Next Article in Journal
Integrated Transcriptome and Microbiome Analyses Reveal Growth- and Stress-Response-Related Genes and Microbes in Mandarin Fish (Siniperca chuatsi)
Next Article in Special Issue
Study on the Reproductive Group Behavior of Schizothorax wangchiachii Based on Acoustic Telemetry
Previous Article in Journal
Molecular Mechanisms and Biomarker-Based Early-Warning Indicators of Heavy Metal Toxicity in Marine Fish
Previous Article in Special Issue
Behavioral Responses of Migratory Fish to Environmental Cues: Evidence from the Heishui River
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Eat First, Fight Later: Competitive Advantage of an Invasive Cichlid over a Native Competitor for Food Resources

by
Leonardo Cirillo
1,2,*,
Caio A. Miyai
1,
Fábio H. C. Sanches
2,
Alexandre L. Arvigo
3 and
Tânia M. Costa
1,4
1
Biosciences Institute, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Coastal Campus, São Vicente 11330-900, SP, Brazil
2
Institute of Marine Science (IMar), Federal University of São Paulo (UNIFESP), Santos 11070-070, SP, Brazil
3
Northern University Center of Espírito Santo (CEUNES), Federal University of Espírito Santo (UFES), São Mateus 29932-540, ES, Brazil
4
Postgraduate Program in Biological Sciences (Zoology), Biosciences Institute, São Paulo State University (UNESP), Botucatu Campus, Botucatu 18618-000, SP, Brazil
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Fishes 2025, 10(7), 340; https://doi.org/10.3390/fishes10070340
Submission received: 31 March 2025 / Revised: 10 May 2025 / Accepted: 16 May 2025 / Published: 10 July 2025
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Behavioral Ecology of Fishes)

Abstract

Competition for resources with invasive species can negatively impact native fauna. Invasive species often exhibit higher aggressiveness and monopolize resources through behavioral interference. However, their feeding behavior also plays a central role in invasion success. We investigated how food resource availability influences agonistic interactions between the invasive cichlid Oreochromis niloticus and the native cichlid Geophagus iporangensis. Specifically, we assessed whether the invasive species interferes with the native species’ feeding behavior. Using neutral arenas, we staged competition trials under two conditions: initially without food and subsequently with food present. The native species served as the focal animal and was exposed to either a conspecific or an invasive competitor. Results showed that native species aggressiveness toward the invasive competitor was three times higher in the absence of food. Although O. niloticus was 1.6 times more aggressive than conspecifics, its presence did not affect the native species’ feeding rate, and no behavioral interference was observed. Instead, the invasive species relied primarily on scramble competition, initiating nine of ten trials and consuming seven times more food than the native cichlid. Our findings suggest that, beyond aggression, feeding behavior and resource exploitation are key mechanisms driving competitive exclusion by invasive species.
Key Contribution: This study advances the understanding of food resource competition between aggressive invasive and native species, demonstrating that feeding efficiency and exploitative behavior—rather than aggressiveness and behavioral interference alone—may be key mechanisms underlying invasion success. These findings provide a conceptual foundation for future research on the behavioral mechanisms driving competitive exclusion in the invasion ecology context.

1. Introduction

Invasive species are a major threat to biodiversity and the stability of ecosystems [1]. The presence of invasive species is associated with changes in community structure [2,3], disrupting interspecific interactions among native species with similar niches [4,5]. Globalization in recent decades has facilitated the spread of species into environments beyond their natural range [6]. Once an invasive species is established, it might utilize similar resources as native species, leading to niche overlap [7,8,9] and positioning itself as a strong competitor of native species [10,11] by presenting ecological functional similarities [12]. Thus, the loss of resources and reduced fitness in native species may lead to population decline and, in extreme cases, local extinction [2,13,14,15].
Competition may manifest in two primary forms: (1) behavioral interference (direct competition; [16]), through aggressive behaviors in territorial contest, aimed at directly excluding opponents from accessing resources; and (2) exploitative competition (indirect competition; [17,18]), with individual behaviors ensuring greater resource acquisition, such as ingesting food quickly and in greater quantities [19], without directly preventing competitors from accessing resources [2,20]. Thus, individuals who obtain more and better-quality resources have higher fitness [21,22] and can increase their reproductive success [23,24,25]. Both behavioral interference and exploitative competition can lead to competitive exclusion or habitat partitioning [13,16,26], although competitive interference affects individual fitness more intensely. The aggressiveness of invasive species plays an important role in the invasion process, often exhibiting highly aggressive behavior that facilitates resource monopolization and increases fitness concerning native species [27].
Agonistic contests during competition demand a high metabolic cost [28], and the decision and intensity of fighting with a similar-sized competitor may be influenced by the resource value [29,30,31,32]. For example, during food resource competition, the native Mexican mojarra Cichlasoma istlanum may exhibit higher levels of aggressiveness than the invasive species convict Cichlid Amatitlania nigrofasciata [33]; however, during nest defense, the convict Cichlid exhibits higher aggressiveness than the Mexican mojarra [34]. The value that the individual attributes to the resource in dispute may vary depending on the type of competitor [26,32] and the status of the competitor [35,36,37], reflecting their motivation to engage in fights [38]. More intense agonistic contests may happen with competitors with greater niche overlap (e.g., conspecifics) [39] and with competitors that impose a great apparent threat [40,41]. Nevertheless, native species may not recognize invasive species as potential competitors because they have not shared an evolutionary context, suffering lower levels of aggression in comparison to sympatric competitors [42]. As a result, aggressive invasive species may directly have advantages in obtaining limited resources over native species [43,44]. Consequently, native species may be at a competitive disadvantage when facing invasive species in resource acquisition.
In aggressive fish species, agonistic behavior is commonly associated with resource monopolization [45,46], and with individuals engaging in fights over territory [47], food [48], or reproductive resources [49]. For juvenile fish, territoriality is more often linked to feeding sites and access to better quality food resources than to reproductive success [48,50]. Consequently, competition between juvenile native and invasive species for food resources can act as a strong population regulation mechanism by indirectly affecting native fish species’ growth and the densities of prey communities [51]. Freshwater invasive species also tend to exhibit higher consumption rates than native counterparts [52], reinforcing the need to better understand the role of feeding behavior in aggressive invasive species as a key competition mechanism underlying the success of biological invasions
Thus, to evaluate the effect of food resource availability on agonistic interactions between aggressive invasive and native species, assessing the feeding behavior of each species and whether the invasive species exert behavioral interference, we utilize as invasive species model the Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) and as the native species model the pearl cichlid (Geophagus iporangensis; previously described as Geophagus brasiliensis [53]). The Nile tilapia is a highly invasive species present in several countries around the world due to its widespread use in fish farming [54,55]. Their common use in aquaculture is due to their generalist diet [56], high resistance to abiotic variations [57], reproduction with oral incubation [58], and accelerated growth [58,59]. These characteristics, along with its aggressive and territorial behavior, make the Nile tilapia a strong competitor, with the ability to competitively exclude native species [8,60]. The population of pearl cichlid has declined due to coexistence with Nile tilapia near urban centers, where habitat structural complexity is lower [61]. By sharing food resources and habitats, and being a territorial species [62], these invasive and native species may compete aggressively. The Nile tilapia exhibits higher aggressiveness and dominance over the pearl cichlid during territorial contests [63], demonstrating as good animal models to address competitive mechanisms underlying the food resource acquisition of aggressive invasive species.
We hypothesized that food availability would intensify the invasive species’ aggressiveness, leading to resource monopolization and a consequent reduction in the food intake by the native species. This study provides novel insights into the behavioral mechanism underlaying direct and indirect competition for food resources between aggressive invasive and native fish species, highlighting the importance of feeding behavior for invasion success even in highly aggressive invasive species.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Fish Acquisition and Holding Conditions

Pearl cichlid and Nile tilapia individuals were acquired from Polettini Fish Farm in the municipality of Mogi Mirim, São Paulo State (Brazil). We used juvenile, sexually immature individuals, as they seem to be more relevant to the process of competitive exclusion between the two species [61,63]. Species were acclimatized in monospecific stock systems (circular 300-L tanks), avoiding physical, visual, or chemical contact between species until the start of the experiment. The individuals were acclimatized for 30 days before the start of the experiments at a temperature of 25 ± 1 °C, with constant aeration provided by air pumps (100% saturation), physical and biological filtration, and a 12/12 photoperiod. Ammonia (<0.5 ppm) and nitrite (<0.5 ppm) levels were kept stable through weekly partial water changes. During this period, the individuals were fed twice a day until apparent satiety with commercial fish feed (32% crude protein; Guabi Pirá).

2.2. Experimental Design

To evaluate the effects of food resource availability on agonistic interactions between aggressive invasive and native species, and to evaluate whether invasive species induce interference competition in native species feeding behavior, we simulated a competition scenario initially without food resources, followed by their introduction (see Figure 1). We exposed the native species pearl cichlid (used as a focal animal) to an invasive competitor treatment (Nile tilapia; n = 10) or conspecific competitor treatment (pearl cichlid; n = 10).
Our experiment involved exposing a focal individual to a competitor (invasive or conspecific) in a neutral arena (experimental aquarium with no previous residence), allowing them to interact first in the absence of food (baseline stage) followed by the introduction of a food resource into the experimental aquarium (feeding stage; see Figure 1). We quantified aggressive behavior during both the basal and feeding stage, while the feeding behavior was assessed only during the feeding stage for both the focal individual and its competitors throughout the experimental trials.

2.3. Procedures

Before the experiments, both focal individuals and competitors (either invasive or conspecifics) were subjected to a 7-day individual acclimatization period to minimize the effects of previous social interaction [64]. One focal individual and one competitor were selected from the stocks, measured (standard length; cm), weighed (grams; g), and placed in separate 20-L acclimatization aquariums (40 × 20 × 10 cm). To avoid size asymmetry effect on aggressiveness during fights [65,66], we paired focal animals and competitors of similar sizes (±0.2 cm) [conspecific treatment: average size 6.40 ± 0.65 cm (paired Wilcoxon test: p = 0.401) and average mass 9.24 ± 2.97 g (paired Wilcoxon test: p = 0.5452; invasive treatment: average size 6.42 ± 0.51 cm (paired t-test: p = 0.912) and average mass 9.32 ± 2.19 g (paired t-test: p = 0.848)]. The acclimatization aquarium was maintained under conditions similar to those of the stock tanks, with food provided once daily, followed by a 24 h fasting period before the start of the experiment. On the seventh day, the focal animal and its respective competitor were removed from the acclimatization aquarium, marked (see Supplementary Materials with non-toxic ink for individual differentiation (red for the focal animal and black for the competitor), and simultaneously introduced into a neutral arena identical in condition and size to the acclimatization aquariums, but without prior residents. The baseline stage then began, allowing both individuals to interact in the absence of food resources for 15 min. After this period, food was introduced into the neutral arena (20 pellets) with an additional 20 pellets provided if all initial food was consumed. This marked the beginning of the feeding stage, in which individuals competed for food resources (see Figure 1). The videos were recorded using a GoPro Hero 6 mounted on a tripod positioned 1.5 m from the experimental aquarium.

2.4. Behavioral Analysis

We quantified the aggressive and feeding behaviors of the focal animal and its competitor of each treatment by analyzing the video recordings experiments using previously published ethograms of Nile tilapia [67] and pearl cichlid [62]. As variables of aggressive behavior, we used total aggression to assess fight intensity [66,68] and aggression latency to indicate fight motivation [69], measuring both across the baseline and feeding stages. Total aggression was calculated as the sum of four commonly exhibited aggressive behaviors: bites (targeting the head, tail, fin, median, or ventral areas of the opponent); lateral displays, involving a lateral body inclination to intimidate the opponent occurring with individuals oriented in the same or opposite directions; mouth wrestling, characterized by both fish interlocking their jaws and pushing against each other; and charge, defined as a rapid approach without physical contact. Aggression latency was defined as the time (in seconds) an individual took to exhibit the first aggressive behavior involving physical contact (bites, lateral displays, or frontal fighting). As feeding behavior variables, we used feeding latency, defined as the time (in seconds) required for an individual to ingest the first food pellet during the feeding stage [70] and ingestion rate, quantified as the ratio between the amount of food ingested and the individual’s weight (in grams) (adapted from [71]), allowing for comparisons of food intake across individuals of different weights.

2.5. Statistical Analysis

The data were tested for normality and homoscedasticity, with a Shapiro–Wilk and Levene test. Since the data were considered non-parametric and exhibited dependency due to the stages of the experiment or two individuals in the same aquarium, we used generalized estimating equations (GEE) to analyze both aggressive and feeding behavior response variables. This analysis consists of a marginal model for correlated structure with non-normally distributed data, being a suitable approach for behavior analysis with repeated measures and dependent data (for further detail, see [72]). GEE analysis was performed using the ‘geepack’ package [73] in R version 4.4.0 [74].
To test the effect of food resource availability on agonistic interaction (response variables: total aggression and aggression latency), we analyzed focal animal and competitor aggressive behavior separately, using as fixed factors the experimental stages (2 levels: basal and feeding stages) and type of competitor (treatments; 2 levels: conspecific and invasive species). By that, we excluded dependent data by analyzing aggressive variables from the focal animal to competitors and from competitors to the focal animal, but still had repeated measures due to the experiment stage. We selected regression models with exchangeable structures with the Poisson family and Logit function using the experiment stage as repeated measures (‘id’ function). For feeding behavior (response variables: feeding latency and ingestion rate), we used as fixed factors the type of competitor (treatment; 2 levels: conspecific and invasive species) and the individual observed (2 levels: focal animal and competitor). Thus, we have dependent data but no repeated measures. We selected regression models with exchangeable structures with the Poisson family and the Logit function using the observed individuals as dependent data (id’ function). The exchangeable correlation structure (“Ar1” function) was used for all analyses due to repeated measures (aggressive behavior variables) or dependent data (feeding behavior variables) on the same individual [72]. Also, we used the Wald test to test the significance of fixed factors, and the post hoc Tukey test was applied to assess differences between factor levels when observing differences in factor interaction. The statistical significance was considered when p < 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Aggressive Behaviors

The focal animal exhibited similar aggression latency regardless of competitor type (GEE; Wald test, χ2 = 1.45, df = 1, p = 0.229) and experimental stage (GEE; Wald test, χ2 = 0.00, df = 1, p = 0.989). However, competitors displayed significantly shorter aggression latency during the baseline stage (GEE; Wald test, χ2 = 19.28, df = 1, p < 0.001; Figure 2a), initiating confrontations 2.5 times faster in the absence of food resources (baseline stage: mean = 211.3 s, standard error = 38.0 s; feeding stage: mean = 539.4 s, standard error = 79.3 s). Despite this difference between experimental stages, aggression latency did not vary between competitor types (GEE; Wald test, χ2 = 0.11, df = 1, p = 0.74; Figure 2a).
Despite the time to start fighting, the total aggression exhibited by the focal animal showed a significant interaction between the competitor type and the experimental stage (GEE; Wald test, χ2 = 18.39, df = 1, p < 0.001). Specifically, aggression toward the invasive competitor was three times higher during the baseline stage (mean = 60.4; standard error = 7.78) compared to the feeding stage (mean = 20.3; standard error = 3.84) (Tukey: Z = 6.40; p < 0.001; Figure 2b). In the presence of food, the focal animal exhibited three times less aggression toward the invasive species (mean = 20.3; standard error = 3.84) than toward its conspecific competitor (mean = 65.5; standard error = 16.7) (Tukey: Z = −3.54; p = 0.002; Figure 2b). In contrast, the competitor’s aggression toward the focal animal did not show a significant interaction between the competitor type and experimental stage (GEE; Wald test, χ2 = 0.14, df = 1, p = 0.712). However, the invasive competitor (mean = 41.7; standard error = 6.33) was 1.6 times more aggressive than the conspecific competitor (mean = 24.9; standard error = 4.97) across all experimental stages (GEE; Wald test, χ2 = 3.89, df = 1, p = 0.049; Figure 2c). Additionally, overall aggression levels were twice as high during the basal stage (mean = 46; standard error = 5.87) compared to the feeding stage (mean = 20.1; standard error = 4.49) (GEE; Wald test, χ2 = 18.21, df = 1, p < 0.001; Figure 2d).

3.2. Feeding Behaviors

When food resources were available in the experimental aquarium, individuals in the invasive competitor treatment fed five times faster (mean = 57.1 s; standard error = 13.6 s) compared to those in the conspecific treatment (mean = 314.1; standard error = 76.9 s) (GEE; Wald test, χ2 = 18.15, df = 1, p < 0.001; Figure 3a). No significant difference was observed between the focal animal and its respective competitor in each contest (GEE; Wald test, χ2 = 0.04, df = 1, p = 0.83).
Food intake exhibited a significant interaction between the type of competitor and the observed individual (GEE; competitor: stage; Wald test, χ2 = 11.0, df = 1, p < 0.001; Figure 3b). The invasive competitor of the invasive treatment (mean = 2.58 pellets ingested/g; standard error = 0.345 pellets ingested/g) consumed a remarkable seven times more food than the focal animal of the invasive treatment (mean = 0.361 pellets ingested/g; standard error = 0.060 pellets ingested/g) (Tukey: Z = 13.81, p < 0.001; Figure 3b), six times more than the focal animal of the conspecific treatment (mean = 0.433 pellets ingested/g; standard error = 0.126 pellets ingested/g) (Tukey: Z = 5.88, p < 0.001; Figure 3b), and five times more than the conspecific competitor of the conspecific treatment (mean = 0.518 pellets ingested/g; standard error = 0.189 pellets ingested/g) (Tukey: Z = 4.38, p < 0.001; Figure 3b). However, no significant differences were observed between the focal animal of the invasive treatment, the focal animal of the conspecific treatment, and the conspecific competitor of the conspecific treatment.

4. Discussion

Our study demonstrated that the presence of food resources reduced the intensity of agonistic interactions with the invasive competitor and did not influence the amount of food consumed by the native species. Thus, behavioral interference was absent during food competition with the invasive competitor in our experiment. Additionally, we observed a faster feeding initiation for both focal animals and the competitors in the invasive competitor treatment, without affecting the food intake of the native species, although the invasive competitor ingested a considerably larger quantity of food. Thus, our results demonstrated behavioral flexibility in response to the presence of food resources, minimizing energy costs and optimizing resource acquisition. Our findings suggest that feeding behavior plays a key role in the competitive exclusion process even for aggressive species, indicating that exploitative competition may contribute directly to differences in native and invasive species population growth.
The increased aggressiveness observed in the absence of food is possibly related to an attempt to establish a territory, given that we used a neutral arena with the same dimensions as the acclimatization tanks. Despite competitors presenting a quicker initiation of fights during the baseline stage, the focal animals used as native species started four of ten trials against the invasive competitors, indicating similar motivation to engage in fights (Chi-square test; χ2 = 0.4, df = 1, p = 0.5271). Although cichlid fish exhibit territoriality primarily associated with reproductive behaviors and intense aggression to monopolize such areas [75], territoriality in juveniles is primarily linked to feeding sites, where aggressive interactions serve to maximize food acquisition [50]. Since the experimental aquarium was a neutral arena (without prior residence), it is plausible that the individuals attempted to establish a hierarchy to ensure better access to food resources during the baseline phase of our experiment. Competition for limited resources, such as the territory of the neutral arena, is often associated with heightened aggressive behaviors as a means of monopolizing the resources [48,76]. Despite the increased aggressiveness observed during the baseline stage of our experiment, the feeding behavior of the native species was not directly affected by the presence of the invasive species, exhibiting similar ingestion rates independently of the competitor present. Therefore, our initial hypothesis of interference competition driven by the aggressiveness of the invasive species was not supported, suggesting a higher influence of resource exploitation during food resource acquisition. However, the reduction of aggressive behaviors during the feeding stage against the invasive competitor may have allowed the invasive species to obtain significantly more food than the native species. Similar findings have been reported in competition for food resources between the native European mudminnow (Umbra krameri) and the invasive Amur sleeper (Perccottus glenii), where the native fish exhibited comparable feeding rates despite taking longer to initiate feeding [77].
In our study, the invasive species appeared to exhibit behavioral flexibility in response to the availability of food resources, shifting the focus of aggressive interactions from baseline stage aggression to resource acquisition in the feeding stage. Although behavioral interference did not occur in our experiment, the feeding behavior of the invasive species highlights the importance of exploitative competition as a mechanism contributing to competitive exclusion. Exploitative competition does not have a direct effect on native species’ fitness in the same way as behavioral interference but influences population dynamics by reducing the quantity and quality of available resources for native fauna [2,16,78]. Impressively, the invasive species ingested sevenfold more food resources than the native species in the contests (focal animal), suggesting that the Nile tilapia may better exploit and use food resources than the native species. Our findings support the Resource Consumption Hypothesis, which suggests higher consumption rates by invasive species than native species of similar trophic niches [52].
Individual characteristics of the invasive species, such as higher food intake and greater energy efficiency compared to native competitors, may facilitate better growth and, consequently, enhanced reproductive performance [79,80], leading to the invasive species outpacing the native species in terms of proliferation. For example, Damas-Moreira et al. [19] observed exploitative competition between the invasive lizard Podarcis siculus and the native lizard Podarcis virescens, where the invasive competitor reached the food source more rapidly, consumed more food, and consequently gained more weight than the native species, partially explaining the displacement of the native species from its habitat. Our results suggest this may also happen to the Nile tilapia and pearl cichlid competition for food resources. Thus, in a natural competition situation, with limited resources, the Nile tilapia may be able to better obtain food resources than the pearl cichlid. In natural feeding areas, the increased consumption by invasive species may reduce the availability of food resources for native species, potentially compromising their fitness and leading to a decline in native populations [81,82]. We highlighted the significant role of feeding behavior in the competition between invasive and native aggressive species, emphasizing the importance of feeding behavior in the success of biological invasions.
The reduction of the aggressiveness by the invasive species appears to have shifted to a scramble competition strategy as a way of limiting food access to the native species without the need to use high levels of energy expenditure through aggression [83,84]. Scramble competition does not monopolize the resources but affects the opponent’s fitness by obtaining more food, more quickly, and in larger quantities [85], as previously reported for Nile tilapia [86]. The use of scramble competition instead of food resource monopolization through aggressive behavior appears to be associated with the unpredictable availability of food items [48], as observed in our experiment. In turn, feeding motivation in the invasive treatment was higher than in the conspecific treatment, as indicated by the lower feeding latency. Despite this difference between treatments, the focal animals and competitors within each treatment exhibited similar feeding motivation during the trials. Nevertheless, in the invasive treatment, the invasive competitor began feeding in 9 out of 10 trials (Chi-square test; χ2 = 6.4, df = 1, p = 0.0114), suggesting that the beginning of feeding by one of the individuals may influence the opponent’s foraging efforts during competition due to social facilitation [87,88]. Thus, the native species secured the acquisition of resources during competition for food, but in smaller quantities compared to the invasive competitor.

5. Conclusions

In summary, our results show that behavioral interference did not occur during feeding competition, as the aggressiveness of the invasive species did not affect the feeding rate of the native species. In the presence of food, the invasive species has reduced its aggressiveness, engaging in scramble competition as the primary strategy to maximize food intake, with significantly higher ingestion rates than the native species. Our findings highlight that feeding behavior, resource exploitation, and exploitative competition play a crucial role in the competitive exclusion of native fauna, as they exhibit a superior ability to acquire food resources compared to native species with a similar trophic niche.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/fishes10070340/s1. Figure S1: Focal animal (pearl Cichlid; Geophagus iporangensis) marked with red on the dorsal fin and conspecific competitor (pearl Cichlid; Geophagus iporangensis) marked with black on the dorsal fin during a trial of the conspecific treatment [89,90,91].

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: L.C., C.A.M., A.L.A., F.H.C.S. and T.M.C.; methodology, L.C., C.A.M., F.H.C.S. and A.L.A.; validation, L.C., C.A.M., A.L.A., F.H.C.S. and T.M.C.; formal analysis, L.C. and F.H.C.S.; investigation, L.C., C.A.M. and A.L.A., resources, T.M.C.; data curation, L.C.; writing—original draft preparation, L.C., C.A.M., A.L.A., F.H.C.S. and T.M.C.; writing—review and editing, L.C., C.A.M., A.L.A., F.H.C.S. and T.M.C.; visualization, L.C.; supervision, C.A.M., A.L.A. and T.M.C.; project administration, L.C., C.A.M., A.L.A. and T.M.C.; funding acquisition, L.C., C.A.M. and T.M.C. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was financially supported by the São Paulo Research Foundation (FAPESP)—2020/11778-6 (L. Cirillo), 2017/20802-5 (C. Miyai); and by the Pro-Rectory of Research (PROPe/UNESP), project 5649 (C. Miyai).

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted following the Ethical Principles of Animal Research established by the National Council for the Control of Animal Experimentation (CONCEA, Brazil) and was approved by the Animal Use Ethics Committee (CEUA) of the Biosciences Institute, São Paulo State University (UNESP), protocol 03/2023, Approval date 12 May 2023. No individual sustained serious injuries or died during the experiments.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

The original data presented in the study are openly available in Mendeley Data repository at https://doi.org/10.17632/kxr26tcwgf.1.

Acknowledgments

We extend our gratitude to Paulo H. Miyai for providing the fish for this study, to the reviewers for their valuable contributions, and to Phylopic for the icons used in the creation of the experimental design figures.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Lowry, E.; Rollinson, E.J.; Laybourn, A.J.; Scott, T.E.; Aiello-Lammens, M.E.; Gray, S.M.; Mickley, J.; Gurevitch, J. Biological invasions: A field synopsis, systematic review, and database of the literature. Ecol. Evol. 2013, 3, 182–196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. David, P.; Thébault, E.; Anneville, O.; Duyck, P.-F.; Chapuis, E.; Loeuille, N. Impacts of Invasive Species on Food Webs. Adv. Ecol. Res. 2017, 56, 1–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Bradley, B.A.; Laginhas, B.B.; Whitlock, R.; Allen, J.M.; Bates, A.E.; Bernatchez, G.; Diez, J.M.; Early, R.; Lenoir, J.; Vilà, M.; et al. Disentangling the abundance–impact relationship for invasive species. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2019, 116, 9919–9924. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Le Louarn, M.; Couillens, B.; Deschamps-Cottin, M.; Clergeau, P. Interference competition between an invasive parakeet and native bird species at feeding sites. J. Ethol. 2016, 34, 291–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Song, Y.; Yang, X.; Zhang, H.; Zhang, D.; He, W.; Wyckhuys, K.A.G.; Wu, K. Interference competition and predation between invasive and native herbivores in maize. J. Pest Sci. 2021, 94, 1053–1063. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Meyerson, L.A.; Mooney, H.A. Invasive alien species in an era of globalization. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2007, 5, 199–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Richter-Boix, A.; Garriga, N.; Montori, A.; Franch, M.; Sebastián, O.S.; Villero, D.; Llorente, G.A. Effects of the non-native amphibian species Discoglossus pictus on the recipient amphibian community: Niche overlap, competition and community organization. Biol. Invasions 2013, 15, 799–815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Zengeya, T.A.; Booth, A.J.; Chimimba, C.T. Broad niche overlap between invasive nile tilapia Oreochromis niloticus and indigenous congenerics in Southern Africa: Should we be concerned? Entropy 2015, 17, 4959–4973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Yalçın Özdilek, Ş.; Partal, N.; Jones, R.I. An invasive species, Carassius gibelio, alters the native fish community through trophic niche competition. Aquat. Sci. 2019, 81, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Britton, J.R.; Ruiz-Navarro, A.; Verreycken, H.; Amat-Trigo, F. Trophic consequences of introduced species: Comparative impacts of increased interspecific versus intraspecific competitive interactions. Funct. Ecol. 2018, 32, 486–495. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Almela, V.D.; South, J.; Britton, J.R. Predicting the competitive interactions and trophic niche consequences of a globally invasive fish with threatened native species. J. Anim. Ecol. 2021, 90, 2651–2662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Gallien, L.; Carboni, M. The community ecology of invasive species: Where are we and what’s next? Ecography 2017, 40, 335–352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Bøhn, T.; Amundsen, P.A.; Sparrow, A. Competitive exclusion after invasion? Biol. Invasions 2008, 10, 359–368. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Moran, E.V.; Alexander, J.M. Evolutionary responses to global change: Lessons from invasive species. Ecol. Lett. 2014, 17, 637–649. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Catford, J.A.; Bode, M.; Tilman, D. Introduced species that overcome life history tradeoffs can cause native extinctions. Nat. Commun. 2018, 9, 2131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Grether, G.F.; Peiman, K.S.; Tobias, J.A.; Robinson, B.W. Causes and Consequences of Behavioral Interference Between Species. Trends Ecol. Evol. 2017, 32, 760–772. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Warren, R.J.; King, J.R.; Bradford, M.A. Disentangling resource acquisition from interspecific behavioral aggression to understand the ecological dominance of a common, widespread temperate forest ant. Insectes Soc. 2020, 67, 179–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Watz, J.; Nyqvist, D. Interspecific competition among terrestrial slugs. J. Molluscan Stud. 2022, 88, eyac007. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Damas-Moreira, I.; Riley, J.L.; Carretero, M.A.; Harris, D.J.; Whiting, M.J. Getting ahead: Exploitative competition by an invasive lizard. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 2020, 74, 117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. White, E.M.; Wilson, J.C.; Clarke, A.R. Biotic indirect effects: A neglected concept in invasion biology. Divers. Distrib. 2006, 12, 443–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Vanni, M.J.; Lampert, W. Food quality effects on life history traits and fitness in the generalist herbivore Daphnia. Oecologia 1992, 92, 48–57. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Grunicke, F.; Wagner, A.; von Elert, E.; Weitere, M.; Berendonk, T. Riparian detritus vs. stream detritus: Food quality determines fitness of juveniles of the highly endangered freshwater pearl mussels (Margaritifera margaritifera). Hydrobiologia 2023, 850, 729–746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Harrison, X.A.; Blount, J.D.; Inger, R.; Norris, D.R.; Bearhop, S. Carry-over effects as drivers of fitness differences in animals. J. Anim. Ecol. 2011, 80, 4–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Barry, K.L. You Are What You Eat: Food Limitation Affects Reproductive Fitness in a Sexually Cannibalistic Praying Mantid. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e78164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Stahlschmidt, Z.R.; Rollinson, N.; Acker, M.; Adamo, S.A. Are all eggs created equal? Food availability and the fitness trade-off between reproduction and immunity. Funct. Ecol. 2013, 27, 800–806. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Oliveira, R.F.; Bshary, R. Expanding the concept of social behavior to interspecific interactions. Ethology 2021, 127, 758–773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Hudina, S.; Hock, K.; Žganec, K. The role of aggression in range expansion and biological invasions. Curr. Zool. 2014, 60, 401–409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Arnott, G.; Elwood, R.W. Information gathering and decision making about resource value in animal contests. Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 529–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Goubault, M.; Cortesero, A.M.; Poinsot, D.; Wajnberg, E.; Boivin, G. Does host value influence female aggressiveness, contest outcome and fitness gain in parasitoids? Ethology 2007, 113, 334–343. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ancona, S.; Drummond, H.; Zaldívar-Rae, J. Male whiptail lizards adjust energetically costly mate guarding to male-male competition and female reproductive value. Anim. Behav. 2010, 79, 75–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Magellan, K.; Kaiser, H. The function of aggression in the swordtail, Xiphophorus helleri: Resource defence. J. Ethol. 2010, 28, 239–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Sacchi, R.; Coladonato, A.J.; Battaiola, M.; Pasquariello, C.; Buratti, S.; Matellini, C.; Mangiacotti, M.; Scali, S.; Zuffi, M.A.L. Subjective resource value affects aggressive behavior independently of resource-holding-potential and color morphs in male common wall lizard. J. Ethol. 2021, 39, 179–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Archundia, M.; Arce, E. Fighting behaviour in native fish: The Mexican mojarra (Cichlasoma istlanum) wins when confronted with the non-native convict cichlid fish (Amatitlania nigrofasciata). J. Ethol. 2019, 37, 67–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Franco, M.; Arce, E. Invasive Cichlids Display Higher Aggression During Nest Defence Compared to the Native Mexican Mojarra. Ecol. Freshw. Fish 2004, 34, e12815. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Johnsson, J.I.; Akerman, A. Watch and learn: Preview of the fighting ability of opponents alters contest behaviour in rainbow trout. Anim. Behav. 1998, 56, 771–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Elias, D.O.; Kasumovic, M.M.; Punzalan, D.; Andrade, M.C.; Mason, A.C. Assessment during aggressive contests between male jumping spiders. Anim. Behav. 2008, 76, 901–910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Arnott, G.; Elwood, R.W. Assessment of fighting ability in animal contests. Anim. Behav. 2009, 77, 991–1004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. O’Connor, C.M.; Reddon, A.R.; Ligocki, I.Y.; Hellmann, J.K.; Garvy, K.A.; Marsh-Rollo, S.E.; Hamilton, I.M.; Balshine, S. Motivation but not body size influences territorial contest dynamics in a wild cichlid fish. Anim. Behav. 2015, 107, 19–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Peiman, K.S.; Robinson, B.W. Ecology and evolution of resource-related heterospecific aggression. Q. Rev. Biol. 2010, 85, 133–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Grether, G.F.; Losin, N.; Anderson, C.N.; Okamoto, K. The role of interspecific interference competition in character displacement and the evolution of competitor recognition. Biol. Rev. 2009, 84, 617–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Grether, G.F.; Anderson, C.N.; Drury, J.P.; Kirschel, A.N.G.; Losin, N.; Okamoto, K.; Peiman, K.S. The evolutionary consequences of interspecific aggression. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 2013, 1289, 48–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Dufour, C.M.S.; Clark, D.L.; Herrel, A.; Losos, J.B. Recent biological invasion shapes species recognition and aggressive behaviour in a native species: A behavioural experiment using robots in the field. J. Anim. Ecol. 2020, 89, 1604–1614. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Champneys, T.; Genner, M.J.; Ioannou, C.C. Invasive Nile tilapia dominates a threatened indigenous tilapia in competition over shelter. Hydrobiologia 2021, 848, 3747–3762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Bush, J.M.; Ellison, M.; Simberloff, D. Impacts of an invasive species (Anolis sagrei) on social and spatial behaviours of a native congener (Anolis carolinensis). Anim. Behav. 2022, 183, 177–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Grant, J.W.A.; Guha, R.T. Spatial clumping of food increases its monopolization and defense by convict cichlids, Cichlasoma nigrofasciatum. Behav. Ecol. 1993, 4, 293–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Grant, J.W.A. Whether or not to defend? The influence of resource distribution. Mar. Behav. Physiol. 1993, 23, 137–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. McCallum, E.S.; Gulas, S.T.; Balshine, S. Accurate resource assessment requires experience in a territorial fish. Anim. Behav. 2017, 123, 249–257. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Grand, T.C.; Grant, J.W.A. Spatial predictability of food influences its monopolization and defence by juvenile convict cichlids. Anim. Behav. 1994, 47, 91–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Desjardins, J.K.; Hofmann, H.A.; Fernald, R.D. Social Context Influences Aggressive and Courtship Behavior in a Cichlid Fish. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e32781. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Satoh, S.; Nishida, Y.; Saeki, T.; Kawasaka, K.; Kohda, M.; Awata, S. The functional role of sibling aggression and “best of a bad job” strategies in cichlid juveniles. Behav. Ecol. 2021, 32, 488–499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Nelson, K.A.; Collins, S.F.; Sass, G.G.; Wahl, D.H. A response-surface examination of competition and facilitation between native and invasive juvenile fishes. Funct. Ecol. 2017, 31, 2147–2166. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Faria, L.; Cuthbert, R.N.; Dickey, J.; Jeschke, J.M.; Ricciardi, A.; Dick, J.T.A.; Vitule, J.R.S. Non-native species have higher consumption rates than their native counterparts. Biol. Rev. 2025. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Argolo, L.A.; López-Fernández, H.; Batalha-Filho, H.; Affonso, P.R.A.d.M. Unraveling the systematics and evolution of the Geophagus brasiliensis (Cichliformes: Cichlidae) species complex. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 2020, 150, 106855. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. FAO. The State of World Fisheries and Aquaculture. In Sustainability in Action; FAO: Rome, Italy, 2020; pp. 1–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Zambrano, L.; Martínez-Meyer, E.; Menezes, N.; Peterson, A.T. Invasive potential of common carp (Cyprinus carpio) and Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus) in American freshwater systems. Can. J. Fish. Aquat. Sci. 2006, 63, 1903–1910. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Njiru, M.; Okeyo-Owuor, J.B.; Muchiri, M.; Cowx, I.G. Shifts in the Food of Nile Tilapia, Oreochromis niloticus (L.) in Lake Victoria, Kenya. Afr. J. Ecol. 2004, 42, 163–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Atwood, H.L.; Tomasso, J.R.; Webb, K.; Gatlin, D.M. Low-temperature tolerance of Nile tilapia, Oreochromis niloticus: Effects of environmental and dietary factors. Aquac. Res. 2003, 34, 241–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Peña-Mendoza, B.; Gómez-Márquez, J.L.; Salgado-Ugarte, I.H.; Ramírez-Noguera, D. Reproductive Biology of Oreochromis niloticus (Perciformes: Cichlidae) at Emiliano Zapata Dam, Morelos, Mexico. Rev. Biol. Trop. 2005, 53, 515–522. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Getabu, A. Growth parameters and total mortality in Oreochromis niloticus (Linnaeus) from Nyanza Gulf, Lake Victoria. Hydrobiologia 1992, 232, 91–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Martin, C.W.; Valentine, M.M.; Valentine, J.F. Competitive interactions between invasive Nile tilapia and native fish: The potential for altered trophic exchange and modification of food webs. PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e14395. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Linde, A.R.; Izquierdo, J.I.; Moreira, J.C.; Garcia-Vazquez, E. Invasive tilapia juveniles are associated with degraded river habitats. Aquat. Conserv. 2008, 18, 891–895. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Kadry, V.O.; Barreto, R.E. Environmental enrichment reduces aggression of pearl cichlid, Geophagus brasiliensis, during resident-intruder interactions. Neotrop. Icthyol. 2010, 8, 329–332. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Sanches, F.H.C.; Miyai, C.A.; Costa, T.M.; Christofoletti, R.A.; Volpato, G.L.; Barreto, R.E. Aggressiveness overcomes body-size effects in fights staged between invasive and native fish species with overlapping niches. PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e29746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Hsu, Y.; Earley, R.L.; Wolf, L.L. Modulation of aggressive behaviour by fighting experience: Mechanisms and contest outcomes. Biol. Rev. 2006, 81, 33–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Moretz, J.A. Aggression and RHP in the Northern Swordtail Fish, Xiphophorus cortez: The Relationship Between Size and Contest Dynamics in Male–Male Competition. Ethology 2003, 109, 995–1008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Reddon, A.R.; Voisin, M.R.; Menon, N.; Marsh-Rollo, S.E.; Wong, M.Y.; Balshine, S. Rules of engagement for resource contests in a social fish. Anim. Behav. 2011, 82, 93–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Alvarenga, C.M.D.; Volpato, G.L. Agonistic profile and metabolism in alevins of the Nile tilapia. Physiol. Behav. 1995, 57, 75–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Earley, R.L.; Edwards, J.T.; Aseem, O.; Felton, K.; Blumer, L.S.; Karom, M.; Grober, M.S. Social interactions tune aggression and stress responsiveness in a territorial cichlid fish (Archocentrus nigrofasciatus). Physiol. Behav. 2006, 88, 353–363. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Torrezani, C.S.; Pinho-Neto, C.F.; Miyai, C.A.; Sanches, F.H.C.; Barreto, R.E. Structural enrichment reduces aggression in Tilapia rendalli. Mar. Freshw. Behav. Physiol. 2013, 46, 183–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Volpato, G.L.; Bovi, T.S.; de Freitas, R.H.A.; da Silva, D.F.; Delicio, H.C.; Giaquinto, P.C.; Barreto, R.E. Red Light Stimulates Feeding Motivation in Fish but Does Not Improve Growth. PLoS ONE 2013, 8, e59134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Arvigo, A.L.; Miyai, C.A.; Sanches, F.H.C.; Barreto, R.E.; Costa, T.M. Combined effects of predator odor and alarm substance on behavioral and physiological responses of the pearl cichlid. Physiol. Behav. 2019, 206, 259–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Pekár, S.; Brabec, M. Generalized estimating equations: A pragmatic and flexible approach to the marginal GLM modelling of correlated data in the behavioural sciences. Ethology 2018, 124, 86–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Halekoh, U.; Højsgaard, S.; Yan, J. The R Package Geepack for Generalized Estimating Equations. J. Stat. Softw. 2006, 15, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2024. [Google Scholar]
  75. Leese, J.M.; Blatt, T. Sex differences in how territory quality affects aggression in convict cichlids. Integr. Organ Biol. 2021, 3, obab028. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Drescher, J.; Feldhaar, H.; Blüthgen, N. Interspecific Aggression and Resource Monopolization of the Invasive Ant Anoplolepis gracilipes in Malaysian Borneo. Biotropica 2011, 43, 93–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Grabowska, J.; Błońska, D.; Kati, S.; Nagy, S.A.; Kakareko, T.; Kobak, J.; Antal, L. Competitive interactions for food resources between the invasive Amur sleeper (Perccottus glenii) and threatened European mudminnow (Umbra krameri). Aquat. Conserv. 2019, 29, 2231–2239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Le Bourlot, V.; Tully, T.; Claessen, D. Interference versus Exploitative Competition in the Regulation of Size-Structured Populations. Am. Nat. 2014, 184, 609–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Zaviezo, T.; Soares, A.O.; Grez, A.A. Interspecific exploitative competition between Harmonia axyridis and other coccinellids is stronger than intraspecific competition. Biol. Control 2019, 131, 62–68. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Ode, P.J.; Vyas, D.K.; Harvey, J.A. Extrinsic Inter- and Intraspecific Competition in Parasitoid Wasps. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 2022, 67, 305–328. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Griffen, B.D.; Altman, I.; Bess, B.M.; Hurley, J.; Penfield, A. The role of foraging in the success of invasive Asian shore crabs in New England. Biol. Invasions 2012, 14, 2545–2558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Nagelkerke, L.A.J.; van Onselen, E.; Van Kessel, N.; Leuven, R.S.E.W. Functional feeding traits as predictors of invasive success of alien freshwater fish species using a food-fish model. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0197636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Castro, N.; Ros, A.F.H.; Becker, K.; Oliveira, R.F. Metabolic costs of aggressive behaviour in the Siamese fighting fish, Betta splendens. Aggress. Behav. 2006, 32, 474–480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Ros, A.F.H.; Becker, K.; Oliveira, R.F. Aggressive behaviour and energy metabolism in a cichlid fish, Oreochromis mossambicus. Physiol. Behav. 2006, 89, 164–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Parker, G.A. Scramble in behaviour and ecology. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B 2000, 355, 1637–1645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Karplus, I.; Zion, B.; Rosenfeld, L.; Grinshpun, Y.; Slosman, T.; Goshen, Z.; Barki, A. Social facilitation of learning in mixed-species schools of common carp Cyprinus carpio L. and Nile tilapia Oreochromis niloticus (L.). J. Fish Biol. 2007, 71, 1023–1034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Ogura, Y.; Matsushima, T. Social facilitation revisited: Increase in foraging efforts and synchronization of running in domestic chicks. Front. Neurosci. 2011, 5, 11069. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Schrandt, M.N.; Powers, S.P. Facilitation and dominance in a schooling predator: Foraging behavior of Florida Pompano, Trachinotus carolinus. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0130095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Branconi, R.; Garner, J.G.; Buston, P.M.; Wong, M.Y. A New Non-Invasive Technique for Temporarily Tagging Coral Reef Fishes. Copeia 2019, 107, 85–91. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Arnold, D.E. Marking Fish with Dyes and Other Chemicals; US Bureau of Sport Fisheries and Wildlife: Washington, DC, USA, 1966; Volume 10.
  91. Klabukov, I.; Shestakova, V.; Krasilnikova, O.; Smirnova, A.; Abramova, O.; Baranovskii, D.; Atiakshin, D.; Kostin, A.A.; Shegay, P.; Kaprin, A.D. Refinement of animal experiments: Replacing traumatic methods of laboratory animal marking with non-invasive alternatives. Animals 2023, 13, 3452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Chronological order of experimental design. The horizontal bar represents the stages of the experiment and the respective duration. Number 1 marks the beginning of the experiment, when both individuals were placed in the experimental aquarium (neutral arena), initiating the basal stage. Number 2 indicates the introduction of food into the experimental aquarium, marking the start of the feeding stage. Number 3 indicates the end of the experiment. Brown dots represent food pellets. White (corresponding to the dark marking used during the experiment) and red dots on the fish’s dorsal fin represent non-toxic ink marks applied to competitors and focal animals, respectively, before the experiment.
Figure 1. Chronological order of experimental design. The horizontal bar represents the stages of the experiment and the respective duration. Number 1 marks the beginning of the experiment, when both individuals were placed in the experimental aquarium (neutral arena), initiating the basal stage. Number 2 indicates the introduction of food into the experimental aquarium, marking the start of the feeding stage. Number 3 indicates the end of the experiment. Brown dots represent food pellets. White (corresponding to the dark marking used during the experiment) and red dots on the fish’s dorsal fin represent non-toxic ink marks applied to competitors and focal animals, respectively, before the experiment.
Fishes 10 00340 g001
Figure 2. (a) Time (in seconds) taken by the competitors to exhibit the first aggressive behavior involving physical contact (bites, lateral display, or mouth wrestling) to the focal animals across experimental stage; (b) Number of aggressions presented by focal animals against the competitors with interaction between the type of competitor and experimental stage. The dark blue color represents total aggression in the baseline stage and the light blue color represents the total aggression in the feeding stage; (c) Number of aggressions presented by competitors (pearl cichlid as conspecific and Nile tilapia as invasive) to the focal animals categorized by competitor type; (d) Number of aggressions presented by competitors (pearl cichlid as conspecific and Nile tilapia as invasive) to the focal animals by experiment stage. Black asterisks (*) or different letters indicate statistically significant differences (p < 0.05). The upper limit of the bar represents the mean value, while the whiskers denote the standard error.
Figure 2. (a) Time (in seconds) taken by the competitors to exhibit the first aggressive behavior involving physical contact (bites, lateral display, or mouth wrestling) to the focal animals across experimental stage; (b) Number of aggressions presented by focal animals against the competitors with interaction between the type of competitor and experimental stage. The dark blue color represents total aggression in the baseline stage and the light blue color represents the total aggression in the feeding stage; (c) Number of aggressions presented by competitors (pearl cichlid as conspecific and Nile tilapia as invasive) to the focal animals categorized by competitor type; (d) Number of aggressions presented by competitors (pearl cichlid as conspecific and Nile tilapia as invasive) to the focal animals by experiment stage. Black asterisks (*) or different letters indicate statistically significant differences (p < 0.05). The upper limit of the bar represents the mean value, while the whiskers denote the standard error.
Fishes 10 00340 g002
Figure 3. (a) Time (in seconds) taken by focal animals and competitors of each treatment to ingest the first food resource (pellets); (b) Proportion between the number of pellets ingested and the individual weight (in grams) for the observed individual (focal animal or competitor) and the type of competitor. The orange bars represent the ingestion rates of the focal animals, and the red bars represent the ingestion rates of the competitors. Black asterisks (*) or different letters indicate statistically significant differences (p < 0.05). The upper limit of the bar represents the mean value, while the whiskers denote the standard error.
Figure 3. (a) Time (in seconds) taken by focal animals and competitors of each treatment to ingest the first food resource (pellets); (b) Proportion between the number of pellets ingested and the individual weight (in grams) for the observed individual (focal animal or competitor) and the type of competitor. The orange bars represent the ingestion rates of the focal animals, and the red bars represent the ingestion rates of the competitors. Black asterisks (*) or different letters indicate statistically significant differences (p < 0.05). The upper limit of the bar represents the mean value, while the whiskers denote the standard error.
Fishes 10 00340 g003
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Cirillo, L.; Miyai, C.A.; Sanches, F.H.C.; Arvigo, A.L.; Costa, T.M. Eat First, Fight Later: Competitive Advantage of an Invasive Cichlid over a Native Competitor for Food Resources. Fishes 2025, 10, 340. https://doi.org/10.3390/fishes10070340

AMA Style

Cirillo L, Miyai CA, Sanches FHC, Arvigo AL, Costa TM. Eat First, Fight Later: Competitive Advantage of an Invasive Cichlid over a Native Competitor for Food Resources. Fishes. 2025; 10(7):340. https://doi.org/10.3390/fishes10070340

Chicago/Turabian Style

Cirillo, Leonardo, Caio A. Miyai, Fábio H. C. Sanches, Alexandre L. Arvigo, and Tânia M. Costa. 2025. "Eat First, Fight Later: Competitive Advantage of an Invasive Cichlid over a Native Competitor for Food Resources" Fishes 10, no. 7: 340. https://doi.org/10.3390/fishes10070340

APA Style

Cirillo, L., Miyai, C. A., Sanches, F. H. C., Arvigo, A. L., & Costa, T. M. (2025). Eat First, Fight Later: Competitive Advantage of an Invasive Cichlid over a Native Competitor for Food Resources. Fishes, 10(7), 340. https://doi.org/10.3390/fishes10070340

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop