Animal Calling Behaviours and What This Can Tell Us about the Effects of Changing Soundscapes
Abstract
:1. The Use of Sound and the Acoustic Modality
2. Acoustic Information Exchange
Signalling and Communicating
3. Functional Uses of Calling
3.1. Group Membership and Definition
3.2. Contact Calling
3.3. Alarm Signalling
3.4. Foraging
3.5. Acoustic Signaling for Wayfinding and Navigation
3.6. Social Interactions and Mating Displays
4. Language and Culture
5. Implications of Changes in Soundscapes
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Rossing, T.D. Springer Handbook of Acoustics; Springer Science and Business Media LLC: New York, NY, USA, 2007. [Google Scholar]
- Ladich, F. Effects of noise on sound detection and acoustic communication in fishes. In Animal Communication and Noise; Brumm, H., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2013; pp. 65–90. [Google Scholar]
- Stoeger, A.S.; Heilmann, G.; Zeppelzauer, M.; Ganswindt, A.; Hensman, S.; Charlton, B.D. Visualizing sound emission of elephant vocalisations: Evidence for two rumble production types. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e48907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Farina, A. Soundscape Ecology, Principles, Patterns, Methods and Applications; Springer Science+Business Media: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Fletcher, N.H. A simple frequency-scaling rule for animal communication. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2004, 115, 2334–2338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gillooly, J.F.; Ophir, A.G. The energetic basis of acoustic communication. Proc. R. Soc. B Boil. Sci. 2010, 277, 1325–1331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Tervo, O.M.; Christoffersen, M.F.; Simon, M.; Miller, L.A.; Jensen, F.H.; Parks, S.E.; Madsen, P.T. High Source Levels and Small Active Space of High-Pitched Song in Bowhead Whales (Balaena mysticetus). PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e52072. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Seddon, N. 2005 Ecological adaptation and species recognition drives vocal evolution in neotropical suboscine birds. Evolution 2005, 59, 200–215. [Google Scholar]
- Ballentine, B. Morphological adaptation influences the evolution of a mating signal. Evolution 2006, 60, 1936–1944. [Google Scholar]
- Arnqvist, G.; Rowe, L. Sexual conflict and arms races between the sexes: A morphological adaptation for control of mating in a female insect. Proc. R. Soc. B Boil. Sci. 1995, 261, 123–127. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoikkala, A.; Klappert, K.; Mazzi, D. Factors affecting male song evolution in Drosophilia montana. Curr. Top. Dev. Biol. 2005, 67, 225–250. [Google Scholar]
- Dale, R.; Lupyan, G. Understanding the origins of morphological diversity: The linguistic niche hypothesis. Adv. Complex Syst. 2012, 15, 1150017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Slabberkoorn, H.; Smith, T.B. Bird song, ecology and speciation. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B 2002, 357, 493–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Heimann, D. Numerical simulations of wind and sound propagation through an idealized stand of trees. Acta Acust. 2003, 89, 779–788. [Google Scholar]
- Boncoraglio, G.; Saino, N. Habitat structure and the evolution of bird song: A meta-analysis of the evidence for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis. Funct. Ecol. 2007, 21, 134–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ey, E.; Fischer, J. The “acoustic adaptation hypothesis”: A review of the evidence from birds, anurans and mammals. Bioacoustics 2009, 19, 21–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morton, E.S. Ecological Sources of Selection on Avian Sounds. Am. Nat. 1975, 109, 17–34. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Hansen, P. Vocal learning: Its role in adapting sound structures to long-distance propagation, and a hypothesis on its evolution. Anim. Behav. 1979, 27, 1270–1271. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Endler, J.A. Signals, signal conditions, and the direction of evolution. Am. Nat. 1992, 139, 125–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schaefer, H.; Ruxton, G. Signal diversity, sexual selection, and speciation. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Evol. Syst. 2015, 46, 573–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arasco, A.G.; Manser, M.; Watson, S.K.; Kyabulima, S.; Radford, A.N.; Cant, M.A.; Garcia, M. Testing the acoustic adaptation hypothesis with vocalizations from three mongoose species. Anim. Behav. 2022, 187, 71–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lombard, E. Le signe de l’elevation de la voix (“The sign of the rise in the voice”) annals maladiers oreille. Larynx Nez Pharynx 1911, 37, 101–119. [Google Scholar]
- Stowe, L.M.; Golob, E.J. Evidence that the Lombard effect is frequency-specific in humans. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2013, 134, 640–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Junqua, J.-C. The influence of acoustics on speech production: A noise-induced stress phenomenon known as the Lombard reflex. Speech Commun. 1996, 20, 13–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zollinger, S.A.; Brumm, H. The Lombard effect. Curr. Biol. 2011, 21, R614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Wisdom, S.; Bowles, A.E.; Anderson, K.E. Development of behaviour and sound repertoire of a rehabilitating gray whale calf. Aquat. Mamm. 2001, 27, 239–255. [Google Scholar]
- Lenneberg, E.H. Biological Foundations of Language; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Marler, P.; Peters, S. Structural changes in song ontogeny in the swamp sparrow Melospiza georgiana. Auk 1981, 99, 446–458. [Google Scholar]
- McCowan, B.; Reiss, D. Vocal learning in captive bottlenose dolphins: A comparison with humans and nonhuman animals. In Social Influences on Vocal Development; Snowdon, C.T., Hausberger, M., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1997; pp. 178–207. [Google Scholar]
- Lipkind, D.; Marcus, G.F.; Bemis, D.K.; Sasahara, K.; Jacoby, N.; Takahasi, M.; Suzuki, K.; Feher, O.; Ravbar, P.; Okanoya, K.; et al. Stepwise acquisition of vocal combinatorial capacity in songbirds and human infants. Nature 2013, 498, 104–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Janik, V.M.; Slater, P.J. Vocal Learning in Mammals. Adv. Study Behav. 1997, 26, 59–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Podos, J.; Nowicki, S.; Peters, S. Permissiveness in the learning and development of song syntax in swamp sparrows. Anim. Behav. 1999, 58, 93–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ollervides, F.J. Gray Whales and Boat Traffic: Movement, Vocal, and Behavioral Responses in Bahia Magdalena, Mexico. Ph.D. Thesis, Texas A and M University, Dallas, TX, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Charles, S.M. Social Context of Gray Whale Eschrichtius robustus Sound Activity. Master’s Thesis, Texas A and M University, Dallas, TX, USA, 2011. [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez, A.A.; Knörnschild, M. Pup directed vocalisations of adult females and males in a vocal learning bat. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2020, 8, 265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jarvis, E.D. Selection for and against vocal learning in birds and mammals. Ornithol. Sci. 2006, 5, 5–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ramus, F.; Hauser, M.D.; Miller, C.; Morris, D.; Mehler, J. Language discrimination by human newborns and by cotton-top tamarin monkeys. Science 2000, 288, 349–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Toro, J.M.; Trobalon, J.B.; Sebastian-Galles, N. The use of prosodic cues in language discrimination tasks by rats. Anim. Cogn. 2003, 6, 131–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Toro, J.M.; Trobalon, J.B.; Sebastian-Galles, N. Effects of backward speech and speaker variability in language discrimination by rats. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. B 2005, 31, 95–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tincoff, R.; Hauser, M.; Tsao, F.; Spaepen, G.; Ramus, F.; Mehler, J. The role of speech rhythm in language discrimination: Further tests with a non-human primate. Dev. Sci. 2005, 8, 26–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Naoi, N.; Watanabe, S.; Maekawa, K.; Hibiya, J. Prosody discrimination by songbirds (Padda oryzivora). PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e47446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wynne, C.D.L.; Udell, M.A.R. Animal Cognition: Evolution, Behavior and Cognition, 2nd revised ed.; Palgrave Macmillan: Basingstoke, UK, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Fox, E.A.; Sitompul, A.F.; Van Schaik, C.P. Intelligent tool use in wild Sumatran orangutans. In The Mentalities of Gorillas and Orangutans: Comparative Perspectives; Parker, S.T., Mitchell Miles, H.L., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1999; pp. 99–116. [Google Scholar]
- Guinee, L.N.; Chu, K.; Dorsey, E.M. Changes over time in the songs of known individual humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae). In Communication and Behavior of Whales; Payne, R., Ed.; Westview Press: Boulder, CO, USA, 1999; pp. 59–80. [Google Scholar]
- Janik, V.M. Pitfalls in the categorization of behaviour: A comparisons of dolphin whistle classification methods. Anim. Behav. 2013, 57, 133–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Becker, P.H. The coding of species-specific characteristics in bird sounds. In Ecology and Evolution of Acoustic Communication in Birds; Kroodsma, D.E., Miller, E.H., Eds.; Cornell University Press: Ithaca, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 136–159. [Google Scholar]
- Kroodsma, D.E.; Byers, B.E. The Function(s) of Bird Song. Am. Zool. 1991, 31, 318–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bradbury, J.W.; Vehrencamp, S.L. Principle of Animal Communication; Sinauer Associates: Sunderland, MA, USA, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Curé, C.; Mathevon, N.; Mundry, R.; Aubin, T. Acoustic cues used for species recognition can differ between sexes and sibling species: Evidence in shearwaters. Anim. Behav. 2012, 84, 239–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maynard-Smith, J.; Harper, D. Animal Signals; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Jones, T.K.; Allen, K.A.; Moss, C.F. Communication with self, friends and foes in active sensing animals. J. Exp. Biol 2021, 224, jeb242637. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zahavi, A. The pattern of vocal signals and the information they convey. Behaviour 1982, 80, 1–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ehret, G. Infant rodent ultrasounds? A gate to the understanding of sound communication. Behav. Genet. 2005, 35, 19–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brudzynski, S.M. Ultrasonic calls of rats as indicator variables of negative or positive states: Acetylcholine–dopamine interaction and acoustic coding. Behav. Brain Res. 2007, 182, 261–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Harris, T.R.; Fitch, W.T.; Goldstein, L.M.; Fashing, P.J. Black and White Colobus Monkey (Colobus guereza) roars as a source of both honest and exaggerated information about body mass. Ethology 2006, 112, 911–920. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pfefferle, D.; Fischer, J. Sounds and size: Identification of acoustic variables that reflect body size in hamadryas baboons, Papio hamadryas. Anim. Behav. 2006, 72, 43–51. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barclay, R.M.R. Interindividual use of echolocation calls: Eavesdropping by bats. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1982, 10, 271–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masters, W.; Raver, K.A.; Kazial, K.A. Sonar signals of big brown bats, Eptesicus fuscus, contain information about individual identity, age and family affiliation. Anim. Behav. 1995, 50, 1243–1260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Seyfarth, R.M.; Cheney, D.L.; Marler, P. Vervet monkey alarm calls: Semantic communication in a free-ranging primate. Anim. Behav. 1980, 28, 1070–1094. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yin, S.; McCowan, B. Barking in domestic dogs: Context specificity and individual identification. Anim. Behav. 2004, 68, 343–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yeon, S.C. The vocal communication of canines. J. Vet. Behav. 2007, 2, 141–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Handelman, B. Canine Behavior: A Photo Illustrated Handbook; Dogwise Publishing: Wenatchee, WA, USA, 2012; ISBN 0976511827. [Google Scholar]
- Pongrácz, P.; Molnár, C.; Miklósi, Á.; Csányi, V. Human listeners are able to classify dog (Canis familiaris) barks recorded in different situations. J. Comp. Psychol. 2005, 119, 136–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ficken, M.S.; Popp, J. A comparative analysis of passerine mobbing calls. Auk 1996, 113, 370–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, C.S.; Evans, L.; Marler, P. On the meaning of alarm calls: Functional reference in an avian vocal system. Anim. Behav. 1993, 46, 23–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Swan, D.C.; Hare, J.F. The first cut is the deepest: Primary syllables of Richardson’s ground squirrel, Spermophilus richardsonii, repeated calls alert receivers. Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 47–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freeberg, T.M. Complexity in the chick-a-dee call of Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis): Associations of context and signaler behavior to call structure. Auk 2008, 125, 896–907. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farnsworth, A.; Lovette, I.J. Evolution of nocturnal flight calls in migrating wood-warblers: Apparent lack of morphological constraints. J. Avian Biol. 2005, 36, 337–347. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, C.S.; Marler, P. Food calling and audience effects in male chickens, Gallus gallus: Their relationships to food availability, courtship and social facilitation. Anim. Behav. 1994, 47, 1159–1170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Marler, P. Bird calls: Their potential for behavioral neurobiology. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2004, 1016, 31–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poole, J.H. Sex differences in the behaviour of African elephants. In The Differences between the Sexes; Short, R.V., Balaban, E., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1994; pp. 331–346. [Google Scholar]
- Langbauer, W.R. Elephant communication. Zoo Biol. 2000, 19, 425–445. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Maciej, P.; Fischer, J.; Hammerschmidt, K. Transmission characteristics of primate vocalizations: Implications for acoustic analyses. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e23015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lakshminarayanan, K.; Ben Shalom, D.; van Wassenhove, V.; Orbelo, D.; Houde, J.; Poeppel, D. The effect of spectral manipulations on the identification of affective and linguistic prosody. Brain Lang. 2003, 84, 250–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rickheit, G.; Herrmann, T.; Deutsch, W. Psycholinguistik. Ein Internationales Handbuch/Psycholingistic. An International Handbook; Walter de Gruyter: Berlin, Germany, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Sidtis, D.; Kreiman, J. In the beginning was the familiar voice: Personally familiar voices in the evolutionary and contemporary biology of communication. Integr. Psychol. Behav. Sci. 2011, 46, 146–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Busnel, R.G. Acoustic communication. In Acoustic Communication: Techniques of Study and Results of Research; Sebeok, T.A., Ed.; Indiana University Press: Bloomington, IN, USA, 1968; p. 127. [Google Scholar]
- Mellinger, D.K.; Nieukirk, S.L.; Matsumoto, H.; Heimlich, S.L.; Dziak, R.P.; Haxel, J.; Fowler, M.; Meinig, C.; Miller, H.V. Seasonal occurrence of North Atlantic right whale (Eubalaena glacialis) vocalizations at two sites on the Scotian Shelf. Mar. Mammal Sci. 2007, 23, 856–867. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Baumgartner, M.F.; Fratantoni, D.M. Diel periodicity in both sei whale vocalisation rates and the vertical migration of their copepod prey observed from ocean gliders. Limnol. Oceanogr. 2008, 53, 2197–2209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Parks, S.E.; Johnson, M.; Nowacek, D.; Tyack, P. Individual right whales call louder in increased environmental noise. Biol. Lett. 2010, 7, 33–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Vu, E.; Risch, D.; Clark, C.; Gaylord, S.; Hatch, L.; Thompson, M.; Wiley, D.; Van Parijs, S. Humpback whale song occurs extensively on feeding grounds in the western North Atlantic Ocean. Aquat. Biol. 2012, 14, 175–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krause, B. The niche hypothesis. Soundsc. Newsl. 1993, 6, 6–10. [Google Scholar]
- Weisman, R.; Ratcliffe, L.; Johnsrude, I.; Hurly, T.A. Absolute and relative pitch production in the song of the black-capped chickadee. Condor 1990, 92, 118–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Sturdy, C.B.; Bloomfield, L.L.; Charrier, I.; Lee, T.T.-Y. Chickadee vocal production and perception: An integrative approach to understanding acoustic communication. In Ecology and Behavior of Chickadees and Titmice: An Integrated Approach; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2007; pp. 153–166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krams, I.; Krama, T. Interspecific reciprocity explains mobbing behaviour of the breeding chaffinches, Fringilla coelebs. Proc. R. Soc. B Boil. Sci. 2002, 269, 2345–2350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bloomfield, L.; Philmore, L.S.; Weisman, R.; Sturdy, C.B. Note types and coding in paired vocalizations. III: The chick-a-dee call of the Carolina chickadee (Poecile carolinensis). Can. J. Zool. 2005, 83, 820–833. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Magrath, R.D.; Pitcher, B.J.; Gardner, J.L. A mutual understanding? Interspecific responses by birds to each other’s aerial alarm calls. Behav. Ecol. 2007, 18, 944–951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goodale, E.; Kotagama, S.W. Response to conspecific and heterospecific alarm calls in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rainforest. Behav. Ecol. 2008, 19, 887–894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Haff, T.M.; Magrath, R.D. Eavesdropping on the neighbours: Fledglings learn to respond to heterospecific alarm calls. Anim. Behav. 2013, 85, 411–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wheatcroft, D.; Price, T.D. Learning and signal copying facilitate communication among bird species. Proc. R. Soc. B Boil. Sci. 2013, 280, 20123070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Date, E.M.; Lemon, R.E.; Weary, D.M.; Richter, A.K. Species identity by birdsong: Discrete or additive information. Anim. Behav. 1991, 41, 111–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Catchpole, C.K.; Slater, P.J.B. Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Peters, S.S.; Searcy, W.A.; Marler, P. Species song discrimination in choice experiments with territorial male swamp and song sparrows. Anim. Behav. 1980, 28, 393–404. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Soha, J.A.; Marler, P. A species-specific acoustic cue for selective song learning in the white-crowned sparrow. Anim. Behav. 2000, 60, 297–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ford, J.K.B.; Fischer, H.D. Killer whale (Orcinus orca) dialects as an indicator of stocks in British Columbia. Rep. Int. Whal. Commision 1982, 32, 671–679. [Google Scholar]
- Ford, J.K.B.; Fischer, H.D. Group specific dialects of killer whale (Orcinus orca) off Vancouver Island, British Columbia. In Communication and Behaviour of Whales; Payne, R., Ed.; Westview Press: Boulder, CO, USA, 1983; pp. 129–161. [Google Scholar]
- Ford, J.K.B. Vocal traditions among resident killer whales (Orcinus orca) in coastal waters of British Columbia. Can. J. Zool. 1991, 69, 1454–1483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Musser, W.B.; Bowles, A.E.; Grebner, D.M.; Crance, J.L. Differences in acoustic features of vocalizations produced by killer whales cross-socialized with bottlenose dolphins. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2014, 136, 1990–2002. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitehead, H.; Weilgart, L. Click rates from sperm whales. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1990, 87, 1798–1806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gero, S.; Whitehead, H.; Rendell, L. Individual, unit and vocal clan level identity cues in sperm whale codas. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2016, 3, 150372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Charrier, I.; Burlet, A.; Aubin, T. Social vocal communication in captive Pacific walruses Odobenus rosmarus divergens. Mamm. Biol. 2010, 76, 622–627. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kastein, H.B.; Winter, R.; Kumar, A.K.V.; Kandula, S.; Schmidt, S. Perception of individuality in bat vocal communication: Discrimination between, or recognition of, interaction partners? Anim. Cogn. 2013, 16, 945–959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Watwood, S.L.; Tyack, P.L.; Wells, R.S. Whistle sharing in paired male bottlenose dolphins, Tursiops truncatus. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2004, 55, 531–543. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fripp, D.; Owen, C.; Quintana-Rizzo, E.; Shapiro, A.; Buckstaff, K.; Jankowski, K.; Wells, R.; Tyack, P. Bottlenose dolphin (Tursiops truncatus) calves appear to model their signature whistles on the signature whistles of community members. Anim. Cogn. 2004, 8, 17–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fay, R.R.; Wilber, L.A. Hearing in Vertebrates: A Psychophysics Databook. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1989, 86, 2044. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Scherer, K.R. Vocal correlates of emotional arousal and affective disturbance. In Handbook of Social Psychophysiology; Wagner, H., Manstead, A., Eds.; John Wiley, Ltd.: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 1989; pp. 165–197. [Google Scholar]
- Fitch, W.T. Production of vocalizations in mammals. In Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics, 2nd ed.; Brown, K., Ed.; Elsevier: Oxford, UK, 2006; Volume 10, pp. 115–121. [Google Scholar]
- Fitch, W.T. The Evolution of Language; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2010; pp. 315–324. [Google Scholar]
- Brown, S.G. Modern whaling in Britain and the northeast Atlantic Ocean. Mammal Rev. 1976, 6, 25–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gelfand, D.L.; McCracken, G.F. Individual variation in the isolation calls of Mexican free-tailed bat pups (Tadarida brasiliensis mexicana). Anim. Behav. 1986, 34, 1078–1086. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Esser, K.-H.; Schmidt, U. Absolute and relative pitch production in the song of the black-capped chickadee. Evidence for acoustic learning. Ethology 1989, 82, 156–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheney, D.; Seyfarth, R. Vocal recognition in free-ranging vervet monkeys. Anim. Behav. 1980, 28, 362–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cheney, D.L.; Seyfarth, R.M. Assessment of meaning and the detection of unreliable signals by vervet monkeys. Anim. Behav. 1988, 36, 477–486. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Snowdon, C.T.; Cleveland, J. Individual recognition of contact calls by Pygmy marmosets. Anim. Behav. 1980, 28, 717–727. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rendall, D.; Rodman, P.S.; Emond, R.E. Vocal recognition of individuals and kin in free-ranging rhesus monkeys. Anim. Behav. 1996, 51, 1007–1015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rendall, D.; Owren, M.J.; Ryan, M.J. What do animal signals mean? Anim. Behav. 2009, 78, 233–240. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sproul, C.; Palleroni, A.; Hauser, M.D. Cottontop tamarin, Saguinus oedipus, alarm calls contain sufficient information for recognition of individual identity. Anim. Behav. 2006, 72, 1379–1385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sliwa, J.; Duhamel, J.-R.; Pascalis, O.; Wirth, S. Spontaneous voice–face identity matching by rhesus monkeys for familiar conspecifics and humans. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2011, 108, 1735–1740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hare, J.F. Juvenile Richardson’s ground squirrels, Spermophilus richardsonii, discriminate among individual alarm callers. Anim. Behav. 1998, 55, 451–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Blumstein, D.T.; Daniel, J.C. Yellow-bellied marmots discriminate between the alarm calls of individuals and are more responsive to calls from juveniles. Anim. Behav. 2004, 68, 1257–1265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holekamp, K.E.; Boydston, E.E.; Szykman, M.; Graham, I.; Nutt, K.J.; Birch, S.; Piskiel, A.; Singh, M. Vocal recognition in the spotted hyaena and its possible implications regarding the evolution of intelligence. Anim. Behav. 1999, 58, 383–395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McCulloch, S.; Boness, D.J. Mother-pup vocal recognition in the grey seal of Sable Island, Nova Scotia, Canada. J. Zool. 2000, 251, 449–455. [Google Scholar]
- Insley, S.J. Mother–Offspring vocal recognition in northern fur seals is mutual but asymmetrical. Anim. Behav. 2001, 61, 129–137. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Charrier, I.; Mathevon, N.; Jouventin, P. How does a fur seal mother recognize the voice of her pup? An experimental study of Arctocephalus tropicalis. J. Exp. Biol. 2002, 205, 603–612. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Charrier, I.; Mathevon, N.; Jouventin, P. Vocal signature recognition of mothers by fur seal pups. Anim. Behav. 2003, 65, 543–550. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Frommolt, K.-H.; Goltsman, M.E.; Macdonald, D.W. Barking foxes, Alopex lagopus: Field experiments in individual recognition in a territorial mammal. Anim. Behav. 2003, 65, 509–518. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Müller, C.A.; Manser, M.B. Mutual recognition of pups and providers in the cooperatively breeding banded mongoose. Anim. Behav. 2008, 75, 1683–1692. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pitcher, B.J.; Harcourt, R.G.; Charrier, I. The memory remains: Long-term vocal recognition in Australian sea lions. Anim. Cogn. 2010, 13, 771–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- McComb, K.; Moss, C.; Sayialel, S.; Bakerab, L. Unusually extensive networks of vocal recognition in African elephants. Anim. Behav. 2000, 59, 1103–1109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Lemasson, A.; Boutin, A.; Boivin, S.; Blois-Heulin, C.; Hausberger, M. Horse (Equus caballus) whinnies: A source of social information. Anim. Cogn. 2009, 12, 693–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Proops, L.; McComb, K.; Reby, D. Cross-modal individual recognition in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2009, 106, 947–951. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lampe, J.F.; Andre, J. Cross-modal recognition of human individuals in domestic horses (Equus caballus). Anim. Cogn. 2012, 15, 623–630. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Watts, J.M.; Stookey, J.M. Effects of restraint and branding on rates and acoustic parameters of vocalization in beef cattle. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1999, 62, 125–135. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Searby, A.; Jouventin, P. Mother-lamb acoustic recognition in sheep: A frequency coding. Proc. R. Soc. B Boil. Sci. 2003, 270, 1765–1771. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Terrazas, A.; Serafin, N.; Hernández, H.; Nowak, R.; Poindron, P. Early recognition of newborn goat kids by their mother: II. Auditory recognition and evidence of an individual acoustic signature in the neonate. Dev. Psychobiol. 2003, 43, 311–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Serrano, A.; Miller, E.H. How vocal are harp seals (Pagophilus groenlandicus)? A captive study of seasonal and diel patterns. Aquat. Mamm. 2000, 26, 253–259. [Google Scholar]
- Janik, V.M.; Sayigh, L.S. Communication in bottlenose dolphins: 50 years of signature whistle research. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2013, 199, 479–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caldwell, D.K.; Caldwell, M.C. The dolphin observed. Nat. Hist. 1968, 77, 58–65. [Google Scholar]
- Caldwell, M.C.; Caldwell, D.K.; Miller, J.F. Statistical evidence for individual signature whistles in the spotted dolphin (Stenella plagiodon). Cetology 1973, 16, 1–21. [Google Scholar]
- Caldwell, M.C.; Caldwell, D.K. Statistical evidence for individual signature whistles in Pacific whitesided dolphins Lagenorhynchus obliquidens. Cetology 1971, 3, 1–9. [Google Scholar]
- Van Parijs, S.M.; Corkeron, P.J. Boat traffic affects the acoustic behaviour of Pacific humpback dolphins, Sousa chinensis. J. Mar. Ass. UK 2001, 81, 533–538. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sayigh, L.S.; Tyack, P.; Wells, R.S.; Scott, M.D. Signature whistles of free-ranging bottlenose dolphins Tursiops truncatus: Stability and mother-offspring comparisons. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1990, 26, 247–260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Darwin, C. The Expression of Emotions in Man and Animals; John Murray: London, UK, 1872. [Google Scholar]
- Freeberg, T.M.; Lucas, J.R. Information theoretical approaches to chick-a-dee calls of Carolina chickadees (Poecile carolinensis). J. Comp. Psychol. 2012, 126, 68–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krams, I.; Krama, T.; Freeberg, T.M.; Kullberg, C.; Lucas, J.R. Linking social complexity and vocal complexity: A paired perspective. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 2012, 367, e1879–e1891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Blumstein, D.T.; Armitage, K.B. Alarm calling in yellow-bellied marmots: I. The meaning of situationally variable alarm calls. Anim. Behav. 1997, 53, 143–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Krebs, J.R.; Ashcroft, R.; Van Orsdol, K. Song matching in the great tit Parus major L. Anim. Behav. 1981, 29, 918–923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beecher, M.D.; Campbell, S.; Burt, J.M.; Hill, C.E.; Nordby, J. Song-type matching between neighbouring song sparrows. Anim. Behav. 2000, 59, 21–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Knörnschild, M.; Jung, K.; Nagy, M.; Metz, M.; Kalko, E. Bat echolocation calls facilitate social communication. Proc. R. Soc. B Boil. Sci. 2012, 279, 4827–4835. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Barklow, W.E. Amphibious communication with sound in hippos, Hippopotamus amphibius. Anim. Behav. 2004, 68, 1125–1132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koyama, N.; Ichino, S.; Nakamichi, M.; Takahata, Y. Long-term changes in dominance ranks among ring-tailed lemurs at Berenty Reserve, Madagascar. Primates 2005, 46, 225–234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brenowitz, E.A.; Wilczynski, W.; Zakon, H.H. Acoustic communication in spring peepers: Environmental and behavioral aspects. J. Comp. Phys. A 1981, 155, 585–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wilczynski, W.; Brenowitz, E.A. Acoustic cues mediate inter-male spacing in a neotropical frog. Anim. Behav. 1988, 36, 1054–1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brenowitz, E.A. Neighbor call amplitude influences aggressive behavior and inter- male spacing in choruses of the Pacific treefrog (Hyla regilla). Ethology 1989, 83, 69–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerhardt, H.C.; Diekamp, B.; Ptacek, M. Inter-male spacing in choruses of the spring peeper, Pseudacris (Hyla) crucifer. Anim. Behav. 1989, 38, 1012–1024. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- O’Connell-Rodwell, C. Keeping an ‘ear’ to the ground: Seismic communication in elephants. Physiology 2007, 22, 287–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gregory, J.E.; McIntyre, A.K.; Proske, U. Vibration-evoked responses from lamellated corpuscles in the legs of kangaroos. Exp. Brain Res. 1986, 62, 648–653. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Shipley, C.; Stewart, B.S.; Bass, J. Seismic communication in northern elephant seals. In A Marine Mammal Sensory Systems; Thomas, J.A., Kastelein, R.A., Ya, A., Eds.; Plenum: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 553–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tyack, P. Interactions between singing Hawaiian humpback whales and conspecifics nearby. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1981, 8, 105–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Helweg, D.A.; Frankel, A.S.; Mobley, J.R.; Herman, L.M. Humpback whale song: Our current understanding. In A Marine Mammal Sensory Systems; Thomas, J.A., Kastelein, R.A., Supin, A.S., Eds.; Plenum: New York, NY, USA, 1992; pp. 459–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Darling, J.D.; Berube, M. Interactions of singing humpback whales with other males. Mar. Mammal Sci. 2001, 17, 570–584. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Darling, J.D.; Jones, M.E.; Nicklin, C.P. Humpback whale songs: Do they organize males during the breeding season? Behaviour 2006, 143, 1051–1101. [Google Scholar]
- Bartholomew, G.A. Model for the evolution of pinniped polygyny. Evolution 1970, 24, 546–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Parijs, S.M.; Lydersen, C.; Kovacs, K.M. Effects of ice cover on the behavioural patterns of aquatic-mating male bearded seals. Anim. Behav. 2004, 68, 89–96. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Opzeeland, I.; Van Parijs, S.; Bornemann, H.; Frickenhaus, S.; Kindermann, L.; Klinck, H.; Plötz, J.; Boebel, O. Acoustic ecology of Antarctic pinnipeds. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 2010, 414, 267–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wells, K.D. The effects of social interactions on anuran vocal behavior. In The Evolution of the Amphibian Auditory System; Fritszch, B., Wilczynski, W., Ryan, M.J., Hetherington, T., Walkowiak, W., Eds.; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1988; pp. 433–454. [Google Scholar]
- Gerhardt, H.C.; Huber, F.; Simmons, A.M. Acoustic Communication in Insects and Anurans; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Würsig, B.; Clark, C.W. Behavior. In The Bowhead Whale; Bums, J.J., Montague, J.J., Cowles, C.J., Eds.; Special Publication Society: Lawrence, KA, USA, 1990; Volume 2, pp. 157–199. [Google Scholar]
- Edds-Walton, P.L. Acoustic Communication Signals of Mysticete Whales. Bioacoustics 1997, 8, 47–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burnham, R.E.; Duffus, D.A.; Mouy, X. Gray whale (Eschrichtius robustus) call types recorded during migration off the west coast of Vancouver Island. Front. Mar. Sci. 2018, 5, 329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Schulz, T.M.; Whitehead, H.; Gero, S.; Rendell, L. Overlapping and matching of codas in vocal interactions between sperm whales: Insights into communication function. Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 1977–1988. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Smolker, R.A.; Mann, J.; Smuts, B.B. Use of signature whistles during separations and reunions by wild bottlenose dolphin mothers and infants. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1993, 33, 393–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Melendez, K.V.; Feng, A.S. Communication calls of little brown bats display individual-specific characteristics. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2010, 128, 919–923. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Doerrie, M.; Schmidt, S.; Suba, M.; Sripathi, K. Contact calls of the bat, Megaderma lyra: A comparison between an Indian and a Sri Lankan Population. Zoology 2001, 104 (Suppl. SIV), 5. [Google Scholar]
- Schmidt, S. Beyond echolocation: Emotional acoustic communication in bats. In From Sounds in Non-Human Mammals to Speech and Music in Man; Altenmuller, E., Schmidt, S., Zimmermann, E., Eds.; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2013; pp. 92–104. [Google Scholar]
- Rees, A.F. Some notes on elephants and their feeding habits. Tanganyika Notes Rec. 1963, 61, 205–208. [Google Scholar]
- Poole, J.H.; Payne, K.; Langbauer, W.R., Jr.; Moss, C.J. The social contexts of some very low-frequency calls of African elephants. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1988, 22, 385–392. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Poole, J.H. Behavioral contexts of elephant acoustic communication. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C.J., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 125–161. [Google Scholar]
- Poole, J.H.; Granli, P. Gestures, and behaviour of African elephants. In The Amboseli Elephants: A Long-Term Perspective on a Long-Lived Mammal; Moss, C.J., Croze, H., Lee, P.C., Eds.; The University of Chicago: Chicago, IL, USA, 2011; pp. 109–124. [Google Scholar]
- Gautier, J.-P.; Gautier-Hion, A. Communication in Old World monkeys. In How Animals Communicate; Seboek, T.A., Ed.; Indiana University Press: Bloomington, IN, USA, 1977; pp. 890–964. [Google Scholar]
- Boinski, S.; Campbell, A.F. Use of Trill Vocalizations to Coordinate Troop Movement among White-Faced Capuchins: A Second Field Test. Nature 1995, 25, 89–104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ingmanson, E.J. Tool-using behavior in wild Pan paniscus: Social and ecological considerations. In Reaching into Thought: The Minds of Great Apes; Russon, A.E., Bard, K.A., Parker, S.T., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1996; pp. 190–210. [Google Scholar]
- Burnham, R.E.; Duffus, D.A. The use of passive acoustic monitoring as a census tool of gray whale (Eschrichtius robustus) migration. Ocean Coast. Manag. 2019, 188, 105070. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Arnold, G.; Boundy, C.; Morgan, P.; Bartle, G. The roles of sight and hearing in the lamb in the location and discrimination between ewes. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 1975, 1, 167–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lenhardt, M.L. Vocal contour cues in maternal recognition of goat kids. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 1977, 3, 211–219. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Walser, E.S.; Walters, E.; Hague, P. Vocal Communication Between Ewes and Their Own and Alien Lambs. Behaviour 1982, 81, 140–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jouventin, P.; Aubin, T.; Lengagne, T. Finding a parent in a king penguin colony: The acoustic system of individual recognition. Anim. Behav. 1999, 57, 1175–1183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Aubin, T.; Jouventin, P.; Hildebrand, C. Penguins use the two-voice system to recognise each other. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 2000, 267, 1081–1087. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lingle, S.; Riede, T. Deer mothers are sensitive to infant distress vocalizations of diverse mammalian species. Am. Nat. 2014, 184, 510–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DuMond, F.V. The squirrel monkey in a seminatural environment. In The Squirrel Monkey; Rosenbloom, L.A., Cooper, R.W., Eds.; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1968; pp. 87–145. [Google Scholar]
- Hohmann, G.M.; Herzog, M.O. Vocal communication in lion-tailed Macaques (Macaca silenus). Folia Primatol. 1985, 4, 148–178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Masataka, N. The Onset of Language; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2003. [Google Scholar]
- Burt, J.M.; Campbell, S.E.; Beecher, M.D. Song type matching as threat: A test using interactive playback. Anim. Behav. 2001, 62, 1163–1170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Owings, D.H.; Hennessy, D.F. The importance of variation in sciurid visual and vocal communication. In The Biology of Ground Dwelling Squirrels; Murie, J.O., Michener, G., Eds.; University of Nebraska Press: Omaha, NE, USA, 1984; pp. 171–200. [Google Scholar]
- Owren, M.J.; Rendall, D. An affect-conditioning model of nonhuman primate vocalizations. In Perspectives in Ethology, Volume 12: Communication; Owing, D.H., Beecher, M.D., Thompson, N.S., Eds.; Plenum: New York, NY, USA, 1997; pp. 299–346. [Google Scholar]
- Owren, M.J.; Rendall, D. Sound on the rebound: Returning form and function to the forefront in understanding non-human primate vocal signaling. Evol. Anthrop. 2001, 10, 58–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Owings, D.H.; Morton, E.S. Animal Vocal Communication; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1998. [Google Scholar]
- Seyfarth, R.M.; Cheney, D.L. Animal cognition: Chimpanzee alarm calls depend on what others know. Curr. Biol. 2012, 22, R51–R52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Meise, K.; Keller, C.; Cowlishaw, G.; Fischer, J. Sources of acoustic variation: Implications for production specificity and call categorization in chacma baboon (Papio ursinus) grunts. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 129, 1631–1641. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ouattara, K.; Lemasson, A.; Zuberbühler, K. Campbell’s monkeys use affixation to alter call meaning. PLoS ONE 2009, 4, e7808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Schlenker, P.; Chemla, E.; Arnold, K.; Lemasson, A.; Ouattara, K.; Kennan, S.; Stephan, C.; Ryder, R.; Zuberbühler, K. Monkey semantics: Two ‘dialects’ of Campbell’s monkey alarm calls. Linguist 2014, 37, 439–501. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hauser, M.D.; Marler, P. Food-associated calls in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta). Socioecological factors. Behav. Ecol. 1993, 4, 194–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Evans, C.S.; Evans, L. Chicken food calls are functionally referential. Anim. Behav. 1999, 58, 307–319. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bugnyar, T.; Kijne, M.; Kotrschal, K. Food calling in ravens: Are yells referential signals? Anim. Behav. 2001, 61, 949–958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Di Bitetti, M. Food-associated calls of tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella nigritus) are functionally referential signals. Behaviour 2003, 140, 565–592. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Slocombe, K.E.; Zuberbühler, K. Functionally referential communication in a chimpanzee. Curr. Biol. 2005, 15, 1779–1784. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kitzmann, C.D.; Caine, N.G. Marmoset (Callithrix geoffroyi) food-associated calls are functionally referential. Ethology 2009, 115, 439–448. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Griffin, D.R. Listening in the Dark: The Acoustic Orientation of Bats and Men; Cornell University Press: New York, NY, USA, 1958. [Google Scholar]
- Au, W.W.L.; Ford, J.K.B.; Horne, J.K.; Allman, K.A.N. Echolocation signals of free-ranging killer whales (Orcinus orca) and modeling of foraging for chinook salmon (Oncorhynchus tshawytscha). J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2004, 115, 901–909. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DeRuiter, S.L.; Bahr, A.; Blanchet, M.-A.; Hansen, S.F.; Kristensen, J.H.; Madsen, P.T.; Tyack, P.L.; Wahlberg, M. Acoustic behaviour of echolocating porpoises during prey capture. J. Exp. Biol. 2009, 212, 3100–3107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ford, J.K.B.; Ellis, G.M. Transients: Mammal-Hunting Killer Whales of British Columbia, Washington, and Southeastern Alaska; UBC Press: Vancouver, BC, Canada, 1999; p. 179. [Google Scholar]
- Thompson, P.O.; Findley, L.T.; Vidal, O.; Cummings, W.C. Underwater sounds of blue whales, Balaenoptera musculus, in the Gulf of California, Mexico. Mar. Mammal Sci. 1996, 12, 288–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gong, Z.; Jain, A.D.; Tran, D.; Yi, D.H.; Wu, F.; Zorn, A.; Ratilal, P.; Makris, N.C. Ecosystem scale acoustic sensing reveals humpback whale behavior synchronous with herring spawning processes and re-evaluation finds no effect of sonar on humpback song occurrence in the Gulf of Maine in fall 2006. PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e104733. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Parks, S.E.; Cusano, D.; Stimpert, A.K.; Weinrich, M.T.; Freilaender, A.S.; Wiley, D.N. Evidence for acoustic communication among bottom foraging humpback whales. Sci. Rep. 2014, 4, 7508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Ellis, S.; Franks, D.W.; Nattrass, S.; Cant, M.A.; Weiss, M.N.; Giles, D.; Balcomb, K.C.; Croft, D.P. Mortality risk and social network position in resident killer whales: Sex differences and the importance of resource abundance. Proc. R. Soc. B Boil. Sci. 2017, 284, 20171313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Matkin, C.O.; Moore, M.J.; Gulland, F.M.D. Review of Recent Research on Southern Resident Killer Whales (SRKW) to Detect Evidence of Poor Body Condition in the Population; Independent Science Panel Report to the SeaDoc Society; Wildlife Health Center: Eastsound, WA, USA, 2017; 3p. [Google Scholar]
- Clay, Z.; Zuberbühler, K. Bonobos Extract Meaning from Call Sequences. PLoS ONE 2011, 6, e18786. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Elowson, A.M.; Tannenbaum, P.L.; Snowdon, C.T. Food-associated calls correlate with food preferences in cotton-top tamarins. Anim. Behav. 1991, 42, 931–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Benz, J.J.; Leger, D.W.; French, J.A. Relation between food preference and food-elicited vocalizations in golden lion tamarins (Leontopithecus rosalia). J. Comp. Psychol. 1992, 106, 142–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Benz, J.J. Food-elicited vocalizations in golden lion tamarins: Design features for representational communication. Anim. Behav. 1993, 45, 443–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Boinski, S.; Campbell, A. The huh vocalization of white-faced capuchins: A spacing call disguised as a food call? Ethology 1995, 102, 826–840. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Roush, R.S.; Snowdon, C.T. Quality, Quantity, Distribution and Audience Effects on Food Calling in Cotton-Top Tamarins. Ethology 2000, 106, 673–690. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gros-Louis, J. Responses of White-Faced Capuchins (Cebus capucinus) to Naturalistic and Experimentally Presented Food-Associated Calls. J. Comp. Psychol. 2004, 118, 396–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Chapman, C.A.; Lefebvre, L. Manipulating foraging group size: Spider monkey food calls at fruiting trees. Anim. Behav. 1990, 39, 891–896. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wladichuk, J.L.; Megill, W.M.; Blondel, P. Passive Biosonar: Ambient Acoustics in Nearshore. In Proceedings of the 4th International Conference on Underwater Acoustic Measurements, Kos, Greece, 20–24 June 2010; pp. 1–8. [Google Scholar]
- Allen, N.A. An Investigation of the Roles of Geomagnetic and Acoustic Cues in Whale Navigation and Orientation. Ph.D. Thesis, Massachusetts Institute of Technology and Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution, Falmouth, MA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Norris, K.S. Whales, Dolphins, and Porpoises; University of California Press: Berkley, CA, USA; Los Angeles, CA, USA, 1966. [Google Scholar]
- Norris, K.S. The echolocation of marine mammals. In The Biology of Marine Mammals; Andersen, H.J., Ed.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1969. [Google Scholar]
- Payne, R.; Webb, D. Orientation by means of long range acoustic signaling in baleen whales. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1971, 188, 110–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Thompson, T.J.; Winn, H.E.; Perkins, P.J. Mysticete sounds. In Behavior of Marine Animals; Winn, H.E., Olla, B.L., Eds.; Perseus: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1979; pp. 403–431. [Google Scholar]
- Ellison, W.T.; Clark, C.W.; Bishop, G.C. Potential use of surface reverberation by bowhead whales, Balaena mysticetus, in under-ice navigation: Preliminary considerations. Rep. Int. Whal. Comm. 1987, 37, 329–332. [Google Scholar]
- Clark, C.W.; Ellison, W.T. Potential use of low-frequency sounds by baleen whales for probing the environment: Evidence from models and empirical measurements. In Echolocation in Bats and Dolphins; Thomas, J.A., Moss, C.F., Vater, M., Eds.; University of Chicago Press: Chicago, IL, USA, 2004; p. 604. [Google Scholar]
- Oleson, E.M.; Calambokidis, J.; Burgess, W.C.; McDonald, M.A.; LeDuc, C.A.; Hildebrand, J.A. Behavioral context of call production by eastern North Pacific blue whales. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 2007, 330, 269–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Oleson, E.M.; Wiggins, S.M.; Hildebrand, J.A. Temporal separation of blue whale call types on a southern California feeding ground. Anim. Behav. 2007, 74, 881–894. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wiley, R.H.; Richards, D.G. Physical constraints on acoustic communication in the atmosphere: Implications for the evolution of animal vocalisations. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1978, 3, 69–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McDonald, M.A.; Hildebrand, J.; Webb, S. Blue and fin whales observed on a seafloor array in the Northeast Pacific. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 1995, 98, 712–721. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, C.W. The acoustic repertoire of the Southern right whale, a quantitative analysis. Anim. Behav. 1982, 30, 1060–1071. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Širović, A.; Williams, L.N.; Kerosky, S.M.; Wiggins, S.M.; Hildebrand, J.A. Temporal separation of two fin whale call types across the eastern North Pacific. Mar. Biol. 2012, 160, 47–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Au, W.W.L.; Pack, A.A.; Lammers, M.O.; Herman, L.M.; Deakos, M.H.; Andrews, K. Acoustic properties of humpback whale songs. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2006, 120, 1103–1110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Parsons, E.C.M.; Wright, A.J.; Gore, M.A. The nature of humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) song. J. Mar. Anim. Ecol. 2008, 1, 21–30. [Google Scholar]
- Wilczynski, W.; Lynch, K.S.; O’Bryant, E.L. Current research in amphibians: Studies integrating endocrinology, behavior, and neurobiology. Horm. Behav. 2005, 48, 440–450. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Cheng, M.-F.; Peng, J.P. Reciprocal talk between the auditory thalamus and hypothalamus: An antidromic study. Neuro Rep. 1997, 8, 653–658. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Veeckman, J.; Ödberg, F. Preliminary studies on the behavioural detection of oestrus in Belgian “warm-blood” mares with acoustic and tactile stimuli. Appl. Anim. Ethol. 1978, 4, 108–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pearce, G.; Hughes, P. An investigation of the roles of boar-component stimuli in the expression of proceptivity in the female pig. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1987, 18, 287–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Leary, C.J. Hormones and acoustic communication in anuran amphibians. Integr. Comp. Biol. 2009, 49, 452–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Langbauer Jr, W.R.; Payne, K.; Charif, R.; Rapport, L.; Osborne, F. African elephants respond to distant playback of low-frequency conspecific calls. J. Exp. Biol. 1991, 157, 35–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Payne, K.B.; Thompson, M.; Kramer, L. Elephant calling patterns as indicators of group size and composition: The basis for an acoustic monitoring system. Afr. J. Ecol. 2003, 41, 99–107. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Garstang, M. Long-distance, low-frequency elephant communication. J. Comp. Physiol. A 2004, 190, 791–805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Slater, P.J.B.; Mann, N.I. Why do the females of many bird species sing in the tropics? J. Avian Biol. 2004, 35, 289–294. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brumm, H.; Zollinger, S.A. Avian Vocal Production in Noise. In Animal Communication and Noise; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2013; pp. 187–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zelick, R.; Mann, D.; Popper, A.N. Acoustic communication in fishes and frogs. In Comparative Hearing: Fish and Amphibians; Popper, A.N., Fay, R.R., Eds.; Springer Science+Business Media, LLC: New York, NY, USA, 1999; pp. 363–411. [Google Scholar]
- Todt, D.; Naguib, M. Vocal interactions in birds: The use of song as a model in communication. Adv. Study Behav. 2000, 247–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hall, M.L. Chapter 3 A Review of Vocal Duetting in Birds. Adv. Study Behav. 2009, 67–121. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Brush, J.S.; Narins, P.M. Chorus dynamics of a neotropical amphibian assemblage: Comparison of computer simulation and natural behaviour. Anim. Behav. 1989, 37, 33–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gerhardt, H.C.; Roberts, J.D.; Bee, M.A.; Schwartz, J.J. Call matching in the quacking frog (Crinia georgiana). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2000, 48, 243–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wells, K.D.; Schwartz, J.J. The behavioral ecology of anuran communication. In Hearing and Sound Communication in Amphibians; Narins, P.M., Feng, A.S., Fay, R.R., Popper, A.N., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2007; pp. 44–86. [Google Scholar]
- Winn, H.E.; Thompson, T.L.; Cummings, W.C.; Hain, J.; Hurlnail, L.; Hays, H.; Steiner, W.W. Song of the Humpback Whale Population Comparisons. Behav. Ecol. Soc. 1981, 8, 41–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ackers, S.H.; Slobodchikoff, C.N. Communication of stimulus size and shape in alarm calls of Gunnison’s prairie dogs, Cynomys gunnisoni. Ethology 1999, 105, 149–162. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Eriksson, D.; Wallin, L. Male bird song attracts females? A field experiment. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1986, 19, 297–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Johnson, L.S.; Searcy, W.A. Female Attraction to Male Song in House Wrens (Troglodytes aedon). Behaviour 1996, 133, 357–366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nowicki, S.; Searcy, W.A. Song function and the evolution of female preferences: Why birds sing, why brains matter. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2004, 1016, 704–723. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Davidson, S.M.; Wilkinson, G.S. Function of male song in the greater white-lined bat, Saccopteryx bilineata. Anim. Behav. 2004, 67, 883–891. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cowlishaw, G. Song function in gibbons. Behaviour 1992, 121, 131–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Payne, R.S.; McVay, S. Songs of humpback whales. Science 1971, 173, 585–597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Payne, K.; Tyack, P.; Payne, R.S. Progressive changes in the song of humpback whales (Megaptera novaeangliae): A detailed analysis of two seasons in Hawaii. In Communication and Behaviour of Whales; Payne, R., Ed.; Westview Press: Boulder, CO, USA, 1983; pp. 9–57. [Google Scholar]
- Payne, K.; Payne, R. Large scale changes over 19 years in songs of humpback whales in Bermuda. Z. Tierpsychol. 1985, 68, 89–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tyack, P.L.; Sayigh, L.S. Vocal learning in cetaceans. In Social Influences on Vocal Development; Snowdon, C.T., Hausberger, M., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1997; pp. 208–233. [Google Scholar]
- Tyack, P.L.; Clark, C.W. Communication and acoustical behavior in dolphins and whales. In Hearing by Whales and Dolphins; Springer Handbook of Auditory Research Springer-Verlag; Au, W.W.L., Popper, A.N., Fay, R.R., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2000; pp. 156–224. [Google Scholar]
- Allen, J.; Weinrich, M.; Hoppitt, W.; Rendell, L. Network-based diffusion analysis reveals cultural transmission of lobtail feeding in humpback whales. Science 2013, 340, 485–488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Janik, V.M. Cetacean vocal learning and communication. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2014, 28, 60–65. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bowling, D.L.; Fitch, W.T. Do animal communication systems have phonemes? Trends Cogn. Sci. 2015, 19, 555–557. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Laland, K.; Janik, V. The animal cultures debate. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2006, 21, 542–547. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kean, D.; Tiddi, B.; Fahy, M.; Heistermann, M.; Schino, G.; Wheeler, B.C. Feeling anxious? The mechanisms of vocal deception in tufted capuchin monkeys. Anim. Behav. 2017, 130, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hammerschmidt, K.; Fischer, J. Constraints in primate vocal production. In The Evolution of Communicative Creativity: Complexity, Creativity, and Adaptability in Human and Animal Communication; Griebel, U., Oller, K., Eds.; MIT Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 2008; pp. 93–119. [Google Scholar]
- Hutchinson, G.E. Concluding remarks. In Cold Spring Harb. Symp. Quant. Biol. 1957, 22, 415–427. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hofmann, V.; Sanguinetti-Scheck, J.I.; Künzel, S.; Geurten, B.; Gómez-Sena, L.; Engelmann, J. Sensory flow shaped by active sensing: Sensorimotor strategies in electric fish. J. Exp. Biol. 2013, 216, 2487–2500. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Richardson, W.J.; Greene, C.R., Jr.; Malme, C.I.; Thomson, D. Marine Mammals and Noise; Academic Press: San Diego, CA, USA, 1995. [Google Scholar]
