Next Article in Journal
Comprehensive Transcriptome and Metabolome Characterization of Peony ‘Coral Sunset’ Petals Provides Insights into the Mechanism of Pigment Degradation
Previous Article in Journal
Grape Yield Prediction Models: Approaching Different Machine Learning Algorithms
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Tactile Stimulation Using an Assortment of Natural Elements on the Psychophysiological Responses of Adults

1
Department of Bio and Healing Convergence, Graduate School, Konkuk University, Seoul 05029, Republic of Korea
2
Department of Systems Biotechnology, Konkuk Institute of Technology, Konkuk University, Seoul 05029, Republic of Korea
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Horticulturae 2023, 9(12), 1293; https://doi.org/10.3390/horticulturae9121293
Submission received: 29 September 2023 / Revised: 27 November 2023 / Accepted: 29 November 2023 / Published: 30 November 2023

Abstract

:
Contact with the natural environment has positive effects on physical and mental health and well-being. This study aimed to investigate the effects of tactile stimulation on the psychophysiological responses of adults, using natural gardening elements. The participants were 30 adults (20–60 years old). The participants received tactile stimulation by touching five natural elements with their hands and feet, and tactile stimulation for each natural element was performed for 90 s. Geranium, tiny ardisia, decomposed granite soil, log hardwood, and culture soil were used as tactile stimulation factors for the hand, and moss, grass, pebble, bark, and culture soil were used as tactile stimulation factors for the foot. Oxyhemoglobin (oxy-Hb) concentrations in the prefrontal cortex during the stimulation as well as blood pressure and pulse rate after each activity were measured. Additionally, the semantic differential method was used to evaluate the psychological effects of contact with the elements on the participants. Compared to before tactile stimulation, the oxy-Hb concentration related to prefrontal lobe cortical activity significantly decreased in some sections using tiny ardisia, log hardwood, and culture soil on the hands, and using grass, moss, pebble, and bark on the feet. Blood pressure also showed a significant decrease after tactile stimulation using geranium, tiny ardisia, and log hardwood. The findings of this study suggest that tactile stimulation using natural gardening elements could be a significant intervention in inducing physiological stability and reducing stress by calming the activity of the prefrontal cortex.

1. Introduction

As natural spaces are transformed into artificial spaces, owing to the development of urban and architectural spaces along with the rapid growth of the economy and industry, many city dwellers are living in artificial environments lacking natural objects [1]. The artificial environments of large cities provide life benefits to humans but cause immune diseases, mental diseases, and infectious diseases due to air pollution, noise, electromagnetic waves, and so on [2,3]. However, as the standard of living of residents in cities increases, the demand for improving their quality of living environment also increases. Accordingly, various activities such as agro-healing, horticultural therapy, horticultural activities, and green plant walls focusing on the healing function of nature are gaining attention.
Additionally, various theories based on the nature-friendly aspect have been argued, and interest in the potential of the natural environment to benefit humans is increasing. The natural environment plays an important role in the modern world to help humans overcome diseases. In the stress recovery theory (SRT), Ulrich [4] argues for more contact with nature. Wilson’s biophilia theory [5] claims that natural reversion is inherent in human genes, so people are instinctively attracted to nature.
Based on this, studies have been conducted on the identification of human health effects caused by nature or horticultural activities. A gardening program for the elderly significantly improved their brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and platelet-derived growth factor (PDGF) levels related to cognitive health factors [6]. Following a horticultural therapy program for people with intellectual disabilities, their interpersonal negotiation strategies, functional adaptive behaviors, and physical abilities for job behaviors, including agility and grasping of the hand, improved significantly [7]. As a result of conducting a healing agriculture program for patients with schizophrenia, their negative symptom responses were significantly reduced and positive effects were found upon rehabilitation [8]. Moreover, a horticultural treatment program was conducted for veterans with mental health problems, and their depression and stress levels significantly reduced [9]. As such, it positively affects the physical, cognitive, and emotional health aspects.
The human health effects of such horticultural activities are generated through the interaction between horticultural and natural healing elements, and humans. To identify this, basic research based on the five human senses using horticulture and natural healing elements is being conducted. In a prior study on visual stimulation in adults, it was observed that increased exposure led to an elevation in the relative theta (RT) power spectrum, an indicator of attentional ability, in the occipital cortex [10]. Additionally, the relative high beta (RHB) power spectrum, an indicator of stress and anxiety, showed a decrease in inducing physiological relaxation [10]. When women’s senses of smell were stimulated with various aroma oils, the EEG of their prefrontal cortex significantly increased the ratio of alpha to high beta (RAHB), an index of relaxation and stability, and the relative low beta (RLB), an index of brain activity. Moreover, their blood pressure was significantly reduced and the participants were psychophysiologically stable [11]. In another study, forest-derived visual, auditory, and combined visual–auditory stimuli significantly reduced the concentration of oxy-Hb in the human prefrontal cortex and increased the parasympathetic nervous system (PSNS) activity, resulting in a relaxed state [12]. In particular, the combined stimuli indicated a more pronounced physiological relaxation state than a single stimulus [12].
Among the five human senses, tactile contact has a particularly active and exploratory form of perception in nature, which can help us to realize various characteristics such as the shape, warmth, weight, and texture of an object through the sense of touch [13]. According to a tactile stimulation study conducted using contact with natural materials, a significant decrease in cerebral blood flow (CBF) was observed when the leaves of real plants were touched by hand, but no physiological changes were observed in the leaves of artificial plants [14]. In the case of barefoot contact with wood, the concentration of oxy-Hb in the left and right prefrontal cortex was shown to decrease immediately after contact, and the value of ln(HF) was found to increase significantly, thus affecting the autonomic nervous system (ANS) and causing physiological relaxation [15,16].
However, the number of natural elements used in studying the interaction between the tactile stimulation of natural objects and human psychophysiological aspects is miniscule; moreover, the original form is modified according to the experimenter’s intention before the research is conducted. To understand the actual healing mechanism of the natural environment, research on various types and original forms of natural objects is necessary. Therefore, this study aims to investigate the psychophysiological effects on humans when their hands and feet meet various natural gardening elements.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Research Participants

Flyers containing information about the research were distributed to apartments and libraries in Gwangjin-gu, Seoul, Republic of Korea. Participants were recruited according to convenience sampling and based on specific criteria to avoid influencing the data. A participant had to be one (a) who did not have any psychopathological disease and did not take related drugs; (b) who was right-hand dominant; (c) who agreed to participate after fully understanding the contents of the research; (d) who had no history of any cardiovascular disease such as high blood pressure, unstable angina, heart attack, and heart surgery; (e) who had no tactile function-related disorders, allergies, respiratory diseases, and insomnia; and (f) who was not pregnant, lactating, or menstruating [11].
A total of 30 adults (7 men and 23 women; average age: 31.2 ± 10.2 years) participated in this study. The researcher explained the contents of the study and precautions to the participants before conducting the experiment and obtained written consent from the participants who agreed to the study. Additional data about their, age, sex, height, weight, and body mass index (ioi 353; Jawon Medical Co., Ltd., Gyeongsan, Republic of Korea) were measured as demographic information. The participants received an incentive (approximately USD 20) after completing the experiment. This study was approved by the Bioethics Committee of Konkuk University (7001355-202106-HR-442).

2.2. Experimental Condition

This study was conducted in a designated experimental space (200 cm × 160 cm) in a laboratory at Konkuk University, Seoul, Republic of Korea. The environmental conditions of the experimental space were as follows: temperature of 24.4 ± 3.1 °C, relative humidity of 15.2 ± 1.8% (O-257; DRETECKOREA Co., Seoul, Republic of Korea), and illuminance of 2679.5 ± 1813.4 Lux. To minimize external visual stimulation, white cardboard was attached to the front of the desk and ivory curtains were attached on both sides.

2.3. Experimental Procedure

This was a crossover experimental study in which each participant performed the experimental protocol presented in Figure 1.
In this study, a psychophysiological measuring device was worn to measure the concentration of oxy-Hb in the participants’ prefrontal lobe cortex during the activity. In addition, a mask, earplugs, and eye patch were worn to minimize external stimulation. After taking a break for 60 s, the participants touched the natural gardening elements for 90 s according to the experimenter’s instructions (Figure 2). The order of elements was randomly assigned to each participant. Changes in oxy-Hb concentration were measured during the 90 s of touching a natural element. Immediately after touching the natural gardening elements for 90 s, the blood pressure and pulse rate of the participants were measured, along with their subjective feelings, which were assessed using the semantic difference method (SDM). The participants were instructed not to make large movements or speak during the experiment. The experiment comprised two parts, tactile stimulation of the hands and that of the feet. A break was given between each experiment. The participants visited the laboratory only once to participate in this study, and all experimental procedures were completed within 80 min per participant.

2.4. Tactile Stimulation Element

Despite there being many different types of natural elements, the subjects in this study were limited to plant leaves, trees, and soil. As for the tactile stimulation elements for the hand, natural elements that can be encountered during horticulture activities were selected. The plant leaves and soil were divided into two textures (smooth and rough). As for the tactile stimulation elements for the feet, natural elements that can be encountered mainly in gardens or care farms were selected.

2.4.1. Hand

Tactile stimulation of the hand was performed using geranium (Pelargonium inquinans), tiny ardisia (Ardisia pusilla A.DC.), log hardwood (Quercus), decomposed granite soil, and culture soil. The geranium (Ø15 cm pot, 6 ea) (Figure 3A) and tiny ardisia (Ø10 cm pot, 8 ea) (Figure 3B) were prepared so that only the leaves could be touched. The decomposed granite soil (3~9 mm, 8 L) was washed in advance and dried before being used in the experiment (Figure 3C). In the case of the log hardwood (Ø20 cm × 30 cm), oak (Quercus) in the form of log hardwood was used in its natural state (Figure 3D). For the culture soil (8 L), a horticultural medium containing coco peat, peat moss, zeolite, perlite, vermiculite, and fertilizer was used (Figure 3E).

2.4.2. Foot

Grass, moss, bark, pebbles, and culture soil were used in the tactile stimulation experiment for the feet. All materials were stored at room temperature in identical boxes (59 × 38 × 15 cm, 27 L). Grass (46 × 36 cm, 1 ea) engrafted to fibrous soil was used (Figure 4A). Dried moss (56 × 36 cm, 1 ea) collected from natural surroundings was soaked in water for a day and used after it was dry (Figure 4B). In the box containing pebbles (1.2~3 cm, 8 L), black gravel and agate stone were mixed in a certain ratio (Figure 4C). The bark (3~6 cm, 8 L) was made of pine and was used without any chemical treatment (Figure 4D). For the culture soil (8 L), a horticultural medium containing coco peat, peat moss, zeolite, perlite, vermiculite, and fertilizer was used (Figure 4E).

2.5. Measurement Items

2.5.1. Near-Infrared Time-Resolved Spectroscopy

NIRS was used for physiological measurements. In this study, functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) was used to measure brain activity when the participants came into contact with the elements of natural origin (Figure 5A). fNIRS is non-invasive and measures concentration changes in oxyhemoglobin (HbO) and deoxyhemoglobin (HbR) based on nerve activation, and is easy to use and safe on the test subjects. It also contributes to measuring functional brain activity and cognitive function [17].
fNIRS data were recorded through a NIRSIT Lite (OBELAB Inc., Seoul, Republic of Korea) comprising 5 light sources, 7 collectors, and 15 channels (Figure 5B). Light intensity data of the PFC region were collected at a sampling rate of 8.138 Hz using 780 and 850 nm wavelengths. The channels are located in the aPFC (CH 2 to 14), DPFC (CH 1), and the orbital part of the IFG (CH 15) in Brodmann’s area. CH 1~7 can be divided into the right prefrontal lobe and CH 9~15 into the left prefrontal lobe.

2.5.2. Blood Pressure and Pulse Rate

Blood pressure and pulse rates are used as measures of the autonomic nervous system (ANS). After tactile stimulation using the natural elements, the participants’ blood pressure was measured using a blood pressure monitor (T4 with Intellisense; Omron Co., Kyoto, Japan) on their left arm while they were resting after the stimulation. The measurement items included their pulse rate (HRV), systolic blood pressure (mmHg), and diastolic blood pressure (mmHg).

2.5.3. Psychological Measurement

The modified SDM [18] was used to evaluate the psychological effect on the participants based on tactile contact and to identify the characteristics of each element. The modified SDM used in this study consisted of seven items, and the Likert score was 13 out of 13: “Comfortable–Uncomfortable”, “Relaxed–Alert”, “Natural–Artificial”, “Warm–Cold”, “Smooth–Uneven”, “Moist–Dry”, and “Soft–Hard”.

2.6. Data Processing and Analysis

fNIRS data were processed through the NIRSIT Lite Analysis Tool v3.2.4 (OBELAB Inc., Seoul, Republic of Korea). The light intensity data measured using the instrument were converted into a time series of changes in HbO and HbR concentrations by applying the modified Beer–Lambert law (MBLL). In this experiment, only HbO data were used; a high-pass filter (cutoff frequency 0.005 Hz) and a low-pass filter (cutoff frequency 0.01 Hz) were applied to the converted data to remove noise, such as the heart rate, respiration, and excessive movement. The signal-to-noise ratio criterion for removing channels with a low signal quality was less than 30 dB, and excessive peaks (more than 10 mM and less than −10 mM) on the △HbO time series graph were considered as noise and removed. All data values were calculated based on 2 s before material contact. Referring to previous studies [19,20], the collected PFC data were divided into R-PFC (CH1-7) and L-PFC (CH9-15), and the average value for 90 s was calculated. Then, for more detailed analysis, they were further divided into data of 30 s and analyzed. Channels corresponding to each region were averaged and used for data statistical processing.
A paired t-test was performed on the processed fNIRS data, blood pressure, pulse rate data, and SDM, using SPSS (version 25 for Windows; IBM Corp., Armonk, NY, USA). Differences before and after touching the natural objects were analyzed. All significance levels were set at p < 0.05. For demographic information, descriptive statistics such as the mean and standard deviation were performed using Microsoft Excel (Office 2016; Microsoft Corp., Redmond, WA, USA).

3. Results

3.1. Demographic Characteristics

Adults aged 31.2 ± 10.2 years participated in this study (seven men, 26.7 ± 2.6 years; 23 women, 32.6 ± 11.3 years) (Table 1). The average height was 164.4 ± 6.6 cm, while the average body weight was 63.2 ± 13.1 kg. The overall average body mass index (BMI) was 23.3 ± 4.1 kg m−2, which is within the normal range as per the criteria specified by the World Health Organization.

3.2. Tactile Stimulation (Hand)

3.2.1. Near-Infrared Time-Resolved Spectroscopy

Figure 6 shows the comparison of the total and section-wise average oxy-Hb concentrations in the prefrontal cortex before and after touching the five natural elements. All data were calculated as the difference to the 2 s baseline period immediately before the participant touched the element. Compared to before tactile stimulation, the concentration of oxy-Hb in the participants’ left prefrontal lobe after touching the tiny ardisia, log hardwood, and culture soil significantly decreased in the 0–30 s interval of the stimulation (p < 0.05; Figure 6A). There was no significant difference in the concentration of oxy-Hb in the right prefrontal lobe (p > 0.05; Figure 6B). However, comparing the changes in the oxy-Hb concentration per second in the left and right prefrontal cortex showed that the oxy-Hb concentration tended to be maintained at a lower level when touching the element than before touching it.

3.2.2. Pulse Rate and Blood Pressure

Comparing the pulse rate and blood pressure before and after tactile stimulation for each element showed that there was no significant difference in the pulse rate (p > 0.05; Table 2), while the systolic blood pressure showed a significant decrease after stimulation with tiny ardisia (p < 0.05; Table 2), and the diastolic blood pressure showed a significant decrease after stimulation with geranium, tiny ardisia and log hardwood (p < 0.05; Table 2).

3.2.3. Semantic Differential Method (SDM)

Figure 7 shows the results of the SDM for the natural elements that were touched by the hand. There was no significant difference in terms of feeling “comfortable–uncomfortable” (p > 0.05; Figure 7A) on touching. Participants were “slightly to moderately relaxed” when touching the geranium (p < 0.01; Figure 7B), and were “moderately natural” when touching the log hardwood (p < 0.001; Figure 7C). This was shown to induce additional relaxation than when tactile stimulation was not performed. In terms of feeling warm–cold, when touching the tiny ardisia, decomposed granite soil, log hardwood, and culture soil, their feelings ranged from “indifferent to slightly cold” (p < 0.05; Figure 7D). When touching the tiny ardisia, decomposed granite soil, and log hardwood, their feelings ranged from “indifferent to finding it moderately uneven” (p < 0.001; Figure 7E). When touching the culture soil, participants felt it to be “slightly to moderately moist,” and when touching the tiny ardisia and decomposed granite soil, they felt it to be “slightly to moderately dry” (p < 0.001; Figure 7F). When touching the tiny ardisia and culture soil, they felt “moderately to very soft”, and when touching the decomposed granite soil and log hardwood, they felt “slightly to moderately hard” (p < 0.001; Figure 7G).

3.3. Tactile Stimulation (Foot)

3.3.1. Near-Infrared Time-Resolved Spectroscopy

Figure 8 shows the comparison of the total and section average oxy-Hb concentrations in the participants’ prefrontal cortex before and after touching the five natural elements. All data were calculated as the difference from the 2 s baseline period immediately before the participant touched the natural element. Compared to before tactile stimulation, the concentration of oxy-Hb in their left prefrontal cortex was significantly lower with grass, moss, pebbles, and bark in the 0–30 s period of stimulation (p < 0.05; Figure 8A). In the 31–60 s period, the oxy-Hb concentrations after touching moss and bark were significantly lower (p < 0.01; Figure 8A). Compared to before tactile stimulation, the concentration of oxy-Hb in the right prefrontal cortex was significantly lower when touching moss and bark in the 0–30-s interval (p < 0.01; Figure 8B). In the 31–60 s period, the concentration of oxy-Hb with the bark was significantly lower (p < 0.05; Figure 8B).

