saccadic eye movements and not that there is a PD-related impairment in vertical saccadic eye movements (as again, there was no significant difference between body sway in vertical saccadic eye movements and fixation). We can provide some reasons to explain why the coupling between gaze shift and postural control was not functional when performing vertical saccadic eye movements.
Firstly, we need to acknowledge that the control of horizontal and vertical eye saccadic movements is distinct. In fact, on one hand, the cortical areas mainly involved in horizontal saccade generation are the contralateral parietal and frontal eye fields [26]. On the other hand, the circuits required to execute vertical saccades involve a higher level of activation in the right frontal eye field, cerebellar posterior lobe, and superior temporal gyrus [4]. Secondly, vertical eye movements are more variable compared to horizontal eye movements [25]. Our results seem to confirm that gaze performance is poorest during vertical eye movements. We found a lower number of fixations during vertical saccadic eye movements compared to horizontal saccadic conditions for both groups. However, we did not assess the variability of eye movements to fully confirm the existing literature. Thirdly, the direction of vertical gaze movement could have promoted slight up/down head rotations, which would have increased sway because the head is a heavy segment. Even if head rotations had been similar in left/right and up/down directions, they have clearly different effects on postural sway [39]. In fact, on one hand, in left/right head rotations, the head center of mass stays in line with the body center of mass, thus not creating any couple of rotations and ultimately not increasing postural sway. On the other hand, in up/down head rotations, the head center of mass is not aligned with the body center of mass anymore [39], thus creating couples of rotations that increase postural sway. Unfortunately, we cannot verify this a posteriori hypothesis, as we did not record head rotation. We can only suggest, from the results, that both groups were able to significantly reduce their sway when performing horizontal saccades but not vertical saccades. One relevant finding, though, is that due to the specificity of 90% in discriminating PD and healthy individuals, vertical saccades could be a biomarker for early diagnosis of PD [26] and postural impairments.
Our results validated the hypothesis that people with PD would have a worse gaze performance than neurologically healthy individuals. In fact, they revealed that people with PD used a higher number of fixations in all visual tasks in both postural tasks compared to healthy individuals, which could be an indication of reduced goal-directed control [40]. This suggests that people with PD have difficulty maintaining their visual attention and may shift their gaze toward irrelevant information, which could increase the number of fixations in the visual scene. This behavior can be dangerous, as shown by Gotardi et al. [41], who found that people with PD had an increased number of collisions while driving due to an overall increase in gaze fixations and a shift in visual attention toward task-irrelevant information. People with PD tend to have difficulties in processing temporal information, leading to deficits in temporal judgment [42]. According to Cruz et al. [43], individuals with PD exhibit delayed body sway to visual stimuli during continuous and predictable driving frequency compared to the control group, indicating some disruption in the visual–motor coupling. The pedunculopontine nucleus area, which is responsible for both saccades and posture functions, may explain some of the dysfunction seen in PD patients [20].
Although novel findings were presented above, some limitations of our study need to be discussed. First, the sample size was small but still fitted with the required minimum sample size. Additionally, all significant differences exhibited large effect sizes, which confirms that the number of participants in the study was adequate. A second limitation is that we refrained from the inclusion of patients in H&Y 4 and 5. So, our findings can be valid only for initial and moderate levels of PD. An extension of the study population to the late stages of PD would be relevant in future studies.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, the novelty of our findings is that individuals with initial and moderate PD and ON-drug are able to couple eye movements and postural sway in order to improve body sway when saccadic horizontal eye movements are performed during standing tasks. Furthermore, these individuals with PD can reduce postural sway as efficiently as neurologically healthy people when performing horizontal saccades. However, vertical saccades could affect gaze behavior in PD, potentially compromising the effect of the efferent mechanism for postural stabilization in this population. Therefore, individuals with PD should better perform saccadic horizontal eye movements, and even exaggerate performing them, and they should be careful when performing saccadic vertical eye movements. Incorporating saccadic eye movements into postural training could be an effective intervention strategy for individuals with PD.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, F.A.B., P.F.P. and S.T.R.; methodology, F.A.B., P.F.P., M.B.B. and S.T.R.; formal analysis, F.A.B., P.F.P., J.A.B., C.T.B., M.B.B. and S.T.R.; writing—original draft preparation, F.A.B., P.F.P., J.A.B., C.T.B., M.B.B. and S.T.R.; writing—review and editing, F.A.B., P.F.P., J.A.B., C.T.B., M.B.B. and S.T.R.; project administration, F.A.B., P.F.P. and S.T.R.; funding acquisition, F.A.B., P.F.P. and S.T.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP), grant numbers 2014/20549-0 and 2015/15928-4; Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior -Brasil (CAPES), grant number 001; National Council for Scientific and Technological Development (CNPq); CAPES-COFECUB, grant number Ma 1005/23 (ID Campus France: 49556YD) and 88881.712030/2022-01.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of the School of Sciences at São Paulo State University (UNESP), Bauru (#11322/46/01/12 and 05/10/2012).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data can be requested for the correspondent author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Stoffregen, T.A.; Bardy, B.G.; Bonnet, C.T.; Pagulayan, R.J. Postural Stabilization of Visually Guided Eye Movements. Ecol. Psychol. 2006, 18, 191–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Guerraz, M.; Bronstein, A.M. Ocular versus extraocular control of posture and equilibrium. Neurophysiol. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2008, 38, 391–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Ferrazzoli, D.; Fasano, A.; Maestri, R.; Bera, R.; Palamara, G.; Ghilardi, M.F.; Pezzoli, G.; Frazzitta, G. Balance Dysfunction in Parkinson’s Disease: The Role of Posturography in Developing a Rehabilitation Program. Parkinson’s Dis. 2015, 2015, 520128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Lemos, J.; Pereira, D.; Almendra, L.; Rebelo, D.; Patrício, M.; Castelhano, J.; Cunha, G.; Januário, C.; Cunha, L.; Freire, A.; et al. Distinct functional properties of the vertical and horizontal saccadic network in Health and Parkinson’s disease: An eye-tracking and fMRI study. Brain Res. 2016, 1648, 469–484. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Horak, F.B.; Dimitrova, D.; Nutt, J.G. Direction-specific postural instability in subjects with Parkinson’s disease. Exp. Neurol. 2005, 193, 504–521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Rocchi, L.; Chiari, L.; Cappello, A.; Horak, F.B. Identification of distinct characteristics of postural sway in Parkinson’s disease: A feature selection procedure based on principal component analysis. Neurosci. Lett. 2006, 394, 140–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Cruz, C.F.; Piemonte, M.E.P.; Okai-Nobrega, L.A.; Okamoto, E.; de Souza Fortaleza, A.C.; Mancini, M.; Horak, F.B.; Barela, J.A. Parkinson’s disease does not alter automatic visual-motor coupling in postural control. Neurosci. Lett. 2018, 686, 47–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. de Carvalho Costa, E.; Santinelli, F.B.; Moretto, G.F.; Figueiredo, C.; von Ah Morano, A.E.; Barela, J.A.; Barbieri, F.A. A multiple domain postural control assessment in people with Parkinson’s disease: Traditional, non-linear, and rambling and trembling trajectories analysis. Gait Posture 2022, 97, 130–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. MMancini; Horak, F.B.; Zampieri, C.; Carlson-Kuhta, P.; Nutt, J.G.; Chiari, L. Trunk accelerometry reveals postural instability in untreated Parkinson’s disease. Park. Relat. Disord. 2011, 17, 557–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Doná, F.; Aquino, C.C.; Gazzola, J.M.; Borges, V.; Silva, S.M.C.A.; Ganança, F.F.; Caovilla, H.H.; Ferraz, H.B. Changes in postural control in patients with Parkinson’s disease: A posturographic study. Physiotherapy 2016, 102, 272–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Palakurthi, B.; Burugupally, S.P. Postural Instability in Parkinson’s Disease: A Review. Brain Sci. 2019, 9, 239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Allen, N.E.; Schwarzel, A.K.; Canning, C.G. Recurrent Falls in Parkinson’s Disease: A Systematic Review. Parkinson’s Dis. 2013, 2013, 906274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Beretta, V.S.; Barbieri, F.A.; Orcioli-Silva, D.; Santos, P.C.R.D.; Simieli, L.; Vitório, R.; Gobbi, L.T.B. Can Postural Control Asymmetry Predict Falls in People with Parkinson’s Disease? Mot. Control 2018, 22, 449–461. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Beretta, V.S.; Gobbi, L.T.B.; Lirani-Silva, E.; Simieli, L.; Orcioli-Silva, D.; Barbieri, F.A. Challenging postural tasks increase asymmetry in patients with Parkinson’s disease. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0137722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Mancini, M.; Carlson-Kuhta, P.; Zampieri, C.; Nutt, J.G.; Chiari, L.; Horak, F.B. Postural sway as a marker of progression in Parkinson’s disease: A pilot longitudinal study. Gait Posture 2012, 36, 471–476. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Takakusaki, K. Functional Neuroanatomy for Posture and Gait Control. J. Mov. Disord. 2017, 10, 1–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Brown, L.A.; Cooper, S.A.; Doan, J.B.; Dickin, D.C.; Whishaw, I.Q.; Pellis, S.M.; Suchowersky, O. Parkinsonian deficits in sensory integration for postural control: Temporal response to changes in visual input. Park. Relat. Disord. 2006, 12, 376–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Terao, Y.; Fukuda, H.; Ugawa, Y.; Hikosaka, O. New perspectives on the pathophysiology of Parkinson’s disease as assessed by saccade performance: A clinical review. Clin. Neurophysiol. 2013, 124, 1491–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Anderson, T.J.; MacAskill, M.R. Eye movements in patients with neurodegenerative disorders. Nat. Rev. Neurol. 2013, 9, 74–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Ewenczyk, C.; Mesmoudi, S.; Gallea, C.; Welter, M.-L.; Gaymard, B.; Demain, A.; Cherif, L.Y.; Degos, B.; Benali, H.; Pouget, P.; et al. Antisaccades in Parkinson disease. Neurology 2017, 88, 853–861. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Beh, S.C.; Frohman, T.C.; Frohman, E.M. Cerebellar Control of Eye Movements. J. Neuro-Ophthalmol. 2017, 37, 87–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Rodrigues, S.T.; Polastri, P.F.; Carvalho, J.C.; Barela, J.A.; Moraes, R.; Barbieri, F.A. Saccadic and smooth pursuit eye movements attenuate postural sway similarly. Neurosci. Lett. 2015, 584, 292–295. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Rodrigues, S.T.; Aguiar, S.A.; Polastri, P.F.; Godoi, D.; Moraes, R.; Barela, J.A. Effects of saccadic eye movements on postural control stabilization. Mot. Rev. Educ. Física 2013, 19, 614–619. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Aguiar, S.A.; Polastri, P.F.; Godoi, D.; Moraes, R.; Barela, J.A.; Rodrigues, S.T. Effects of saccadic eye movements on postural control in older adults. Psychol. Neurosci. 2015, 8, 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Polastri, P.F.; Barbieri, F.A.; Brito, M.B.; Bonfim, J.V.A.A.; Gotardi, G.C.; Kuga, G.K.; Rodrigues, S.T. Semi tandem base of support degrades both saccadic gaze control and postural stability particularly in older adults. Neurosci. Lett. 2019, 705, 227–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Waldthaler, J.; Tsitsi, P.; Svenningsson, P. Vertical saccades and antisaccades: Complementary markers for motor and cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. NPJ Park. Dis. 2019, 5, 11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Stoffregen, T.A.; Smart, L.J.; Bardy, B.G.; Pagulayan, R.J. Postural stabilization of looking. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform. 1999, 25, 1641–1658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Oullier, O.; Bardy, B.G.; Stoffregen, T.A.; Bootsma, R.J. Postural coordination in looking and tracking tasks. Hum. Mov. Sci. 2002, 21, 147–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Kowler, E. Eye movements: The past 25 years. Vis. Res. 2011, 51, 1457–1483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Hughes, A.J.; Daniel, S.E.; Kilford, L.; Lees, A.J. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: A clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 1992, 55, 181–184. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Goetz, C.G.; Poewe, W.; Rascol, O.; Sampaio, C.; Stebbins, G.T.; Counsell, C.; Giladi, N.; Holloway, R.G.; Moore, C.G.; Wenning, G.K.; et al. Movement Disorder Society Task Force report on the Hoehn and Yahr staging scale: Status and recommendations. Mov. Disord. 2004, 19, 1020–1028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Almeida, O.P. The Mini-Mental State Examination and the Diagnosis of Dementia in Brazil. Arq. Neuropsiquiatr. 1998, 56, 605–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Goetz, C.G.; Fahn, S.; Martinez-Martin, P.; Poewe, W.; Sampaio, C.; Stebbins, G.T.; Stern, M.B.; Tilley, B.C.; Dodel, R.; Dubois, B.; et al. Movement Disorder Society-sponsored revision of the Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale (MDS-UPDRS): Process, format, and clinimetric testing plan. Mov. Disord. 2007, 22, 41–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Araújo-Silva, F.; Santinelli, F.B.; Imaizumi, L.F.I.; Silveira, A.P.B.; Vieira, L.H.P.; Alcock, L.; Barbieri, F.A. Temporal dynamics of cortical activity and postural control in response to the first levodopa dose of the day in people with Parkinson’s disease. Brain Res. 2022, 1775, 147727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Barbieri, F.A.; Simieli, L.; Orcioli-Silva, D.; Baptista, A.M.; Pestana, M.B.; Beretta, V.S.; Santos, P.C.R.D.; Gobbi, L.T.B. Obstacle Avoidance Increases Asymmetry of Crossing Step in Individuals with Parkinson’s Disease and Neurologically Healthy Individuals. J. Mot. Behav. 2018, 50, 17–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Richardson, J.T.E. Eta squared and partial eta squared as measures of effect size in educational research. Educ. Res. Rev. 2011, 6, 135–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Bonnet, C.T.; Delval, A.; Singh, T.; Kechabia, Y.-R.; Defebvre, L. New insight into Parkinson’s disease-related impairment of the automatic control of upright stance. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2020, 52, 4851–4862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Feller, K.J.; Peterka, R.J.; Horak, F.B. Sensory Re-weighting for Postural Control in Parkinson’s Disease. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Fukushima, J.; Asaka, T.; Fukushima, K. Postural changes during eye–head movements. Prog. Brain Res. 2008, 171, 335–338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Derakshan, N.; Ansari, T.L.; Hansard, M.; Shoker, L.; Eysenck, M.W. Anxiety, Inhibition, Efficiency, and Effectiveness. Exp. Psychol. 2009, 56, 48–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Gotardi, G.C.; Barbieri, F.A.; Simão, R.O.; Pereira, V.A.; Baptista, A.M.; Imaizumi, L.F.; Moretto, G.; Navarro, M.; Polastri, P.F.; Rodrigues, S.T. Parkinson’s disease affects gaze behaviour and performance of drivers. Ergonomics 2022, 65, 1302–1311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Lucas, M.; Chaves, F.; Teixeira, S.; Carvalho, D.; Peressutti, C.; Bittencourt, J.; Velasques, B.; Menéndez-González, M.; Cagy, M.; Piedade, R.; et al. Time perception impairs sensory-motor integration in Parkinson’s disease. Int. Arch. Med. 2013, 6, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Cruz, C.F.; Barela, A.M.F.; Doná, F.; Genoves, G.G.; Ferraz, H.B.; de Azevedo Silva, S.M.C.; Barela, J.A. Parkinson’s disease delays predictable visual cue processing without affecting complex and unpredictable visual cue processing in postural control. Brain Res. 2021, 1751, 147209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. A representation of postural tasks and visual conditions: (a) horizontal saccadic eye movements, (b) fixation, (c) vertical saccadic eye movements, (d) feet in side-by-side stance, and (e) tandem stance. The red dot is the target for eye movement tasks.
Figure 1. A representation of postural tasks and visual conditions: (a) horizontal saccadic eye movements, (b) fixation, (c) vertical saccadic eye movements, (d) feet in side-by-side stance, and (e) tandem stance. The red dot is the target for eye movement tasks.
Biomechanics 04 00032 g001
Figure 2. Means and standard deviations of CoP parameters for side-by-side stance during fixation and eye saccadic movements in horizontal and vertical directions in people with PD (PD group) and neurologically healthy individuals (control group). AP—anterior–posterior; ML—medial–lateral. # indicates significant group differences; + indicates significant differences between fixation condition and horizontal eye saccadic movements (both groups); * indicates significant differences between horizontal and vertical eye saccadic movements for the PD group.
Figure 2. Means and standard deviations of CoP parameters for side-by-side stance during fixation and eye saccadic movements in horizontal and vertical directions in people with PD (PD group) and neurologically healthy individuals (control group). AP—anterior–posterior; ML—medial–lateral. # indicates significant group differences; + indicates significant differences between fixation condition and horizontal eye saccadic movements (both groups); * indicates significant differences between horizontal and vertical eye saccadic movements for the PD group.
Biomechanics 04 00032 g002
Figure 3. Means and standard deviations of CoP parameters for tandem stance during fixation and eye saccadic movements in horizontal and vertical directions in people with PD (PD group) and neurologically healthy individuals (control group). AP—anterior–posterior; ML—medial–lateral. # indicates significant group differences; + indicates significant differences between fixation condition and horizontal eye saccadic movements (both groups); * indicates significant differences between horizontal and vertical eye saccadic movements for the PD group.
Figure 3. Means and standard deviations of CoP parameters for tandem stance during fixation and eye saccadic movements in horizontal and vertical directions in people with PD (PD group) and neurologically healthy individuals (control group). AP—anterior–posterior; ML—medial–lateral. # indicates significant group differences; + indicates significant differences between fixation condition and horizontal eye saccadic movements (both groups); * indicates significant differences between horizontal and vertical eye saccadic movements for the PD group.
Biomechanics 04 00032 g003
Figure 4. Means and standard errors of gaze parameters for side-by-side and tandem stances during fixation and eye saccadic movements in horizontal and vertical directions in people with PD (PD group) and neurologically healthy individuals (control group). # indicates significant group differences; * indicates significant differences between vertical and horizontal saccadic eye movements (both groups).
Figure 4. Means and standard errors of gaze parameters for side-by-side and tandem stances during fixation and eye saccadic movements in horizontal and vertical directions in people with PD (PD group) and neurologically healthy individuals (control group). # indicates significant group differences; * indicates significant differences between vertical and horizontal saccadic eye movements (both groups).
Biomechanics 04 00032 g004
Table 1. Anthropometric and clinical characteristics of the participants.
Table 1. Anthropometric and clinical characteristics of the participants.
PD GroupControl Group
Age (years)66.0 ± 6.069.8 ± 3.4
Body mass (kg)67.4 ± 16.3-
Height (m)1.64 ± 0.05-
H&Y stage2.2 ± 0.4-
UPDRS-III (pts)19.3 ± 6.1-
MMSE (pts)28.2 ± 1.828.5 ± 1.1
H&Y—Hoehn and Yahr scale; Unified Parkinson’s Disease Rating Scale—motor part—UPDRS-III; MMSE—Mini-Mental State Examination.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Barbieri, F.A.; Polastri, P.F.; Barela, J.A.; Bonnet, C.T.; Brito, M.B.; Rodrigues, S.T. People with Parkinson’s Disease Are Able to Couple Eye Movements and Postural Sway to Improve Stability. Biomechanics 2024, 4, 460-472. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomechanics4030032

AMA Style

Barbieri FA, Polastri PF, Barela JA, Bonnet CT, Brito MB, Rodrigues ST. People with Parkinson’s Disease Are Able to Couple Eye Movements and Postural Sway to Improve Stability. Biomechanics. 2024; 4(3):460-472. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomechanics4030032

Chicago/Turabian Style

Barbieri, Fabio Augusto, Paula Favaro Polastri, José Angelo Barela, Cédrick T. Bonnet, Matheus Belizario Brito, and Sergio Tosi Rodrigues. 2024. "People with Parkinson’s Disease Are Able to Couple Eye Movements and Postural Sway to Improve Stability" Biomechanics 4, no. 3: 460-472. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomechanics4030032

APA Style

Barbieri, F. A., Polastri, P. F., Barela, J. A., Bonnet, C. T., Brito, M. B., & Rodrigues, S. T. (2024). People with Parkinson’s Disease Are Able to Couple Eye Movements and Postural Sway to Improve Stability. Biomechanics, 4(3), 460-472. https://doi.org/10.3390/biomechanics4030032

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop