Variable Message Signs in Traffic Management: A Systematic Review of User Behavior and Future Innovations
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Research Method
3. Results
4. Discussion
4.1. VMS Influence on User Behavior
- i.
- Message Content and Characteristics
- ii.
- Complementary Sources
- iii.
- Demographics
- iv.
- VMS Location
- v.
- Reliance on VMS
4.2. Display of Non-Critical Information on VMS
4.3. New Concept: Virtual Dynamic Message Signs and Vehicle-to-Everything
4.4. Research Gaps and Future Directions
- i.
- Advanced Integration with V2X Technologies
- ii.
- Empirical Validation in Complex, Real-World Scenarios
- iii.
- Impact of Non-Critical Information in Modern Traffic Systems
- iv.
- Enhancing User Engagement Through Personalization and Interactivity
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Data Availability Statement
Conflicts of Interest
Appendix A
Section and Topic | Item # | Checklist Item | Location Where Item Is Reported |
---|---|---|---|
Title | |||
Title | 1 | Identify the report as a systematic review. | 1 |
Abstract | |||
Abstract | 2 | See the PRISMA 2020 for Abstracts checklist. | 1 |
Introduction | |||
Rationale | 3 | Describe the rationale for the review in the context of existing knowledge. | 1–2 |
Objectives | 4 | Provide an explicit statement of the objective(s) or question(s) the review addresses. | 1–2 |
Methods | |||
Eligibility criteria | 5 | Specify the inclusion and exclusion criteria for the review and how studies were grouped for the syntheses. | 2–4 |
Information sources | 6 | Specify all databases, registers, websites, organisations, reference lists and other sources searched or consulted to identify studies. Specify the date when each source was last searched or consulted. | 2–4 |
Search strategy | 7 | Present the full search strategies for all databases, registers and websites, including any filters and limits used. | 2–4 |
Selection process | 8 | Specify the methods used to decide whether a study met the inclusion criteria of the review, including how many reviewers screened each record and each report retrieved, whether they worked independently, and if applicable, details of automation tools used in the process. | 2–4 |
Data collection process | 9 | Specify the methods used to collect data from reports, including how many reviewers collected data from each report, whether they worked independently, any processes for obtaining or confirming data from study investigators, and if applicable, details of automation tools used in the process. | 2–4 |
Data items | 10a | List and define all outcomes for which data were sought. Specify whether all results that were compatible with each outcome domain in each study were sought (e.g., for all measures, time points, analyses), and if not, the methods used to decide which results to collect. | 2–4 |
10b | List and define all other variables for which data were sought (e.g., participant and intervention characteristics, funding sources). Describe any assumptions made about any missing or unclear information. | 2–4 | |
Study risk of bias assessment | 11 | Specify the methods used to assess risk of bias in the included studies, including details of the tool(s) used, how many reviewers assessed each study and whether they worked independently, and if applicable, details of automation tools used in the process. | 2–4 |
Effect measures | 12 | Specify for each outcome the effect measure(s) (e.g., risk ratio, mean difference) used in the synthesis or presentation of results. | 2–4 |
Synthesis methods | 13a | Describe the processes used to decide which studies were eligible for each synthesis (e.g., tabulating the study intervention characteristics and comparing against the planned groups for each synthesis (item #5)). | 2–4 |
13b | Describe any methods required to prepare the data for presentation or synthesis, such as handling of missing summary statistics, or data conversions. | 2–4 | |
13c | Describe any methods used to tabulate or visually display results of individual studies and syntheses. | 2–4 | |
13d | Describe any methods used to synthesize results and provide a rationale for the choice(s). If meta-analysis was performed, describe the model(s), method(s) to identify the presence and extent of statistical heterogeneity, and software package(s) used. | 2–4 | |
13e | Describe any methods used to explore possible causes of heterogeneity among study results (e.g., subgroup analysis, meta-regression). | 2–4 | |
13f | Describe any sensitivity analyses conducted to assess robustness of the synthesized results. | 2–4 | |
Reporting bias assessment | 14 | Describe any methods used to assess risk of bias due to missing results in a synthesis (arising from reporting biases). | 2–4 |
Certainty assessment | 15 | Describe any methods used to assess certainty (or confidence) in the body of evidence for an outcome. | 2–4 |
Results | |||
Study selection | 16a | Describe the results of the search and selection process, from the number of records identified in the search to the number of studies included in the review, ideally using a flow diagram. | 5–11 |
16b | Cite studies that might appear to meet the inclusion criteria, but which were excluded, and explain why they were excluded. | 5–11 | |
Study characteristics | 17 | Cite each included study and present its characteristics. | 5–11 |
Risk of bias in studies | 18 | Present assessments of risk of bias for each included study. | 5–11 |
Results of individual studies | 19 | For all outcomes, present, for each study: (a) summary statistics for each group (where appropriate) and (b) an effect estimate and its precision (e.g., confidence/credible interval), ideally using structured tables or plots. | 5–11 |
Results of syntheses | 20a | For each synthesis, briefly summarise the characteristics and risk of bias among contributing studies. | 5–11 |
20b | Present results of all statistical syntheses conducted. If meta-analysis was done, present for each the summary estimate and its precision (e.g., confidence/credible interval) and measures of statistical heterogeneity. If comparing groups, describe the direction of the effect. | 5–11 | |
20c | Present results of all investigations of possible causes of heterogeneity among study results. | 5–11 | |
20d | Present results of all sensitivity analyses conducted to assess the robustness of the synthesized results. | 5–11 | |
Reporting biases | 21 | Present assessments of risk of bias due to missing results (arising from reporting biases) for each synthesis assessed. | 5–11 |
Certainty of evidence | 22 | Present assessments of certainty (or confidence) in the body of evidence for each outcome assessed. | 5–11 |
Discussion | |||
Discussion | 23a | Provide a general interpretation of the results in the context of other evidence. | 11–15 |
23b | Discuss any limitations of the evidence included in the review. | 11–15 | |
23c | Discuss any limitations of the review processes used. | 11–15 | |
23d | Discuss implications of the results for practice, policy, and future research. | 11–15 | |
Other information | |||
Registration and protocol | 24a | Provide registration information for the review, including register name and registration number, or state that the review was not registered. | Review not registered. |
24b | Indicate where the review protocol can be accessed, or state that a protocol was not prepared. | Protocol not prepared. | |
24c | Describe and explain any amendments to information provided at registration or in the protocol. | Not applicable. | |
Support | 25 | Describe sources of financial or non-financial support for the review, and the role of the funders or sponsors in the review. | 16 |
Competing interests | 26 | Declare any competing interests of review authors. | 16 |
Availability of data, code and other materials | 27 | Report which of the following are publicly available and where they can be found: template data collection forms; data extracted from included studies; data used for all analyses; analytic code; or any other materials used in the review. | 16 |
References
- Leitner, G.; Hitz, M.; Melcher, R. The Role of Usability in the Design and Evaluation of Dynamic Traffic Displays. In Lecture Notes in Computer Science; Holzinger, A., Ed.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Khoo, H.L.; Ong, G.P. Evaluating perceived quality of traffic information system using structural equation modeling. KSCE J. Civ. Eng. 2013, 17, 837–849. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, W.; Ma, Z.; Yang, K.; Huang, H.; Monsuur, F.; Lee, J. Impacts of variable message signs on en-route route choice behavior. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 2020, 139, 335–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Spyropoulou, I.; Antoniou, C. Determinants of driver response to variable message sign information in Athens. IET Intell. Transp. Syst. 2015, 9, 453–466. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ma, Z.; Shao, C.; Song, Y.; Chen, J. Driver response to information provided by variable message signs in Beijing. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2014, 26, 199–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ma, Z.; Luo, M.; Chien, S.I.J.; Hu, D.; Zhao, X. Analyzing drivers’ perceived service quality of variable message signs (VMS). PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0239394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xu, C.; Wang, W.; Chen, J.; Wang, W.; Yang, C.; Li, Z. Analyzing travelers’ intention to accept travel information: Structural equation modeling. Transp. Res. Rec. 2010, 2156, 93–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Page, M.J.; McKenzie, J.E.; Bossuyt, P.M.; Boutron, I.; Hoffmann, T.C.; Mulrow, C.D.; Shamseer, L.; Tetzlaff, J.M.; Akl, E.A.; Brennan, S.E.; et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: An updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moher, D.; Liberati, A.; Tetzlaff, J.; Altman, D.G.; The PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. BMJ 2009, 339, b2535. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ahn, E.; Kang, H. Introduction to systematic review and meta-analysis. Korean J. Anesthesiol. 2018, 71, 103–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Munn, Z.; Peters, M.D.; Stern, C.; Tufanaru, C.; McArthur, A.; Aromataris, E. Systematic review or scoping review? Guidance for authors when choosing between a systematic or scoping review approach. BMC Med. Res. Methodol. 2018, 18, 1–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jahan, N.; Naveed, S.; Zeshan, M.; Tahir, M.A. How to Conduct a Systematic Review: A Narrative Literature Review. Cureus 2016, 8, e864. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Haddaway, N.R.; Collins, A.M.; Coughlin, D.; Kirk, S. The role of google scholar in evidence reviews and its applicability to grey literature searching. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0138237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Schmucker, C.M.; Blümle, A.; Schell, L.K.; Schwarzer, G.; Oeller, P.; Cabrera, L.; Von Elm, E.; Briel, M.; Meerpohl, J.J. Systematic review finds that study data not published in full text articles have unclear impact on meta-analyses results in medical research. PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0176210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bolic Baric, V.; Larsson Ranada, Å.; Berg, J. The whole journey chain with public transport for people with autism spectrum disorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder—A scoping review. Transp. Rev. 2024, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Faulkner, G.E.J.; Buliung, R.N.; Flora, P.K.; Fusco, C. Active school transport, physical activity levels and body weight of children and youth: A systematic review. Prev. Med. 2009, 48, 3–8. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jaydarifard, S.; Behara, K.; Baker, D.; Paz, A. Driver fatigue in taxi, ride-hailing, and ridesharing services: A systematic review. Transp. Rev. 2024, 44, 572–590. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Scheepers, C.E.; Wendel-Vos, G.C.W.; den Broeder, J.M.; van Kempen, E.E.M.M.; van Wesemael, P.J.V.; Schuit, A.J. Shifting from car to active transport: A systematic review of the effectiveness of interventions. Transp. Res. Part A Policy Pract. 2014, 70, 264–280. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ardeshiri, A.; Jeihani, M.; Peeta, S. Driving simulator-based study of compliance behaviour with dynamic message sign route guidance. IET Intell. Transp. Syst. 2015, 9, 765–772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Banerjee, S.; Jeihani, M.; Brown, D.D.; Ahangari, S. Comprehensive Analysis of Dynamic Message Sign Impact on Driver Behavior: A Random Forest Approach. Urban Sci. 2020, 4, 49. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Caicedo, F. The use of space availability information in “PARC” systems to reduce search times in parking facilities. Transp. Res. Part C Emerg. Technol. 2009, 17, 56–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Fancello, G.; Serra, P.; Pinna, C. Visual perception and understanding of variable message signs: The influence of the drivers’ age and message layout. Safety 2021, 7, 60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gan, H.; Ye, X. Urban freeway users’ diversion response to variable message sign displaying the travel time of both freeway and local street. IET Intell. Transp. Syst. 2012, 6, 78–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gan, H.-C.; Bai, Y.; Wei, J. Why do people change routes? Impact of information services. Ind. Manag. Data Syst. 2013, 113, 403–422. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harms, I.M.; Dijksterhuis, C.; Jelijs, B.; de Waard, D.; Brookhuis, K.A. Don’t shoot the messenger: Traffic-irrelevant messages on variable message signs (VMSs) might not interfere with traffic management. Transp. Res. Part F 2019, 65, 564–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Imants, P.; Theeuwes, J.; Bronkhorst, A.W.; Martens, M.H. Effect of multiple traffic information sources on route choice: A driving simulator study. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2021, 81, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koller-Matschke, I.; Belzner, H.; Glas, R. Traffic Information and Individual Driver Behaviour; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2013; Volume 200 LNEE, ISBN 9783642338373. [Google Scholar]
- Ma, J.; Zhou, F. Virtual dynamic message signs: A future mode for basic public traveller information. IET Intell. Transp. Syst. 2016, 10, 476–482. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Megat-Johari, M.-U.; Megat-Johari, N.; Savolainen, P.T.; Gates, T.J.; Kassens-Noor, E. Does Displaying Safety Messages on Dynamic Message Signs have Measurable Impacts on Crash Risk? Transp. Res. Rec. 2022, 2676, 516–528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schlaich, J. Analyzing route choice behavior with mobile phone trajectories. Transp. Res. Rec. 2010, 2157, 78–85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sharples, S.; Shalloe, S.; Burnett, G.; Crundall, D. Journey decision making: The influence on drivers of dynamic information presented on variable message signs. Cogn. Technol. Work 2016, 18, 303–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Shen, J.; Wang, H. Effects of urban road variable message signs on driving behavior. Adv. Mech. Eng. 2019, 11, 1687814019841843. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Yan, X.; Wu, J. Effectiveness of variable message signs on driving behavior based on a driving simulation experiment. Discret. Dyn. Nat. Soc. 2014, 2014, 206805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhao, D.; Shao, C. Empirical study of drivers’ learning behavior and reliance on VMS. In Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Intelligent Transportation Systems, Proceedings, ITSC, Funchal, Madeira Island, Portugal, 19–22 September 2010; pp. 1614–1619. [Google Scholar]
Inclusion Criteria | Exclusion Criteria |
---|---|
|
|
References and Country | Study Objective | Methodology and Sample | Main Findings | Limitations and Future Work |
---|---|---|---|---|
[19] USA | Analyze driver compliance with dynamic message sign (DMS) route guidance and factors influencing route choice decisions. | A hybrid approach using a driving simulator (DS) and a stated preference (SP) survey, with over 100 participants in a 400 km2 network near Baltimore. | - Key factors: past DMS exposure, travel time savings, DMS reliability, and learning from experience. - Divergence between DS experiment decisions and SP survey responses. - Higher compliance with DMS in DS experiments compared to SP scenarios. | - Limitation: Lack of personalized navigation systems like GPS may affect generalizability. - Future work: Include navigation systems to explore their impact on driver compliance. |
[20] USA | Investigate the impact of different DMS types (including color-coded and alphanumeric) on driver behavior, focusing on route diversion, choice, and compliance. | A high-fidelity driving simulator at Morgan State University with six virtual scenarios was used. Sixty-five participants from diverse backgrounds experienced various DMS messages. Data were collected from pre- and post-simulation surveys and driving behavior. | - Route diversion: DMSs with delay and lane closure information were most effective. - Route choice: color-coded DMSs and “avoid route” advice had a strong impact. - DMS compliance: color-coded DMSs and crash-related advice increased compliance; time-distance alternate routes reduced compliance. | - Limitation: No navigation system was included. - Future work: Study the interaction between GPS and DMS and explore standards for autonomous vehicle integration. |
[21] Spain | Evaluate two PARC (Parking Access and Revenue Control) systems—level 4 and level 2—and their impact on user search times and parking choices. | Simulation of a parking facility using a 3D model, comparing level 4 (censoring info) and level 2 (zoning with vehicle detection) PARC systems. | - Level 4 PARC is effective in small facilities but has little impact on search times. - Level 2 PARC reduces search times by 16.2%. - Availability information helps users make better parking choices. - Manipulating info in level 2 systems reduces search times. | - Limitation: impact of informing users about specific level shortages needs further study. - Future work: field experiments or real-world implementations to validate results. |
[22] Italy | Investigate how different VMS message displays (uppercase/lowercase letters and familiar/less familiar pictograms) affect reading time and information perception, focusing on drivers of different ages and speeds. | A simulated driving scene in a controlled environment at the University of Cagliari. Messages were displayed at speeds of 50 km/h and 80 km/h. Eye-tracking technology was used to measure reading times for 30 drivers, categorized into young, middle-aged, and elderly groups. | - Uppercase vs. lowercase letters did not significantly impact reading times. - Unfamiliar pictograms hindered message perception. - Elderly drivers had more difficulty perceiving messages compared to younger groups. | - Limitation: the simulation environment may not fully replicate real-world conditions. - Future work: use a physical driving simulator for more realistic experiments. |
[23] China | Investigate urban freeway users’ diversion responses to a dual variable message sign (D-VMS) showing travel times for both freeway and local streets and examine factors influencing route choice behavior. | An on-site stated preference survey in Shanghai with 140 drivers. Participants were surveyed based on gender, age, driving experience, frequency of freeway use, and driver type, using scenario-based surveys with D-VMS displaying travel times and causes of delays. | - D-VMS significantly impacts diversion decisions. - Key factors: travel time savings, years of driving experience, traffic lights on local streets, frequency of freeway use, age, and driver type. - Panel models provide more robust statistical results than cross-sectional models. | - Limitation: small sample size due to road changes. - Future work: focus on advanced traveler information systems and improving survey and model design in urban transportation. |
[24] China | Identify factors influencing drivers’ route choice response to travel time information provided by variable message signs (VMSs) on arterial roads. | A stated preference survey was conducted in Shanghai with 228 drivers, resulting in 1512 choice observations from 189 drivers. Generalized Estimating Equations (GEEs) were used to model driver responses, comparing four different correlation structures. | - Key factors: driving experience, expressway delay, cause of delay (accident), and the number of traffic lights on local streets. - Differences in behavior among drivers of employer-provided cars, taxis, and private cars. - The GEEs method was effective, with the exchangeable structure being the most suitable. | - Limitation: study limited to Shanghai. - Future work: cross-country comparisons and collecting revealed preference data after implementing enhanced VMS services. |
[25] Netherlands | Study the impact of using VMS for both traffic management and displaying non-traffic messages, such as ads or mottos. | Experimental design with 32 participants divided into two groups—control (no ads) and experimental (ads)—using a driving simulator. | - Non-traffic messages on VMS do not negatively affect traffic management if ergonomic guidelines are followed. - Compliance with critical traffic instructions was higher in the group exposed to ads. - Conscious attention was not required for compliance. - Familiarity with VMS through repeated exposure positively influenced driver behavior. | - Limitation: ads were designed to minimize distraction, which may not fully represent typical commercial ads. - Future work: investigate the impact of non-ergonomic messages and real-world distraction thresholds. |
[26] Netherlands | Study how drivers respond to different sources of traffic information, such as variable message signs (VMSs) and in-car navigation devices. | A driving simulator was used to simulate route choice scenarios with varying traffic information sources (VMS and in-car navigation). Participants rated their trust in the information after each trial, and a Bayesian model was used to analyze route-switching likelihood. Twenty-four participants took part in the study. | - Trust in traffic information was higher when provided by two sources versus one. - High compliance with the preferred source, mainly VMS. - Participants were more likely to switch routes when the alternative route was shorter. - Age, gender, and yearly mileage did not significantly affect route-switching propensity. | - Limitation: the sample consisted of experienced drivers, limiting generalizability. - Future work: explore how other traffic information sources interact and influence behavior towards VMS. |
[27] Germany | Investigate the impact of individual and collective on-trip traffic management systems on traffic flow in urban networks and how traffic information design influences route choice behavior. | Three systems were considered: radio traffic service, variable traffic signs, and navigation systems. Evaluation methods included ANPR, GPS-logging, interviews, and a driving simulator. The study tracked 300 individuals in Munich over 8 weeks. | - Route choice behavior was mainly influenced by travel time. - Drivers preferred the main route unless there was a significant travel time increase. - Radio traffic services and navigation systems significantly influenced individual decisions. - The driving simulator highlighted the role of familiarity and subjective motivations in route choice. | - Limitation: difficulty studying individual behavior in detail. - Future work: investigate the impact of emerging systems and reliability of route choices. |
[1] Austria | Design a dynamic traffic display that effectively conveys information to drivers and promotes behavioral change, exploring various design categories based on four dimensions. | The study used a conceptual design based on four dimensions: emotionality, complexity, information content, and familiarity. An online survey was conducted to eliminate poor designs, followed by a lab evaluation with 12 participants sitting in front of a simulated car cockpit. | - “Icon oriented”, “schedule oriented”, and “traffic sign oriented” designs were rated higher than “emotion oriented” signs. - Icon-oriented signs were clear favorites, being unique, memorable, and easily interpreted. | - Limitation: subjectivity in design evaluation was noted. - Future work: further exploration of designs that support long-term behavioral change, with a focus on clarity for individuals with foreign mother tongues, is needed. |
[28] USA | Propose and evaluate the concept of virtual dynamic message signs (VDMSs) as a future method for delivering public traveler information, comparing it to traditional dynamic message signs (DMS) in terms of user acceptance, message comprehension, and driver distraction. | A smartphone-based VDMS prototype was developed. The study included a user experience survey with 21 participants from Northern Virginia who used the VDMS app for two weeks and a driving simulator study with 42 participants to assess message comprehension and driver distraction in various driving scenarios. | - Positive user experience with VDMS, with high ratings for usefulness and safety. - VDMS was seen as safer and more comfortable than traditional DMS. - The driving simulator study showed VDMS performed better than DMS, especially under complex driving conditions. - VDMS was as effective as DMS under simple conditions but significantly better in complex scenarios. | - Limitation: on-road assessments are needed to confirm simulator findings. - Future work: explore additional factors like gender and education level and evaluate the effectiveness of improved auditory VDMS messages in real-world settings. |
[29] USA | Assess the relationship between traffic crashes and the frequency of safety messages displayed on dynamic message signs (DMSs) on Michigan freeways, examining if these messages influence driver behavior and crash risk. | Data from 202 fixed DMS on Michigan freeways (2014–2018) were integrated with traffic volume, roadway geometry, and crash data. Random parameters negative binomial models were used to analyze total, speeding-related, and nighttime crashes. | - No significant difference in total crashes related to message frequency. - Marginal, statistically insignificant declines in nighttime crashes with more impaired driving messages. - Significant reduction in speeding-related crashes near DMS with frequent messages about speeding or tailgating. | - Limitation: uncertainty about how many drivers read or retain messages and the influence of urban DMS density. - Future work: field research to explore the impact of safety messages on individual driver behavior. |
[30] Germany | Evaluate how information from variable message signs (VMSs), broadcast traffic updates, and other factors influence drivers’ route choices, focusing on the impact of VMS recommendations and broadcasted traffic information. | The study used floating car data (FCD) from mobile phones to analyze route choices through maximum likelihood estimations, logit models, and utility functions. Two cases were studied: the motorway quadrangle and Stuttgart. | Motorway quadrangle: - Drivers respond to congestion proximity in broadcast traffic updates. - VMS recommendations diverted 30% of through-traffic to alternative routes. Stuttgart: - Results were statistically insignificant, with only 3% to 17% of drivers accepting VMS recommendations. | - Limitation: Infrequent VMS activation in Stuttgart led to inconclusive results. - Future work: investigate external factors (e.g., weather or events) and how drivers adapt their route choices based on dynamic variables beyond traffic congestion. |
[31] United Kingdom | Explore the influence of dynamic information on VMS on driving behavior and decision-making, focusing on factors like familiarity, information wording, and context. | Two methods were used: a scenario-based approach and a medium-fidelity driving simulator. Data were collected from 82 UK drivers (aged 21–65) through scenarios, questionnaires, and driving tasks. | - Repetition of information increases trust and encourages drivers to slow down. - Additional details about traffic conditions (e.g., delay length) affect driver behavior. - Specific information, like mentioning an “accident”, prompts more cautious driving. - Context and prior experience influence decision-making. | - Limitation: caution is needed when implementing complex signs in real-world contexts. - Future work: explore the role of new information sources (e.g., sensor-based tech and social media) for more effective traffic information design. |
[32] China | Investigate the effects of VMS on driving behaviors in urban areas, focusing on factors such as age, driving experience, familiarity with the road network, and attention to VMS. The study also aimed to develop a logistic model to analyze the influence of VMS on driving behavior. | A questionnaire survey was conducted with 578 samples from car repair shops and parking lots in Yangzhou, China, with 402 valid responses. A logistic model was developed based on survey data and real-world traffic data from video cameras, along with correlation analysis. | - Main factors affecting driving behavior: age, driving experience, road familiarity, and attention to VMS. - Drivers aged 36–55 and those with 6–10 years of driving experience were more likely to reduce speed to read VMS. - Drivers unfamiliar with the road network also tended to reduce speed to read VMS. - The logistic model confirmed that VMS significantly affects driving behavior in urban areas. | - Limitation: sample bias from data collected in Yangzhou, China, may limit generalizability. - Future work: further research on the optimal design of VMS messages, including size, color, and layout, is recommended. |
[4] Greece | Investigate factors influencing driver response to VMS in Athens, focusing on route diversion due to incidents. The study examines the impact of message characteristics, trip characteristics, and driver characteristics on diversion behavior. | A stated preference questionnaire survey was conducted with 120 Athenian drivers familiar with VMS. The survey covered traffic information preferences, trip details, and incident scenarios. A random-effect ordered probit model was used for analysis, with data collected through on-site interviews. | - Message characteristics (incident type, impact, and alternative route) significantly influenced diversion decisions. - Accidents were the most influential incident type, followed by demonstrations and roadworks, with congestion being the least influential. - Qualitative descriptions of impact (e.g., ‘long delays’) had a stronger effect than quantitative descriptions. - Drivers were more likely to divert when an alternative route was suggested via VMS. - Trip and driver characteristics (e.g., transport mode, VMS experience, attitudes towards VMS) also influenced decisions. | - Limitation: the findings are based on Greek drivers and may not generalize to other urban areas. - Future work: explore diversion behavior in other driver populations (e.g., professional drivers) and tailor VMS information to specific driver groups. The study also recommends improving VMS reliability by withdrawing messages once incidents end. |
[33] China | Test the effectiveness of VMS on driving behavior, focusing on how VMS location and information format influence route choice, speed control, and lane-changing behaviors. | Participants drove three simulated scenarios using a high-fidelity driving simulator. Two parts of the network were designed to test the impact of VMS location and information format. Fifty-seven subjects participated, with data collected on route choice, speed, lane changes, and other behaviors. | - VMS location, information format, and driver characteristics (age, gender) significantly influenced driving behavior. - VMS placed 150–200 m upstream from the diverging point was optimal for safety and traffic flow. - Graphic information on VMS was more effective than text-only formats. - Older drivers were less likely to change routes, while male and professional drivers were more influenced by VMS. | - Limitation: the study did not account for socioeconomic factors or confidence in VMS information. - Future work: explore more factors, including VMS content, to better understand their combined impact on driving behavior. |
[34] China | Analyze the causal relationships among drivers’ attributes, learning behavior, reliance on VMS, and route choice behavior in the context of road networks in Beijing. | Data were gathered from the “Inducing Effect Survey of VMS in Beijing”, with 311 valid responses from 350 distributed questionnaires. The study focused on traffic information from 228 VMS devices across major trip generators in Beijing. A structural equation model (SEM) was used for analysis. | - Drivers’ learning from VMS significantly impacts their reliance on VMS. - Learning behavior is a key factor in route choice decisions, influenced primarily by driving years and age. - Drivers who trust and rely on VMS are more likely to follow alternative routes suggested by the signs. | - Limitation: the data used in the study had limitations, and more observed variables should be considered. - Future work: further research should include more observed variables to strengthen the findings. |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Lagoa, P.; Galvão, T.; Campos Ferreira, M. Variable Message Signs in Traffic Management: A Systematic Review of User Behavior and Future Innovations. Infrastructures 2024, 9, 184. https://doi.org/10.3390/infrastructures9100184
Lagoa P, Galvão T, Campos Ferreira M. Variable Message Signs in Traffic Management: A Systematic Review of User Behavior and Future Innovations. Infrastructures. 2024; 9(10):184. https://doi.org/10.3390/infrastructures9100184
Chicago/Turabian StyleLagoa, Paula, Teresa Galvão, and Marta Campos Ferreira. 2024. "Variable Message Signs in Traffic Management: A Systematic Review of User Behavior and Future Innovations" Infrastructures 9, no. 10: 184. https://doi.org/10.3390/infrastructures9100184
APA StyleLagoa, P., Galvão, T., & Campos Ferreira, M. (2024). Variable Message Signs in Traffic Management: A Systematic Review of User Behavior and Future Innovations. Infrastructures, 9(10), 184. https://doi.org/10.3390/infrastructures9100184