- Brakes, P.; Dall, S.R.X. Marine mammal behaviour: A review of conservation implication. Front. Mar. Sci. 2016, 3, 87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Whitehead, H.; Rendell, L.; Osborne, R.W.; Würsig, B. Culture and conservation of non-humans with reference to whales and dolphins: Review and new directions. Biol. Conserv. 2004, 120, 427–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Whitehead, H. Conserving and managing animals that learn socially and share cultures. Learn. Behav. 2010, 38, 329–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Janik, V.M.; Slater, P.J. The different roles of social learning in vocal communication. Anim. Behav. 2000, 60, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- McDonald, M.; Hildebrand, J.; Mesnick, S. Worldwide decline in tonal frequencies of blue whale songs. Endanger. Species Res. 2009, 9, 13–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Weary, D.M. How great tits use song-note and whole-song features to categorize. Auk 1991, 108, 187–190. [Google Scholar]
- Nelson, D.A. Auditory perception in birds. In Acoustic Communication in Birds; Kroodsma, D.E., Miller, E.H., Eds.; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1982; Volume 1, pp. 95–129. [Google Scholar]
- Krams, I. Communication in crested tits and the risk of predation. Anim. Behav. 2001, 61, 1065–1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Erbe, C.; Reichmuth, C.; Cunningham, K.; Lucke, K.; Dooling, R. Communication masking in marine mammals: A review and research strategy. Mar. Pollut. Bull. 2016, 103, 15–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Otter, K.A.; Ratcliffe, L.; Njegovan, M.; Fotheringham, J. Importance of frequency and temporal song matching in black-capped chickadees: Evidence from interactive playback. Ethology 2002, 108, 181–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Phillmore, L.S.; Sturdy, C.B.; Turyk, M.; Weisman, R.G. Discrimination of individual vocalizations by black-capped chickadees (Poecile atricapillus). Anim. Learn. Behav. 2002, 30, 43–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Phillmore, L.S.; Sturdy, C.B.; Weisman, R.G. Does reduced social contact affect discrimination of distance cues and in-dividual vocalizations? Anim. Behav. 2003, 64, 911–922. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Rutz, C.; Loretto, M.-C.; Bates, A.E.; Davidson, S.C.; Duarte, C.M.; Jetz, W. COVID-10 lockdown allows researchers to quantify the effects of human activity on wildlife. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2020, 4, 1156–1159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bates, A.E.; Primack, R.B.; Biggar, B.S.; Bird, T.J.; Clinton, M.E.; Command, R.J.; Richards, C.; Shellard, M.; Geraldi, N.R.; Vergara, V.; et al. Global COVID-19 lockdown highlights humans as both threats and custodians of the environment. Biol. Conserv. 2021, 263, 109175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Perkins, S.E.; Shilling, F.; Collinson, W. Anthropause opportunities: Experimental perturbation of road traffic and the potential effects of wildlife. Front. Ecol. Evol. 2022, 10, 833129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Derryberry, E.P.; Phillips, J.N.; Derryberry, G.E.; Blum, M.J.; Luther, D. Singing in a silent spring: Birds respond to a half-century soundscape reversion during the COVID-19 shutdown. Science 2020, 370, 575–579. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Krause, B.; Farina, A. Using ecoacoustic methods to survey the impacts of climate change on biodiversity. Biol. Conserv. 2016, 195, 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lengagne, T.; Slater, P.J.B. The effects of rain on acoustic communication: Tawny owls have good reason for calling less in wet weather. Proc. R. Soc. B Boil. Sci. 2002, 269, 2121–2125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shannon, C.E.; Weaver, W. The Mathematical Theory of Communication; University of Illinois Press: Urbana, IL, USA, 1949; 117p. [Google Scholar]
- Laiolo, P. Characterizing the spatial structure of song-bird cultures. Ecol. Appl. 2008, 18, 1774–1780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Laiolo, P.; Tella, J.L. Landscape bioacoustics allow detection of the effects of habitat patchiness on population structure. Ecology 2006, 87, 1203–1214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Botero, C.A.; Boogert, N.J.; Vehrencamp, S.L.; Lovette, I.J. Climatic patterns predict the elaboration of song displays in mockingbirds. Curr. Biol. 2009, 19, 1151–1155. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Brewer, P.; Hester, K. Ocean acidification and the increasing transparency of the ocean to low-frequency sound. Oceanography 2009, 22, 86–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Derryberry, E.P. Ecology shapes birdsong evolution: Variation in morphology and habitat explains variation in white-crowned sparrow song. Am. Nat. 2009, 174, 24–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Burnham, R. Acoustic disturbance, risk estimates and mitigation strategies: An animal-centric approach. In Proceedings of the Effects of Noise on Aquatic Life Conference, Berlin, Germany, 10–15 July 2022; Popper, A., Hawkins, A., Thomsen, F., Eds.; January 2023. [Google Scholar]
- Burnham, R.; Vagle, S. Potential interference of communication and echolocation of southern resident killer whales resulting from soundscape modification with implications on foraging behaviours. In Proceedings of the Effects of Noise on Aquatic Life Conference, Berlin, Germany, 10–15 July 2022; Popper, A., Hawkins, A., Thomsen, F., Eds.; January 2023. [Google Scholar]
- Cantor, M.; Whitehead, H. The interplay between social networks and culture: Theoretically and among whales and dolphins. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2013, 368, 20120340. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Clark, C.; Ellison, W.; Southall, B.; Hatch, L.; Van Parijs, S.; Frankel, A.; Ponirakis, D. Acoustic masking in marine ecosystems: Intuitions, analysis, and implication. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 2009, 395, 201–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Burnham, R.E. Whale geography: Acoustics, biogeography, and whales. Prog. Phys. Geog. 2017, 41, 676–685. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freeberg, T.M.; Dunbar, R.I.M.; Ord, T.J. Social complexity as a proximate and ultimate factor in communicative complexity. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 2012, 367, 1785–1801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Bejder, L.; Samuels, A.; Whitehead, H.; Finn, H.; Allen, S. Impact assessment research: Use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in describing wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 2009, 395, 177–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holt, M.M.; Noren, D.P.; Emmons, C.K. Effects of noise levels and call types on the source levels of killer whale calls. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 2011, 130, 3100–3106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thomsen, F.; McCully, S.R.; Weiss, L.R.; Wood, D.T.; Warr, K.J.; Barry, J.; Law, R.J. Cetacean stock assessment in relation to exploration and production industry activity and other human pressures: Review and data needs. Aquat. Mamm. 2011, 37, 1–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Veirs, S.; Veirs, V.; Wood, J. Ship noise in an urban estuary extends to frequencies used for echolocation by endangered killer whales. PeerJ PrePrints 2016, 4, e1657. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Farina, A.; Gage, S.H. Ecoacoustics: A New Science. In Ecoacoustics: The Ecological Role of Sounds; Farino, A., Gage, S.H., Eds.; Wiley and Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2023 by the author. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Burnham, R. Animal Calling Behaviours and What This Can Tell Us about the Effects of Changing Soundscapes. Acoustics 2023, 5, 631-652. https://doi.org/10.3390/acoustics5030039
Burnham R. Animal Calling Behaviours and What This Can Tell Us about the Effects of Changing Soundscapes. Acoustics. 2023; 5(3):631-652. https://doi.org/10.3390/acoustics5030039
Chicago/Turabian StyleBurnham, Rianna. 2023. "Animal Calling Behaviours and What This Can Tell Us about the Effects of Changing Soundscapes" Acoustics 5, no. 3: 631-652. https://doi.org/10.3390/acoustics5030039
APA StyleBurnham, R. (2023). Animal Calling Behaviours and What This Can Tell Us about the Effects of Changing Soundscapes. Acoustics, 5(3), 631-652. https://doi.org/10.3390/acoustics5030039