Next Article in Journal
The Influence of User-Adapted, Instructive Information on Participation in a Recycling Scheme: A Case Study in a Medium-Sized Swedish City
Previous Article in Journal
Improving Fly Ash Brightness with Carbon and Iron Oxide Removal
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Household Waste Sorting Participation in Developing Countries—A Meta-Analysis

Swedish Centre for Resource Recovery, University of Borås, 501 90 Borås, Sweden
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Recycling 2020, 5(1), 6; https://doi.org/10.3390/recycling5010006
Submission received: 10 December 2019 / Revised: 23 January 2020 / Accepted: 23 March 2020 / Published: 24 March 2020

Abstract

:
Given the increasing efforts at improving waste management in developing countries, this study aimed to analyze factors that influence participation in household waste sorting. It thereby is the first review that extends the published literature on this topic. A meta-analysis was conducted that analyzed twelve influencing factors. A moderate correlation was found for the most strongly influential factors—attitude, moral norm, subjective norm and perceived behavior control—which indicates that people’s perception of waste sorting is most influencing in prompting participation in household waste sorting in developing countries. The results of this meta-analysis indicate that knowledge, situational factors, such as physical conditions, and governmental incentives can influence participation in household waste sorting in developing countries but the relationship between those factors and other factors with high correlations should be studied further. Notably, socio-demographic factors have the weakest influence on the participation in waste sorting in developing countries despite a large body of research on such factors. It can be constructive to take the relationship across the identified factors and the participation in waste sorting into consideration when aiming to implement measures to increase the participation in waste management schemes through waste sorting. The outcome of this study may contribute to recommendations and policy suggestions regarding the promotion of sustainable waste management through household waste sorting in developing countries.

1. Introduction

Global economic development has accelerated in recent decades and is expected to grow starkly in the future [1]. Simultaneously, the world population is growing and is expected to exceed 8 billion by 2024 and up to 11.2 billion by 2100 [2]. Hand in hand with these developments goes an incredible increase in the generation of municipal solid waste (MSW) [3]. Currently, the global MSW generation is 2.01 billion tons annually, which is predicted to increase by 70% to 3.4 billion tons by 2050 unless action is taken [4].
Internationally recognized classifications for the development of a sovereign state include Gross National Income [5], as well as Human Development Index, Gross Domestic Product, political stability, industrialization and freedom [6]. Countries can thus also be classified as threshold, low income, lower-middle income, upper-middle income or high income countries depending on the Gross National Income [7]. In the context of this meta-analysis, an article was considered to be on a developing country when the country the study took place in had a Human Development Index of less than 0.8 [6] in the United Nations Human Development Report [8] and a Gross Domestic Product that ranged from less than USD 4000 to at least USD 25,000 [9,10] at the time when the study was published. Furthermore, countries were considered to be “developing” concerning waste infrastructure provision when, per Kaza et al. [4], one or more of the following aspects were true: more than 62% of the waste was unaccounted for, more than 42% was openly dumped, more than 34% was send to landfills or when there was no data available.
Waste management infrastructures and service provision vary significantly across the countries that are developing by this classification. As an example, the infrastructure coverage may not be consistent. Parts of China, Malaysia and South Africa have established comprehensive and constructive coverage of waste services in some places and cities, while other parts of these countries are not covered and have poor or no waste services at all. In developing countries with high population densities, managing and treating the accumulating amount of waste has become challenging given undeveloped urban waste management systems, lack of relevant technologies or limited available resources for waste management [11,12,13,14,15]. In some countries, insufficient waste management severely impacts both human health and well-being, as well as urban development [16]. For example, uncontrolled burning of waste is the largest source of dioxins, which are among the most toxic chemicals known to threaten human life [17]. Landfilling, in which untreated waste is piled up to heaps or dug into the ground, is still the most common method of MSW disposal worldwide [18]. Landfills, however, are a major source of soil and groundwater contamination and transmittable diseases [16,19]. Many developing countries mainly rely on landfills in managing their waste [4]. MSW is typically landfilled near urban areas, which causes cities to become surrounded by waste gradually. Despite some efforts aimed at improving the status quo of waste management throughout developing countries [20,21], landfills are particularly concerning given the health and environmental impact affiliated with them. Moreover, the ever-increasing volume of waste to be managed in the future is additionally concerning, especially since landfills are already overwhelmed at present [4].
Efficient waste management systems are thus needed in order to improve the current situation and protect human health and the environment, especially so given that many materials in household waste can be recycled, which contributes to the conservation of energy and resources. Waste management systems are often set up in a way in which the goal is to landfill as little as possible and recycle, reduce and reuse as many resources as possible [22]. In order to achieve this, waste management systems need to encourage the public’s participation because people can make a direct impact by making waste sorting part of their everyday routines [23].
The participation in waste sorting is also referred to as “source separation”, which describes the action of sorting waste into its different fractions which can take place in many contexts, such as at factories or industries. The focus of this meta-analysis, however, is on waste sorting within the context of households and by people who form society. In developing countries, the participation in waste sorting can take place as part of recycling schemes that may be provided by the municipality, the private sector or nongovernmental organizations. The way in which waste management infrastructures are set up can have a significant influence on the level of participation in them [22]. In countries that are in the focus of this study, sorted waste may be picked up at the house or householders may be asked to bring their waste to recycling stations or collection points. However, the participation in waste sorting can also take shape in the selling of materials for a livelihood, claiming deposits on recyclable materials such as glass, aluminum or plastic bottles or by using recyclables as a currency to pay for goods [24]. Financial benefits can motivate the participation in sorting by householders and informal collectors alike. However, it is important to address the benefits of the contribution that informal collectors make in regards to enabling higher recycling rates in developing countries, which needs to be taken into account when waste management systems are to be reconstructed or adapted [24].
Waste sorting is a particularly valuable tool to improve waste management and focus has increasingly been directed at efforts to increase waste sorting participation in developing countries [25]. Because household waste sorting occurs at a person’s home, it is linked to individual behavioral patterns. Factors that impact waste sorting participation are thus highly important when aiming to improve the efficiency of waste management by encouraging people to sort the waste in developing countries without integrated waste management systems. Varying factors that impact household waste sorting have been in the focus of previous research across developing countries, but the specific factors those studies focused on and their findings vary. As an example, multiple studies found that there are differences in attitude towards waste sorting between younger and older generations [26,27,28] while others concluded that age does not impact participation in sorting [29]. Social surveys conducted in higher education institutes in developing countries found that sixty percent of surveyed students in Bolivia participate in waste sorting by sorting up to half of their household waste, even though recycling rates are generally low [30]. Whereas, roughly half of the students who participated in a survey in China claimed to sort their waste despite much higher participation rates among their parents (around one in three) [31].
Different research aims and designs may contribute to varying outcomes, but likewise, so can the researchers’ different backgrounds direct the way studies on this subject are concluded. This study aims to meta-analyze the current state of the literature on factors influencing household waste sorting in developing countries. The results of this study can complement the determination of decision-makers and waste management providers that aim to improve waste management system schemes in developing countries. It can also determine which factors need to be investigated further in future research on people’s participation in waste sorting in developing countries.

2. Methods

Given the aim of this study, a meta-analysis is the most appropriate method because a statistical analysis combines the results of multiple scientific studies [32]. By meta-analysis, the number of research samples can be expanded, the performance of statistical tests improved and the reliability of the resulting estimates increased. The results can thus be elaborated rather than narratively reviewed. Furthermore, by conducting a thorough meta-analysis of the factors that influence participation in household waste sorting in developing countries, quantitative conclusions can be drawn to evaluate each factor [32].

2.1. Selection of the Studies

The articles found were screened through the application of the following criteria:
  • Publication date, topic and language: published between 1997 and 2018 and related to factors that influence the participation in waste sorting in developing countries. Articles in English were included
  • Paper format: due to the aim of this meta-analysis, only peer-review publications that included data and results on factors that affect the participation in waste sorting in developing countries were included. Literature reviews and analytical reviews were not considered due to the lack of quantitative datasets required to fulfill the aim of this study. The data quality included means, standard deviations, t and F statistics tests, frequencies or counts, contingency table and Chi-square tests
The stages of the identification of relevant articles are presented in Figure 1.
In order to identify relevant literature, databases were searched using keywords, such as “household waste recycling behavior”; “municipal solid waste + factors”; “factors influencing waste management” and “household waste separation”. Two thousand three hundred and twenty-six articles were so identified. Titles and abstracts were screened in order to ensure the first inclusion criterion was met. Two hundred and eighty-two articles met this requirement and were considered potentially relevant and imported to Endnote. Eighty-eight articles that were cited in potentially relevant articles were additionally considered. A total of 370 articles were analyzed for eligibility and whether they entailed the data (i.e., quantitative datasets, such as means, standard deviations, t and F statistical tests, frequencies or counts, contingency table and Chi-square tests) necessary for the meta-analysis. Thirty-nine articles fulfilled these criteria. Articles that reached the final selection were read in detail and analyzed in their entirety so to ensure the accuracy and validity of the data and therefore suitability for the meta-analysis. No articles were dismissed during this stage, and 39 articles were included in the meta-analysis.

2.2. Data Analysis

The effect size is quantified as the correlation r, between the participation in household waste sorting in developing countries and the factors influencing it. Some of the effect sizes are directly quoted in the source articles, for example, where Pearson coefficients (ESr) or Spearman coefficients are given. Some were calculated on the basis of the original data, such as contingency tables, t-test or F values using statistical calculation. The effect size of all included studies was then computed using the “Fisher Z” transformation [32]. In order to do this, the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis (CMA) 2.0 [33] was used to calculate the overall effect size (correlation r). The average effect size was calculated for each factor by considering the data found in each study to be statistically independent [33]. The degree of influence of correlation (r) was categorized as small (±0.1 to ±0.29), medium (±0.3 to ±0.49) or large (±0.5 to ±1.0) [34].
True variation was considered likely due to the effects of moderator variables, such as the year of publication and country in which the research was conducted. A random-effect model was therefore appropriate for this meta-analysis. It should, however, be noted that this is simply an assumption that required verification. To verify this assumption and to determine the heterogeneity of the studies, the Q test was performed. Using the Q test, the selected model was tested by advancing the hypothesis that all studies have the same true effect size (fixed-effect model). Q follows a central chi-square distribution with df = k−1, where k is the number of studies and df is the degrees of freedom. The p-value was obtained from Q in the chi-square distribution table. If the p-value is less than 0.05, a hypothesis should be rejected, and the random-effect model should be chosen [35]. The Q test showed that the random-effect model was appropriate for all factors in this study. Besides Q test, the I-square test was also conducted to test the homogeneity between studies. According to Higgins, et al. [36], I-square scores higher than 25%, 50% and 75% correspond to low, medium and high heterogeneity, respectively.
Publication bias, which refers to positive results getting published more than negative ones, can be assessed using Rothstein’s failsafe number, Nfs. When Nfs is smaller than 5N + 10 (N = number of studies which discussed this factor), the possible impact of publication bias should be recognized with caution [37]. This coefficient thus refers to the number of studies needed to reverse the conclusion when the summary effect size of the meta-analysis is statistically significant.

3. Results

Through the rigorous literature screening process, 39 studies that assessed factors that influence the participation in household waste sorting in developing countries were identified and included in this meta-analysis. These studies were published between 1997 and 2018 and were conducted in Bangladesh, Malaysia, Iran, Hong Kong, Taiwan, China, Mexico, South Africa, Ecuador, Nigeria, Guinea, Cuba, Vietnam, Ghana, Thailand, Ethiopia and the Philippines.
Except for one study, all included articles met the inclusion criteria at the time of publication and would still do so now. Only the study conducted in Hong Kong [38] met the inclusion criteria because of the HDI/GDI in the year of publication but has since emerged in that regard. Despite its progress regarding the Human Development Index and Gross National Product, 66% of Hong Kong’s waste is still landfilled according to Kaza, et al. [4].
Each included study used self-reporting questionnaires to collect data. Thirty-three of the included studies were based on actual participation in household waste sorting [28,31,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51,52,53,54,55,56,57,58,59,60,61,62,63,64,65,66] while six were based on intentions to participate in it [25,67,68,69,70,71]. All of the included studies were conducted in cities and based on households as samples except for five. Ghani, et al. [68] conducted a survey among the staff of a university campus. Ifegbesan [47] surveyed school students. Ramayah, et al. [58] and Zhang, et al. [31] conducted a survey on students at university campus. Lastly, Tatlonghari and Jamias [60] conducted their survey on households in a village. All but two studies were conducted in the context of long-term recycling programs. The studies by Hernandez, et al. [46] and Xiao, et al. [62] were conducted within the context of pilot recycling programs.
Each article was analyzed systematically in order to categorize the factors that impact participation in household waste sorting in developing countries. Detailed information on the authors, factors discussed, the statistical methodologies used, the countries the studies were conducted in and the sample size of the included articles are presented in Appendix A Table A1.
The three main variables identified are presented in Figure 2. These are (i) governmental incentives and factors; (ii) psychological variables, including perceived behavior control, perceived consequence, attitude, subjective norm and moral norm; and (iii) situational variables, including situational factors, knowledge and the socio-demographic factors, including age, gender, income and education.
The majority of included studies investigated a combination of at least two of these factors. Due to the diverse data collated, some factors had similar meanings but were named differently. On such occasions, these aspects were integrated using the meta-analysis approach employing umbrella terms. The aspects included are described below for each factor along with the respective results.
Table 1 presents the results of the meta-analysis for each factor, including the correlation (r), Q and I-square test and the number of studies that each factor discussed. The factors investigated and included in this meta-analysis as well as the effect-size and 95% CI (confidence interval) calculations are presented in Appendix B Figure A1.
The correlation (r) for governmental incentives and factors is 0.26. The correlations (r) for perceived behavior control and perceived consequence are 0.25 and 0.28 respectively. Moderate correlations were found for factors that are part of this variable: attitude (r = 0.39), moral norm (r = 0.35) and subjective norm (r = 0.30). The correlation (r) found for situational factors is 0.19 and for knowledge it is 0.14. Low correlations were found between the socio-demographic factors of age (r= 0.08), gender (r = 0.11), education (r = 0.09) and income (r = 0.02).
Consulting the chi-square table for the Q-values, I-square values were identified at approximately 90%. The result is in line with the conclusion of the Q test which found that all factors are highly heterogeneous. The result also confirmed the appropriateness of a random effect model for this meta-analysis. The failsafe numbers for all variables are higher than 5N + 10 except for the income variable (failsafe n = 16). Publication bias must thus be considered for the income variable making results on income potentially unreliable. Furthermore, the correlation (r) for knowledge (r = 0.14) and income (r = 0.02) were not statistically significant.

4. Discussion

The results for each variable and factor, as well as remarks, policy implications and limitations of this study will be discussed in the next section.

4.1. Governmental Incentives and Factors

Governmental incentives include financial rewards for people who participate in sorting or penalties when they fail to do so, as well as mandatory regulations and laws. Other aspects that were analyzed within the “governmental incentive” factor are aspects that the included studies focused on and which relate to incentives that governments can influence. These include rewards, market incentives [63], the value of recyclable waste [58], awareness of the benefits of household waste sorting, the cost-value ratio as well as regulations.
The correlation (r) for governmental incentives is 0.26, which indicates a moderate positive influence on household waste sorting participation. Monetary rewards or regulations offered by governments can encourage householders to sort in developing countries. Even though studies in developed countries have concluded that governmental incentives are a significant predictor of participation in waste sorting [72,73,74,75], how and if rewards or penalties increase the participation in sorting is not understood [76].
It is often municipalities who are responsible for the provision of efficient waste management [13], but the organizational, financial, complexity and multidimensionality related issues they face [77] often reach beyond the capacities of municipal authorities [78]. In response, private recycling companies were found in cities across developing countries. These private companies appreciate the value of recyclable materials but their business models also increased the participation in household waste sorting [13]. Government subsidies, promotions and investments in private microenterprises and waste management companies might hence promote higher participation rates in household waste sorting by making waste collection services more affordable [15].
Trust in governmental waste management systems, policies and programs can influence participation in household waste sorting in developing countries. As an example, when the government in Xiamen, China, requested every household to pay 60 RMB biannually towards the improvement of waste management, less than 25% paid the fees. Moreover, when 10–20 communities were chosen every year as waste recycling demonstration communities, the initiative was soon called off for lack of traction [62]. This is consistent with other findings that show that trust and satisfaction with the waste management system and laws introduced by the government have a significant effect on waste sorting participation [79]. Furthermore, increased participation rates can be achieved when people get the chance to take part in decision making regarding the waste management system in which they are supposed to participate [13].
Increased trust and satisfaction, through means such as transparency in the system and in the authorities that offer them may be the most important element to consider when aiming to increase the participation of households in waste sorting. It can be assumed that when trust and satisfaction are improved, the influence of incentives, knowledge-provision and other waste sorting related actions by the government will also be more impactful.

4.2. Psychological Variables

4.2.1. Perceived Behavior Control

Perceived behavior control refers to how individuals perceive their ability to participate in sorting [80,81]. Numerous studies found an influence of perceived behavior control on participation in sorting [56,58,64,67]. Perceived difficulties [25,58,67], perceived lack of recycling facilities [82] and perceived barriers [58] were considered aspects that link to perceived behavior control since they are obstacles to waste sorting and impact the participation in it.
The correlation (r) for perceived behavior control was found to be 0.28. Multiple studies found a high positive relationship between perceived behavior control and participation in waste sorting [56,67]. The result of the factor perceived behavior control of this study is in line with results that show that self-efficacy is influential in household waste sorting [43]. This indicates that the influence that self-efficacy has on the participation in waste sorting in developing countries can be enhanced through means such as supervision and motivation among communities. Because subjective norms among society impact peoples’ perceived behavior control, the public should be provided with detailed information addressing not only the benefits of sorting but also the responsibility carried by every individual [64].
In summary, the results indicate a small to moderate positive relationship between perceived behavior control and participation in waste sorting. Given that perceived behavior control is influenced by the availability of waste facilities, the probability that people participate in waste sorting increases the stronger people perceive satisfaction with the availability, user-friendliness and accessibility of these facilities. This, however, indicates that governments and responsible institutions need to increase their efforts in supplying the population with targeted information about waste sorting in order to reverse the common perception that it is challenging to participate in waste management schemes [25,67].

4.2.2. Perceived Consequence

Perceived consequence refers to a person’s perception of the outcome of participating in sorting. Several aspects were analyzed within the umbrella factor perceived consequences. Among these are the perceived usefulness of participating in waste sorting which refers to a person’s perception of the usefulness of waste sorting as well as consequences of waste sorting. Moreover, parts of the perceived consequence factor are also the relative social benefits that refer to social benefits which can be gained through participation in waste sorting and motives which refer to a person’s choice to participate in sorting and the consequences associated with that choice.
The correlation (r) of perceived consequence is 0.25 which indicates that participation in household waste sorting in developing countries is more likely when it is perceived to have positive consequences. Lacking awareness of the positive outcomes that can be achieved through waste sorting is a significant hindrance to participation in sorting [83]. Because one’s attitude is affected by the perception of outcomes, people are more likely to have positive attitudes after realizing that waste sorting can have positive outcomes. Perceived consequence thus contributes to positive attitudes towards waste sorting [25,61].

4.2.3. Attitude

Attitude describes a person’s favorable or unfavorable evaluation of participation in waste sorting. Attitude can be operationalized in two ways. One of which evaluates the results of waste sorting to determine attitudes [25]. The other more commonly used way evaluates attitudes by focusing on a person’s assessment of whether the participation in waste sorting is favorable or unfavorable while dismissing the consequences of that personal assessment [52,61,64,68,70]. Moreover, attitudes towards the environment [25,49] were also analyzed within the umbrella factor “attitude”.
Attitude is the most significant factor that influences household waste sorting participation in developing countries because positive attitudes towards or being in favor of waste sorting proposedly increase the likelihood of participation in sorting [25,43,49,56,67,69,71]. However, several studies found the contrary and concluded that attitude has no significant correlation with participation in waste sorting [59,64,68,84]. Rather, these studies suggest that participation in sorting tends to be low despite positive attitudes. One explanation for this could be that changing patterns and maintaining altered habits can be challenging when people are used to doing tasks in a certain way [85].
Household waste sorting participation may be low despite positive attitudes for a variety of other reasons, such as aspects like convenience, accessibility of facilities or knowledge. Most study participants in developing countries highlighted the lack of adequate facilities or efficient waste management systems. This unavailability likely discourages people from waste sorting despite positive attitudes. This would be congruent with research that found a lower correlation between actual behavior and attitude than between behavioral intention and attitude [38]. However, a negative correlation of factors with attitude would indicate that there can be participation in waste sorting despite negative attitudes towards it. For example, lower levels of education and income in developing countries can create an incentive to collect recyclable material for resale and financial gain rather than ideological commitments to the environmental benefits of effective waste management.
Environmental concern in this context refers to how much a person cares about the impact participation in waste sorting on the environment and is another aspect of attitude that can influence the participation in waste sorting in developing countries. Part of environmental concern is also environmental attitude, awareness of the consequences of inefficient waste management and environmental consideration.
In developing countries, people might remark that it is difficult to participate in waste sorting due to poor waste infrastructure even though they would like to do so given their environmental concern [49]. This is in line with some of the findings mentioned above and highlights the positive relation with attitudes. It is furthermore proposed that environmental concern influences the participation in waste sorting in developing countries both directly and indirectly [49].
Moreover, strengthening positive public attitudes or changing negative attitudes towards waste sorting without financial means is challenging for governments in developing countries [61]. Nongovernmental organizations and community-based organizations significantly contribute to waste management systems [86,87]. Such organizations can also contribute to strengthening positive public attitudes by involving people from the civil society in designing and implementing waste management systems [88]. Furthermore, nongovernmental and community-based organizations establish cooperation between informal waste collectors as well as private recycling companies with local authorities [86] which can also lead to an increase in positive attitude towards waste sorting among society.
In response to the difficulties governments face when attempting to change public attitudes without financial means, it was suggested that campaigns with constant succinct information can be used as a strategy to alter attitudes when these emphasize the importance of waste sorting [56,68]. However, campaigns are thought to be insufficient in changing attitudes towards waste sorting because they do not sufficiently raise awareness of the importance of waste sorting and do not provide enough knowledge on how to sort [64]. Ways to improve attitudes and participation in household waste sorting may, therefore, include providing knowledge about it and encouraging children early on to sort as part of educational curricula [20,21,28,47]. Furthermore, Ramayah, et al. [58] suggest that governments should encourage the private sector to increase the efficiency of green technologies by investing in them and publicizing such developments among society.
Given that attitude is the most substantial influencing factor in the participation in household waste sorting in developing countries (r = 0.39), participation in waste sorting is thus relatively likely when there are positive attitudes and external conditions facilitating sorting participation.

4.2.4. Subjective Norm

Subjective norm refers to the perception of expectations to participate in waste sorting [64,89]. Part of subjective norms are social norm [66,67,69,70,71], social motivation [25,44,45,48,68,84], social pressure [25,58,67,90] and perceived reputation [58,61], i.e., the way opinions of others, and the importance of those opinions, are perceived in terms of the standing in society.
Given that 19 out of 39 included articles mentioned subjective norm, this is one of the second most investigated factors that influence the participation in household waste sorting in developing countries. In this context, subjective norm refers to a person’s perception of the role social pressure plays in whether or not one participates in waste sorting.
Subjective norms are part of the patterns which engage people to participate in waste sorting in the context of their daily life and the culture they find themselves in. The social pressure that people may perceive from the broader society influences moral norms regarding participation in waste sorting [64], given that people might look at the habits of the broader society before choosing to behave in specific ways [91]. However, subjective norms also center around how that engagement takes place while recognizing the role that culture may play in shaping the significance and expectation of the participation in sorting [23].
Multiple included studies found that subjective norms significantly positively influence participation in household waste sorting in developing countries [66,67,69,70,71]. On the other hand, some studies found that subjective norms less significantly influence the tendency to sort the waste in developing countries [25,44,45,46,68,84] because internally generated motivation is thought to be more likely to lead to participation in sorting than the influence of family, peers or others [68].
The results of this meta-analysis found a correlation (r) of 0.30 for subjective norm, which indicates that it is a moderately influencing factor in the participation of household waste sorting in developing countries. The result is in line with research that suggests that social influences are an influential part of the development of sustained participation in household waste sorting [31,92].
People are more likely to sort if they worry that they will be criticized for failing to do so by members of their family, community or colleagues. However, most of the data that contributed to these results originate from countries in the far East, such as China, Malaysia and Vietnam where cultural structures impact people’s sensitivity to and consideration of how others judge them. In some collectivist countries, people might even consider the opinions of others more important than their own [58].
Nonetheless, there is a positive relationship between subjective norms and perceived difficulties, which indicates that people who face intense social pressure still tend to sort even if the conditions are inconvenient [67]. Subjective norms also affect waste sorting participation indirectly through perceived behavior control [64]. Future research would thus do well to consider the indirect influences of other variables.
In sum, the subjective norm, directly and indirectly, impacts the participation in waste sorting in developing countries with a correlation (r) of 0.30, which is considerable. Targeting subjective norms through social pressure can thus be an effective way to encourage higher participation rates in a waste sorting system.

4.2.5. Moral Norm

Moral norm referred to the perception of the moral obligation to participate in waste sorting [93] and was mentioned in several included pieces of literature as an influential factor [56,61,64,67,70]. Three aspects that relate to the moral norms were analyzed within the umbrella factor “moral norm”. These include moral obligation which refers to a person’s perception of whether it is morally right or wrong to participate in waste sorting [94] as well as the personal norm which refers to a person’s individual opinion of whether or not the participation in sorting is right [64]. Lastly, responsibility is an essential factor that should be considered when evaluating moral norm and it refers to a person’s participation in waste sorting due to a sense of responsibility [58,64].
Nine studies concluded that moral norms profoundly influence waste sorting participation, and some concluded that moral norms are the most important influencing factor [56,64,67,70]. As mentioned earlier, many studies on household waste sorting in developing countries were conducted in the far East. In many eastern cultures, norms are perceived as more important than attitudes. In such cultures, aiming to establish a social-ethical climate through campaigns and education might be worthwhile when targeting waste sorting to become a moral norm [61]. Perceived consequences and responsibility are also key factors that influence moral norms [64], which indicates the need for future research to understand these relationships better. Learning from people who made sorting their waste an everyday routine can aid tailoring campaigns aimed at moral norms. It can do so by providing knowledge and raising awareness about how individual action can improve the environment and human well-being locally and globally [23].
The correlation (r) of moral norm regarding the participation in waste sorting is 0.35, making it the second most impactful factor and leading to the conclusion that targeting moral obligations may be a relevant way to encourage higher participation rates in household waste sorting in developing countries.

4.3. Situational Variables

4.3.1. Situational Factors

Situational factors are physical conditions that encourage or discourage participation in waste sorting. These include convenience, time available for waste sorting, having enough space for waste or even separate waste bins and the cooperation of other residents. Additional situational factors are the type of residential area, distance to a recycling center or waste container and the condition of this waste facility. Moreover, situational factors also include the accessibility or availability of recycling infrastructure, containers and a collection point.
With a correlation (r) of 0.19, situational factors are modestly influential factors that impact household waste sorting in developing countries. The result is in line with research that has shown that participation in waste sorting is more likely when factors such as time to sort, space to accommodate sorted waste and available waste infrastructure are satisfied [53,95,96]. The lack of waste service provision likely keeps people from sorting their waste because people do not know what they are meant to do with their sorted waste. This is why service provision is essential if a waste management system is to succeed.
The factor of space has commonly been discussed as an inconvenience for householders [97,98,99,100] given that separate bins for the different fractions of the waste may take up space in the household. The time it takes to sort the waste is commonly perceived as a factor that discourages participation in waste sorting [97,101]. The provision of easily accessible and nearby waste facilities significantly affects the participation in household waste sorting [102] as well as the number of fractions people tend to sort [13] in developing countries.
Situational factors not only have a direct influence on the participation in waste sorting but they also influence attitudes and perceived behavior control, thereby influencing waste sorting participation indirectly [45,52,58]. As mentioned earlier, regardless of principally positive attitudes to waste sorting, low participation rates can occur when people find it hard to adapt to waste sorting because it is not (yet) a habitual task of daily routines or a learned behavior [103].
Situational factors are likely more influential than the result of this meta-analysis indicates given that only the direct influence was analyzed. However, situational factors also indirectly influence participation in household waste sorting in developing countries. Low participation rates can occur when there are no waste facilities but also when there is no cooperation of others within the household and when the waste management system is inconvenient. That is because these aspects impact both people’s attitudes and their perceived behavior control. Future research might be able to gain a lot of constructive knowledge from studying the indirect influence of situational factors on participation in household waste sorting in developing countries.
The results of this meta-analysis furthermore indicate that it is crucial to efforts at encouraging household participation in waste sorting to focus on the convenience of waste management systems since this strengthens the impact of situational factors on participation as well as the influence of attitudes and perceived behavior control.

4.3.2. Knowledge

Knowledge in this context refers to two critical aspects. One of which is intrinsic knowledge, i.e., pre-existing knowledge. The other aspect is extrinsic knowledge, which a person can gain from external sources, such as in school or university [28,31,47,50], pictures on trash bins and information campaigns [25,28,61,67]. In addition, information and communication were considered part of the factor of knowledge.
The correlation (r) for knowledge is 0.14. Knowledge thus has a positive influence on waste sorting participation. The more intrinsic or extrinsic knowledge a person has, the more probable is participation in waste sorting. Not knowing how to sort or where to bring sorted waste prevents participation in waste sorting [97,104,105]. Insufficient information provided to the public about what resource recovery can do and the difference that waste sorting can make might be a reason for low participation, given that such knowledge may otherwise be an incentive to sort the waste [23]. Given that language can be a barrier, visual and nonverbal communication, such as symbols or color-coded systems, may help instruct how to sort the different fractions of the waste [106]. As an example, the use of stickers in Sweden helped decrease the amount of missorted waste by 70% [107].

4.3.3. Socio-Demographic Factors

Socio-demographic factors are the most commonly studied predictors of participation in waste sorting generally. However, 20 of the 39 included articles studied socio-demographic factors in developing countries making them the least most studied factors contributing to this meta-analysis. Data on income, gender, education and age were analyzed.

Income

The aspect of income is least frequently reported in studies on waste sorting participation in developing countries and is the weakest predictor among the factors considered given its correlation (r) of 0.02. This result is inconsistent with a body of research that indicates that high income and high education typically go hand-in-hand and thus enable environmental knowledge, which in turn increases the likelihood of participation in waste sorting [108,109,110,111,112,113]. However, the majority of these studies were conducted in EU member states rather than developing countries.
It is not uncommon in developing countries for people to participate in waste management by sorting recyclables for economic reasons or even a living [42,114,115,116]. However, low-income areas tend to have inadequate waste facilities and infrastructure, which also prohibits participation in waste sorting [53]. Whether or not people live in an affluent area can also be an important factor in enabling access to waste management services, given that the provision of such services can vary within different areas of a country or even within cities.
Given such conflicting results on the influence of income in the literature, it is perhaps not surprising that the overall result of this meta-analysis fails to identify a correlation between income and the participation in waste sorting.

Gender

Gender is a more frequently studied aspect of waste sorting participation in developing countries. The correlation (r) for gender is 0.11 in which case −1 indicates men and +1 indicate women. Women are thus more likely to participate in waste sorting than men, which is consistent with Zhang, et al. [31] who found that female students in China are more likely to participate in sorting than males [31]. More than half of the literature on household participation in waste sorting in developing countries included in this meta-analysis is based on studies conducted in the East. In many eastern cultures, women are often perceived to be responsible for domestic household work whereas men are perceived as being responsible for work outside of it [117]. When this is the case, women spend more of their time and energy on household work. People who are more involved in running a household are also more likely to acquire knowledge on waste management [50], which may impact their motivation to sort. Given that societies develop towards gender equality, and increasingly, both men and women find employment in developing countries [117], many women might find less time for household chores [118,119]. As part of such developments, preconceptions about who is responsible for the household may shift, and both genders may engage more equally in waste sorting.

Education

The correlation (r) for education is 0.09, which indicates a minor positive influence of the level of education on the participation in waste sorting in developing countries.
Empirical research concluded that education influences waste sorting participation positively because higher education enables better access to environmental knowledge [31,41,47,51,55,56,60,65]. However, other studies found that particularly in developing countries, education does not significantly influence the participation of householders in waste sorting [31] because among the well-educated greater focus is laid on money and status than waste sorting [38,50,54,57,68,120]. Waste sorting is also associated with dirty and unhygienic labor [69] that is predominantly pursued by the poor [121].

Age

Age is the most studied socio-demographic variable studied in developing countries and the correlation (r) for age is 0.08 in which -1 indicates young people and +1 older people. The result indicates that older people are more likely to participate in sorting. Various studies found that all ages are relevant in terms of participation in waste sorting. The younger generation is likely to sort waste due to the attention paid to environmental knowledge provided in schools [42,47]. However, people between the age of 25 and 35 years of age were found to be more likely to sort than their younger peers [28]. However, the older generation is most willing to sort [28,39,41,50,54,56,59].
One explanation for this could be that older people who are about to retire or already are retired have more time which can be used towards waste sorting than people of working age with families have. Another aspect that influences the results on age is likely lack of education due to which younger people in developing countries may be less inclined to sort the waste. However, given the current efforts in educating school children in developing countries about effective waste sorting and the difference it can make, the raised awareness may lead to increased participation of that generation in sorting [28,50].

4.4. Remarks

The results of this meta-analysis show that government incentives and factors that relate to a person’s perception of waste sorting, such as attitude, moral norm, subjective norm, perceived behavior control and perceived consequence are most influential on the participation in waste sorting in developing countries. Whereas situational factors and knowledge are the second most influencing factors and socio-demographic factors are the least influential factors.
The results of the meta-analysis also indicate that people’s perception of waste sorting is the most important factor in developing countries that prompts participation in it. This could, however, be due to the poor conditions that waste management systems often have in developing countries. People who participated in studies in places where the waste management system is dysfunctional may not consider situational factors or knowledge relevant to their participation in waste sorting when they are resentful towards governmental incentives.
Government incentives were found to be influential which indicates that trust and satisfaction with the waste management system and laws introduced by the government have a significant effect on waste sorting participation. Despite having a lesser correlation than other factors, the results of this meta-analysis indicate knowledge and situational factors can nonetheless influence participation in sorting in developing countries. More so, these factors can influence factors that relate to consequences, norms, attitudes and perceptions and thereby prompt participation in waste sorting [28,45,52,58]. However, this indirect influence could not be analyzed in this meta-analysis due to an insufficient amount of quantitative data on it. The socio-demographical factors showed a very small correlation to participation in waste sorting, despite being the most studied factors included in this meta-analysis, which leads to doubts around the constructiveness of increased research efforts on them.

4.5. Policy Implications

In developing countries, it can be constructive to take the relationship across the identified factors and the participation in waste sorting into consideration when aiming to implement measures to increase participation in recycling schemes through waste sorting. Given that behavioral aspects are crucial in prompting participation in waste sorting [122,123,124,125], factors that prevent or discourage people from sorting should be addressed. The results of this meta-analysis showed that these factors that relate to behavior could be psychological, which included attitudes, perceived consequences, motivation and both subjective and moral norms. As such, strategies to increase participation in household waste sorting in developing countries could include knowledge provision as part of educational curricula. Teaching young generations about the importance and benefits of waste sorting can address attitudes and shift behavioral norms among the young society and the next generations to come.
However, it is also important to reach all of society with information, campaigns and strategic efforts that aim to increase participation in household waste sorting in developing countries. More so, user-adapted waste systems make participation in waste sorting likely. They can be achieved by engaging people in both designing waste management and involving them in decision-making processes. Because nongovernmental and community-based organizations already often do this, there should be cooperation among such organizations and local authorities or other waste service providers. Campaigns and practical strategies, such as transparency, employed by governments may strengthen credibility, trust and satisfaction of citizens making it more likely that people will act according to established regulations.
In order to resonate with factors such as attitude, moral norms and perceived consequences, emphasis should be laid on strategically communicating the undesirable ecological and human health-related consequences of mismanaged waste as well as the degradation of natural resources due to nonrecycling in developing countries. Moreover, the important work of informal collectors should be addressed, given the significant contribution they make to waste collection in developing countries. People should be informed that the work of informal collectors and their participation in waste sorting can make a direct difference not only to better living standards for themselves but also for people in their society as well as across the globe. This is because people’s participation in waste sorting aids the correct management of waste, which, in turn, can avoid health issues resulting from uncontrolled dumping [19]. People should also have access to information that highlights that waste sorting enables sustainable waste management which avoids landfills and thereby helps minimize breeding grounds for communicable diseases that can flourish in landfills, improves air pollution and soil quality and avoids the depletion of resources which lowers the impact that the production of new materials has on the climate.
Targeted efforts at increasing the participation in household waste sorting in developing countries should also consider the results of this study in terms of situational variables. In order to address perceived behavior control, information provision on why and how to sort waste may encourage people to trust that they have the ability to participate in waste sorting and sound waste management. Lastly, in order to enhance the convenience of participating in waste management through sorting, the waste management infrastructure should be adapted to the needs of the people that engage in it.

4.6. Limitations

While meta-analyses are a constructive method in concluding multiple studies, sources of uncertainty exist which could direct future research. The homogeneity test verified that a random effect model was most appropriate in this study. However, this can mean that moderator variables could have impacted the final result to some extent through variables such as the year of publication or the country in which a study was conducted. These moderator variables and their impact on the accuracy of the results should be considered in future research. In addition, there is no unified standard questionnaire design regarding each of the factors analyzed in this meta-analysis. Responses thus may have different implications depending on the designs of the questionnaires across the included studies. In addition, the complexities of the waste systems across countries presents an additional challenge in interpreting the findings of this meta-analysis. Furthermore, there is no unified classification of the analyzed factors. Both of these moderator variables may have contributed to inaccuracies in the results. The establishment of unified standards for factors influencing the participation in waste sorting would enhance the assessment of their meaning as well as their comparability in future research.

5. Conclusions

This was the first time that a meta-analysis was conducted with the aim to provide a quantitative assessment of the current state of the literature on factors that influence participation in household waste sorting in developing countries. The results indicate that:
  • Meta-analysis as a method is effective in concluding factors that influence the participation in waste sorting from multiple studies. However, categorizing these factors and their relations to the participation in sorting is both constructive and challenging.
  • The most strongly influencing factors are attitude, perceived behavior control and moral norms, which indicates that people’s perception of waste sorting is most influencing in prompting participation in household waste sorting in developing countries.
  • The results of this meta-analysis indicate that knowledge, situational factors and government incentives can influence participation in household sorting in developing countries but the relationship between those factors and attitude, perceived behavior control and moral and subjective norms should be studied further.
  • Among the most notable results is that even though socio-demographic factors were the most studied factors that contributed to this meta-analysis, they have the weakest influence on participation in waste sorting in developing countries.
The results of this meta-analysis may contribute to recommendations and policy suggestions regarding the promotion of effective waste management aided by household waste sorting in developing countries. Further research can complement such effort further by focusing not merely on either factor that relates to attitudes and perception or on situational factors, knowledge and government incentives but rather the complexity, which is how the relationship among such factors prompts the participation in household waste sorting.

Author Contributions

For research articles with several authors, a short paragraph specifying their individual contributions must be provided. The following statements should be used “conceptualization, L.Z. and K.R.; methodology, L.Z. and K.R.; software, L.Z.; validation, L.Z. and K.R.; formal analysis, L.Z., C.H. and K.R.; investigation, L.Z. and K.R.; resources, L.Z. and K.R.; data curation, L.Z. and K.R.; writing—original draft preparation, L.Z. and C.H.; writing—review and editing, C.H. and K.R.; visualization, C.H., L.Z. and K.R.; supervision, K.R. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript

Funding

The authors are grateful for the financial support from Sparbankstiftelsen Sjuhärd.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest

Appendix A

Table A1. Authors: influencing factors on household waste sorting, statistical analysis and sample sizes and countries studied *.
Table A1. Authors: influencing factors on household waste sorting, statistical analysis and sample sizes and countries studied *.
AuthorFactors Investigated in Meta-AnalysisStatistical AnalysisSample SizeCountry
Afroz, et al. [28]Situational factor (space for storage at home, distance to collection point), age, gender, incomeMean value, t-value456Bangladesh
Akil and Ho [39]AgeMean value, frequency and contingency table670Malaysia
Akil, Foziah and Ho [40]AgeMean value, frequency and contingency table600Malaysia
Babaei, et al. [41]Age, education, genderMean value, frequency and contingency table2400Iran
Chan [38]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective norm, age, education, genderMean value, frequency and contingency table167/173Hong Kong
Chen and Tung [67]Attitude, perceived behavior control (perceived difficulty for lack of recycling facilities), subjective norm, moral norm, perceived consequencesCorrelation r, t-value541Taiwan
Chu and Chiu [25]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective norm, moral norm (ethical and moral aspects in engaging in recycling), perceived consequences (protecting natural resources and reducing societal cost as a consequence for recycling), situational factor (space for storage at home and convenience of recycling facilities), governmental incentives (rewards)Mean vale, correlation r386Taiwan
Chung and Poon [42]Age, education, gender, incomeMean value, Z-value370/738China
Corral-Verdugo [43]Attitude, perceived behavior control, perceived consequencesMean value, correlation r100Mexico
Du Toit, et al. [44]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective normCorrelation r, t-value298South Africa
Du Toit, et al. [45]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective norm, situational factor (space for storage at home and convenience of curbside and collection point schemes)t-value, frequency and contingency table, correlation r290South Africa
Ghani, et al. [68]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective norm, situational factor, age, education, genderFrequency table, correlation r204Malaysia
Hernandez, et al. [46]Attitude, subjective norm, knowledgeMean value, correlation r, t-value410Ecuador
Ifegbesan [47]Knowledge, age, genderMean value, frequency and contingency table, t-value650Nigeria
Janmaimool [48]Subjective norm, moral normMean value, correlation r193Mexico
Jekria and Daud [49]AttitudeCorrelation r, t-value143Malaysia
Li [50]Age, education, gender, incomeMean value, frequency and contingency table, odds ratio391China
Mamady [51]Situational factor (access to recycling schemes in residential area and distance to the collection point, age, education, gender, incomeMean value, frequency and contingency table, odds ratio1093Guinea
Mosler, et al. [69]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective norm, governmental incentives (trust to the government and local authorities)Correlation r, t-value299Cuba
Nguyen, et al. [70]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective norm, moral norm, perceived consequences as waste problem threat health, governmental incentives (rewards and benefit from recycling and trust to local authorities)Frequency and contingency table, correlation r180Vietnam
Nomura, et al. [52]Attitude, moral norm, situational factor, governmental incentivesFrequency and contingency table, correlation r357Vietnam
Omran, et al. [53]Age, education, gender, incomeMean value, frequency and contingency table389Malaysia
Owusu, et al. [54]Age, education, genderMean value, t-value, frequency and contingency table384Ghana
Oyekale [55]Governmental incentives (regulation for recycling), age, education, incomeMean value, frequency and contingency table23518South Africa
Pakpour, et al. [56]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective norm, moral norm, age, educationMean value, frequency and contingency table, correlation r1676Iran
Ying [57]Perceived behavior control (perceived difficulty for recycling), subjective norm, educationMean value, frequency and contingency table, correlation r618China
Ramayah, et al. [58]Attitude, perceived behavior control (cost for recycling), subjective norm, situational factor (availability of recycling infrastructure)Correlation r, t-value200Malaysia
Singhirunnu-sorn, et al. [59]Attitude, knowledge, age, incomeMean value, frequency and contingency table, correlation r151Thailand
Strydom [84]Attitude, behavior control, subjective normMean value, frequency and contingency table, correlation r2004South Africa
Tadesse [120]Attitude, situational factor (access to container for recycling), governmental incentives (regulation for recycling), age, education, gender, incomeMean value, correlation r400Ethiopia
Tatlonghari and Jamias [60]Attitude, knowledge, education, incomeMean value, F-value, correlation r100Philippines
Tamas, et al. [71]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective normCorrelation r, t-value765Cuba
Tang, et al. [61]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective norm, moral norm, perceived consequences, situational factor, knowledgeCorrelation r, t-value765China
Xiao, et al. [62]Subjective norm, knowledge, governmental incentives (rewards and regulation)Mean value, frequency table correlation r712China
Xu, et al. [63]Knowledge, governmental incentives (rewards and regulation)Mean value, Correlation r631China
Yuan, et al. [64]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective norm, moral normMean value, frequency and contingency table, correlation r362China
Zhang, et al. [65]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective norm, moral norm, situational factor, knowledge, age, education, gender, incomeMean value, frequency and contingency table, correlation r208China
Zhang, et al. [66]Attitude, perceived behavior control, subjective normMean value, F-value, correlation r250China
Zhang, et al. [31]GenderMean value, frequency and contingency table509China
* The studies were conducted in cities with households as the source of the samples, except for the following studies: Ghani, et al. [68] conducted a survey among the staff of a university campus, Ifegbesan [47] surveyed school students, Ramayah, et al. [58] and Zhang, et al. [31] conducted a survey on students at university campus, and Tatlonghari and Jamias [60] conducted their survey on households in a village. All studies were conducted in the context of long-term recycling programs, except for two studies by Hernandez, et al. [46] and Xiao, et al. [62] which were within the context of pilot recycling programs.

Appendix B

Figure A1. Factors investigated and included in this meta-analysis. The effect-size and 95% confidence interval (CI) was calculated for each study. The overall synthesized for each factor shows under a group of studies; it is highlighted and underlined.
Figure A1. Factors investigated and included in this meta-analysis. The effect-size and 95% confidence interval (CI) was calculated for each study. The overall synthesized for each factor shows under a group of studies; it is highlighted and underlined.
Recycling 05 00006 g0a1aRecycling 05 00006 g0a1bRecycling 05 00006 g0a1c

References

  1. OECD. Overview: Development Strategies for the 21st century; Organisation for Economic Co-operation and Development: Paris, France, 2019; pp. 1–15. [Google Scholar]
  2. Bongaarts, J. Development: Slow down population growth. Nat. News 2016, 530, 409. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  3. OECD. Municipal Waste. Available online: https://data.oecd.org/waste/municipal-waste.htm (accessed on 4 September 2019).
  4. Kaza, S.; Yao, L.; Perinaz, B.-T.; Van Woerden, F. Appendix B: Waste Treatment and Disposal by Country or Economy. In What a Waste 2.0 - A Global Snapshot of Solid Waste Management to 2050; International Bank for Reconstruction and Development: Washington DC, USA, 2018; pp. 231–257. [Google Scholar]
  5. World Bank Group. Why use GNI per capita to classify economies into income groupings? Available online: https://datahelpdesk.worldbank.org/knowledgebase/articles/378831-why-use-gni-per-capita-to-classify-economies-into (accessed on 15 January 2019).
  6. WPR. Developed Countries List 2020 (HDI). Available online: http://worldpopulationreview.com/countries/developed-countries-list/ (accessed on 15 January 2019).
  7. WorldBank. World Bank Country and Lending Groups. Available online: https://datahelpdesk.worldbank.org/knowledgebase/articles/906519-world-bank-country-and-lending-groups (accessed on 15 January 2019).
  8. UNDP. Human Development Index (HDI). Available online: http://hdr.undp.org/en/indicators/137506 (accessed on 17 January 2019).
  9. Investopedia. Top 25 Developed and Developing Countries. Available online: https://www.investopedia.com/updates/top-developing-countries/ (accessed on 15 January 2019).
  10. WPR. Developing Countries 2020. Available online: http://worldpopulationreview.com/countries/developing-countries/ (accessed on 15 January 2019).
  11. Minghua, Z.; Xiumin, F.; Rovetta, A.; Qichang, H.; Vicentini, F.; Bingkai, L.; Giusti, A.; Yi, L. Municipal solid waste management in Pudong new area, China. Waste Manag. 2009, 29, 1227–1233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Kaza, S.; Yao, L.C.; Bhada-Tata, P.; Van Woerden, F. What a Waste 2.0: A Global Snapshot of Solid Waste Management to 2050; World Bank: Washington, DC, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  13. Guerrero, L.A.; Maas, G.; Hogland, W. Solid waste management challenges for cities in developing countries. Waste Manag. 2013, 33, 220–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  14. Marshall, R.E.; Farahbakhsh, K. Systems approaches to integrated solid waste management in developing countries. Waste Manag. 2013, 33, 988–1003. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Sharholy, M.; Ahmad, K.; Mahmood, G.; Trivedi, R. Municipal solid waste management in Indian cities–A review. Waste Manag. 2008, 28, 459–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. UN. Transforming Our World: The 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development; United Nations General Assembly, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  17. EPA. The Hazards of Backyard Burning; United States Environmental Protection, Ed.; National Service Center for Environmental Publications (NSCEP): Washington, DC, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  18. Millati, R.; Cahyono, R.B.; Ariyanto, T.; Azzahrani, I.N.; Putri, R.U.; Taherzadeh, M.J. Agricultural, Industrial, Municipal, and Forest Wastes: An Overview. In Sustainable Resource Recovery and Zero Waste Approaches; Taherzadeh, M.J., Bolton, K., Wong, J., Pandey, A., Eds.; Elsevier: St. Louis, MO, USA, 2019; pp. 1–22. [Google Scholar]
  19. Bolton, K.; de Mena, B.; Schories, G. Sustainable Management of Solid Waste. In Resource Recovery to Approach Zero Municipal Waste; Taherzadeh, M.J., Richards, T., Eds.; CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA, 2016; pp. 23–40. [Google Scholar]
  20. Susanto, N.; Davidesyta, L.; Nurkertamanda, D.; Putranto, T.T. The influence of behavioral prediction factors and intention in improving 3R (reduce, reuse, recycle) household behavior in Tanjung Mas, Semarang, Indonesia. In Proceedings of the AIP Conference Proceedings; p. 030002.
  21. Ulhasanah, N.; Goto, N. Assessment of citizens’ environmental behavior toward municipal solid waste management for a better and appropriate system in Indonesia: A case study of Padang City. J. Mater. Cycles Waste Manag. 2018, 20, 1257–1272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Rousta, K. Household Waste Sorting at the Source: A Procedure for Improvement; University of Borås: Borås, Sweden, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  23. Hellwig, C.; Häggblom-Kronlöf, G.; Bolton, K.; Rousta, K. Household Waste Sorting and Engagement in Everyday Life Occupations After Migration—A Scoping Review. Sustainability 2019, 11, 4701. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  24. Wilson, D.C.; Velis, C.; Cheeseman, C. Role of informal sector recycling in waste management in developing countries. Habitat Int. 2006, 30, 797–808. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Chu, P.Y.; Chiu, J.F. Factors influencing household waste recycling behavior: Test of an integrated model 1. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 33, 604–626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Meneses, G.D.; Palacio, A.B. Recycling behavior: A multidimensional approach. Environ. Behav. 2005, 37, 837–860. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Knussen, C.; Yule, F.; MacKenzie, J.; Wells, M. An analysis of intentions to recycle household waste: The roles of past behaviour, perceived habit, and perceived lack of facilities. J. Environ. Psychol. 2004, 24, 237–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Afroz, R.; Hanaki, K.; Tuddin, R.; Ayup, K. A survey of recycling behaviour in households in Dhaka, Bangladesh. Waste Manag. Res. 2010, 28, 552–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Kurz, T.; Linden, M.; Sheehy, N. Attitudinal and community influences on participation in new curbside recycling initiatives in Northern Ireland. Environ. Behav. 2007, 39, 367–391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Ferronato, N.; D’Avino, C.; Ragazzi, M.; Torretta, V.; De Feo, G. Social surveys about solid waste management within higher education institutes: A comparison. Sustainability 2017, 9, 391. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Zhang, H.; Liu, J.; Wen, Z.-g.; Chen, Y.-X. College students’ municipal solid waste source separation behavior and its influential factors: A case study in Beijing, China. J. Clean. Prod. 2017, 164, 444–454. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.V.; Higgins, J.P.; Rothstein, H.R. Introduction to Meta-Analysis; John Wiley & Sons: West Sussex, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  33. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.V.; Higgins, J.P.T.; Rothstein, H.R. Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 2; Biostat: Englewood, CO, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  34. Cohen, J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed.; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hilsdale, NJ, USA, 1988. [Google Scholar]
  35. Borenstein, M.; Hedges, L.V.; Higgins, J.P.T.; Rothstein, H.R. Introduction to Meta-Analysis; John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.: West Sussex, UK, 2009. [Google Scholar]
  36. Higgins, J.P.; Thompson, S.G.; Deeks, J.J.; Altman, D.G. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Bmj 2003, 327, 557–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Rothstein, H.R.; Sutton, A.J.; Borenstein, M. Publication Bias in Meta-Analysis: Prevention, Assessment and Adjustments; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  38. Chan, K. Mass communication and pro-environmental behaviour: Waste recycling in Hong Kong. J. Environ. Manag. 1998, 52, 317–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Akil, A.; Ho, C. Towards sustainable solid waste management: Investigating household participation in solid waste management. In Proceedings of the IOP Conference Series: Earth and Environmental Science, Kuching, Malaysia, 26–29 August 2014; p. 012163. [Google Scholar]
  40. Akil, A.M.; Foziah, J.; Ho, C. Households Inclination in Waste Separation Program. Asian J. Qual. Life 2017, 2, 19–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  41. Babaei, A.A.; Alavi, N.; Goudarzi, G.; Teymouri, P.; Ahmadi, K.; Rafiee, M. Household recycling knowledge, attitudes and practices towards solid waste management. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2015, 102, 94–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Chung, S.S.; Poon, C.S. A comparison of waste-reduction practices and new environmental paradigm of rural and urban Chinese citizens. J. Environ. Manag. 2001, 62, 3–19. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Corral-Verdugo, V.C. Dual ‘realities’ of conservation behavior: Self-reports vs. observations of re-use and recycling behavior. J. Environ. Psychol. 1997, 17, 135–145. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. du Toit, J.; Wagner, C.; Fletcher, L. Factors affecting household recycling behaviour in townhouses in Pretoria, South Africa. In Proceedings of the Smart and Sustainable Built Environment (SASBE) Conference Pretoria, Pretoria, South Africa, 2015; pp. 119–126. [Google Scholar]
  45. du Toit, J.; Wagner, C.; Fletcher, L. Socio-Spatial Factors Affecting Household Recycling in Townhouses in Pretoria, South Africa. Sustainability 2017, 9, 2033. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  46. Hernandez, O.; Rawlins, B.; Schwartz, R. Voluntary recycling in Quito: Factors associated with participation in a pilot programme. Environ. Urban. 1999, 11, 145–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Ifegbesan, A. Exploring Secondary School Students’ Understanding and Practices of Waste Management in Ogun State, Nigeria. Int. J. Environ. Sci. Educ. 2010, 5, 201–215. [Google Scholar]
  48. Janmaimool, P. The role of descriptive social norms, organisational norms and personal norms in explaining solid waste management behaviours in workplaces. J. Organ. Chang. Manag. 2017, 30, 184–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Jekria, N.; Daud, S. Environmental concern and recycling behaviour. Procedia Econ. Financ. 2016, 35, 667–673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Li, S. Recycling behavior under China’s social and economic transition: The case of metropolitan Wuhan. Environ. Behav. 2003, 35, 784–801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Mamady, K. Factors influencing attitude, safety behavior, and knowledge regarding household waste management in Guinea: A cross-sectional study. J. Environ. Public Health 2016, 2016. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Nomura, H.; Takahashi, Y.; Yabe, M. Psychological driving forces behind households’ behaviors toward municipal organic waste separation at source in Vietnam: A structural equation modeling approach. J. Mater. Cycles Waste Manag. 2017, 19, 1052–1060. [Google Scholar]
  53. Omran, A.; Mahmood, A.; Abdul Aziz, H.; Robinson, G.M. Investigating households attitude toward recycling of solid waste in malaysia: A case study. Int. J. Environ. Res. 2009, 3, 275–288. [Google Scholar]
  54. Owusu, V.; Adjei-Addo, E.; Sundberg, C. Do economic incentives affect attitudes to solid waste source separation? Evidence from Ghana. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2013, 78, 115–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Oyekale, A. Factors explaining households’ cash payment for solid waste disposal and recycling behaviors in South Africa. Sustainability 2015, 7, 15882–15899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Pakpour, A.H.; Zeidi, I.M.; Emamjomeh, M.M.; Asefzadeh, S.; Pearson, H. Household waste behaviours among a community sample in Iran: An application of the theory of planned behaviour. Waste Manag. 2014, 34, 980–986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Ying, Q. The Study on Factors Influencing Resident’s Behavior for Source Separation of Household Waste. J. Appl. Stat. Manag. 2011, 1. Available online: http://en.cnki.com.cn/Article_en/CJFDTotal-SLTJ201101008.htm (accessed on 1 January 2011).
  58. Ramayah, T.; Lee, J.W.C.; Lim, S. Sustaining the environment through recycling: An empirical study. J. Environ. Manag. 2012, 102, 141–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Singhirunnusorn, W.; Donlakorn, K.; Kaewhanin, W. Contextual factors influencing household recycling behaviours: A case of waste bank project in Mahasarakham municipality. Procedia-Soc. Behav. Sci. 2012, 36, 688–697. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  60. Tatlonghari, R.V.; Jamias, S.B. Village-level knowledge, attitudes and practices on solid waste management in Sta. Rosa City, Laguna, Philippines. J. Environ. Sci. Manag. 2010, 13. Available online: https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Serlie_Jamias/publication/298946129_Village-Level_Knowledge_Attitudes_and_Practices_on_Solid_Waste_Management_in_Sta_Rosa_City_Laguna_Philippines/links/5db43638299bf111d4ca29c1/Village-Level-Knowledge-Attitudes-and-Practices-on-Solid-Waste-Management-in-Sta-Rosa-City-Laguna-Philippines.pdf (accessed on 1 January 2010).
  61. Tang, Z.; Chen, X.; Luo, J. Determining socio-psychological drivers for rural household recycling behavior in developing countries: A case study from Wugan, Hunan, China. Environ. Behav. 2011, 43, 848–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Xiao, L.; Zhang, G.; Zhu, Y.; Lin, T. Promoting public participation in household waste management: A survey based method and case study in Xiamen city, China. J. Clean. Prod. 2017, 144, 313–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Xu, L.; Ling, M.; Lu, Y.; Shen, M. External influences on forming residents’ waste separation behaviour: Evidence from households in Hangzhou, China. Habitat Int. 2017, 63, 21–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Yuan, Y.; Nomura, H.; Takahashi, Y.; Yabe, M. Model of Chinese household kitchen waste separation behavior: A case study in Beijing city. Sustainability 2016, 8, 1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Zhang, D.; Huang, G.; Yin, X.; Gong, Q. Residents’ waste separation behaviors at the source: Using SEM with the theory of planned behavior in Guangzhou, China. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2015, 12, 9475–9491. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  66. Zhang, S.; Zhang, M.; Yu, X.; Ren, H. What keeps Chinese from recycling: Accessibility of recycling facilities and the behavior. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2016, 109, 176–186. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Chen, M.-F.; Tung, P.-J. The moderating effect of perceived lack of facilities on consumers’ recycling intentions. Environ. Behav. 2010, 42, 824–844. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Ghani, W.A.W.A.K.; Rusli, I.F.; Biak, D.R.A.; Idris, A. An application of the theory of planned behaviour to study the influencing factors of participation in source separation of food waste. Waste Manag. 2013, 33, 1276–1281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Mosler, H.-J.; Tamas, A.; Tobias, R.; Rodríguez, T.C.; Miranda, O.G. Deriving interventions on the basis of factors influencing behavioral intentions for waste recycling, composting, and reuse in Cuba. Environ. Behav. 2008, 40, 522–544. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Nguyen, T.T.P.; Zhu, D.; Le, N.P. Factors influencing waste separation intention of residential households in a developing country: Evidence from Hanoi, Vietnam. Habitat Int. 2015, 48, 169–176. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Tamas, A.; Mosler, H.-J.; Tobias, R.; Rodríguez, T.C.; Miranda, O.G. Factors determining the intentions to reuse, separate and compost household waste in the city of Santiago De Cuba. Waste: The Social Context (2005) 2005, 736–744. [Google Scholar]
  72. Saphores, J.-D.M.; Nixon, H. How effective are current household recycling policies? Results from a national survey of US households. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2014, 92, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Wertz, K.L. Economic factors influencing households’ production of refuse. J. Environ. Econ. Manag. 1976, 2, 263–272. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Acuff, K.; Kaffine, D.T. Greenhouse gas emissions, waste and recycling policy. J. Environ. Econ. Manag. 2013, 65, 74–86. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Uwasu, M.; Naito, T.; Yabar, H.; Hara, K. Assessment of Japanese recycling policies for home electric appliance: Cost-effectiveness analysis and socioeconomic and technological implications. Environ. Dev. 2013, 6, 21–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Shaw, P.; Maynard, S. The potential of financial incentives to enhance householders’ kerbside recycling behaviour. Waste Manag. 2008, 28, 1732–1741. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Burnley, S.J. A review of municipal solid waste composition in the United Kingdom. Waste Manag. 2007, 27, 1274–1285. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Sujauddin, M.; Huda, S.; Hoque, A.R. Household solid waste characteristics and management in Chittagong, Bangladesh. Waste Manag. 2008, 28, 1688–1695. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Harring, N.; Jagers, S.C.; Nilsson, F. Recycling as a large-scale collective action dilemma: A cross-country study on trust and reported recycling behavior. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2019, 140, 85–90. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Bandura, A. Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychol. Rev. 1977, 84, 191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Bandura, A. Self-efficacy mechanism in human agency. Am. Psychol. 1982, 37, 122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Triandis, H.C. Values, attitudes, and interpersonal behavior. In Proceedings of the Nebraska symposium on motivation, Lincoln, NE, USA, 1979. [Google Scholar]
  83. Rajendran, K.; Lin, R.; Wall, D.M.; Murphy, J.D. Influential Aspects in Waste Management Practices. In Sustainable Resource Recovery and Zero Waste Approaches; Taherzadeh, M.J., Bolton, K., Wong, J., Pandey, A., Eds.; Elsevier: St. Louis, MO, USA, 2019; pp. 65–78. [Google Scholar]
  84. Strydom, W. Highlights of a recycling behaviour study in South Africa’s large urban areas. In Proceedings of the 4th CSIR Biennial Conference: Real problems relevant solutions, Pretoria, South Africa, 9–10 October 2012. [Google Scholar]
  85. Romaine, D.S.; Marlatt, G.A. The Complete Idiot’s Guide to Changing Old Habits for Good; Penguin: London, UK, 2008. [Google Scholar]
  86. Baud, I.; Grafakos, S.; Hordijk, M.; Post, J. Quality of life and alliances in solid waste management: Contributions to urban sustainable development. Cities 2001, 18, 3–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Ahsan, A.; Alamgir, M.; Imteaz, M.; Daud, N.N.; Islam, R. Role of NGOs and CBOs in waste management. Iran. J. Public Health 2012, 41, 27. [Google Scholar]
  88. Singh, C.R.; Dey, M. Role of NGO’s in Solid Waste Management: A Study in Different Municipalities of Manipur, India. Curr. World Environ. 2015, 10, 161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  89. Fornara, F.; Carrus, G.; Passafaro, P.; Bonnes, M. Distinguishing the sources of normative influence on proenvironmental behaviors: The role of local norms in household waste recycling. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 2011, 14, 623–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Oskamp, S.; Harrington, M.J.; Edwards, T.C.; Sherwood, D.L.; Okuda, S.M.; Swanson, D.C. Factors influencing household recycling behavior. Environ. Behav. 1991, 23, 494–519. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Bourdieu, P. Structures, Habitus, Practices. In Social theory: The multicultural and classic readings; Original work published 1990 ed.; Lemert, C., Ed.; Westview Press: Boulder, CO, USA, 2004; pp. 435–440. [Google Scholar]
  92. Gonzalez-Torre, P.L.; Adenso-Diaz, B. Influence of distance on the motivation and frequency of household recycling. Waste Manag. 2005, 25, 15–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Schwartz, B. Maintenance of key pecking by response-independent food presentation: The role of the modality of the signal for food 1. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 1973, 20, 17–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  94. Ajzen, I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Process. 1991, 50, 179–211. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Strydom, W. Barriers to household waste recycling: Empirical evidence from South Africa. Recycling 2018, 3, 41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  96. Afroz, R.; Hanaki, K.; Tuddin, R. The role of socio-economic factors on household waste generation: A study in a waste management program in Dhaka City, Bangladesh. Res. J. Appl. Sci. 2010, 5, 183–190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Miafodzyeva, S.; Brandt, N.; Andersson, M. Recycling behaviour of householders living in multicultural urban area: A case study of Järva, Stockholm, Sweden. Waste Manag. Res. 2013, 31, 447–457. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. McDonald, S.; Oates, C. Reasons for non-participation in a kerbside recycling scheme. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2003, 39, 369–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Ando, A.W.; Gosselin, A.Y. Recycling in multifamily dwellings: Does convinience matter? Econ. Inq. 2005, 43, 426–438. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Jesson, J. Household waste recycling behavior: A market segmentation model. Soc. Mark. Q. 2009, 15, 25–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Miafodzyeva, S.; Brandt, N.; Olsson, M. Motivation recycling: Pre-recycling case study in Minsk, Belarus. Waste Manag. Res. 2010, 28, 340–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Tadesse, T.; Ruijs, A.; Hagos, F. Household waste disposal in Mekelle city, Northern Ethiopia. Waste Manag. 2008, 28, 2003–2012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  103. Yerxa, E.J. Habits in Context: A Synthesis, with Implications for Research in Occupational Science. Otjr: Occup. Particip. Health 2002, 22, 104S–110S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  104. De Young, R. Recycling as appropriate behavior: A review of survey data from selected recycling education programs in Michigan. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 1990, 3, 253–266. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  105. Vicente, P.; Reis, E. Factors influencing households’ participation in recycling. Waste Manag. Res. 2008, 26, 140–146. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Bonatti, V. Taking out the garbage: Migrant women’s unseen environmental work. Eur. J. Women’s Stud. 2018, 25, 41–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Rousta, K.; Bolton, K.; Lundin, M.; Dahlen, L. Quantitative assessment of distance to collection point and improved sorting information on source separation of household waste. Waste Manag 2015, 40, 22–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Barr, S.; Ford, N.J.; Gilg, A.W. Attitudes towards recycling household waste in Exeter, Devon: Quantitative and qualitative approaches. Local Environ. 2003, 8, 407–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Belton, V.; Crowe, D.V.; Matthews, R.; Scott, S. A survey of public attitudes to recycling in Glasgow (UK). Waste Manag. Res. 1994, 12, 351–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Berglund, C. The assessment of households’ recycling cost: The role of personal motives. Ecol. Econ. 2006, 56, 560–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. Hage, O.; Söderholm, P. An econometric analysis of regional differences in household waste collection: The case of plastic packaging waste in Sweden. Waste Manag. 2008, 28, 1720–1731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  112. Vencatasawmy, C.P.; Öhman, M.; Brännström, T. A survey of recycling behaviour in households in Kiruna, Sweden. Waste Manag. Res. 2000, 18, 545–556. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. Williams, I.; Kelly, J. Green waste collection and the public’s recycling behaviour in the Borough of Wyre, England. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2003, 38, 139–159. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  114. Gutberlet, J.; Carenzo, S.; Kain, J.-H.; Mantovani Martiniano de Azevedo, A. Waste picker organizations and their contribution to the circular economy: Two case studies from a global south perspective. Resources 2017, 6, 52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  115. Joshi, N. Low-income women’s right to sanitation services in city public spaces: A study of waste picker women in Pune. Environ. Urban. 2018, 30, 249–264. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Fergutz, O.; Dias, S.; Mitlin, D. Developing urban waste management in Brazil with waste picker organizations. Environ. Urban. 2011, 23, 597–608. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  117. WorldBank. Toward Gender Equality in East Asia and the Pacific: A Companion to the World Development Report; World Bank Publications: Herndon, VA, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  118. Bianchi, S.M.; Milkie, M.A.; Sayer, L.C.; Robinson, J.P. Is anyone doing the housework? Trends in the gender division of household labor. Soc. Forces 2000, 79, 191–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Berk, S.F. Women and household labor. Soc. Sci. 1983, 11, 55–63. [Google Scholar]
  120. Tadesse, T. Environmental concern and its implication to household waste separation and disposal: Evidence from Mekelle, Ethiopia. Resour. Conserv. Recycl. 2009, 53, 183–191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Becker, N. Increasing High Recycling Rates: Socio-Demographics as an Additional Layer of Information to Improve Waste Management; IIIEE: Lund, Sweden, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  122. Tucker, P. Understanding recycling behaviour. Pap. Technol. 2001, 42, 51–54. [Google Scholar]
  123. Madden, T.J.; Ellen, P.S.; Ajzen, I. A Comparison of the Theory of Planned Behaviour and the Theory of Reasoned Action. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 1992, 18, 3–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  124. Barr, S. Household Waste in Social Perspective; Values, Attitudes, Situation and Behaviour; Ashgate: Burlington, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  125. Ölander, F.; Thøgersen, J. Understanding of consumer behaviour as a prerequisite for environmental protection. J. Consum. Policy 1995, 18, 345–385. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. Literature selection process.
Figure 1. Literature selection process.
Recycling 05 00006 g001
Figure 2. Identified variables and factors that influence the participation in household waste sorting in developing countries.
Figure 2. Identified variables and factors that influence the participation in household waste sorting in developing countries.
Recycling 05 00006 g002
Table 1. Results of the analysis of the determined factors.
Table 1. Results of the analysis of the determined factors.
Factor AnalyzedCorrelation r (95% CI)Q-ValueI-Square (%)Failsafe NNo. of Studies
Governmental Incentives and Factors
Governmental Incentives and Factors0.26 [0.16, 0.36] **255 **96.193511
Psychological Variables
Perceived Behavior Control0.28 [0.22, 0.34] **159 **88.7373419
Perceived Consequence0.25 [0.03, 0.44] *89 **95.51695
Attitude0.39 [0.24, 0.52] **1559 **98.6836723
Subjective Norm0.30 [0.22, 0.38] **369 **94.8436419
Moral Norm0.35 [0.22, 0.46] **171 **95.313569
Situational Variables
Situational Factors0.19 [0.10, 0.29] **222 **93.7121315
Knowledge0.14 [−0.05, 0.32]226 **96.92008
Age0.08 [0.02, 0.14] **257 **93.440918
Gender0.11 [0.01, 0.21] *247 **95.124913
Education0.09 [0.02, 0.15] **209 **93.883914
Income0.02 [−0.07, 0.12]325 **96.01614
* p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01; For gender, the positive value (r) is interpreted as female are correlated to waste sorting.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Rousta, K.; Zisen, L.; Hellwig, C. Household Waste Sorting Participation in Developing Countries—A Meta-Analysis. Recycling 2020, 5, 6. https://doi.org/10.3390/recycling5010006

AMA Style

Rousta K, Zisen L, Hellwig C. Household Waste Sorting Participation in Developing Countries—A Meta-Analysis. Recycling. 2020; 5(1):6. https://doi.org/10.3390/recycling5010006

Chicago/Turabian Style

Rousta, Kamran, Liu Zisen, and Coralie Hellwig. 2020. "Household Waste Sorting Participation in Developing Countries—A Meta-Analysis" Recycling 5, no. 1: 6. https://doi.org/10.3390/recycling5010006

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop