Socio-Cognitive Determinants of Pedestrians’ Intention to Cross on a Red Light Signal: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour
Abstract
:1. Introduction
1.1. Background
1.2. Theory of Planned Behaviour (TPB)
1.3. Extended Theoretical Framework of the TPB
1.4. Present Study
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Participants and Procedure
2.2. Questionnaire Measures
2.2.1. Behavioural Intention
2.2.2. Cognitive Attitude
2.2.3. Affective Attitude
2.2.4. Subjective Norm
2.2.5. Perceived Behavioural Control
2.2.6. Descriptive Norm
2.2.7. Normative Norm
2.2.8. Personal Norm
2.2.9. Habit
2.3. Statistical Analyses
3. Results
4. Discussion
5. Conclusions
Author Contributions
Funding
Institutional Review Board Statement
Informed Consent Statement
Data Availability Statement
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- World Health Organization. Global Status Report on Road Safety 2023; World Health Organization: Geneva, Switzerland, 2023. [Google Scholar]
- National Center for Statistics and Analysis. Pedestrians: 2021 Data (Traffic Safety Facts. Report No. DOT HS 813 458); National Highway Traffic Safety Administration: Washington, DC, USA, 2023.
- Adminaité-Fodor, D.; Jost, G. Safer Roads, Safer Cities: How to Improve Urban Road Safety in the EU; European Transport Safety Council: Brussels, Belgium, 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Lassarre, S.; Papadimitriou, E.; Yannis, G.; Golias, J. Measuring Accident Risk Exposure for Pedestrians in Different Micro-Environments. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2007, 39, 1226–1238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hölzl, R.; Steckhan, L.; Lehsing, C.; Savage, S.W.; Bowers, A.R. Driving with Hemianopia VIII: Effects of a Vibro-Tactile Assistance System on Safety and Gaze Behavior in Pedestrian Crossing Situations. Safety 2021, 7, 18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mikusova, M.; Wachnicka, J.; Zukowska, J. Research on the Use of Mobile Devices and Headphones on Pedestrian Crossings—Pilot Case Study from Slovakia. Safety 2021, 7, 17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bhagavathula, R.; Gibbons, R.B. Lighting Strategies to Increase Nighttime Pedestrian Visibility at Midblock Crosswalks. Sustainability 2023, 15, 1455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tiwari, G.; Mohan, D.; Fazio, J. Conflict Analysis for Prediction of Fatal Crash Locations in Mixed Traffic Streams. Accid. Anal. Prev. 1998, 30, 207–215. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Park, S.-H.; Bae, M.-K. Effects Influencing Pedestrian–Vehicle Crash Frequency by Severity Level: A Case Study of Seoul Metropolitan City, South Korea. Safety 2020, 6, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Díaz, E.M. Theory of Planned Behavior and Pedestrians’ Intentions to Violate Traffic Regulations. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2002, 5, 169–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Preusser, D.F.; Wells, J.K.; Williams, A.F.; Weinstein, H.B. Pedestrian Crashes in Washington, DC and Baltimore. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2002, 34, 703–710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, D.; Norman, P. Understanding Pedestrians’ Road Crossing Decisions: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Health Educ. Res. 1998, 13, 481–489. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, D.; Norman, P. Predicting Adolescent Pedestrians’ Road-Crossing Intentions: An Application and Extension of the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Health Educ. Res. 2003, 18, 267–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rosenbloom, T. Crossing at a Red Light: Behaviour of Individuals and Groups. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2009, 12, 389–394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wu, Y.; Guo, Y.; Yin, W. Real Time Safety Model for Pedestrian Red-Light Running at Signalized Intersections in China. Sustainability 2021, 13, 1695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Koh, P.P.; Wong, Y.D. Gap Acceptance of Violators at Signalised Pedestrian Crossings. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2014, 62, 178–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- King, M.J.; Soole, D.; Ghafourian, A. Illegal Pedestrian Crossing at Signalised Intersections: Incidence and Relative Risk. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2009, 41, 485–490. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gårder, P. Pedestrian Safety at Traffic Signals: A Study Carried out with the Help of a Traffic Conflicts Technique. Accid. Anal. Prev. 1989, 21, 435–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Räsänen, M.; Lajunen, T.; Alticafarbay, F.; Aydin, C. Pedestrian Self-Reports of Factors Influencing the Use of Pedestrian Bridges. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2007, 39, 969–973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Khatoon, M.; Tiwari, G.; Chatterjee, N. Impact of Grade Separator on Pedestrian Risk Taking Behavior. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2013, 50, 861–870. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Demiroz, Y.I.; Onelcin, P.; Alver, Y. Illegal Road Crossing Behavior of Pedestrians at Overpass Locations: Factors Affecting Gap Acceptance, Crossing Times and Overpass Use. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2015, 80, 220–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Singh, P.; Pasha, J.; Khorram-Manesh, A.; Goniewicz, K.; Roshani, A.; Dulebenets, M.A. A Holistic Analysis of Train-Vehicle Accidents at Highway-Rail Grade Crossings in Florida. Sustainability 2021, 13, 8842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Şimşekoğlu, Ö.; Lajunen, T. Social Psychology of Seat Belt Use: A Comparison of Theory of Planned Behavior and Health Belief Model. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2008, 11, 181–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Quine, L.; Rutter, D.R.; Arnold, L. Predicting and Understanding Safety Helmet Use among Schoolboy Cyclists: A Comparison of the Theory of Planned Behaviour and the Health Belief Model. Psychol. Health 1998, 13, 251–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lajunen, T.; Räsänen, M. Can Social Psychological Models Be Used to Promote Bicycle Helmet Use among Teenagers? A Comparison of the Health Belief Model, Theory of Planned Behavior and the Locus of Control. J. Safety Res. 2004, 35, 115–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Norman, P.; Conner, P. Predicting Health Behaviour: A Social Cognition Approach. Predict. Health Behav. 2005, 1, 17–18. [Google Scholar]
- Ajzen, I. From Intentions to Actions: A Theory of Planned Behavior. In Action Control: From Cognition to Behavior; Kuhl, J., Beckmann, J., Eds.; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 1985; pp. 11–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ajzen, I. Attitudes, Traits, and Actions: Dispositional Prediction of Behavior in Personality and Social Psychology; Berkowitz, L., Ed.; Advances in Experimental Social Psychology; Academic Press: Cambridge, MA, USA, 1987; Volume 20, pp. 1–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ajzen, I. The Theory of Planned Behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ajzen, I.; Fishbein, M. Understanding Attitudes and Predicting Social Behavior; Prentice-Hall: Englewood Cliffs, NJ, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
- Trafimow, D.; Sheeran, P. Some Tests of the Distinction between Cognitive and Affective Beliefs. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 34, 378–397. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lawton, R.; Conner, M.; Parker, D. Beyond Cognition: Predicting Health Risk Behaviors from Instrumental and Affective Beliefs. Health Psychol. 2007, 26, 259–267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conner, M.; Godin, G.; Sheeran, P.; Germain, M. Some Feelings Are More Important: Cognitive Attitudes, Affective Attitudes, Anticipated Affect, and Blood Donation. Health Psychol. 2013, 32, 264–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conner, M.; McEachan, R.; Taylor, N.; O’Hara, J.; Lawton, R. Role of Affective Attitudes and Anticipated Affective Reactions in Predicting Health Behaviors. Health Psychol. 2015, 34, 642–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- McEachan, R.R.C.; Conner, M.; Taylor, N.J.; Lawton, R.J. Prospective Prediction of Health-Related Behaviours with the Theory of Planned Behaviour: A Meta-Analysis. Health Psychol. Rev. 2011, 5, 97–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- White, K.M.; Smith, J.R.; Terry, D.J.; Greenslade, J.H.; McKimmie, B.M. Social Influence in the Theory of Planned Behaviour: The Role of Descriptive, Injunctive, and in-Group Norms. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2009, 48, 135–158. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parker, D.; Manstead, A.S.R.; Stradling, S.G.; Reason, J.T.; Baxter, J.S. Intention to Commit Driving Violations: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior. J. Appl. Psychol. 1992, 77, 94–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Parker, D.; Manstead, A.S.R.; Stradling, S.G. Extending the Theory of Planned Behaviour: The Role of Personal Norm. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 1995, 34, 127–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Forward, S.E. The Theory of Planned Behaviour: The Role of Descriptive Norms and Past Behaviour in the Prediction of Drivers’ Intentions to Violate. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2009, 12, 198–207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xu, Y.; Li, Y.; Zhang, F. Pedestrians’ Intention to Jaywalk: Automatic or Planned? A Study Based on a Dual-Process Model in China. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2013, 50, 811–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Le, L.V.; Nguyen, L.X.; Chu, M.C.; Huynh, N. Personality Traits Affecting Risky Riding Behavior: An Application of an Extended Theory of Planned Behavior. Sustainability 2023, 15, 16586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhou, R.; Horrey, W.J.; Yu, R. The Effect of Conformity Tendency on Pedestrians’ Road-Crossing Intentions in China: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behavior. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2009, 41, 491–497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhou, H.; Romero, S.B.; Qin, X. An Extension of the Theory of Planned Behavior to Predict Pedestrians’ Violating Crossing Behavior Using Structural Equation Modeling. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2016, 95, 417–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Suo, Q.; Zhang, D. Psychological Differences toward Pedestrian Red Light Crossing between University Students and Their Peers. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0148000. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Xiao, Y.; Liu, Y.; Liang, Z. Study on Road-Crossing Violations among Young Pedestrians Based on the Theory of Planned Behavior. J. Adv. Transp. 2021, 2021, 6893816. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- De Pelsmacker, P.; Janssens, W. The Effect of Norms, Attitudes and Habits on Speeding Behavior: Scale Development and Model Building and Estimation. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2007, 39, 6–15. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jovanović, D.; Šraml, M.; Matović, B.; Mićić, S. An Examination of the Construct and Predictive Validity of the Self-Reported Speeding Behavior Model. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2017, 99, 66–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Eagly, A.H.; Chaiken, S. The Psychology of Attitudes; Harcourt Brace Jovanovich College Publishers: Orlando, FL, USA, 1993; p. 22. [Google Scholar]
- Triandis, H.C. Interpersonal Behavior; Brooks/Cole Pub. Co.: Monterey, CA, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Aarts, H.; Verplanken, B.; van Knippenberg, A. Predicting Behavior from Actions in the Past: Repeated Decision Making or a Matter of Habit? J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 28, 1355–1374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gardner, B.; Abraham, C.; Lally, P.; de Bruijn, G.-J. Towards Parsimony in Habit Measurement: Testing the Convergent and Predictive Validity of an Automaticity Subscale of the Self-Report Habit Index. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2012, 9, 102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Beck, L.; Ajzen, I. Predicting Dishonest Actions Using the Theory of Planned Behavior. J. Res. Personal. 1991, 25, 285–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conner, M.; McMillan, B. Interaction Effects in the Theory of Planned Behavior: Studying Cannabis Use. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 1999, 38, 195–222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Field, A.P. Discovering Statistics Using IBM SPSS Statistics, 5th ed.; Sage edge; SAGE Publications: London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Hutcheson, G. The Multivariate Social Scientist; SAGE Publications, Ltd.: London, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kline, R.B.; Santor, D.A. Principles & Practice of Structural Equation Modelling. Can. Psychol. 1999, 40, 381. [Google Scholar]
- Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences; Elsevier Science: Burlington, NJ, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Johnson, J.W. A Heuristic Method for Estimating the Relative Weight of Predictor Variables in Multiple Regression. Multivar. Behav. Res. 2000, 35, 1–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tonidandel, S.; LeBreton, J.M. Relative Importance Analysis: A Useful Supplement to Regression Analysis. J. Bus. Psychol. 2011, 26, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Holland, C.; Hill, R. The Effect of Age, Gender and Driver Status on Pedestrians’ Intentions to Cross the Road in Risky Situations. Accid. Anal. Prev. 2007, 39, 224–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Zhou, R.; Horrey, W.J. Predicting Adolescent Pedestrians’ Behavioral Intentions to Follow the Masses in Risky Crossing Situations. Transp. Res. Part F Traffic Psychol. Behav. 2010, 13, 153–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Trafimow, D.; Sheeran, P.; Lombardo, B.; Finlay, K.A.; Brown, J.; Armitage, C.J. Affective and Cognitive Control of Persons and Behaviours. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2004, 43 Pt 2, 207–224. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Loewenstein, G.F.; Weber, E.U.; Hsee, C.K.; Welch, N. Risk as Feelings. Psychol. Bull. 2001, 127, 267–286. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cialdini, R.B.; Reno, R.R.; Kallgren, C.A. A Focus Theory of Normative Conduct: Recycling the Concept of Norms to Reduce Littering in Public Places. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 1990, 58, 1015–1026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Deutsch, M.; Gerard, H.B. A Study of Normative and Informational Social Influences upon Individual Judgment. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 1955, 51, 629–636. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rivis, A.; Sheeran, P. Descriptive Norms as an Additional Predictor in the Theory of Planned Behaviour: A Meta-Analysis. Curr. Psychol. J. Diverse Perspect. Diverse Psychol. Issues 2003, 22, 218–233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Conner, M.; Armitage, C.J. Extending the Theory of Planned Behavior: A Review and Avenues for Further Research. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 1998, 28, 1429–1464. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Godin, G.; Conner, M.; Sheeran, P. Bridging the Intention–Behaviour Gap: The Role of Moral Norm. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 2005, 44, 497–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ajzen, I. Residual Effects of Past on Later Behavior: Habituation and Reasoned Action Perspectives. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2002, 6, 107–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bamberg, S.; Ajzen, I.; Schmidt, P. Choice of Travel Mode in the Theory of Planned Behavior: The Roles of Past Behavior, Habit, and Reasoned Action. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 25, 175–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gardner, B.; de Bruijn, G.-J.; Lally, P. A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Applications of the Self-Report Habit Index to Nutrition and Physical Activity Behaviours. Ann. Behav. Med. Publ. Soc. Behav. Med. 2011, 42, 174–187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Criterion | Percentage of Sample | Percentage of Population |
---|---|---|
Gender | ||
Male | 43.6 | 46.5 |
Female | 56.4 | 53.5 |
Age | ||
18–24 | 12.0 | 11.2 |
25–34 | 24.8 | 18.5 |
35–44 | 22.2 | 18.0 |
45–54 | 17.8 | 20.9 |
55–64 | 8.6 | 14.0 |
65+ | 14.6 | 17.4 |
Item | Rotated Factor Loadings | ||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
SN | PN | NN | PBC | Hbt | Int | DN | CA | AA | |
Respecting a ‘Don’t walk’ signal makes me nervous. | −0.068 | 0.144 | 0.012 | −0.083 | 0.097 | 0.098 | 0.048 | −0.210 | 0.652 |
Respecting a ‘Don’t walk’ signal is monotonous. | −0.005 | −0.150 | −0.087 | 0.003 | −0.044 | −0.166 | −0.035 | 0.035 | 0.885 |
Respecting a ‘Don’t walk’ signal irritates me. | −0.015 | 0.103 | 0.035 | −0.024 | 0.057 | 0.035 | −0.003 | −0.055 | 0.788 |
Crossing on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal is reckless. | 0.034 | 0.043 | −0.077 | 0.059 | 0.056 | 0.136 | −0.030 | 0.826 | −0.085 |
Crossing on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal is dangerous, even when it’s done carefully. | −0.026 | −0.011 | 0.042 | −0.077 | −0.017 | −0.072 | 0.043 | 0.846 | −0.042 |
Crossing on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal increases the risk of partaking in a road accident. | −0.004 | 0.066 | −0.003 | −0.008 | 0.033 | 0.071 | 0.028 | 0.855 | −0.020 |
My best friends think that I should cross on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal. | 0.727 | 0.031 | 0.021 | −0.119 | −0.119 | 0.004 | 0.084 | −0.043 | 0.011 |
My classmates/colleagues think that I should cross on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal. | 0.843 | −0.050 | −0.039 | 0.021 | 0.008 | −0.066 | 0.040 | −0.037 | 0.020 |
My partner/spouse thinks that I should cross on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal. | 0.591 | 0.091 | −0.165 | 0.108 | 0.305 | 0.153 | 0.028 | −0.026 | 0.025 |
My parents/children think that I should cross on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal. | 0.685 | −0.014 | 0.096 | 0.018 | 0.011 | 0.118 | −0.125 | 0.040 | −0.160 |
Most people that are important to me think that I should cross on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal. | 0.863 | −0.058 | 0.153 | −0.045 | −0.052 | −0.244 | 0.018 | 0.061 | 0.031 |
How often do your best friends cross on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal? | 0.080 | −0.016 | −0.071 | −0.024 | −0.045 | 0.147 | 0.797 | −0.075 | −0.103 |
How often do your classmates/colleagues cross on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal? | 0.023 | −0.055 | −0.033 | 0.036 | −0.021 | 0.139 | 0.827 | −0.079 | −0.094 |
How often do other pedestrians cross on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal? | −0.053 | 0.035 | −0.072 | 0.062 | −0.029 | −0.036 | 0.820 | 0.086 | 0.064 |
Most people in your city do not comply with the ‘Don’t walk’ signal. | 0.005 | 0.034 | 0.167 | 0.001 | 0.098 | −0.312 | 0.609 | 0.137 | 0.140 |
If I crossed on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal, I would regret it afterwards. | −0.111 | 0.791 | 0.180 | −0.083 | −0.117 | −0.020 | 0.072 | −0.006 | −0.049 |
I would feel guilty if I crossed on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal. | −0.018 | 0.777 | −0.094 | 0.015 | 0.108 | −0.001 | 0.003 | −0.073 | −0.009 |
Crossing on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal violates my principles. | 0.108 | 0.716 | −0.169 | −0.010 | −0.013 | 0.029 | −0.091 | 0.098 | 0.084 |
I would feel really bad if I crossed on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal. | 0.014 | 0.571 | −0.041 | −0.039 | −0.025 | −0.130 | 0.024 | 0.190 | −0.069 |
I have a strong personal obligation not to cross on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal. | −0.041 | 0.522 | 0.144 | 0.183 | −0.068 | −0.133 | −0.017 | 0.024 | 0.078 |
It is more important to cross the road when other pedestrians do it than to respect a ‘Don’t walk’ signal. | 0.025 | 0.148 | 0.722 | −0.077 | 0.071 | 0.142 | −0.016 | −0.111 | −0.177 |
I cross the road on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal when I see other pedestrians doing it. | 0.014 | −0.094 | 0.687 | 0.099 | 0.005 | 0.146 | −0.086 | 0.068 | 0.138 |
When a ‘Don’t walk’ signal is on, I often rely on other pedestrians’ choices, and I act as they do. | −0.007 | −0.077 | 0.718 | 0.026 | 0.093 | −0.030 | 0.062 | 0.055 | 0.039 |
Usually, when a ‘Don’t walk’ signal is on, pedestrians around me are the ones who decide if we are going to cross or not. | 0.084 | 0.012 | 0.838 | 0.027 | −0.039 | −0.039 | −0.038 | −0.039 | −0.036 |
Crossing on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal is something I do automatically. | 0.180 | 0.003 | 0.016 | 0.040 | 0.476 | 0.070 | −0.057 | 0.024 | 0.114 |
Crossing on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal is something I do without consciously remembering doing so. | −0.048 | −0.056 | −0.007 | −0.021 | 0.807 | 0.014 | 0.001 | 0.056 | 0.064 |
Crossing on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal is something I do without thinking. | −0.038 | −0.080 | 0.051 | −0.082 | 0.862 | −0.179 | −0.014 | 0.033 | −0.040 |
Crossing on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal is something I start doing before I realize I’m doing it. | −0.009 | 0.083 | 0.058 | 0.056 | 0.697 | 0.175 | 0.018 | −0.035 | −0.021 |
How easy or difficult would it be for you to respect a ‘Don’t walk’ signal when you are in a hurry? | −0.015 | 0.058 | −0.011 | 0.769 | 0.101 | −0.058 | 0.084 | −0.087 | −0.061 |
How easy or difficult would it be for you to respect a ‘Don’t walk’ signal when there are no vehicles in the vicinity? | −0.146 | −0.023 | 0.148 | 0.749 | 0.049 | −0.087 | 0.016 | 0.020 | −0.037 |
How easy or difficult would it be for you to respect a ‘Don’t walk’ signal when you are excited or nervous? | 0.065 | 0.018 | −0.099 | 0.792 | −0.186 | 0.141 | −0.075 | 0.076 | 0.133 |
How easy or difficult would it be for you to respect a ‘Don’t walk’ signal when the weather is bad (rain, snow, etc.)? | 0.035 | −0.046 | 0.022 | 0.749 | −0.022 | −0.051 | 0.055 | −0.029 | −0.105 |
How often, in the next two weeks, do you intend to cross the road on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal? | −0.009 | −0.041 | 0.149 | −0.050 | 0.016 | 0.592 | 0.112 | 0.044 | 0.106 |
I will try not to cross the road on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal in the future. (reverse coded) | −0.027 | 0.036 | 0.013 | 0.017 | 0.035 | 0.794 | −0.078 | −0.090 | −0.275 |
I think that in the future I will cross the road when a ‘Don’t walk’ signal is on. | 0.107 | 0.016 | 0.127 | 0.074 | −0.130 | 0.630 | −0.006 | 0.025 | 0.236 |
My intention of not crossing the road on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal in the future is high. (reverse coded) | −0.143 | −0.168 | −0.077 | 0.004 | 0.054 | 0.789 | 0.021 | 0.150 | 0.037 |
During the following two weeks, how probable is it that you will cross the road on a ‘Don’t walk’ signal? | 0.015 | −0.048 | 0.093 | −0.166 | −0.059 | 0.633 | 0.050 | 0.082 | 0.060 |
Eigenvalues | 8.83 | 3.02 | 2.30 | 2.20 | 1.83 | 1.43 | 1.38 | 1.22 | 1.03 |
% of variance | 23.86 | 8.18 | 6.22 | 5.95 | 4.95 | 3.87 | 3.72 | 3.29 | 2.77 |
Cronbach’s alpha | 0.815 | 0.783 | 0.808 | 0.785 | 0.760 | 0.803 | 0.763 | 0.804 | 0.722 |
M | SD | CA | SN | PBC | AA | DN | NN | PN | Hbt | Int | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
CA | 5.79 | 1.41 | - | ||||||||
SN | 1.92 | 1.02 | −0.25 ** | - | |||||||
PBC | 4.15 | 1.51 | 0.17 ** | −0.09 | - | ||||||
AA | 3.00 | 1.51 | −0.11 * | 0.26 ** | −0.21 ** | - | |||||
DN | 3.96 | 1.28 | 0.01 | 0.32 ** | −0.04 | 0.16 ** | - | ||||
NN | 2.08 | 1.13 | −0.19 ** | 0.37 ** | −0.20 ** | 0.37 ** | 0.16 ** | - | |||
PN | 4.36 | 1.49 | 0.30 ** | −0.19 ** | 0.29 ** | −0.35 ** | −0.16 ** | −0.28 ** | - | ||
Hbt | 2.31 | 1.33 | −0.17 ** | 0.31 ** | −0.16 ** | 0.35 ** | 0.16 ** | 0.44 ** | −0.19 ** | - | |
Int | 2.27 | 1.12 | −0.32 ** | 0.44 ** | −0.29 ** | 0.43 ** | 0.23 ** | 0.46 ** | −0.50 ** | 0.42 ** | - |
Predictor | β (Step 1) | β (Step 2) | β (Step 3) | β (Step 4) | β (Step5) | β (Step 6) | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
TPB components | CA | −0.19 ** | −0.19 ** | −0.20 ** | −0.18 ** | −0.12 ** | −0.11 ** |
SN | 0.37 ** | 0.30 ** | 0.27 ** | 0.22 ** | 0.22 ** | 0.21 ** | |
PBC | −0.22 ** | −0.16 ** | −0.16 ** | −0.14 ** | −0.09 * | −0.08 * | |
Additional components | AA | 0.30 ** | 0.29 ** | 0.23 ** | 0.16 ** | 0.14 ** | |
DN | 0.09 * | 0.09 * | 0.06 | 0.05 | |||
NN | 0.22 ** | 0.19 ** | 0.15 ** | ||||
PN | −0.28 ** | −0.28 ** | |||||
Hbt | 0.15 ** | ||||||
R2 | 0.29 | 0.37 | 0.38 | 0.41 | 0.47 | 0.49 | |
R2 adjusted | 0.28 | 0.36 | 0.37 | 0.40 | 0.46 | 0.47 | |
ΔR2 | 0.29 ** | 0.08 ** | 0.01 * | 0.03 ** | 0.06 ** | 0.02 ** |
RW | Percentage | |
---|---|---|
Personal norm | 0.12 | 24.6 |
Subjective norm | 0.08 | 16.7 |
Normative norm | 0.07 | 14.6 |
Affective attitude | 0.06 | 13.3 |
Habit | 0.06 | 12.8 |
Cognitive attitude | 0.04 | 8.3 |
Perceived behavioural control | 0.03 | 6.0 |
Descriptive norm | 0.02 | 3.8 |
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content. |
© 2024 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Matović, B.; Petrović, A.; Damjanović, M.; Bulajić, A.; Ilić, V. Socio-Cognitive Determinants of Pedestrians’ Intention to Cross on a Red Light Signal: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Safety 2024, 10, 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/safety10010033
Matović B, Petrović A, Damjanović M, Bulajić A, Ilić V. Socio-Cognitive Determinants of Pedestrians’ Intention to Cross on a Red Light Signal: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Safety. 2024; 10(1):33. https://doi.org/10.3390/safety10010033
Chicago/Turabian StyleMatović, Boško, Aleksandra Petrović, Milanko Damjanović, Aleksandar Bulajić, and Vladimir Ilić. 2024. "Socio-Cognitive Determinants of Pedestrians’ Intention to Cross on a Red Light Signal: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour" Safety 10, no. 1: 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/safety10010033
APA StyleMatović, B., Petrović, A., Damjanović, M., Bulajić, A., & Ilić, V. (2024). Socio-Cognitive Determinants of Pedestrians’ Intention to Cross on a Red Light Signal: An Application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour. Safety, 10(1), 33. https://doi.org/10.3390/safety10010033