3.3.2. Pulse Rate and Blood Pressure

Comparing the pulse rate and blood pressure before and after tactile stimulation using the five elements, no significant difference was seen in the pulse rate (p > 0.05; Table 3). On the other hand, the difference in systolic blood pressure increased significantly after touching the grass, moss, and culture soil (p < 0.05; Table 3), but there was no significant difference in the diastolic blood pressure (p > 0.05; Table 3).

3.3.3. SDM

Figure 9 shows the SDM results of the participants’ feelings when their feet came into contact with the natural elements. They felt “slightly to moderately comfortable and relaxed” when touching grass (p > 0.05; Figure 9A,B), which was found to induce significantly more relaxation than when no tactile stimulation was performed. When touching grass, moss, and culture soil, they felt the elements to be “slightly to very natural” (p < 0.001; Figure 9C), more so than the other elements. They felt “indifferent to moderately cold” when touching all the elements (p < 0.05; Figure 9D). When touching a pebble and bark, they felt “slightly to moderately uneven” (p < 0.001; Figure 9E). When touching the moss, grass, and culture soil, they felt “slightly to very moist” (p < 0.001; Figure 9F). When touching the moss, grass, and culture soil, they felt “slightly soft to very soft” (p < 0.001; Figure 9G), and when touching the pebbles and bark, they felt “indifferent to moderately hard” (p < 0.01; Figure 9G).

4. Discussion

This study investigated the psychophysiological response in 30 adults to tactile stimulation using natural gardening elements. To compare the activation of the prefrontal lobe cortex and autonomic nervous system of the participants, their oxy-Hb concentration, blood pressure, and pulse rate were measured. Additionally, by measuring the SDM of the participants, we tried to measure the psychological effects of contact with natural elements. As a result, in some sections with geranium, log hardwood, and culture soil, the change in oxy-Hb concentration with the tactile stimulus to the hand was maintained at a lower level than before touching the natural element (Figure 6). In addition, the blood pressure decreased significantly with the geranium, tiny ardisia, and log hardwood when touching the natural elements (p < 0.05; Table 2). In some sections with grass, moss, bark, and pebbles, the change in oxy-Hb concentration with the tactile stimulus to the feet was significantly reduced after touching the natural elements compared to before touching them (Figure 8). These results suggest that exposure to the natural environment has positive effects on physiological relaxation and stress recovery.
The frontal lobe is located in the anterior part of the cerebral hemisphere and plays an important role in various cognitive processes and emotional processing [21]. In particular, the prefrontal cortex not only accepts and processes information from all other cortical areas, but also accepts information from the sensory receptor system and enables immediate responses [22]. NIRS used in this study is a non-invasive method for monitoring oxy-Hb concentrations, since is not affected by head motions due to the temporal resolution of the device [23,24]. Near-infrared light spectroscopy is a method that can continuously measure the hemodynamic parameters of the prefrontal cortex [23,25]. When brain activity increases, cerebral blood flow increases, and the level of cerebral blood flow affects the concentration of oxy-Hb [26,27]. According to a previous study, olfactory stimulation using perilla essential oil reduced the concentration of oxy-Hb in the prefrontal cortex and induced physiological relaxation [28]. In addition, touching wood with the palm of the hand, more than other materials, decreased the concentration of oxy-Hb, calming the activity of the prefrontal cortex and inducing physiological relaxation [29]. A decrease in oxy-Hb concentration shows physiological and psychological relaxing effects.
With tactile stimulation from tiny ardisia, log hardwood, and culture soil on the hand, the concentration of oxy-Hb in the left prefrontal cortex significantly decreased in the 0–30 s interval (Figure 6), but there was no significant difference in the right prefrontal cortex (Figure 6). A previous study showed a transient decrease in oxy-Hb concentrations in the left prefrontal cortex immediately after contact with all substances [16]. In addition, most previous NIRS studies reported that only one frontal lobe region was significantly activated [28,29], and hemispheric differences were seen in brain activation [30]. However, the mechanism underlying hemispheric regions making an asymmetrical contribution to the results is unknown and requires further research [19,31]. With tactile stimulation from grass, moss, bark, and pebbles on the foot, the concentration of oxy-Hb in the left/right prefrontal lobes in the 0–60 s period decreased significantly after touching the natural elements compared to before touching them (Figure 8). These results are consistent with previous studies showing that the concentration of oxy-Hb is lowered when tactile stimulation is through contact with wood, a natural element [16,18]. The results of this study indicate that physiological stability is induced when tactile stimulation is performed using natural elements.
Blood pressure is regulated physiologically by the autonomic nervous system [32], and it has been shown to induce changes in blood pressure under the influence of stress or relaxation [11,33]. Blood pressure increases under stressful conditions and decreases under relaxed emotional conditions [34,35,36]. In a previous study, adults were randomly divided into four groups (control group, horticultural activity group with green plants, horticultural activity group without green plants, and green exercise group), and the findings showed that in the group whose horticultural activity involved green plants, the participants’ blood pressure was significantly lowered post-activity compared to beforehand and the other green exercise groups [37]. In a gardening program conducted for elderly women, their blood pressure was significantly reduced, which had positive effects on lowering stress [38]. In this study, blood pressure was found to decrease after performing tactile stimulation through the hand using natural gardening factors. Thus, tactile stimulation performed through the hand using natural gardening elements can induce physiological relaxation and is effective in reducing stress.
However, unlike the results of tactile stimulation of the hand, when the participants’ feet touched grass, moss, or culture soil, their systolic blood pressure increased. Therefore, it is necessary to consider the characteristics of these three natural resource factors. The three natural gardening factors were evaluated as moist, natural, and cold by the experiment participants compared with other factors in common. According to a study by Filingeri [39], the lower the temperature, the higher the sensitivity of the skin to humidity. A study by Ikei [15] mentioned the possibility that human physiological changes according to the sense of touch can be affected by the temperature of the material. Depending on the nature of these factors, they can affect blood pressure. The results in this experiment appear to have been influenced by the differences in the temperature of the materials. Therefore, future studies need to consider these temperature differences.
Before the urbanization of regions caused by industrialization, humans lived in close contact with nature [19]. In urban areas, people are constantly exposed to stimuli that require their attention, causing mental fatigue [40]. Miyazaki [41,42] argued for the “back to nature” theory, claiming that human physiological functions are best adapted to the natural environment. In addition, Ulrich [43] said that contact with nature can induce human psychological stability, reduce fatigue, and relieve stress. It can also prevent illness and promote recovery to a positive state of well-being [44,45]. A previous study on high-school students showed that viewing green plants in an indoor environment was associated with the stabilization of the autonomic nervous system and parasympathetic nervous activity [46]. The visual stimulation of four types of houseplants (real plants, artificial plants, plant photos, and no plants) to adults showed that RT increased in both occipital lobes when observing real plants, whereas RHB showed a decrease, thereby reducing stress, anxiety, and tension [10]. On observing pots with and without houseplants, in male college students in their 20s, the concentration of oxy-Hb in the prefrontal lobe cortex was significantly lowered when they saw the houseplants, indicating a biological state of armament [47]. Further, comparing the emotional state of elementary-school students during horticultural and non-horticultural activities showed that they felt more natural and comfortable during horticultural activities [48]. Therefore, a state of physiological relaxation is induced when humans are exposed to natural environments or stimuli derived from nature. Moreover, exposure to a restorative environment, such as a natural environment, can contribute to psychological and physiological recovery [49,50]. In this way, healing through nature has been used as an efficient way to improve human physical and mental health and well-being. These experiences with nature provide people with various benefits to their health and well-being. To identify the mechanism through which these benefits are delivered, an integrated interpretation is needed through additional research in the future [51].
This study investigated the effects of tactile stimulation according to natural gardening elements on the psychophysiological responses of adults. This study expanded to more diverse natural elements than previous studies and used two places, the hand and foot, as the contact points of the body. In addition, an unprocessed, natural state was used. In future studies, the following are considered necessary: First, the oxy-Hb concentration was maintained at a lower level during tactile stimulation than before tactile stimulation, but no significant difference was observed when tactile stimulation was prolonged. Compared to other senses, Meissner’s corpuscle, which accepts tactile sensations, always has the characteristic of reacting sensitively. As the measurement was made for a relatively short time, it is necessary to measure the change when the tactile element is touched for a long time in future research. Second, there were no psychophysiologically significant differences in some of the natural factors used in this study. Among the tactile stimulation elements, no significant difference in decomposed granite soil (hand) and cultured soil (foot) was found. Research on the tactile stimulation of natural elements is very insufficient, so generalizing the results only from this study is difficult. Therefore, verifying the results of tactile stimulation with natural elements through further research is necessary. Additional research on tactile stimulation with various natural elements is considered necessary. Third, the material surface temperature was not measured. The natural gardening elements used in this experiment were maintained at room temperature (25 °C), but Ikei [19] also mentioned the possibility that human psychophysiological changes could be affected by the temperature of the material. Therefore, considering these differences in the future is necessary. Fourth, the results for the hands and feet were different in this study. In most gardening activities, your hands are likely to be exposed to natural elements. However, the feet are relatively less exposed to natural elements, and this difference may have influenced the results. Therefore, in future research, it is necessary to discuss whether it has a practical impact when applied to horticultural activities. Finally, as the activity of only the prefrontal lobe was measured in this study, it is necessary to expand the range to measure psychophysiological changes in the parietal lobe, which is responsible for motor and somatosensory functions [52].

5. Conclusions

This study was conducted to investigate the effects of tactile stimulation using natural plant products on the psychophysiological responses of adults. Compared to before tactile stimulation, the oxy-Hb concentration related to prefrontal lobe cortical activity significantly decreased in some sections. The participants’ blood pressure also showed a significant decrease after tactile stimulation. These results suggest that tactile stimulation using natural elements such as plants and soil, among others, can be a significant intervention in reducing stress by inducing physiological relaxation and calming the activity of the prefrontal cortex. In other words, exposure to a natural environment has positive effects on mental recovery and well-being. The results of this study can be useful fundamental data for constructing a horticultural therapy program, considering the purpose of the program and the characteristics of the natural elements used for stimulation.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, S.-A.P.; methodology, S.-A.P.; data Curation, Y.-J.K. and S.-W.C.; writing—original draft preparation, Y.-J.K. and S.-W.C.; writing—review and editing, Y.-J.K. and S.-A.P.; project administration, Y.-J.K.; funding acquisition, S.-A.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was carried out with the support of the “Cooperative Research Program for Agriculture Science and Technology Development (Project No.: RS-2021-RD009877)”, Rural Development Administration, Republic of Korea.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Institutional Review Board of Konkuk University (protocol code 7001355-202106-HR-442; June 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

The data presented in this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to lab data privacy.

Acknowledgments

This paper was supported by the KU Research Professor Program of Konkuk University.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bae, S.J.; Kim, S.J.; Kim, D.S. Priority analysis of activation policies for agro-healing services. J. Korea Soc. Rural Plan. 2019, 25, 89–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Hoisington, A.J.; Stearns-Yoder, K.A.; Schuldt, S.J.; Beemer, C.J.; Maestre, J.P.; Kinney, K.A.; Postolache, T.T.; Lowry, C.A.; Brenner, L.A. Ten questions concerning the built environment and mental health. Build Environ. 2019, 155, 58–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Van den Bosch, M.; Meyer-Lindenberg, A. Environmental exposures and depression: Biological mechanisms and epidemiological evidence. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2019, 40, 1–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Ulrich, R.S.; Simons, R.F.; Losito, B.D.; Fiorito, E.; Miles, M.A.; Zelson, M. Stress recovery during exposure to natural and urban environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 1991, 11, 201–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Wilson, E.O. Biophilia and the conservation ethic. In Evolutionary Perspectives on Environmental Problems; Routledge: Oxfordshire, UK, 2017; pp. 250–258. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Park, S.A.; Lee, A.Y.; Park, H.G.; Lee, W.L. Benefits of gardening activities for cognitive function according to measurement of brain nerve growth factor levels. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Son, H.J.; Kim, D.S.; Park, S.A. Horticultural Therapy for Improving the Work Performance and Interpersonal Relationships of Persons with Intellectual Disabilities. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 13874. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Oh, Y.A.; Park, S.A.; Ahn, B.E. Assessment of the psychopathological effects of a horticultural therapy program in patients with schizophrenia. Complement. Ther. Med. 2018, 36, 54–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Siu, A.M.; Kam, M.; Mok, I. Horticultural therapy program for people with mental illness: A mixed-method evaluation. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2020, 17, 711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Jeong, J.E.; Park, S.A. Physiological and Psychological Effects of Visual Stimulation with Green Plant Types. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 12932. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Choi, N.Y.; Wu, Y.T.; Park, S.A. Effects of olfactory stimulation with aroma oils on psychophysiological responses of female adults. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 5196. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Miyazaki, Y. Effects of forest-derived visual, auditory, and combined stimuli. Urban For. Urban Green 2021, 64, 127253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Matthew, F. The unity of haptic touch. Philos. Psychol. 2011, 24, 493–516. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Koga, K.; Iwasaki, Y. Psychological and physiological effect in humans of touching plant foliage-using the semantic differential method and cerebral activity as indicators. J. Physiol. Anthropo. L 2013, 32, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Ikei, H.; Song, C.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological effects of touching wood. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 801. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Ikei, H.; Miyazaki, Y. Positive physiological effects of touching sugi (Cryptomeria japonica) with the sole of the feet. J. Wood Sci. 2020, 66, 29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Pinti, P.; Tachtsidis, I.; Hamilton, A.; Hirsch, J.; Aichelburg, C.; Gilbert, S.; Burgess, P.W. The present and future use of functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) for cognitive neuroscience. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sc. 2020, 1464, 5–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Osgood, C.E.; Suci, G.J.; Tannenbaum, P.H. The Measurement of Meaning; University of Illinois Press: Champaign, IL, USA, 1957; Available online: https://books.google.co.kr/books?id=Qj8GeUrKZdAC&printsec=frontcover&hl=ko&source=gbs_ge_summary_r&cad=0#v=onepage&q&f=false (accessed on 30 January 1997).
  19. Ikei, H.; Song, C.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological effects of touching coated wood. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2017, 14, 773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Ikei, H.; Song, C.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological effects of touching hinoki cypress (Chamaecyparis obtusa). J Wood Sci. 2018, 64, 226–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Chayer, C.; Freedman, M. Frontal lobe functions. Curr. Neurol. Neurosci. Rep. 2001, 1, 547–552. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Kolb, B.; Mychasiuk, R.; Muhammad, A.; Li, Y.; Frost, D.O.; Gibb, R. Experience and the developing prefrontal cortex. PNAS 2012, 109 (Suppl. 2), 17186–17193. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Jöbsis, F.F. Noninvasive, infrared monitoring of cerebral and myocardial oxygen sufficiency and circulatory parameters. Science 1977, 198, 1264–1267. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Perry, S. NIRS for measuring cerebral hemodynamic responses during exercise. In Functional Neuroimaging in Exercise and Sport Sciences; Boecker, H., Hillman, C.H., Scheef, L., Strüder, H.K., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2012; pp. 335–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Perrey, S. Non-invasive NIR spectoroscopy of human brain function during exercise. Methods 2008, 45, 289–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Fox, P.T.; Raichle, M.E. Focal physiological uncoupling of cerebral blood flow and oxidative metabolism during somatosensory stimulation in human subjects. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 1986, 83, 1140–1144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Hoshi, Y.; Kobayashi, N.; Tamura, M. Interpretation of near-infrared spectroscopy signals: A study with a newly developed perfused rat brain model. J. Appl. Physiol. 2001, 90, 1657–1662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Igarashi, M.; Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Miyazaki, Y. Effects of olfactory stimulation with perilla essential oil on prefrontal cortex activity. J. Altern. Complement. Med. 2014, 20, 545–549. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  29. Igarashi, M.; Song, C.; Ikei, H.; Miyazaki, Y. Effect of Stimulation by Foliage Plant Display Images on Prefrontal Cortex Activity: A Comparison with Stimulation using Actual Foliage Plants. J. Neuroimaging 2015, 25, 127–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Homae, F. A brain of two halves: Insights into interhemispheric organization provided by near-infrared spectroscopy. Neuroimage 2014, 85, 354–362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Damasio, A.R. The Feeling of What Happens. Body and Emotion in Making of Consciousness; Heinemann: London, UK, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  32. Waxenbaum, J.A.; Reddy, V.; Varacallo, M. Anatomy, Autonomic Nervous System; Stat Pearls Publishing LLC.: Treasure Island, FL, USA, 2019; Available online: https://europepmc.org/article/nbk/nbk539845 (accessed on 30 April 2019).
  33. Schwartz, G.E.; Weinberger, D.A.; Singer, J.A. Cardiovascular differentiation of happiness, sadness, anger, and fear following imagery and exercise. Psychosom. Med. 1981, 43, 343–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Ulrich, R.S. View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science 1984, 224, 420–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Tsunetsugu, Y.; Lee, J.; Park, B.J.; Tyrväinen, L.; Kagawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and Psychological Effects of Viewing Urban Forest Landscapes Assessed by Multiple Measurements. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013, 113, 90–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Hassan, A.; Tao, J.; Li, G.; Jiang, M.; Ai, L.; Zhihui, J.; Zongfang, L.; Qibing, C. Effects of walking in bamboo forest and city environments on brainwave activity in young adults. Evid. Based Complement. Altern. Med. 2018, 2018, 9653857. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Tao, M.; Lu, L.; Gao, J.; He, X. Horticultural activities can achieve the same affect improvement effect of green exercise: A randomized field controlled trial. Front. Psychol. 2022, 13, 989919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Park, S.A.; Lee, A.Y.; Park, H.G.; Son, K.C.; Kim, D.S.; Lee, W.R. Gardening intervention as a low-to moderate-intensity physical activity for improving blood lipid profiles, blood pressure, inflammation, and oxidative stress in women over the age of 70: A pilot study. HortScience 2017, 52, 200–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Filingeri, D.; Fournet, D.; Hodder, S.; Havenith, G. Why wet feels wet? A neurophysiological model of human cutaneous wetness sensitivity. J. Neurophysiol. 2014, 112, 1457–1469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Kragsig Peschardt, K.; Karlsson Stigsdotter, U. Associations between park characteristics and perceived restorativeness of small public urban green spaces. Landsc. Urban Plan. 2013, 112, 26–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Miyazaki, Y.; Park, B.-J.; Lee, J. Nature therapy. Designing Our Future: Perspectives on Bioproduction, Ecosystems and Humanity; Osaki, M.B.A., Nakagami, K., Eds.; United Nations University Press: Tokyo, Japan, 2011; Volume 4, pp. 407–412. Available online: https://www.cabdirect.org/cabdirect/abstract/20113192530 (accessed on 30 December 2011).
  42. Miyazaki, Y. Shinrin-Yoku: The Japanese Way of Forest Bathing for Health and Relaxation; Aster: London, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  43. Ulrich, R.S. Health benefits of gardens in hospitals. In Proceedings of the Plants for People International Exhibition Floriade, Haarlemmermeer, The Netherlands, 6 April–20 October 2002; Available online: https://jardinessanadores.cl/wp-content/uploads/2019/09/Health_Benefits (accessed on 30 January 2002).
  44. Velarde, M.D.; Fry, G.; Tveit, M. Health Effects of Viewing Landscapes: Landscape Types in Environmental Psychology. Urban For. Urban Green. 2007, 6, 199–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Van den Berg, A.; Jorgensen, A.; Wilson, E.R. Evaluating restoration in urban green spaces: Does setting type make a difference? Landsc. Urban Plan. 2014, 127, 173–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Ikei, H.; Song, C.; Igarashi, M.; Namekawa, T.; Miyazaki, Y. Physiological and psychological relaxing effects of visual stimulation with foliage plants in high school students. Adv. Hortic. Sci. 2014, 28, 111–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Park, S.A.; Song, C.; Choi, J.Y.; Son, K.C.; Miyazaki, Y. Foliage plants cause physiological and psychological relaxation as evidenced by measurements of prefrontal cortex activity and profile of mood states. HortScience 2016, 51, 1308–1312. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Kim, S.O.; Jeong, J.E.; Oh, Y.A.; Kim, H.R.; Park, S.A. Comparing Concentration Levels and Emotional States of Children Using Electroencephalography During Horticultural and Nonhorticultural Activities. HortScience 2021, 56, 324–329. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Joye, Y.; van den Berg, A. Restorative environments: An introduction. In Environmental Psychology; Steg, L., van den Berg, A., Eds.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2018; pp. 58–66. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Laumann, K.; Gärling, T.; Morten Stormark, K. Rating scale measures of restorative components of environments. J. Environ. Psychol. 2001, 21, 31–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Hartig, T.; Mitchell, R.; de Vries, S.; Frumkin, H. Nature and health. Ann. Rev. Public Health 2014, 35, 207–228. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Berlucchi, G.; Vallar, G. The history of the neurophysiology and neurology of the parietal lobe. Handb. Clin. Neurol. 2018, 151, 3–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Study procedure. NIRSIT, near-infrared time-resolved spectroscopy.
Figure 1. Study procedure. NIRSIT, near-infrared time-resolved spectroscopy.
Horticulturae 09 01293 g001
Figure 2. Experimental procedure. (A) Experimental scene of tactile stimulation (hand); (B) experimental scene of tactile stimulation (foot).
Figure 2. Experimental procedure. (A) Experimental scene of tactile stimulation (hand); (B) experimental scene of tactile stimulation (foot).
Horticulturae 09 01293 g002
Figure 3. Materials used for the hand tactile experiment. (A) Geranium (Pelargonium inquinans); (B) tiny ardisia (Ardisia pusilla A.DC.); (C) decomposed granite soil; (D) log hardwood (Quercus); and (E) culture soil.
Figure 3. Materials used for the hand tactile experiment. (A) Geranium (Pelargonium inquinans); (B) tiny ardisia (Ardisia pusilla A.DC.); (C) decomposed granite soil; (D) log hardwood (Quercus); and (E) culture soil.
Horticulturae 09 01293 g003
Figure 4. Materials used for the foot tactile experiment. (A) Grass; (B) moss; (C) pebbles; (D) bark; and (E) culture soil.
Figure 4. Materials used for the foot tactile experiment. (A) Grass; (B) moss; (C) pebbles; (D) bark; and (E) culture soil.
Horticulturae 09 01293 g004
Figure 5. Wearing instrument: (A) brain imaging equipment (NIRSIT Lite, OBELAB Inc., Seoul, Republic of Korea); (B) location of channel, source, and detector (NIRSIT Lite Channel Information, OBELAB Inc., Seoul, Republic of Korea).
Figure 5. Wearing instrument: (A) brain imaging equipment (NIRSIT Lite, OBELAB Inc., Seoul, Republic of Korea); (B) location of channel, source, and detector (NIRSIT Lite Channel Information, OBELAB Inc., Seoul, Republic of Korea).
Horticulturae 09 01293 g005
Figure 6. Comparison by section according to materials (hand). (A) Left; (B) right. Statistical significance as determined using the paired t-test. *, ** significant at p < 0.05 or 0.01, respectively; NS, not significant.
Figure 6. Comparison by section according to materials (hand). (A) Left; (B) right. Statistical significance as determined using the paired t-test. *, ** significant at p < 0.05 or 0.01, respectively; NS, not significant.
Horticulturae 09 01293 g006
Figure 7. Comparison of responses using an SDM for stimulation using natural elements (hand). (A) feeling comfortable; (B) feeling relaxed; (C) feeling natural; (D) feeling warm; (E) feeling uneven; (F) feeling moist; and (G) feeling soft. Statistical significance was determined using the paired t-test. *, **, *** significant at p < 0.05, 0.01 or 0.001, respectively; NS, not significant.
Figure 7. Comparison of responses using an SDM for stimulation using natural elements (hand). (A) feeling comfortable; (B) feeling relaxed; (C) feeling natural; (D) feeling warm; (E) feeling uneven; (F) feeling moist; and (G) feeling soft. Statistical significance was determined using the paired t-test. *, **, *** significant at p < 0.05, 0.01 or 0.001, respectively; NS, not significant.
Horticulturae 09 01293 g007aHorticulturae 09 01293 g007b
Figure 8. Comparison by section according to materials (foot). (A) Left; (B) right. Statistical significance was determined using the paired t-test. *, **, *** significant at p < 0.05, 0.01 or 0.001, respectively; NS, not significant.
Figure 8. Comparison by section according to materials (foot). (A) Left; (B) right. Statistical significance was determined using the paired t-test. *, **, *** significant at p < 0.05, 0.01 or 0.001, respectively; NS, not significant.
Horticulturae 09 01293 g008
Figure 9. Comparison of an SDM according to agricultural resources elements (foot). (A) Feeling comfortable; (B) feeling relaxed; (C) feeling natural; (D) feeling warm; (E) feeling uneven; (F) feeling moist; and (G) feeling soft. Statistical significance was determined using the paired t-test. *, **, *** significant at p < 0.05, 0.01 or 0.001, respectively; NS, not significant.
Figure 9. Comparison of an SDM according to agricultural resources elements (foot). (A) Feeling comfortable; (B) feeling relaxed; (C) feeling natural; (D) feeling warm; (E) feeling uneven; (F) feeling moist; and (G) feeling soft. Statistical significance was determined using the paired t-test. *, **, *** significant at p < 0.05, 0.01 or 0.001, respectively; NS, not significant.
Horticulturae 09 01293 g009aHorticulturae 09 01293 g009b
Table 1. Demographic information of the participants in this study.
Table 1. Demographic information of the participants in this study.
VariableMale (n = 7)Female (n = 23)Total (N = 30)
Mean ± SD
Age (years)26.71 ± 2.5632.57 ± 11.2831.20 ± 10.21
Height 1 (cm)173.50 ± 2.19161.50 ± 4.46164.40 ± 6.58
Body weight 2 (kg)75.90 ± 8.7859.10 ± 11.6663.20 ± 13.12
Body mass index 3 (kg∙m−2)25.20 ± 2.8022.60 ± 4.3423.30 ± 4.13
1 Height was measured using an anthropometer (Ok7979; Samhwa, Seoul, Republic of Korea) without shoes; 2 body weight was measured using a body fat analyzer (ioi 353; Jawon Medical, Republic of Korea); 3 body mass index was calculated using the formula: [weight (kg)]/[height (m)2].
Table 2. Change in pulse rate and blood pressure due to tactile stimulation using five natural elements (hand).
Table 2. Change in pulse rate and blood pressure due to tactile stimulation using five natural elements (hand).
VariableNo Touch
(Control)
GeraniumTiny ArdisiaDecomposed Granite SoilLog HardwoodCulture Soil
Mean ± SD 1
Pulse rate77.73 ± 9.8776.63 ± 9.4977.73 ± 9.1678.10 ± 9.5077.07 ± 9.4077.93 ± 8.80
Significance 2 0.3171.0000.7240.4870.857
Systolic pressure117.07 ± 16.60114.37 ± 12.95113.97 ± 13.95116.33 ± 16.43115.40 ± 15.46118.37 ± 15.38
Significance 2 0.0660.012 *0.5320.2060.255
Diastolic pressure75.30 ± 11.8273.33 ± 10.0473.07 ± 9.4174.93 ± 9.7072.87 ± 10.8174.80 ± 10.93
Significance 2 0.045 *0.034 *0.7120.020 *0.644
1 SD: standard deviation. 2 * p < 0.05 using the paired t-test.
Table 3. Change in pulse rate and blood pressure due to tactile stimulation using the five natural elements (foot).
Table 3. Change in pulse rate and blood pressure due to tactile stimulation using the five natural elements (foot).
VariableNo Touch
(Control)
GrassMossPebblesBarkCulture Soil
Mean ± SD 1
Pulse rate75.73 ± 9.6775.47 ± 10.3676.50 ± 10.6475.70 ± 11.3575.93 ± 9.6277.30 ± 11.00
Significance 2 0.8130.4730.9780.8610.266
Systolic pressure114.03 ± 14.28116.83 ± 15.24116.07 ± 15.14114.53 ± 14.08114.90 ± 13.66118.27 ± 16.83
Significance 2 0.036 *0.047 *0.7250.4160.001 **
Diastolic pressure73.97 ± 10.7274.90 ± 10.1075.43 ± 11.4975.50 ± 11.4974.37 ± 8.9775.13 ± 10.42
Significance 2 0.3580.1650.2010.6600.325
1 SD: standard deviation. 2 * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01 using the paired t-test.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Kim, Y.-J.; Choi, S.-W.; Park, S.-A. Effects of Tactile Stimulation Using an Assortment of Natural Elements on the Psychophysiological Responses of Adults. Horticulturae 2023, 9, 1293. https://doi.org/10.3390/horticulturae9121293

AMA Style

Kim Y-J, Choi S-W, Park S-A. Effects of Tactile Stimulation Using an Assortment of Natural Elements on the Psychophysiological Responses of Adults. Horticulturae. 2023; 9(12):1293. https://doi.org/10.3390/horticulturae9121293

Chicago/Turabian Style

Kim, Yun-Jin, Soo-Wan Choi, and Sin-Ae Park. 2023. "Effects of Tactile Stimulation Using an Assortment of Natural Elements on the Psychophysiological Responses of Adults" Horticulturae 9, no. 12: 1293. https://doi.org/10.3390/horticulturae9121293

APA Style

Kim, Y. -J., Choi, S. -W., & Park, S. -A. (2023). Effects of Tactile Stimulation Using an Assortment of Natural Elements on the Psychophysiological Responses of Adults. Horticulturae, 9(12), 1293. https://doi.org/10.3390/horticulturae9121293

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop