Next Article in Journal
Catfish Glycoprotein, a Highly Powerful Safe Preservative of Minced Beef Stored at 4 °C for 15 Days
Next Article in Special Issue
Australian Consumers’ Preferences for Food Attributes: A Latent Profile Analysis
Previous Article in Journal
Scientific Approaches on Extraction, Purification and Stability for the Commercialization of Fucoxanthin Recovered from Brown Algae
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Factors Underlying Food Choice Motives in a Brazilian Sample: The Association with Socioeconomic Factors and Risk Perceptions about Chronic Diseases

by
Camila de Mello Marsola
1,
Luís Miguel Cunha
2,
Joana Pereira de Carvalho-Ferreira
1 and
Diogo Thimoteo da Cunha
1,*
1
Laboratório Multidisciplinar em Alimentos e Saúde, Faculdade de Ciências Aplicadas, Universidade Estadual de Campinas—UNICAMP, R. Pedro Zaccaria, 1300, Limeira SP 13484-350, Brazil
2
GreenUPorto, DGAOT, Faculty of Sciences, University of Porto, Campus Agrário de Vairão, R. da Agrária, 747, 4485–646 Vila do Conde, Portugal
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Foods 2020, 9(8), 1114; https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9081114
Submission received: 23 July 2020 / Revised: 5 August 2020 / Accepted: 10 August 2020 / Published: 13 August 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Individual Determinants of Food Choice in a New Decade)

Abstract

:
This study aimed to evaluate the food choice motives in a sample in Brazil and to identify how socioeconomic characteristics and risk perceptions about chronic diseases and weight gain affect these motives. The Food Choice Questionnaire (FCQ) is an instrument to analyze the motivations for food choice. The FCQ was completed by 525 adult individuals in Brazil. The participants were asked about their perceived risk of gaining weight, developing diabetes, and hypertension. Confirmatory factor analysis led to the rejection of the original FCQ structure, and, after exploratory factor analysis, 30 items of the FCQ were maintained in eight factors: Nutritional Composition, Preparation Convenience, Purchase Convenience, Mood, Sensory Appeal, Health, Price, and Familiarity. Sensory Appeal and Familiarity were, respectively, the most and the least important factors involved in food choices in this sample. A high education level, high income, age, and female sex positively affected FCQ factors (except for the Price factor). On the basis of FCQ scores, we defined five clusters: Health Driven, Practicality Concerned, Shape Concerned, Food Concerned, and Cooking Enthusiasts. In general, individuals were optimistic regarding the risk of developing diabetes, hypertension, and gaining weight, especially those from the Shape Concerned cluster. The differences in food choice motives presented here reinforce the existence of different niches of food consumption. Different types of products can attract specific target groups at the time of choice.

Graphical Abstract

1. Introduction

Making choices is a complex mental process. The choice itself is preceded by the judgment of different options available [1]. According to Sobal et al. [2], “food choice involves the selection and consumption of foods and beverages, considering what, how, when, where, and with whom people eat, as well as other aspects of their food and eating behaviors. Food choices play an important role in the symbolic, economic, and social aspects of life by expressing preferences, identities, and cultural meanings”. In this sense, studying food choices is relevant, as it allows for the identification of consumer demand for supplies, consumption of nutrients, and health issues [2], and also because food patterns change over time.
It is known that the nutritional profile of the Brazilian population has transformed in recent decades. Since the 1970s, the prevalence of undernutrition is decreasing while overweight is increasing [3]. Through a survey called VIGITEL (Chronic Disease by Telephone Survey) that was carried out in all capitals of the 26 Brazilian states and the Federal District, researchers monitored the frequency and distribution of risk and protective factors of noncommunicable chronic diseases. An increase in the prevalence of overweight and obesity in all Brazilian capitals was observed through comparing the results of both the 2006 and 2018 surveys [4,5].
Unhealthy food choices are among the leading risk factors for the development of obesity [6,7,8]. As an example, the recent change in the dietary pattern of the Brazilian population is characterized, among other things, by a reduction in the consumption of fresh foods (e.g., fruits, vegetables, beans, and rice) and an increase in the consumption of ultra-processed and ready-to-eat (RTE) foods [4,5,9]. Data from the 2008–2009 Household Budget Survey (Pesquisa de Orçamentos Familiares, in Portuguese) [10]—a study carried out in Brazil every 10 years that aims to model the consumption profile of the population—indicated an important reduction in consumption of traditional Brazilian foods such as rice, beans, sweet potatoes, cassava, and fish. At the same time, an increase in RTE food consumption, such as bread, stuffed cookies, frozen food, and soft drinks, was observed in the urban population [10].
Several different aspects can influence consumers’ food choices and the transition in the eating patterns of a population over the years [11,12]. However, it is common for these factors to be studied in isolation, devoting little emphasis on the fact that decisions are interconnected, contextualized, and interspersed with different aspects of the consumer’s life [13]. Therefore, it seems imperative to study the influences, combinations, and sets of factors involved in food choice to understand the connections in this process [13]. Some of those factors are consumer-related as the desire for social approval (i.e., leading the individual to follow social norms at the moment of choice), food consumption in the presence of other people [14], income, nutritional status, appetite, mood, pleasure, desires, and cultural factors [1,13,15]. There are also aspects related to food that affect food choices, such as its intrinsic aspects (e.g., appearance, taste, ingredients, aroma) and extrinsic aspects (e.g., packaging, brand, price, origin, souvenirs) [16,17].
An issue not yet studied is how the perceived risk and control of diseases such as diabetes and hypertension can affect—or be affected by—food choice motives. It is also common that people are concerned about gaining weight, which can affect their practices and choices. Risk perception has also been shown to affect the individual’s behavior directly [15]. Therefore, it is likely that a high perceived risk of food-related diseases would influence individuals to make more prudent and healthier food choices, following the Health Belief Model theory [18]. At the same time, when a hazard is perceived as controllable, individuals tend to be more optimistic and exert riskier behaviors [19].
In order to analyze the motivations for food choices, Steptoe, Pollard, and Wardle created a multidimensional instrument called the Food Choice Questionnaire (FCQ) in 1995. The 36-item FCQ aimed to measure the importance of different motives for daily food choices, which are related to health, price, and convenience, among others [20]. As the FCQ is a multidimensional instrument with a unique rating scale, it is possible to identify, within a population, the factors considered most important for food choice [20].
The FCQ is widely used in several countries to assess the food choice motives within different populations, with cultural adaptations and translations made accordingly [21]. It has already been observed that samples from different countries present different motives for food choice [21]. For example, in a study that compared FCQ answers between four countries, Belgium, Hungary, Romania, and the Philippines, it showed that in the first three, the most important factor for food choice was the sensory appeal. At the same time, for Filipinos, it was the health factor [22]. The hierarchy of factors attributed by the study population appears to depend mainly on the culture, educational level, and circumstances of the countries in which the studies are developed [23]. For example, country-of-origin of foods may affect food choices of Europeans [24], but less likely to affect Brazilians, since they produce most of the food they consume [25]. Moreover, although in some populations, convenience is an important factor in food choice [20], in countries with high consumption of home-made foods and with limited access to the food industry, convenience does not seem to be an important factor [23].
The FCQ was translated into Portuguese and adapted for the Brazilian culture by Heitor et al. [26]. This same research group conducted a study with a Brazilian sample aiming to analyze the dimensional structure and reliability of the FCQ [27]. However, no study was conducted in Brazil to explore other factors underlying food choices, such as risk perceptions, for example.
Considering the above, it is possible to realize the importance of a more in-depth study of the motivations linked to daily food choice considering the complexity of this process in a Brazilian sample. Therefore, the present study aimed to evaluate the food choice motives in a sample in Brazil, to identify whether and how socioeconomic issues affect food choice motives, as well as how food choice motives associate with risk perceptions about chronic diseases and weight gain.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Sample

In this cross-sectional study, individuals over 18 years old from the region of Limeira (São Paulo State, Brazil) were included.
First, a pilot study was carried out with 50 people to estimate the necessary sample. Considering the results, the sample was calculated considering beta error = 1%; alpha error = 5%; effect size = 0.30, requiring 204 participants [28]. As the study involved factor and cluster analysis, as well as regression models, a more robust and conservative sample of 525 individuals was chosen. This sample would allow the use of up to 17 predictors variables in the regression model, following the recommendations of Pedhazur and Schmelkin (1991) [29].
Recruitment took place in different locations to cover different socioeconomic contexts, including a university, public squares, health posts, and condominiums. The participants were recruited face-to-face, using a convenient, intentional, and reasoned sampling with predetermined quotas for age and gender [30]. The sample was collected in order to be equivalent to population characteristics, such as sex (52% female), age (10.5% elderly), and income (50% with income below four times the minimum wage). The education level was categorized as low (incomplete elementary school to incomplete high school) and high (complete high school to complete higher education). The family income was categorized as low (one to four times the minimum wage per month) and high (more than five times the minimum wage per month).
Three trained researchers administered the questionnaires face-to-face. All the participants signed an informed consent form before participating in the study. The Ethics Committee of the State University of Campinas approved the study (CAAE number: 91222418.5.0000.5404).

2.2. Evaluation of Food Choices

First, the individuals answered questions about their socioeconomic profiles, such as family income, education level, marital status, and the number of children, in order to characterize the sample.
The FCQ was developed by Steptoe, Pollard, and Wardle [20], and adapted for Brazilian culture by Heitor et al. [26]. The questionnaire has 36 items and is organized initially into nine factors: health, mood, convenience, sensory appeal, natural content, price, weight control, familiarity, and ethical concern. Answers were given using a seven-point scale, with the description in all scale points, ranging from (1) strongly disagree to (7) strongly agree. This scale adaptation was already widely used [23].

2.3. Evaluation of Risk and Control Perceptions

The participants were asked about their risk of developing diabetes, hypertension, and gaining weight, as listed below:
  • R1—What is the chance of you developing diabetes, maintaining your current lifestyle?/Qual a chance de você desenvolver diabetes, mantendo seu estilo de vida atual?
  • R2—What is the chance of you developing hypertension, maintaining your current lifestyle?/Qual a chance de você desenvolver hipertensão, mantendo seu estilo de vida atual?
  • R3—What is the chance that you will gain 3 to 10 kg in the next 12 months, maintaining your current lifestyle?/Qual a chance de você engordar 3 a 10 kg nos próximos 12 meses, mantendo seus hábitos atuais?
The risk perception questions were made through considering the recommendation of Helweg-Larsen and Shepperd [31]. The risk perception assessment was measured through a structured psychometric scale with seven options of intensity descriptors from −3 (extremely low) to +3 (extremely high) [32]. All scale points had a specific descriptor.
To verify the perceived control over health and avoiding chronic diseases such as diabetes and hypertension, we used the following two statements:
  • C1—“I believe I have control over my health”/Eu acredito que tenho controle sobre a minha saúde.
  • C2—“There is not much I can do to prevent the development of diseases such as diabetes and hypertension”/Não existe muito o que eu possa fazer para evitar o desenvolvimento de doenças como diabetes e hipertensão.
The control statements were made following the same referential as risk perception [31]. Answers were given using the same aforementioned seven-point scale.

2.4. Data Analysis

The theoretical distributions of the variables were analyzed using means, deviations, and the histogram of distribution. The Kolmogorov–Smirnov’s test (with Lillefors correction) was used to check normality of data.
The FCQ structure was evaluated by confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) and later by exploratory factor analysis (EFA).
CFA, using a covariance-based structural equation modeling (SEM), was performed to verify the adequacy of the data for the original FCQ structure. The validity of the FCQ factors was assessed using Cronbach’s alpha, with values above 0.60 being considered adequate. The model adjustment was analyzed according to Hu and Bentler [33] using the comparative fit index (CFI) >0.90, standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR) <0.10, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) <0.06, and PClose (p-value for testing the null hypothesis that RMSEA is no higher than 0.05).
Since an adequate fit of the original FCQ structure was not observed, we performed EFA. In the EFA, valid items from the FCQ were extracted, considering only those with factorial loads above 0.40. EFA was performed with varimax rotation. A Kaiser–Meyer–Olkin (KMO) value greater than 0.70 was used to verify the adequacy. The new model was again submitted to CFA to assess the fit.
The FCQ factors were compared using repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with the Bonferroni post hoc test.
Generalized linear models (GLM) were used to determine which variables were associated with food choice factors (normalized variables). First, multiple linear models were developed, however, these models did not present adequate fit. For this reason, linear mixed effect models were chosen. The independent variables in each model were those variables that presented a Pearson correlation coefficient greater than 0.30, and differences in Student’s t-test. Homoscedasticity and model fit were evaluated by residual analysis and the chi-squared test.
K-means cluster analysis was used to identify patterns in food choices. The normalized scores for each FCQ factor were included in the analysis. The cluster’s quality was observed through comparing the averages of the scores of each cluster through one-way ANOVA. The comparison of risk perceptions between clusters was made using one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni’s post hoc test. Levene’s test assessed the equality of variance. The clusters’ names were given on the basis of salient factors of FCQ.
The CFA was conducted using the Analysis of Moment Structures—AMOS v.26 software. All the other tests were conducted using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences—SPSS 20.0.1.

3. Results

A total of 525 individuals, with a mean age of 34.5 years old (standard deviation (SD) of 16.4), were enrolled in this study. The majority are women (61%) and single (56.8%). The socio-demographic characterization of the sample can be seen in Table 1.

3.1. Food Choice Questionnaire’s Structure

The final model, with a better fit, was different from the original FCQ structure. A model with the following indicators was found: CMIN/DF = 0.882; SRMR = 0.067; RMSEA = 0.065; PClose = 0.01; CFI = 0.88. After EFA, 5 of the 36 items originally present were removed. A total of 31 items were kept in the confirmatory factor analysis (CFA), and these comprised the 8 factors (Table 2). According to the rearrangement of the FCQ structure from CFA, some factors were renamed. Nutritional Composition was a new factor formed with items present in the former questionnaire in the Weight Control and Natural Content factors. The items that originally formed the Convenience factor were separated into two new factors named Convenience of Preparation and Convenience of Purchase. The Ethical Concern factor did not appear to be related to the other items evaluated in the instrument and was excluded for a better model fit.
Among the eight factors of the FCQ, Sensory Appeal presented the highest importance score attributed by the sample (Table 1). The factors of Price, Convenience of Purchase, and Health were the second most important, with no differences between them. Thirdly were the factors of Convenience of Preparation, Mood, and Nutritional Composition. Finally, the Familiarity factor was the least important one.
It is possible to observe an adequate discriminant validity, with r > 0.30 in most correlations (Table 3). A moderate positive correlation was found between Health and Nutritional Composition.

3.2. Food Choice Motives

The mean scores attributed by women, in all factors of the FCQ, were significantly higher (p < 0.001) than the scores attributed by men.
Five well-defined clusters were created, namely, Health Driven, Practicality Concerned, Shape Concerned, Food Concerned, and Cooking Enthusiasts (Table 4). The cluster names refer to the FCQ factors with the highest and lowest averages within each cluster. For the cluster named Health Driven, the most salient factors were Health and Convenience of Preparation, and the least important were Familiarity and Mood, indicating the appreciation of healthy food that is easily prepared, but that keeps satisfactory sensory characteristics. In terms of Practicality Concerned, individuals were identified as people who value practicality above other motives when it comes to eating, i.e., prefer food that is easy to buy, easily prepared, and with high sensory appeal. Individuals in this cluster showed lower concern for aspects such as Nutritional Composition and Health. In Shape Concerned, individuals showed concern for food with satisfying sensory characteristics, fair price, and proper nutritional composition. Antagonistically, people in this cluster showed less concern for Health, indicating that the concern with Nutritional Composition is probably related to issues of diet and body weight. In Food Concerned, all factors showed high averages. In this cluster, higher importance was attributed to Nutritional Composition, Preparation Convenience, and Price. Finally, in Cooking Enthusiasts, the individuals showed importance for foods with excellent sensory characteristics that are good for health and that are easy to buy. The counterpoint was the low importance of the practicality of the preparation, suggesting that people in this cluster probably take pleasure in preparing their meals.

3.3. Risk Perceptions and the Relationship with Food Choice Motives

In general, individuals show an optimistic risk perception of having diabetes, hypertension, and weight gain (Figure 1A), i.e., they perceived, in general, a low personal risk for the indicated hazards, indicated by the negative average values. Moreover, they perceived a high control about their health and low lack of control about diabetes and hypertension (Figure 1B), i.e., they believe that they can prevent the development of diabetes and hypertension.
When comparing the differences between sex, men presented a lower risk perception of gaining weight (−0.93) than women (−0.52; p = 0.016).
However, individuals in cluster 2 (Practicality Concerned) indicated that they perceived a somewhat higher risk of having diabetes and hypertension and a regular risk for weight gain. Individuals in cluster 3 (Shape Concerned) seemed quite optimistic about the risk of weight gain, especially compared to clusters 2 and 4 (Figure 2).
Regarding the perception of health control (C1), in all clusters the mean value was higher than 4.1, with 1—Health Driven (5.0 ± 1.4); 2—Practicality Concerned (4.1 ± 1.6); 3—Shape Concerned (4.4 ± 2.3), 4—Food Concerned (5.1 ± 1.6); and 5—Cooking Enthusiasts (4.6 ± 1.7). Clusters 1 and 4 showed higher perceived control than clusters 2 and 5. There was no difference between the clusters regarding the control to prevent diabetes and hypertension (C2).
Table 5 shows the effects of the independent variables on the FCQ factors. Odds ratio (OR) < 1.0 denotes a negative effect (i.e., reduction) and OR > 1.0 denotes a positive effect (i.e., increasing) of the independent variables on the FCQ factors.
Socioeconomic factors affect, in different ways, the food choice motives. A negative effect of the male sex was observed in the scores of Nutritional Composition, Sensory Appeal, Health, Price, and Familiarity. However, with increasing age, men care more about Nutritional Composition. Having no children reduced individuals’ perceived importance of the Nutritional Composition, Convenience of Preparation, and Familiarity factors. According to the interactions observed in models 5 and 6, individuals without children tended to increase the importance of Familiarity and Convenience of Preparation with age. Notwithstanding, age did not affect these factors independently. The perceived control over health had a positive effect on the Nutritional Composition and Health factors.
A low education level and a high family income seemed to reduce the importance of food choice motives. There was a negative effect of less education on the scores of Sensory Appeal, Health, Familiarity, and Convenience of Purchase. In contrast, high income was related to lower scores for Price and Convenience of Preparation. However, an interaction between high income and age was also related to lower scores for Familiarity. It was decided not to keep the education and income variables in the same models in order to avoid collinearity, with only the one with higher explanatory power and better adjustment remaining.
No significant model emerged from the adjustment of the Mood factor.

4. Discussion

4.1. FCQ’s Structure

When CFA was applied in the present study, we changed the structure of the original questionnaire for a better model fit, with the exclusion of six items and rearrangement of the factors. These modifications are common and expected in different populations, and are explained by cultural differences, including regionalism, access to food, socioeconomic aspects, and food culture [23,34]. Even though the study conducted by Heitor et al. [27] was also carried out in Brazil, our results ended up differing in terms of the resulting FCQ structure. The differences in the regions where the studies were conducted, as well as the heterogeneity of the country, may explain these differences.
It appears to be a trend in some studies that the items related to Health, Natural Content, and Weight Control converge into a single factor [34,35]. In the present study, items of Natural Content and Weight Control were merged into the new Nutritional Composition factor, on the basis of the EFA. Although Health items were not included in this same factor, as shown in previous studies [34,35], it is worth mentioning that there was a moderate positive correlation between Health and Nutritional Composition. Moreover, it was found that Convenience can be differently perceived when food preparation or purchase is considered. The segregation of the Convenience factor was also observed in the Hungarian population [35]. It is suggested that, for some people, the complexity or time used to cook may not be a nuisance, but it may be necessary for those people that food is easily accessible for purchase.

4.2. Food Choice Motives

When comparing the eight FCQ factors, Sensory Appeal was the one with the highest average, indicating this factor as the leading food choice motive, which has been previously shown in other countries [23,35,36]. This factor is related to the pleasure of eating. Indeed, it is well established that food intake goes beyond the objective of nourishing the body [1], and hedonic issues related to food are also important in this process [37]. The central issue regarding the appreciation of sensory aspects is that food is considered a natural reward, since gratification from food consumption leads to the production of dopamine that activates reward and pleasure centers in the brain, especially considering highly palatable foods [15,38]. The problem lies when consumers overvalue sensory appeal and choose high palatable foods. This repetitive behavior, associated with an obesogenic environment, can override the signs of hunger and satiety, leading to excesses and less healthy food choices [15]. A positive hedonic interpretation of food increases the feeling of well-being and joy after eating and is associated with fullness during and after eating [39].
As examples of hyper-palatable foods, there are ultra-processed foods, which have a low cost and high convenience (of availability and preparation), making them an easy choice [40]. Examples of ultra-processed foods with these characteristics are soft drinks, chocolate, margarine, dehydrated soups, sausages, and instant noodles [41]. It is known that ultra-processed products dominate the food supply in high-income countries, and their consumption has overgrown in middle-income countries by replacing home-cooked meals with RTE foods [42]. There is some evidence that the high consumption of ultra-processed foods is harmful to health [43,44], due to high amounts of calories, fats, and sugars. Given this, the results of the present study reinforce the importance of attention to these aspects, since, as noted, health and an adequate nutritional composition are not the primary food choice motives. It is worth mentioning that the sensory aspects of eating includes a number of other aspects besides palatability, such as visual cues, smell, taste, and texture [45]. Therefore, health professionals and the food industry have a role on stimulating these other sensory aspects on less palatable and more healthy foods, making them as wanted as palatable foods, which would potentially contribute to better food choices and weight management.

4.3. Risk Perceptions and Food Choice Patterns

The individuals presented a low-risk perception of having diabetes, hypertension, and gaining weight, especially for the latter. In contrast, a high perceived control about their health was shown. Events perceived as controllable tend to be expressed with more optimism by people [46]. If the person thinks he/she is in control of the situation, he/she probably believes that his/her actions can increase the chances of a positive outcome [19]. This explains why the perception of risk about weight gain was lower than the risk perception of having diabetes and hypertension, despite Brazil ranking third in the world for the absolute number of obese adult men and the fifth position for the number of obese women during 2014 [47]. Some differences in the perceived risk were observed among different food choices clusters, which is further discussed.
As a result of the food choice motives, the studied population could be divided into five well-defined clusters, defining food choice patterns. The Food Concerned cluster was labeled as so, because in this cluster all the factors of the FCQ were somehow important for the choice of food and because it can be composed of people who pay attention to their food choices as a whole. In contrast, all other clusters had distinctions in the characteristics that define them.
The Cooking Enthusiasts cluster was characterized by individuals looking for accessible foods with excellent sensory characteristics, but they do not seem to care about the complexity of the preparation. The label assigned to this cluster was based on people who dedicate themselves to preparing meals and take pleasure in doing so. Home cooking is strongly encouraged by the Food Guide for the Brazilian Population of the Ministry of Health [41]. The pleasure in home cooking is an essential predictor of culinary skills, which correlate positively with the weekly consumption of vegetables and negatively with the consumption of “convenient foods” and RTE food [48]. It can be suggested that people in this cluster would potentially have positive health-related behavior.
The Practicality Concerned cluster is, in a way, antagonistic to the Cooking Enthusiasts cluster since the primary motive observed for food choices was the Preparation Convenience and the least important were related to Health and Nutritional Composition of the food. This importance attributed to Convenience may be associated with the choice of ultra-processed foods [49], whose consumption has been linked to poor quality of diet and, among other factors, to the increase in the prevalence of chronic diseases [50,51]. Individuals in the Practicality Concerned cluster have a lower perception of control over their health than individuals in the Health and Food Concerned clusters. Moreover, they have a higher perception of personal risk for diabetes and hypertension than other clusters and a neutral perception of the risk of weight gain. This may indicate that the members of this cluster may be aware of the possible harms of their food choices, and deliberately neglect this aspect. The low score of the Health factor in this cluster reinforces this hypothesis. Contrary to the Cooking Enthusiasts, the Practically Concerned cluster indicates a risky health-related behavior, and this is useful information considering the use of FCQ in research and clinical practice.
Individuals from the Shape Concerned cluster attributed importance to Nutritional Composition and Sensory Appeal but also attributed lower scores to the Health factor. This result provokes reflections about food composition being overrated, with particular attention to the amount of fat or caloric value, but not necessarily due to health concerns. Another hypothesis is that a food low on calories, fat, and without additives may be what this group of consumers understand as healthy food, especially due to the concern with weight or body shape for aesthetic reasons. This kind of thinking presents several implications in food behavior. For example, eating food perceived as healthy was previously associated with an increase in hunger [52], and a compensatory effect occurs when eating a perceived low-fat food in comparison to a perceived high-fat food [53], which can count for unsuccessful weight loss diets and be related to psychological distress. There are many diets extolled by the media, known as fad diets, in which the nutritional composition (carbohydrate, protein, or fat content), caloric value, and the final result of the diet on the body are often highlighted [54]. Moreover, information about diets is published in blogs and magazines, including menus, with the attraction of quick results, which can potentially put consumers who follow these recommendations without professional counseling at risk [55,56]. These considerations raise attention to the interdisciplinary aspect of food choices/behavior, which requires, consequently, interdisciplinary approaches in health and policy practices.
Although the score attributed to the Health factor by the individuals in the Shape Concerned cluster was lower, they were quite optimistic about their own risk of developing diabetes; hypertension; and above all, gaining weight. Individuals in this cluster believe that they may be healthy with the consumption of foods low in calories, fat, and additives. However, they fail to identify the importance of nutrients, fiber, and other motives such as mood and sensory appeal. When the stereotypes attributed to high- and low-fat consumption were investigated, a low-fat diet was associated with a “thin” and “healthy” person. In contrast, a high-fat diet was associated with “unhealthy” and “overweight” people, but who are happier [57]. These results demonstrate the dichotomy between health and eating pleasure, as well as the relationship between health and body weight with the nutritional composition of the food eaten. Probably, when concerned with aspects related to food, such as having little fat or low calories, individuals already believe that, even in an indirect way, they are taking a protective attitude towards their health, even if they do not think directly of health at the time of food choice.

4.4. Modelling Food Choice Motives

Finally, the use of linear models sought to evaluate the variables that affect the FCQ factors. It was observed that the importance of factors like Nutritional Composition and Health increased with age, as well as the interaction between male gender and age, increasing the importance given to Nutritional Composition. Data from 2008–2009 Household Budget Survey (Pesquisa de Orçamentos Familiares, in Portuguese) [10] showed that the consumption of ultra-processed foods reduces with increasing age. These changes in food consumption can be understood as a form of self-care. This self-care seems to increase over time when the feeling of being closer to the exposure of diseases linked to poor health habits is less latent. Younger people have a lower risk perception of adverse events that they understand as controllable, such as chronic noncommunicable diseases, and consequently, they exert riskier behaviors [58,59]. It was observed that the perception of control over health positively affected the factors of Health and Nutritional Composition, as well as scoring higher for the Health Driven and Food Concerned clusters. However, the relationship between practices and risk perception is complex. Practices understood as adequate might reduce the risk perception (self-efficacy). On the other hand, a low-risk perception might lead the individual to exert risky behaviors since the hazard is perceived as controllable (the illusion of control) [60].
The comparison between sexes indicated that higher scores were attributed by women to all the investigated food choice motives, as well as being male having an independent negative effect on the scores of Nutritional Composition, Sensory Appeal, Health, Price, and Familiarity. Higher scores were also attributed by women in London, with the attribution of greater importance to Natural Content and Weight Control being associated with a better quality of diet [49].
Men presented less risk perception to gain weight. Previous findings demonstrate that women tend to have higher body dissatisfaction and concern about weight gain [61,62]. Being female and being afraid of gaining weight are associated with the practice of diets, even in women with normal weight [61,63], which may be related to taking actions to avoid weight gain and thus reduce the perceived risk [64].
Women tend to have healthier food consumption than men, as well as more concern for food quality and healthcare, justifying this result [5,65,66]. These results may also be related to the fact that women are primarily responsible for preparing meals at home and have better culinary ability [48]. Women seem to be more health-conscious and have more pleasure in cooking than men [48]. According to the cited authors, men’s motivation to cook is possibly because they want to do it, instead of being an everyday obligation.
As expected, socioeconomic aspects such as income and education also affected food choice motives. According to the 2017–2018 POF [67], food was equivalent to 17.5% of the average consumption expenses of Brazilian families, with higher expenses for housing (36.6%) and transportation (18.1%). The survey cites a reduction in this percentage, highlighting that in the 2008–2009 survey, the contribution of food to consumption expenses was 19.8%. Therefore, although food still has a significant representation in Brazilian citizens’ expenses, this reduction may represent a greater interest in saving family resources, which may be related to the findings in the present study, since Price had the second-highest importance. This need to save money can also be reflected in the quality of the food, since most of the ultra-processed foods are also low cost [42].

5. Conclusions

It was possible to observe a heterogeneous profile of the motives for choosing food in Brazil. The choices were strongly linked to socioeconomic factors, but valuing sensorial aspects, with a well-presented role for tasty and nice-smelling food. For this reason, Sensory Appeal was the factor with the highest score. The factors of Price, Convenience of Purchase, and Health also showed higher averages than the other factors. The perception of control over health also affected the motives of food choice. Most participants perceived themselves as low risk for diabetes, hypertension, and weight gain, independently of their food choice motives. However, those more concerned with practicality when eating showed a less optimistic view about these hazards.
The identified clusters demonstrated that the factors must be understood together, facilitating the identification of patterns. Individuals with motivations geared to fitness (Shape Concerned) or quick meals (Practically Concerned) may have a poor diet, requiring more attention by health professionals. Moreover, traditional health messages and campaigns may not be effective for this audience.
The hierarchy of factors does seem to depend on the country in which the FCQ is applied. Factors such as education, income, age, and sex can affect the valuation of the factors. Therefore, this highlights the importance of the reproducibility of this type of research, investigating the reasons involved in food choices across different populations.
The interrelation of factors demonstrates the importance of public policies and health campaigns. Professionals in this sector need to have a comprehensive view of these aspects to have more effective practices. Policies for food access, information campaigns to raise awareness about nutritional quality, and factors related to the development of chronic diseases are essential, but equally as important to review how these messages are addressed. Thus, a focus on nutritional value and health alone may not be sufficient for everyone, with a potential strategy being to expand the approach to other factors.

Author Contributions

C.d.M.M. was responsible for conceptualization, methodology, funding acquisition, formal analysis, investigation, and writing—original draft; L.M.C. was responsible for visualization, funding acquisition, and writing—review and editing; J.P.d.C.-F. was responsible for visualization, funding acquisition, and writing—review and editing; D.T.d.C. was responsible for conceptualization, formal analysis, funding acquisition, methodology, project administration, resources, supervision, visualization, and writing—review and editing. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Fundação de Amparo à Pesquisa do Estado de São Paulo, FAPESP (process number: 2018/12625-9 and 2019/10936-0). This study was financed in part by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior—Brasil (CAPES)—Finance Code 001. Author L.M. Cunha acknowledges financial support from the national funds by Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (FCT), Portugal, within the scope of UIDB/05748/2020 and UIDP/05748/2020

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Alvarenga, M.; Figueiredo, M.; Timerman, F.; Antonaccio, C. Nutrição Comportamental, 1st ed.; Coutinho, W.L., Hosaka, A.M.d.S., Eds.; Editora Manole Ltda: Barueri, Brasil, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  2. Sobal, J.; Bisogni, C.A.; Devine, C.M.; Jastran, M. A Conptual Model of the Food Choice Progress over the Life Course. In The Psychology of Food Choice; Shepherd, R., Raats, M., Eds.; Biddles Ltd.: King’s Lynn, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
  3. Filho, M.B.; Rissin, A. Nutritional transition in Brazil: Geographic and temporal trends. Cad. Saude Publica 2003, 19, S181–S191. [Google Scholar]
  4. Brasíl Ministério da Saúde. Vigitel Brasil 2006: Vigilância de Fatores de Risco e Proteção Para Doenças Crônicas Por Inquérito Telefônico; Brasíl Ministério da Saúde: Brasília, Brazil.
  5. Brasil Ministério da Saúde. Vigitel Brasil 2018: Vigilância de Fatores de Risco e Proteção Para Doenças Crônicas Por Inquérito Telefônico. Brasil Ministério da Saúde: Brasília, Brazil, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  6. Lee, E.Y.; Yoon, K.-H. Epidemic obesity in children and adolescents: Risk factors and prevention. Front. Med. 2018, 12, 658–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  7. Canella, D.S.; Levy, R.B.; Martins, A.P.B.; Claro, R.M.; Moubarac, J.C.; Baraldi, L.G.; Cannon, G.; Monteiro, C.A. Ultra-processed food products and obesity in Brazilian households (2008–2009). PLoS ONE 2014, 9, e92752. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  8. Monteiro, C.A.; Moubarac, J.-C.; Levy, R.B.; Canella, D.S.; da Costa Louzada, M.L.; Cannon, G. Household availability of ultra-processed foods and obesity in nineteen European countries. Public Health Nutr. 2018, 21, 18–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  9. Martins, A.P.B.; Levy, R.B.; Claro, R.M.; Moubarac, J.C.; Monteiro, C.A. Increased contribution of ultra-processed food products in the Brazilian diet (1987–2009). Rev. Saúde Pública 2013, 47, 656–665. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. IBGE-Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística. Pesquisa de Orçamentos Familiares—2008/2009; IBGE: Rio de Janeiro, Brazil, 2011.
  11. Sibai, A.M.; Nasreddine, L.; Mokdad, A.H.; Adra, N.; Tabet, M.; Hwalla, N. Nutrition Transition and Cardiovascular Disease Risk Factors in Middle East and North Africa Countries: Reviewing the Evidence. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2010, 57, 193–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Popkin, B.M. The shift in stages of the nutrition transition in the developing world differs from past experiences! Public Health Nutr. 2002, 5, 205–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Sobal, J.; Bisogni, C.A.; Jastran, M. Food Choice Is Multifaceted, Contextual, Dynamic, Multilevel, Integrated, and Diverse. Mind Brain Educ. 2014, 8, 6–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Higgs, S. Social norms and their influence on eating behaviours. Appetite 2015, 86, 38–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Singh, M. Mood, food and obesity. Front. Psychol. 2014, 5, 1–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Chaya, C.; Eaton, C.; Hewson, L.; Vázquez, R.F.; Fernández-Ruiz, V.; Smart, K.A.; Hort, J. Developing a reduced consumer-led lexicon to measure emotional response to beer. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 45, 100–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Deliza, R.; Macfie, H.J.H. The generation of sensory expectation by external cues and its effect on sensory perception and hedonic ratings: A review. J. Sens. Stud. 1996, 11, 103–128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Champion, V.L.; Skinner, C.S. The health belief model. In Health Behavior and Health Education: Theory, Research, and Practice; Glanz, K., Rimer, B.K., Viswanath, K., Eds.; John Wiley & Sons: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2008; pp. 45–65. [Google Scholar]
  19. McKenna, F.P. It won’t happen to me: Unrealistic optimism or illusion of control? Br. J. Psychol. 1993, 84, 39–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Steptoe, A.; Pollard, T.M.; Wardle, J. Development of a Measure of the Motives Underlying the Selection of Food: The Food Choice Questionnaire. Appetite 1995, 25, 267–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  21. Cunha, L.M.; Cabral, D.; Moura, A.P.; de Almeida, M.D.V. Application of the Food Choice Questionnaire across cultures: Systematic review of cross-cultural and single country studies. Food Qual. Prefer. 2018, 64, 21–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Januszewska, R.; Pieniak, Z.; Verbeke, W. Food choice questionnaire revisited in four countries. Does it still measure the same? Appetite 2011, 57, 94–98. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Cabral, D.; de Almeida, M.D.V.; Cunha, L.M. Food Choice Questionnaire in an African country—Application and validation in Cape Verde. Food Qual. Prefer. 2017, 62, 90–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Knight, J.G.; Holdsworth, D.K.; Mather, D.W. Country-of-origin and choice of food imports: An in-depth study of European distribution channel gatekeepers. J. Int. Bus. Stud. 2007, 38, 107–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. de Onis, J. Brazil’s Big Moment: A South American Giant Wakes Up. Foreign Aff. 2008, 87, 110–122. [Google Scholar]
  26. Heitor, S.F.D.; Estima, C.C.P.; Das Neves, F.J.; De Aguiar, A.S.; de Souza Castro, S.; de Souza Ferreira, J.E. Tradução e adaptação cultural do questionário sobre motivo das escolhas alimentares (Food Choice Questionnaire—FCQ) para a língua portuguesa. Cienc. Saude Coletiva 2015, 20, 2339–2346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Heitor, S.F.D.; Reichenheim, M.E.; de Souza Ferreira, J.E.; de Souza Castro, S. Validity of the factorial structure of the brazilian version scale of the food choice questionnaire. Cienc. Saude Coletiva 2019, 24, 3551–3561. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Dean, A.G.; Sullivan, K.M.; Soe, M.M. Epi Info and OpenEpi in Epidemiology and Clinical Medicine: Health Applications of Free Software; CreateSpace: Scotts Valley, CA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  29. Pedhazur, E.J.; Schmelkin, L.P. Measurement, Design, and Analysis: An Integrated Approach; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Hillside, NJ, USA, 1991. [Google Scholar]
  30. Malhotra, N.K.; Nunan, D.; Birks, D.F. Marketing Research, 5th ed.; Pearson Education: Edinburgh, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  31. Helweg-Larsen, M.; Shepperd, J.A. Do Moderators of the Optimistic Bias Affect Personal or Target Risk Estimates? A Review of the Literature. Personal. Soc. Psychol. Rev. 2001, 5, 74–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Raats, M.M.; Sparks, P.; Geekie, M.A.; Shepherd, R. The effects of providing personalized dietary feedback. A semi-computerized approach. Patient Educ. Couns. 1999, 37, 177–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Hu, L.T.; Bentler, P.M. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct. Equ. Model. A Multidiscip. J. 1999, 6, 1–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Milošević, J.; Žeželj, I.; Gorton, M.; Barjolle, D. Understanding the motives for food choice in Western Balkan Countries. Appetite 2012, 58, 205–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Szakály, Z.; Kontor, E.; Kovács, S.; Popp, J.; Pető, K.; Polereczki, Z. Adaptation of the Food Choice Questionnaire: The case of Hungary. Br. Food J. 2018, 120, 1474–1488. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Carrillo, E.; Varela, P.; Salvador, A.; Fiszman, S. Main Factors Underlying Consumers’ Food Choice: A First Step For The Understanding Of Attitudes Toward “Healthy Eating”. J. Sens. Stud. 2011, 26, 85–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Carvalho-Ferreira, J.P.d.; Finlayson, G.; da Cunha, D.T.; Caldas, G.; Bandoni, D.; de Rosso, V.V. Adiposity and binge eating are related to liking and wanting for food in Brazil: A cultural adaptation of the Leeds Food Preference Questionnaire. Appetite 2019, 133, 174–183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Berthoud, H.-R. Homeostatic and Non-homeostatic Pathways Involved in the Control of Food Intake and Energy Balance. Obesity 2006, 14, 197S–200S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Duerlund, M.; Andersen, B.V.; Grønbeck, M.S.; Byrne, D.V. Consumer reflections on post-ingestive sensations. A qualitative approach by means of focus group interviews. Appetite 2019, 142, 104350. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Monteiro, C.A.; Cannon, G.; Levy, R.B.; Moubarac, J.-C.; Louzada, M.L.; Rauber, F.; Khandpur, N.; Cediel, G.; Neri, D.; Martinez-Steele, E.; et al. Ultra-processed foods: What they are and how to identify them. Public Health Nutr. 2019, 22, 936–941. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Departamento de Atenção Básica, Secretaria de Atenção à Saúde, Ministério da Saúde. Guia Alimentar Para a População Brasileira; Ministério da saúde: Brasília, Brazil, 2014; ISBN 9788533421769.
  42. Monteiro, C.A.; Moubarac, J.C.; Cannon, G.; Ng, S.W.; Popkin, B. Ultra-processed products are becoming dominant in the global food system. Obes. Rev. 2013, 14, 21–28. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  43. Fiolet, T.; Srour, B.; Sellem, L.; Kesse-Guyot, E.; Allès, B.; Méjean, C.; Deschasaux, M.; Fassier, P.; Latino-Martel, P.; Beslay, M.; et al. Consumption of ultra-processed foods and cancer risk: Results from NutriNet-Santé prospective cohort. BMJ 2018, 360, k322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  44. Srour, B.; Fezeu, L.K.; Kesse-Guyot, E.; Allès, B.; Méjean, C.; Andrianasolo, R.M.; Chazelas, E.; Deschasaux, M.; Hercberg, S.; Galan, P.; et al. Ultra-processed food intake and risk of cardiovascular disease: Prospective cohort study (NutriNet-Santé). BMJ 2019, 365, 11451. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. McCrickerd, K.; Forde, C.G. Sensory influences on food intake control: Moving beyond palatability. Obes. Rev. 2016, 17, 18–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  46. Weinstein, N.D. Unrealistic optimism about future life events. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 1980, 39, 806–820. [Google Scholar]
  47. Gomes, D.C.K.; Sichieri, R.; Junior, E.V.; Boccolini, C.S.; de Moura Souza, A.; Cunha, D.B. Trends in obesity prevalence among Brazilian adults from 2002 to 2013 by educational level. BMC Public Health 2019, 19, 965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Hartmann, C.; Dohle, S.; Siegrist, M. Importance of cooking skills for balanced food choices. Appetite 2013, 65, 125–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Pollard, T.M.; Steptoe, A.; Wardle, J. Motives underlying healthy eating: Using the food choice questionnaire to explain variation in dietary intake. J. Biosoc. Sci. 1998, 30, 165–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. da Costa Louzada, M.L.; Martins, A.P.B.; Canella, D.S.; Baraldi, L.G.; Levy, R.B.; Claro, R.M.; Moubarac, J.C.; Cannon, G.; Monteiro, C.A. Ultra-processed foods and the nutritional dietary profile in Brazil. Rev. Saude Publica 2015, 49, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  51. Monteiro, C.A. Nutrition and health. The issue is not food, nor nutrients, so much as processing. Public Health Nutr. 2009, 12, 729–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  52. Finkelstein, S.R.; Fishbach, A. When Healthy Food Makes You Hungry. J. Consum. Res. Inc. 2010, 37, 357–367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  53. Carvalho-Ferreira, J.P.d.; da Cunha, D.T.; Finlayson, G.; Caldas, G.; Jamar, G.; Bandoni, D.H.; Pisani, L.P.; de Rosso, V.V. Differential impact of consuming foods perceived to be high or low in fat on subsequent food reward. Food Qual. Prefer. 2020, 85, 103977. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Kuchkuntla, A.R.; Limketkai, B.; Nanda, S.; Hurt, R.T.; Mundi, M.S. Fad Diets: Hype or Hope? Curr. Nutr. Rep. 2018, 7, 310–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. de Assis BRAGA, D.C.; Coletro, H.N.; de Freitas, M.T. Nutritional composition of fad diets published on websites and blogs. Rev. Nutr. 2019, 32, e170190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Marangoni, J.S.; Maniglia, F.P. Nutrition analysis of diets published in magazines for women. Rev. Assoc. Bras. Nutr. 2017, 7894, 31–36. [Google Scholar]
  57. Barker, M.E.; Tandy, M.; Stookey, J.D. How are consumers of low-fat and high-fat diets perceived by those with lower and higher fat intake? Appetite 1999, 33, 309–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Silva, D.A.S.; Peres, K.G.; Boing, A.F.; González-Chica, D.A.; Peres, M.A. Clustering of risk behaviors for chronic noncommunicable diseases: A population-based study in southern Brazil. Prev. Med. (Baltim.) 2013, 56, 20–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Weinstein, N.D. Unrealistic optimism about susceptibility to health problems. J. Behav. Med. 1982, 5, 441–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Weinstein, N.D. Optimistic Biases About Personal Risks. Science 1989, 246, 1233–1234. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Slof-Op’t Landt, M.C.T.; van Furth, E.F.; van Beijsterveldt, C.E.M.; Bartels, M.; Willemsen, G.; de Geus, E.J.; Ligthart, L.; Boomsma, D.I. Prevalence of dieting and fear of weight gain across ages: A community sample from adolescents to the elderly. Int. J. Public Health 2017, 62, 911–919. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. del Mar Bibiloni, M.; Coll, J.L.; Pich, J.; Pons, A.; Tur, J.A. Body image satisfaction and weight concerns among a Mediterranean adult population. BMC Public Health 2017, 17, 39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  63. Ferreira, C.; Trindade, I.A.; Martinho, A. Explaining rigid dieting in normal-weight women: The key role of body image inflexibility. Eat. Weight Disord.-Stud. Anorex. Bulim. Obes. 2016, 21, 49–56. [Google Scholar]
  64. Brewer, N.T.; Weinstein, N.D.; Cuite, C.L.; Herrington, J.E. Risk Perceptions and Their Relation to Risk Behavior. Ann. Behav. Med. 2004, 27, 125–130. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Assumpção, D.D.; Martins, S.; Domene, Á.; Fisberg, R.M.; Canesqui, A.M.; Barros, M.B.D.A. Differences between men and women in the quality of their diet: A study conducted on a population in Campinas, São Paulo, Brazil. Cienc. Saude Coletiva 2017, 22, 347–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Guibu, I.A.; Moraes, J.C.d.; Junior, A.A.G.; Costa, E.A.; Acurcio, F.d.A.; Costa, K.S.; Karnikowski, M.G.d.O.; Soeiro, O.M.; Leite, S.N.; Álvares, J. Características principais dos usuários dos serviços de atenção primária à saúde no Brasil. Rev. Saude Publica 2017, 51, 1s–13s. [Google Scholar]
  67. IBGE—Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística. Pesquisa de Orçamentos Familiares 2017/2018, Primeiros Resultados; IBGE—Instituto Brasileiro de Geografia e Estatística: Rio de Janeiro, Brazil, 2019.
Figure 1. (A) Mean risk perception (scaled −3 to 3) about diabetes, hypertension, and gaining weight, with 95% confidence intervals (CI); (B) mean perceived control (scaled 1 to 7) about health and having diabetes and hypertension, with 95% confidence intervals (CI).
Figure 1. (A) Mean risk perception (scaled −3 to 3) about diabetes, hypertension, and gaining weight, with 95% confidence intervals (CI); (B) mean perceived control (scaled 1 to 7) about health and having diabetes and hypertension, with 95% confidence intervals (CI).
Foods 09 01114 g001
Figure 2. Perceived risk between FCQ clusters. Different letters within each risk perception indicate significant difference—Bonferroni’s test (p < 0.050).
Figure 2. Perceived risk between FCQ clusters. Different letters within each risk perception indicate significant difference—Bonferroni’s test (p < 0.050).
Foods 09 01114 g002
Table 1. Socioeconomic characteristics of the sample (n = 525).
Table 1. Socioeconomic characteristics of the sample (n = 525).
VariableFrequency (n)Valid Percentage (%)
Age (years old)
Young adults (18–34)30157.3
Adults (35–59)16731.8
Elderly (>60)5710.9
Marital status
Single29856.8
Married17032.4
Divorced336.3
Widower244.6
Have children
No31459.9
Yes21040.1
Education level
Incomplete primary education438.2
Complete primary education305.7
Incomplete high school214.0
Complete high school6712.8
Incomplete higher education25849.1
Complete higher education10620.2
Family income
1 to 2 times the minimum wage *9919.1
Between 2 and 3 times the minimum wage8817.0
Between 3 and 4 times the minimum wage7013.5
Between 4 and 5 times the minimum wage6111.8
More than 5 times the minimum wage19938.5
For some variables, the total sum is less than 525 due to missing values. * Brazilian 2019 minimum wage per month = BRL 954.00 ≅ USD 261.36.
Table 2. Food Choice Questionnaire (FCQ)’s structure.
Table 2. Food Choice Questionnaire (FCQ)’s structure.
FactorItems—“It Is Important to Me That the Food I Eat on a Typical Day...”LoadingMean (SD) *Cronbach’s α
Factor 1Nutritional Composition 5.46 c (1.23)0.863
Contains natural ingredients0.7245.80 (1.37)
Contains no additives0.7865.47 (1.60)
Contains no artificial ingredients0.7615.37 (1.65)
Is low in fat0.6125.51 (1.49)
Is low in calories0.5745.16 (1.58)
Helps me control my weight0.5445.57 (1.53)
Factor 2Mood 5.48 c (1.15)0.794
Helps me cope with stress0.6764.98 (1.78)
Helps me cope with life0.7375.22 (1.78)
Helps me relax0.7755.23 (1.58)
Makes me feel good0.5216.21 (1.14)
Cheers me up0.7105.76 (1.41)
Factor 3Health 5.94 b (1.15)0.833
Is nutritious0.7156.25 (1.19)
Is high in fibre and roughage0.5375.62 (1.47)
Contains a lot of vitamins and minerals0.6445.80 (1.34)
Is good for my skin/teeth/hair/nails etc.0.4205.66 (1.61)
Keeps me healthy0.4976.37 (1.00)
Factor 4Sensory Appeal 6.19 a (0.90)0.778
Looks nice0.7616.08 (1.27)
Has a pleasant texture0.7665.96 (1.32)
Tastes good0.4316.56 (0.82)
Smells nice0.6796.16 (1.20)
Factor 5Price 6.01 b (1.02)0.763
Is not expensive0.7675.86 (1.36)
Is cheap0.8035.79 (1.33)
Is good value for money0.5496.38 (0.98)
Factor 6Preparation Convenience 5.59 c (1.31)0.852
Is easy to prepare0.7275.77 (1.43)
Takes no time to prepare0.8995.47 (1.53)
Can be cooked very simply0.8665.52 (1.51)
Factor 7Familiarity 4.99 d (1.34)0.712
Is familiar to me0.5375.48 (1.51)
Is what I usually eat0.7105.45 (1.60)
Is like the food I ate when I was a child0.6964.03 (1.94)
Factor 8Purchase Convenience 5.95 b (1.18)0.625
Is easily available in shops and supermarkets0.6605.91 (1.47)
Can be bought in shops close to where I live or work0.4265.99 (1.30)
* Scale from 1—totally disagree to 7—totally agree; different letters indicate significant differences in factors according to Bonferroni’s test (p < 0.050).
Table 3. Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r) between the FCQ factors (discriminant validity).
Table 3. Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r) between the FCQ factors (discriminant validity).
FCQ Factors FCQ Factors
12345678
Nutritional composition (1)1.000 .
Mood (2)0.3981.000
Health (3)0.6840.4771.000
Sensory Appeal (4)0.4870.3530.4001.000
Price (5)0.3840.2910.2710.3751.000
Convenience of Preparation (6)0.1920.2580.2260.1910.4221.000
Familiarity (7)0.4250.3670.3340.4960.3170.2931.000
Convenience of Purchase (8)0.2520.3070.3290.3360.4750.4150.2841.000
All correlations presented p < 0.001.
Table 4. Average (SD) FCQ factor scores for each cluster.
Table 4. Average (SD) FCQ factor scores for each cluster.
FCQ FactorsClustersF-Value *
1—Health Driven
(n = 106)
2—Practicality Concerned (n = 77)3—Shape Concerned (n = 18)4—Food Concerned (n = 209)5—Cooking Enthusiasts (n = 88)
Sensory Appeal5.22 c,B (0.94)6.04 b,A (0.84)6.33 ab,A (1.10)6.69 a,A (0.42)6.24 b,A (0.73)72.70
Price5.15 c,BC (1.03)5.91 b,A (0.90)6.01 b,AB (1.33)6.62 a,A (0.52)5.67 b,B (0.98)49.45
Purchase Convenience 5.18 c,B (1.15)5.96 b,A (0.92)3.38 d,C (1.74)6.58 a,A (0.63)5.84 b,B (0.99)89.24
Health5.86 b,A (0.87)4.62 c,C (1.06)4.76 c,BC (1.26)6.58 a,A (0.48)5.84 b,B (0.77)50.93
Preparation Convenience5.51 b,AB (1.00)6.07 a,A (0.90)4.42 c,BC (1.62)6.31 a,B (0.65)3.78 c,D (1.06)27.77
Mood4.74 c,C (1.13)5.18 b,B (1.25)4.94 bc,BC (1.25)6.10 a,B (0.82)5.17b,C (0.92)102.01
Nutritional Composition5.13 b,BC (0.99)3.92 d,D (0.91)5.80 a,AB (1.22)6.37 a,B (0.64)5.13 c,C (1.00)79.57
Familiarity 4.14 b,D (1.21)4.62 b,C (1.11)4.90 b,BC (1.43)5.77 a,C (1.06)4.29 b,D (1.26)38.00
F-value **115.2119.7832.07118.1042.15-
* One-way ANOVA test among clusters with p < 0.001; N total = 498. a, b, c, d—homogeneous groups among clusters according to Bonferroni’s test (p < 0.050), to be read horizontally for each factor. ** Repeated measures ANOVA among the factors with p < 0.001. A, B, C, D—homogeneous groups among factors according to Bonferroni’s test (p < 0.050), to be read vertically for each cluster.
Table 5. Odds ratio values of the mixed linear models developed for the final FCQ constructs.
Table 5. Odds ratio values of the mixed linear models developed for the final FCQ constructs.
Independent VariablesDependent Variables
Model 1—Nut CompModel 2—Sens AppModel 3—HealthModel 4—PriceModel 5—FamModel 6—Prep ConvModel 7—Purch Conv
Odds Ratio [CI 95%]
Sex (base value = female)0.32
(0.21–0.49)
0.72
(0.62–0.84)
0.66
(0.55–0.78)
0.83
(0.69–0.99)
0.72
(0.59–0.88)
--
Age (years)1.01
(1.00–1.02)
1.01
(1.00–1.02)
1.01
(1.00–1.02)
-1.00 *
(0.99—1.01)
--
Children in household (base level = no)0.72
(0.54–0.94)
---0.31
(0.17–0.58)
0.37
(0.20–0.67)
-
Income (base level = low income)0.88 *
(0.73–1.06)
--0.62
(0.52–0.75)
-0.81
(0.68–0.97)
-
Perceived health control (scale from 1 to 7)1.10
(1.04–1.16)
-1.11
(1.06–1.17)
----
Education level (base level = high education level)NS0.77
(0.63–0.93)
0.80
(0.64–1.00)
-0.53
(0.41–0.69)
-0.54
(0.45–0.66)
Interaction
(male sex and age)
1.02
(1.01–1.03)
------
Interaction (high income and age)----0.99
(0.99–1.00)
--
Interaction (have no children and age)----1.02
(1.01–1.04)
1.01
(1.00–1.02)
-
OR = odds ratio; CI 95% = confidence interval of 95%. Bold values indicate significant OR. * Adjust variable. Nut Comp = Nutritional Composition; Sens App = Sensory Appeal; Fam = Familiarity; Prep Conv = Preparation Convenience; Purch Conv = Purchase Convenience.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Marsola, C.d.M.; Cunha, L.M.; Carvalho-Ferreira, J.P.d.; da Cunha, D.T. Factors Underlying Food Choice Motives in a Brazilian Sample: The Association with Socioeconomic Factors and Risk Perceptions about Chronic Diseases. Foods 2020, 9, 1114. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9081114

AMA Style

Marsola CdM, Cunha LM, Carvalho-Ferreira JPd, da Cunha DT. Factors Underlying Food Choice Motives in a Brazilian Sample: The Association with Socioeconomic Factors and Risk Perceptions about Chronic Diseases. Foods. 2020; 9(8):1114. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9081114

Chicago/Turabian Style

Marsola, Camila de Mello, Luís Miguel Cunha, Joana Pereira de Carvalho-Ferreira, and Diogo Thimoteo da Cunha. 2020. "Factors Underlying Food Choice Motives in a Brazilian Sample: The Association with Socioeconomic Factors and Risk Perceptions about Chronic Diseases" Foods 9, no. 8: 1114. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9081114

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop