Next Article in Journal
Analysis of the Anthocyanin Degradation in Blue Honeysuckle Berry under Microwave Assisted Foam-Mat Drying
Next Article in Special Issue
Perception of the Attributes of Sherry Wine and Its Consumption in Young People in the South of Spain
Previous Article in Journal
Ternary Cross-Modal Interactions between Sweetness, Aroma, and Viscosity in Different Beverage Matrices
Previous Article in Special Issue
Ethnic Food Consumption in Italy: The Role of Food Neophobia and Openness to Different Cultures
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Effects of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Visual Cues on Consumer Emotion and Purchase Intent: A Case of Ready-to-Eat Salad

by
Pitchayapat Chonpracha
1,
Ryan Ardoin
1,
Yupeng Gao
1,
Pamarin Waimaleongora-ek
2,
Georgianna Tuuri
1 and
Witoon Prinyawiwatkul
1,*
1
School of Nutrition and Food Sciences, Louisiana State University, Agricultural Center, Baton Rouge, LA 70803, USA
2
Institute of Nutrition, Mahidol University, Phutthamonthon Rd., Salaya, Phutthamonthon, Nakhon Pathom 73170, Thailand
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Foods 2020, 9(4), 396; https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040396
Submission received: 27 February 2020 / Revised: 24 March 2020 / Accepted: 30 March 2020 / Published: 31 March 2020

Abstract

:
With increasing demand for ready-to-eat (RTE) fresh vegetables, it is important to understand how visual information cues, both intrinsic and extrinsic, affect consumer perception of these products. This study developed an emotional and wellness lexicon related to RTE salads. Subsequent questionnaires with images of salads were used to quantify consumer (N = 150) emotional and hedonic perceptions related to green color shade, shape/size of pieces, multicolor scheme, product name, and packaging. The different visual cues significantly impacted emotions and their intensities. Qualitatively, feelings of health and wellness predominated across salad samples. Negative emotions were more influenced by size of piece and green-color (intrinsic), while positive emotions were influenced by viewing salads of multiple colors (intrinsic) and packaging (extrinsic). Pale green salads were generally less liked than darker green ones. Values, in one case, ranged from 4.39 to 7.28 (on a 9-point hedonic scale), but naming the product (“iceberg lettuce”) did raise the lowest score to 5.75. The addition of vegetables with orange and purple colors to the salad mix had a positive impact on the perception of pale green salads. This study demonstrated that intrinsic and extrinsic visual cues significantly influenced consumer emotions, hedonic perception and purchase intent of RTE salads, but the effects of extrinsic cues were generally less prominent.

1. Introduction

Consumers use various intrinsic and extrinsic informational cues to form impressions about the quality of food products and to make subsequent purchase and consumption decisions. Intrinsic attributes are inherent to the product itself and cannot be manipulated without affecting its physical properties, such as appearance, taste and texture. In contrast, extrinsic sources of information are related to the product but are not physically a part of it, such as labeling, packaging, marketing information, or situational contexts [1,2]. In the product development process, both intrinsic and extrinsic cues can be directed toward generating positive sensory expectations, which can then dictate judgments of experienced quality upon consumption [3].
Sight is usually the first sense connected to our evaluation of foods [4]. While taste is reported to have the greatest influence on food choice [5], visual information is typically processed prior to food entering the mouth [6] and can even influence flavor perception [7]. These visual cues are not limited to intrinsic characteristics of the product itself (e.g., portion size, shape, color), but also pertain to extrinsic characteristics such as food packaging [4]. In addition to packaging design (e.g., color scheme, transparency/opacity, pictures), written information presented thereupon can also affect perceived value and expected outcomes, including how the product will be liked [8,9]. Ultimately, visual cues can be very impactful and influence consumer sensory expectations, hedonic evaluations, and emotions associated with food products and the overall eating experience [10,11,12,13,14].
Evaluating food-evoked emotions has recently gained interest in the fields of sensory and consumer research [15]. Emotional data have demonstrated added value, compared to traditional liking scores alone, in discriminating products, predicting food choice, and determining purchase intent. Furthermore, consumer emotional profiles are differentially affected by intrinsic and extrinsic product characteristics [10]. Visually, for example, food color intensity (intrinsic) and exposure to packaging (extrinsic) [16] have been shown to affect consumer product-related emotions and product liking [16,17]. Various methods for assessing food–elicited emotions have been employed [15], and the current study combined a few of these techniques to obtain emotional profiles for ready-to-eat salads based on visual evaluation.
The benefits of convenience and portability have driven the growing market for ready-to-eat (RTE) food products [18]. The demand for fresh–cut or minimally processed vegetable salads has also risen worldwide due to changes in demographics, lifestyles, and eating habits [19]. Thus, the hypothesis of this study was that visual intrinsic (green color shade, single vs. multiple vegetable colors, and size/shape of pieces) and extrinsic (product name and packaging) cues of RTE salads would affect consumer emotion expression (terms and their intensities), liking and purchase intent. The research experiment was divided into two main parts: (i) to develop an emotion lexicon related to RTE salads using the individual sample description technique and online questionnaire, and (ii) to investigate the effects of the above-mentioned intrinsic and extrinsic visual cues on consumer liking, emotion, and purchase intent of RTE salads. Data should shed light on salient emotions related to RTE salads, and guide methods to effectively employ visual cues in enhancing perceived product quality.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Development of Emotion Lexicon Related to Ready-To-Eat (RTE) Salads

Twelve RTE salads were purchased (from local supermarkets in Baton Rouge, LA, USA.) for evaluation. Selection was based on vegetable variety (e.g., iceberg lettuce, green cabbage, romaine lettuce, spring mix, and spinach), convenience orientation (complete salad kit with and without dressing), and packaging (elastic plastic bag and solid plastic container). The salads were stored under refrigeration (3–5 °C) one day before visual evaluations.
Twenty-three consumers (Figure 1) who frequently purchased or consumed RTE salads (at least twice a week) were recruited to participate in the session. Emotion terminology was generated using a modified individual sample description technique [20]. Consumers were presented with the entire set of 12 salad samples at once but asked to evaluate them one at a time and write down perceived emotions associated with each salad. The typical total evaluation time was approximately 30–35 min (modified from [20]).
The selection of emotion descriptors began with sorting of terms within each salad. Terms with similar meaning were grouped together, and the most representative term from each group was chosen based on frequency of usage, and redundancies were eliminated. For example, if “safe” occurred more often than “secure” in relation to a salad sample, “safe” was retained. Then, emotion terms were pooled across all salad samples, and the same grouping and selection/elimination procedure was employed. This process yielded a list of 35 emotions pertaining to the RTE salads (Table 1).
These 35 terms were combined with those from the EsSense ProfileTM [21], an existing set of 39 common food-evoked emotions. A resulting list of 58 emotion terms was obtained after consolidating 16 duplicate terms (Table 2). In order to further identify those emotion terms most relevant to RTE salads, an online survey (Qualtrics, Provo, UT, USA.) was developed, and completed by an additional 118 consumers (consuming salads at least twice a week). This online survey presented consumers with photographs of RTE salads and asked them to select, in a check-all-that-apply (CATA) format, emotion terms (from a list of 58 terms) associated with the salads. The most relevant emotion terms, based on a selection rate of at least 30%, were chosen for rating measurement in the subsequent consumer study.

2.2. The Impact of Visual Cues on Consumer Liking, Emotions and Purchase Intent

2.2.1. Visual Cues Tested in a Consumer Study

Photographs of RTE salads were captured, uploaded in an online questionnaire, and presented as the visual cues for the consumer study. These photograph images were employed as surrogates for fresh salads to maintain visual consistency [22] and to mitigate effects of sensory attributes other than appearance. The four visual cues of interest were: shade of green color, shape/size of vegetable pieces, single color or multicolor salad, and packaging (with or without) (see Table 3). The green color varied in visual green color shade to an extent deemed obvious to the normal human eye. From lightest to darkest green, the images depicted: square-cut iceberg lettuce (Sample A), shredded iceberg lettuce (Sample B), romaine lettuce (Sample C), or spinach (Sample D). The shape/size component was based on cut of the iceberg lettuce: square cut, considered large (square-L), or shredded, considered small (shredded-S). Additionally, the impact of product name (named or unnamed) was evaluated within these factors. Single-color salads consisted of lettuce only, and multicolor salad contained lettuce, shredded carrots (orange hue), and shredded red cabbage (purple hue). Multicolor salads were presented with and without their respective package. The visual effects of packaging presentation and multicolor were compared between pale green (PG; iceberg and romaine lettuce mix) and dark green (DG; romaine lettuce and spinach) salads.

2.2.2. Consumer Study

Consumers were recruited from the Louisiana State University (LSU; Baton Rouge, LA, USA.) campus to participate in this study, which was approved by the LSU Agricultural Center Institutional Review Board (IRB HE#18-22). Selection criteria were that participants were regular salad consumers (at least twice per week) and over 18 years of age. Consumers (N = 150) completed the three questionnaires in 3 consecutive days. Using Compusense five® Software (Compusense Inc., Ontario, Canada), online questionnaires were administered in partitioned booths under cool white lighting. Images of salads (Table 3) were presented in a randomized order. On day 1, the first questionnaire evaluated liking (a 9-point hedonic scale; 1 = dislike extremely, 5 = neither like nor dislike, 9 = like extremely) of green color and liking of size and cut for unnamed ready-to-eat salads. Each hedonic question was followed by a rating of emotions (a 5-point intensity scale; 1 = not at all, 2 = slightly, 3 = moderately, 4 = very much, 5 = extremely [21]; see justification for emotion term inclusion in Section 3.1). From each product image, purchase intent (PI) of the ‘actual product’ was evaluated on a ‘Yes’/’No’ scale. On day 2, the second questionnaire was administered with the only difference being that salads were named (i.e., “[square/shredded] iceberg lettuce,” “romaine lettuce,” or “spinach”). On day 3, the third questionnaire evaluated effects of single color, multicolor, and multicolor with packaging on liking of green color, liking of overall appearance, emotions, and PI for both PG and DG salads.

2.2.3. Statistical Analysis

Analysis of variance (ANOVA with the Tukey’s HSD post-hoc test) and the Student’s t-test were used to determine whether mean liking and emotional ratings were significantly different at α = 0.05. In order to identify significant predictors for purchase intent, Logistic Regression Analysis (LRA) was used to model purchase intent as a function of liking and/or emotion intensity. These data were analyzed with SAS® software (version 9.4, 2003). Correlation between emotion profiles and visual cues (green color, shape/size, multicolor and package) was demonstrated via a bi-plot by Principal Component Analysis (PCA). Emotion-driven green color liking scores were modeled using Partial Least Squares Regressions (PLSR), in which the standardized regression coefficient was used to further identify which emotions influenced liking scores. These analyses were carried out using XLSTAT® software (Addinsoft Inc., 2015).

3. Results and Discussion

3.1. Development of Emotion Lexicon Related to Ready-To-Eat (RTE) Salads

Although the modified individual sample description technique [20] facilitated reporting of both positive and negative emotions, RTE salads seemed to elicit more positive than negative feelings (Table 1). The tendency for consumers to report more positive than negative associations with foods, or “hedonic asymmetry,” [23] has been previously reported and was not unexpected. In the present study, 26 of the 35 emotions identified from the individual sample description approach were considered positive, eight were negative, and the one remaining response “feel different” was neutral.
Specifically, the term “feel healthy” was repeatedly mentioned across samples. The idea of healthiness is often associated with the feelings of wellness, which is of interest to product developers. Wellness has been used to describe a subjective, multidimensional aspect of health, viewed with positive valence [15]. While the scientific literature has attempted to distinguish between the two concepts, it is not necessarily assumed that these differences are realized by all consumers, and for the purpose of this qualitative assessment the terms were considered separately (Table 1).
From the online CATA survey including 58 emotions (Table 2), nine were selected by at least 30% of the 118 participating consumers and used for further measurement in the subsequent consumer study. They were, in order of selection frequency: feel healthy, feel wellness, safe, satisfied, active, good, happy, interested, and refreshing. The terms “feel healthy” (79%) and “feel wellness” (58%) were the two most common responses and the only two reported by over half of the participants. Previous research of implicit color associations and emotional responses suggested that green color could elicit “energized” feeling [24], and using only food images (photographs), as in the present work, researchers demonstrated consumer capacity to experience “desire” for the actual product [22,25]. Therefore, these two emotions were included. Despite the hedonic asymmetry suggested from our emotion screening, the negative emotions “bored,” “disgusted,” “guilty,” and “worried” were also included based on their significant impact (typically negative) on purchase intent for various products with health benefits [26,27,28]. Although not reaching the 30% selection rate from the online questionnaire, the feeling “special” was also included due to its prominence in the open-ended individual sample description session. Accounting for these additional feelings would help depict a more comprehensive consumer perceptions related to RTE salads. As such, these seven additional emotion terms were incorporated for rating; hence, a total of 16 terms used for the subsequent consumer study.

3.2. The Effect of Visual Cues on Consumer Liking of Ready-To-Eat (RTE) Salads

Liking of “green color” and liking of “size of the cut pieces” were analyzed separately, based on combinations of green color shade and naming, or shape/size of pieces and naming, respectively (Table 4). Shade of green color had a significant (p < 0.05) impact on hedonic scores independent of whether names were presented with the images. For unnamed samples, the two darkest salads (Sample C and Sample D) were liked most in terms of green color (p < 0.05), with mean scores of 7.28 and 7.09, respectively. In practical terms, these mean ratings, above “like moderately,” for darker green salads indicated substantial hedonic superiority compared to Sample B (below the “like slightly” criterion) and the most pale Sample A, which was scored on the negative, or “dislike,” side of the scale (mean of 4.39). More intense green color may have implied greater freshness [29].
When salads were named, the darker “romaine” salad scored significantly higher (7.28) than both of the pale “iceberg lettuce” samples (5.75 and 6.0, Table 4). The only statistically significant (p < 0.05) effect of naming, however, was observed when comparing Sample A (iceberg lettuce but unnamed) with “iceberg lettuce” (named). Perception of this pale green color, which was “disliked” at face value, was boosted by identification of the vegetable (increase from 4.39 to 5.75). The effect of green color shade was slightly less pronounced when salads were named, but the darker green shades still clearly predominated in terms of hedonic judgments.
When provided information about the salad constituent (iceberg lettuce, romaine lettuce, or spinach), consumers’ attentions may have turned to recollections of recent eating experiences and/or expectations of how the food would make them feel [30]. By calling upon these higher-level cognitive processes after product information was disclosed, green color cues may have subsequently become less salient in consumer evaluations. Additionally, consumers’ levels of familiarity with the salad samples (once the identity was made known) may have influenced the importance of extrinsic information on value judgments [31].
On the other hand, no statistically significant differences in size/cut liking between Square-L and Shredded-S salads were observed, whether or not they were identified as iceberg lettuce (Table 4). Liking of size of piece was, however, directionally higher for Shredded-S than Square-L salads (mean differences of 0.40 (unnamed) and 0.49 (named)) on the 9-point hedonic scale). With other food products, such as cut/sliced meats [32], small/large snack foods [33], and shaped pasta [34], consumer perceptions were largely influenced by size and shape. Based on the present results, it seems that the cut and size of iceberg lettuce was less influential to liking than the green color shade and product identification (naming) among adult regular salad consumers. However, to influence children’s consumption of vegetables, shape has shown to be important [35].
When evaluating pictures of single-color salads (green lettuce only), multicolor salads (lettuce, carrots, and red cabbage), and multicolor salads with package (see Table 3 for images), only the phrase “ready-to-eat salad” was used to describe the products. Liking of green color and overall appearance was measured, for both pale green (PG) and dark green (DG) lettuce variations (Table 5). With PG lettuce, liking of green color was significantly higher (p < 0.05 statistically; >1-point increase practically) for multicolor (with and without packaging) than single color salads; liking of overall appearance was higher for multicolor PG salad than both the single color and packaged items. This suggests that for pale green colored salads, such as those made with iceberg lettuce, color addition (here, orange and purple) had a positive visual impact on consumers. It has been suggested that, when balanced appropriately within the presentation, a variety of colors enhanced the attractiveness of a dish, in the same way it did with artwork [36].
Consistent with the previously discussed results comparing four shades of green salad (Table 4), single color DG salads were liked more (p < 0.05) in terms of green color and overall appearance than single color PG versions (Table 5). This was also the case for green color liking when packaging was included in the image. Differences in liking were minimized between multicolor PG and DG salads, suggesting that including vegetables with a variety of colors increased the salad’s appeal. In other studies, vegetables with higher chroma and vivid colors have been positively associated with freshness and quality, compared to dull colored vegetables [37,38,39].
For DG salads, directional but insignificant increases in both visual liking dimensions were observed (from single color, to multicolor, to multicolor with package). Overall, it may become more difficult to achieve statistical improvement in liking scores for DG than PG salads. Our evidence suggested visual appeal of DG salad varieties relies more on their intrinsic green color than extrinsic cues. Furthermore, sensitivity to detect differences is often reduced for highly acceptable products using the 9-point hedonic scale [40]. In this study, dark green salad varieties generally received relatively high liking scores, approaching or exceeding the “like moderately” mark.

3.3. The Effect of Visual Cues on Consumer Emotions of Ready-To-Eat (RTE) Salads

Different shades of green color were not only important to visual liking, but also significantly (p < 0.05) affected consumer emotional reactions to images of salads presented without (Figure 2a) and with product name (the extrinsic information) (Figure 2b). Overall, darker green salads (samples C and D) elicited higher intensities of positive emotions (ranging from 2.20 to 3.88 on a 5-point intensity scale) than pale green salads (samples A and B, ranging from 1.56–2.95). Accordingly, the opposite was observed for negative emotions, with paler green salads generating scores ranging from 1.36 to 2.37 versus 1.13 to 1.69 for darker green. Eight of the measured emotions (active, bored, energetic, feel wellness, good, healthy, interested, and satisfied) showed statistically significant differences among salad samples. In fact, a difference of >0.3 units (meaning increased positive feelings and decreased negative ones) in the mean emotional intensity scores was observed between dark green and pale green samples across all eight emotion terms. Based on the current scale, these changes in effect can also be considered practically significant [41].
As previously mentioned, and substantiated in the current study, green color may be associated with memories and expectations about healthy foods, thereby influencing liking and emotions [16]. Interestingly, only consumers’ “guilty” feelings did not distinguish (p > 0.05) among salad samples (scores ranging between 1.20 and 1.50), although they did in similarly designed investigations of different product-types [17,27,28]. Perhaps the nature of the sample, green salads in this case, and their evocation of health and wellness overrode any expectations of guilt associated with consumption.
Presentation of salad name clearly affected emotional profiles (Figure 2a vs. Figure 2b), particularly for Sample A/iceberg lettuce (Figure 2b). Intensities of positive emotions (energetic, happy, healthy, interested, refreshing, satisfied and special) were all significantly higher for the named product-image. On the other hand, there was no effect on negative emotion intensity. As with liking scores, identifying the salad constituent by name may have elicited more specific memories and experienced emotions than the more general “ready-to-eat salad” label. For example, if experience dictated that iceberg lettuce is always pale, confirmed expectations may lead to more positive reactions. Previous research suggested that product naming had more impact on dieters than non-dieters [42], implying that health-conscious eaters were more sensitive to this external cue. In the present study, all participating consumers reported consuming salad at least twice per week (based on our initial screening), perhaps lending to the positive emotional effect of naming information.
Comparing emotions related to size/shape of iceberg lettuce pieces (without inclusion of salad name), only healthy and wellness feelings proved to be significantly different (p < 0.05, Figure 3) between Square-L and Shredded-S salads, with the larger square-cut (Square-L) salads yielding higher intensity scores. When the vegetable name was given (iceberg lettuce), these two emotional intensities increased (p < 0.05). The same mechanism suggested above, for color-elicited emotions, is suspected. To formally differentiate between “health” and “wellness” is beyond the scope of this study and is an issue that is not fully resolved in the literature. However, health is sometimes viewed as a more objective measure of physical well-being, while wellness incorporates physical, emotional, and spiritual aspects [15]. In the present context of self-reported emotions, both are considered innately subjective, and it is not expected that consumers adhered to such formal definitions.
Consumer emotions showed a similar trend to those of liking of overall appearance and green color as affected by monochromatic salads versus multicolor salads with and without package. Significant effects of multicolor and package on consumer emotions were observed for PG colored salads (p < 0.05) (Figure 4a) but not for DG salads (Figure 4b). Notably, the feeling “special” showed a mean increase of >0.4 units with addition of vegetables with orange and purple hues and package depiction, compared to the single-color PG sample. The single pale color also induced significantly more boredom (higher “bored” intensity) than its multicolor counterparts. Indeed, a meal consisting of a single color or only white foods was perceived as boring by some consumers [43]. As with liking scores, the high intrinsic acceptability of green color shade for DG salads may have left little room for further increasing emotional intensities from added color components or the package cue. It was suggested elsewhere [44] that intrinsic product characteristics are more associated with emotions than packaging, as was also observed here.

3.4. Correlation between Emotional Profiles and Visual Cues of Ready-To-Eat (RTE) Salads

Figure 5 shows the PCA bi-plot depicting correlations between the four visual cues (green color, size, multicolor and package) and emotion. Results indicated that the visual cues significantly influenced the emotion ratings, as both PC components can explain the variance up to 95.34%. The differences between positive and negative emotions were heavily seen in the first PC dimension, which accounted for 67.46% of the total variance. In the first dimension, negative emotions were on the left while positive ones on the right. The green color effect was correlated with the emotion “bored.” Conversely, most of the positive emotion terms were characterized by the multicolor and package cues. The second dimension accounted for 27.88% of the total variance. It can also be seen that the rest of the negative emotions (disgusted, guilty, and worried) were loaded on this dimension. These negative emotions were generally associated with the cut size of salads.

3.5. Correlation between Emotions and Consumer Liking of Green Color of Ready-To-Eat (RTE) Salads

Figure 6 shows correlations between emotional attributes evoked by green color cues and liking of green color of salads at a confidence interval 95% using PLRS. It was observed that consumer liking scores for green color salads were driven by the positive emotions. The standardized regression coefficients loaded across all variables were 0.033 to 0.097. The emotion “special” showed the highest standardized regression coefficient value, whereas the term “safe” showed the lowest value. This implied that feeling “special” largely influenced liking scores of green color salads. In contrast, the negative emotions (bored, disgusted, and worried) contributed to decreased liking of green color of salads. As expected, the term “bored” with a standardized regression coefficient of −1.06 was an influential emotion associated with negative liking. Indeed, the liking score would decrease if consumers rated those negative terms with higher scores. Interestingly, the term “guilty” was positively correlated with green color liking scores. It is possible that consumers consciously considered health benefits of salad consumption and thus, would not feel guilty about consuming these products, even the pale green versions. Consequently, this term was positively correlated with green color of salads and did not decrease the liking scores.

3.6. The Effect of Visual Cues on Purchase Intent (PI) of Ready-To-Eat (RTE) Salads

The odds of positive PI (“Yes” response) were modeled separately based on liking and emotion scores for each of the four visual cues (green color, size, multicolor and package) (Table 6). A simple model using liking alone as a predictor showed a significant positive effect on PI (p < 0.05) from liking of size, multicolor and package. A one unit increased (on a 9–point hedonic scale) was associated with an increase in PI odds of 52%, 168%, and 171%, respectively. However, any effect of green color liking did not meet the criterion for significance (p = 0.5255).
When considering the nine emotional intensities as predictors of PI in a model, the impact of individual emotions depended on the eliciting condition under investigation. Results showed that increasing positive emotions would increase odds of positive PI, and more intense negative emotions would have the opposite effect (Table 6). The emotion “active” showed the most consistent significance for three of four visual cues (except shape/size), with each one-unit increase raising predicted PI odds by 2.5 to 5 times. The “disgusted” emotion scores for green color and multicolor would reduce PI odds by 85% and 78%, respectively, and a one-unit increase in “healthy” score was associated with a 3.8 to 5 times predicted rise in PI odds.
When including both liking and emotion in the model, green color and multicolor eliciting conditions became more significant predictors (p < 0.05). Further model selection techniques should be employed to obtain the most practical models. These results do, however, demonstrate the importance of the visual experience in food choice and purchase intent, influenced by both intrinsic and extrinsic information sources.

4. Conclusions

A multifaceted approach to qualitative lexicon development, as it pertains to the appearance of food products, may enhance the quality of consumer emotional profile data. The present combination of methods narrowed the most relevant emotions associated with RTE salads down from a list of 58 to 16 terms. Of these, several proved to distinguish among salad samples, depending on the prominent visual cues presented to consumers. Given the observed perceptions of RTE salads, future studies should elucidate between the product-evoked feelings of “healthy” vs. “wellness.” The effects of both intrinsic and extrinsic visual cues on emotion and liking were evident; however, color may be more important to positive product perceptions of salad than size of the salad pieces. The intrinsic value of a darker green color-shade invoked greater impressions of hedonic and emotional benefits for the salad consumers, repressing the potential for feelings of guilt and even overriding the marked improvement in consumer liking obtained by adding color variety to pale monochromatic salads. Our results suggested that liking of intrinsic visual characteristics of salads may moderate the effects of extrinsic cues. Providing additional product information such as naming or information on packaging may help reinforce positive tendencies toward making healthy food choices and purchase intent.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization: P.C., R.A., Y.G., P.W., G.T. and W.P.; Performing the research work: P.C., R.A. and Y.G.; Data analysis and interpretation: P.C., R.A., Y.G. and P.W.; Writing—original draft preparation: P.C., R.A. and Y.G.; Writing—review and editing: P.W., G.T. and W.P.; Supervision: G.T. and W.P.; Project administration and resource/funding acquisition: W.P. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was partially supported by the USDA National Institute of Food and Agriculture Hatch project (accession number 1006697; LAB94291) and the LSU AgCenter.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge Ashley Gutierrez, the lab manager, for facilitating the use of the LSU AgCenter Sensory Services Lab for the entire study.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript, or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Olson, J.C.; Jacoby, J. Cue Utilization in the Quality Perception Process. In Proceedings of the Third Annual Conference of the Association for Consumer Research, Chicago, IL, USA, 3–5 November 1972; pp. 167–179. [Google Scholar]
  2. Piqueras–Fiszman, B.; Spence, C. Sensory expectations based on product–extrinsic food cues: An interdisciplinary review of the empirical evidence and theoretical accounts. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 40, 165–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Acebrón, B.L.; Dopico, C.D. The importance of intrinsic and extrinsic cues to expected and experienced quality: An empirical application for beef. Food Qual. Prefer. 2000, 11, 229–238. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Wadhera, D.; Capaldi–Phillips, E.D. A review of visual cues associated with food on food acceptance and consumption. Eat. Behav. 2014, 15, 132–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Glanz, K.; Basil, M.; Maibach, E.; Goldberg, J.; Snyder, D. Why Americans eat what they do: Taste, nutrition, cost, convenience, and weight control concerns as influences on food consumption. J. Am. Diet. Assoc. 1998, 98, 1118–1126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Zellner, A.D. Effect of Visual Cues on Sensory and Hedonic Evaluation of Food. In Nutrition and Sensation; Hirsch, A.R., Ed.; CRC Press: New York, NY, USA, 2015; pp. 159–171. [Google Scholar]
  7. Shankar, M.U.; Levitan, C.A.; Prescott, J.; Spence, C. The influence of color and label information on flavor perception. Chemosens. Percept. 2009, 2, 53–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Burger, K.S.; Cornier, M.A.; Ingebrigtsen, J.; Johnson, S.L. Assessing Food Appeal and Desire to Eat: The Effect of Portion Size & Energy Density. Int. J. Behav. Nutr. Phys. Act. 2011, 8, 101. [Google Scholar]
  9. Apaolaza, V.; Hartmann, P.; Echebarria, C.; Barrutia, J.M. Organic label’s halo effect on sensory and hedonic experience of wine: A pilot study. J. Sens. Stud. 2017, 32, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Gutjar, S.; Dalenberg, J.R.; de Graaf, C.; de Wijk, R.A.; Palascha, A.; Renken, R.J.; Jager, G. What reported food–evoked emotions may add: A model to predict consumer food choices. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 45, 140–148. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kostyra, E.; Wasiak–Zys, G.; Rambuszek, M.; Waszkiewicz–Robak, B. Determining the sensory characteristics, associated emotions and degree of liking of the visual attributes of smoked ham. A multifaceted study. Food Sci. Technol. 2016, 65, 246–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Schifferstein, H.N.J.; Desmet, P.M.A. Hedonic asymmetry in emotional responses to consumer products. Food Qual. Prefer. 2010, 21, 1100–1104. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Hurling, R.; Shepherd, R. Eating with your eyes: Effect of appearance on expectations of liking. Appetite 2003, 41, 167–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Zhang, B.; Seo, H.S. Visual attention toward food–item images can vary as a function of background saliency and culture: An eye–tracking study. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 41, 172–179. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Meiselman, H.L. A review of the current state of emotion research in product development. Food Res. Int. 2015, 76, 192–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Wardy, W.; Chonpracha, P.; Chokumnoyporn, N.; Sriwattana, S.; Prinyawiwatkul, W.; Jirangrat, W. Influence of Package Visual Cues of Sweeteners on the Sensory-Emotional Profiles of Their Products. J. Food Sci. 2017, 82, 500–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Sukkwai, S.; Kijroongrojana, K.; Chonpracha, P.; Pujols, K.D.; Alonso-Marenco, J.R.; Ardoin, R.; Prinyawiwatkul, W. Effects of colorant concentration and ‘natural colour’ or ‘sodium content’ claim on saltiness perception, consumer liking and emotion, and purchase intent of dipping sauces. Int. J. Food Sci. Technol. 2018, 53, 1246–1254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Stratakos, A.C.; Koidis, A. Suitability, efficiency and microbiological safety of novel physical technologies for the processing of ready–to–eat meals, meats and pumpable products. Int. J. Food Sci. Technol. 2015, 50, 1283–1302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Zhang, J.; Yuan, L.; Liu, W.; Lin, Q.; Wang, Z.; Guan, W. Effects of UV–C on antioxidant capacity, antioxidant enzyme activity and color of fresh–cut red cabbage during storage. Int. J. Food Sci. Technol. 2017, 52, 626–634. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Fiszman, S.; Salgado, N.; Orrego, C.E.; Ares, G. Comparison of methods for generating sensory vocabulary with consumers: A case study with two types of satiating foods. Food Qual. Pref. 2015, 44, 111–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. King, S.C.; Meiselman, H.L. Development of a method to measure consumer emotions associated with foods. Food Qual. Prefer. 2010, 21, 168–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Maughan, C.; Chambers, E.; Sandria, I.V. A procedure for validating the use of photographs as surrogates for samples sin sensory measurement of appearance: An example with color of cooked turkey patties. J. Sens. Stud. 2016, 31, 507–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Desmet, P.M.; Schifferstein, H.N. Sources of positive and negative emotions in food experience. Appetite 2008, 50, 290–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Gilbert, N.A.; Fridlund, J.A.; Lucchina, A.L. The color of emotion: A metric for implicit color associations. Food Qual. Prefer. 2016, 52, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Barthomeuf, L.; Rousset, S.; Droit–Volet, S. Emotion and food. Do the emotions expressed on other people’s faces affect the desire to eat liked and disliked food products? Appetite 2009, 52, 27–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Poonnakasem, N.; Pujols, K.D.; Chaiwanichsiri, S.; Laohasongkram, K.; Prinyawiwatkul, W. Different oils and health benefit statements affect physicochemical properties, consumer liking, emotion, and purchase intent: A case of sponge cake. J. Food Sci. 2016, 81, 165–173. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  27. Carabante, K.M.; Ardoin, R.; Scaglia, G.; Malekian, F.; Khachaturyan, M.; Janes, M.E.; Prinyawiwatkul, W. Consumer Acceptance, Emotional Response, and Purchase Intent of Rib-Eye Steaks from Grass-Fed Steers, and Effects of Health Benefit Information on Consumer Perception. J. Food Sci. 2018, 83, 2560–2570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Pujols, K.D.; Ardoin, R.; Chaiya, B.; Tuuri, G.; Prinyawiwatkul, W. Low-sodium roasted peanuts: Effects of salt mixtures (NaCl, KCl and glycine) on consumer perception and purchase intent. Int. J. Food Sci. Technol. 2019, 54, 2754–2762. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Lee, S.M.; Lee, K.T.; Lee, S.H.; Song, J.K. Origin of human colour preference for food. J. Food Eng. 2013, 119, 508–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Higgs, S. Cognitive processing of food rewards. Appetite 2016, 104, 10–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Borgogno, M.; Favotto, S.; Corazzin, M.; Cardello, A.V.; Piasentier, E. The role of product familiarity and consumer involvement on liking and perceptions of fresh meat. Food Qual. Prefer. 2015, 44, 139–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Reisfelt, H.H.; Gabrielsen, G.; Aaslyng, M.D.; Bjerre, M.S.; Møller, P. Consumer preferences for visually presented meals. J. Sens. Stud. 2009, 24, 182–203. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Weijzen, P.L.G.; Liem, D.G.; Zandstra, E.H.; de Graaf, C. Sensory specific satiety and intake: The difference between nibble– and bar–size snacks. Appetite 2008, 50, 435–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  34. Rolls, B.J.; Rowe, E.A.; Rolls, E.T. How sensory properties of foods affect human feeding behavior. Physiol. Behav. 1982, 29, 409–417. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Olsen, A.; Ritz, C.; Kramer, L.; Møller, P. Serving styles of raw snack vegetables. What do children want? Appetite 2012, 59, 556–562. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Zellner, D.A.; Lankford, M.; Ambrose, L.; Locher, P. Art on the plate: Effect of balance and color on attractiveness of, willingness to try and liking for food. Food Qual. Prefer. 2010, 21, 575–578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Jantathai, S.; Sungsri–in, M.; Mukprasirt, A.; Duerrschmid, K. Sensory expectations and perceptions of Austrian and Thai consumers: A case study with six colored Thai desserts. Food Res. Int. 2014, 64, 65–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Schloss, K.B.; Poggesi, R.M.; Palmer, S.E. Effects of university affiliation and “school spirit” on color preferences: Berkeley versus Stanford. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 2012, 18, 498–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Manninen, H.; Paakki, M.; Hopia, A.; Franzarn, R. Measuring the green color of vegetables from digital images using image analysis. Food Sci. Technol. 2015, 63, 1184–1190. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. da Silva, A.N.; dos Santos Navarro, R.D.C.; Ferreira, M.A.M.; Minim, V.P.R.; da Costa, T.D.M.T.; Perez, R. Performance of hedonic scales in sensory acceptability of strawberry yogurt. Food Qual. Prefer. 2013, 30, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. King, S.C.; Meiselman, H.L.; Carr, B.T. Measuring emotions associated with foods: Important elements of questionnaire and test design. Food Qual. Prefer. 2013, 28, 8–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Irmak, C.; Vallen, B.; Robinson, S.R. The Impact of Product Name on Dieters’ and Non dieters’ Food Evaluations and Consumption. J. Consum. Res. 2011, 38, 390–405. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  43. Hutchings, J.B. Expectations and the Food Industry: The Impact of Color and Appearance; Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2003; pp. 141–175. [Google Scholar]
  44. Ng, M.; Chaya, C.; Hort, J. The influence of sensory and packaging cues on both liking and emotional, abstract and functional conceptualizations. Food Qual. Prefer. 2013, 29, 146–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. A consumer performing the modified individual sample description technique [20] with RTE salads.
Figure 1. A consumer performing the modified individual sample description technique [20] with RTE salads.
Foods 09 00396 g001
Figure 2. Mean emotion scores elicited by green color cues: (a) without product name and (b) with product name. * indicates significant difference among mean emotion scores (p < 0.05). Refer to Table 3 for sample (A–D) image and description.
Figure 2. Mean emotion scores elicited by green color cues: (a) without product name and (b) with product name. * indicates significant difference among mean emotion scores (p < 0.05). Refer to Table 3 for sample (A–D) image and description.
Foods 09 00396 g002
Figure 3. Mean emotion scores elicited by salad size, without (WO) and with product name. Refer to Table 3 for sample (A and B) image and description.
Figure 3. Mean emotion scores elicited by salad size, without (WO) and with product name. Refer to Table 3 for sample (A and B) image and description.
Foods 09 00396 g003
Figure 4. Mean emotion scores elicited by visual cues: single color, multicolor and package for (a) pale green color salad and (b) darker green color salad. * indicates significant difference among mean emotion scores (p < 0.05). Refer to Table 3 for sample image and description
Figure 4. Mean emotion scores elicited by visual cues: single color, multicolor and package for (a) pale green color salad and (b) darker green color salad. * indicates significant difference among mean emotion scores (p < 0.05). Refer to Table 3 for sample image and description
Foods 09 00396 g004
Figure 5. A PCA-biplot of emotion terms elicited by four visual cues (green color, size, multicolor and package) of ready-to-eat (RTE) salads.
Figure 5. A PCA-biplot of emotion terms elicited by four visual cues (green color, size, multicolor and package) of ready-to-eat (RTE) salads.
Foods 09 00396 g005
Figure 6. Correlations between emotional attributes evoked by green color cues and the liking of green color based on the partial least squares regression analysis (PLSR).
Figure 6. Correlations between emotional attributes evoked by green color cues and the liking of green color based on the partial least squares regression analysis (PLSR).
Foods 09 00396 g006
Table 1. Emotion terms (35) elicited by ready-to-eat (RTE) salads, as generated by the modified individual sample description technique [20].
Table 1. Emotion terms (35) elicited by ready-to-eat (RTE) salads, as generated by the modified individual sample description technique [20].
Emotion Terms
Accomplished (+)Excited (+)Peaceful * (+)
Bored * (-)Feel different (N)Pleasant * (+)
Calm * (+)Feel healthy (+)Pleased * (+)
Comfortable (+)Feel special (+)Refreshing (+)
Confident (+)Feel wellness (+)Safe * (+)
Creative (+)Fullness (+)Satisfied * (+)
Curious (+)Guilty * (-)Steady * (+)
Dangerous (-)Happy * (+)Trust (+)
Desired (+)Interested * (+)Unique (+)
Disappointed (-)Joyful *(+)Warm * (+)
Discouraged (-)Mad (-)Worried * (-)
Disgusted * (-)Nostalgic * (+)
* indicates 16 terms shared with the EsSense ProfileTM [21]. ( ) indicates the emotion status; + = positive emotion, - = negative emotion, N = neutral.
Table 2. Percentage of emotion terms (58) elicited by ready-to-eat (RTE) salads and mentioned by consumers (N = 118).
Table 2. Percentage of emotion terms (58) elicited by ready-to-eat (RTE) salads and mentioned by consumers (N = 118).
Emotion TermsPercentageEmotion TermsPercentageEmotion TermsPercentage
Feel healthy79%Loving15%Glad4%
Feel wellness *58%Peaceful15%Affectionate4%
Safe45%Joyful14%Darling3%
Satisfied44%Bored14%Disgusted3%
Active39%Calm14%Merry3%
Good37%Understanding13%Tender2%
Happy31%Accomplished11%Tame2%
Interested31%Excited10%Nostalgic1%
Refreshing30%Feel different8%Polite1%
Pleased28%Free8%Dangerous
Trust27%Mild1%Mad1%
Confident25%Unique7%Quiet1%
Desired25%Eager6%Wild1%
Energetic25%Curious6%Aggressive0%
Comfortable24%Disappointed6%Creative0%
Feel special23%Guilty5%Discouraged0%
Good–natured22%Adventurous5%Fullness0%
Pleasant22%Enthusiastic5%Steady0%
Friendly21%Warm4%
Worried18%Whole4%
* Also referred to as “engaging in a wellness lifestyle”.
Table 3. Salad images used to determine visual cue effects.
Table 3. Salad images used to determine visual cue effects.
Visual AttributeReady-to-Eat (RTE) Salad Samples
UnnamedLightness/DarknessSample A
Foods 09 00396 i001
Sample B
Foods 09 00396 i002
Sample C
Foods 09 00396 i003
Sample D
Foods 09 00396 i004
Shape/SizeSquare-L
Foods 09 00396 i005
Shredded-S
Foods 09 00396 i006
NamedLightness/DarknessIceberg Lettuce
Foods 09 00396 i007
Iceberg Lettuce
Foods 09 00396 i008
Romaine Lettuce
Foods 09 00396 i009
Spinach
Foods 09 00396 i010
Shape/Size



Square Iceberg lettuce
Foods 09 00396 i011
Shredded Iceberg lettuce
Foods 09 00396 i012
UnnamedPale green
(PG)

Single-color
Foods 09 00396 i013
Multicolor
Foods 09 00396 i014
Multicolor with Package
Foods 09 00396 i015
Dark green
(DG)
Foods 09 00396 i016 Foods 09 00396 i017 Foods 09 00396 i018
Table 4. Mean liking scores of green color and size of ready-to-eat (RTE) salads.
Table 4. Mean liking scores of green color and size of ready-to-eat (RTE) salads.
Visual Cue FactorsProduct
Naming
RTE Salads *
Sample ASample BSample CSample D
Green colorNo4.39 c5.40 b7.28 a7.09 a
Yes5.75 b6.00 b7.28 a6.52 a,b
p–value0.000 **0.070.9610.074
Square-LShredded-S
SizeNo4.96 ns5.36 nsNDND
Yes5.27 ns5.76 nsNDND
p–value0.3820.178
* Sample A—Square Iceberg; Sample B—Shredded Iceberg; Sample C—Romaine; Sample D—Spinach; Square-L—square-cut iceberg lettuce (large piece); Shredded-S—shredded iceberg lettuce (small piece). a, b, c indicated significant differences of mean scores in each row (p < 0.05). ns indicated no significant differences of mean scores (p > 0.05). ** indicated significant differences of mean scores in each column using the Student’s t-test. ND not determined.
Table 5. Liking scores of overall appearance and liking scores of green color for ready-to-eat (RTE) salads with a single color, multicolor and package.
Table 5. Liking scores of overall appearance and liking scores of green color for ready-to-eat (RTE) salads with a single color, multicolor and package.
Visual Cue FactorsLiking of Green ColorLiking of Overall Appearance
PG *DG *p ValuePGDGp Value
Single color5.49 b6.92 ns<0.0001 **5.28 b6.39 ns0.001 **
Multicolor6.65 a6.93 ns0.2936.51 a6.56 ns0.829
Package6.37 a7.10 ns0.029 **6.24 b6.68 ns0.078
* PG: pale green lettuce, DG: dark green lettuce. (see Table 3 for images). a, b, c: indicated significant differences of mean scores in each column (p < 0.05). ns indicated no significant differences of mean scores (p > 0.05). ** indicated significant differences of mean scores in each row using the Student’s t-test.
Table 6. Predicting purchase intent of the ready-to-eat (RTE) salads by sensory visual cues.
Table 6. Predicting purchase intent of the ready-to-eat (RTE) salads by sensory visual cues.
Variables Used in ModelSensory Visual Cues Effects
Green ColorSizeMulticolorPackage
p-ValueOR ˆp-ValueORp-ValueORp-ValueOR
Liking only
Liking0.52550.9330.00761.521<0.00012.681<0.00012.711
Emotions only
Active0.0233.6520.9551.0300.0502.5230.0355.028
Desired0.1850.4300.5360.7940.4681.4410.0943.274
Energetic0.7301.2430.5991.3190.0900.3600.7530.805
Wellness0.3540.4640.1200.3890.4261.4000.1880.320
Good0.3482.3240.0962.8830.1710.4400.2143.411
Happy0.9081.0790.8740.9130.3471.7670.0290.130
Healthy0.0543.8690.6721.2130.1740.4510.0435.047
Interested0.5200.6010.6290.7800.7440.8310.2202.541
Refreshing0.7890.8380.0412.7510.0912.8220.1780.349
Safe0.0490.2990.7491.1360.1180.4970.0560.280
Satisfied0.0494.2650.5410.7400.0642.9960.0745.002
Special0.6370.7210.5001.4010.6911.1910.6201.462
Bored0.5650.8320.2140.5660.4880.7660.0950.481
Disgusted0.0340.1480.5551.3370.0340.2190.9511.062
Guilty0.2160.3940.8661.0810.4621.6260.9130.909
Worried0.0141.6460.3460.6190.8361.1420.1380.183
Liking and emotions
Liking0.2801.2560.2221.3540.0092.9940.0267.686
Active0.0164.7350.9430.9620.0502.6850.02213.732
Desired0.2000.4330.4900.7710.9351.0440.1024.055
Energetic0.9141.0710.7001.2300.4800.6110.9030.897
Wellness0.3250.4400.1050.3620.6931.2030.2490.204
Good0.3582.2910.1132.8170.0900.3290.4312.495
Happy0.9271.0630.8140.8690.9430.9510.0450.097
Healthy0.0434.2910.4691.4280.5650.6770.0427.597
Interested0.7900.7990.7860.8680.8351.1290.1447.635
Refreshing0.7960.8430.0472.7110.2102.4260.0970.134
Safe0.0310.2500.6031.2330.1300.4770.0430.096
Satisfied0.0504.3590.3600.6220.0903.1130.10813.822
Special0.4800.6030.5531.3570.6410.7880.4660.472
Bored0.6950.8790.2720.5920.6841.2130.8471.154
Disgusted0.0230.1230.3851.5890.0700.2150.1458.596
Guilty0.2050.3810.6731.2320.3161.9790.1980.224
Worried0.0101.6540.2610.5430.9260.9370.0660.051
^ OR = odds ratio.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Chonpracha, P.; Ardoin, R.; Gao, Y.; Waimaleongora-ek, P.; Tuuri, G.; Prinyawiwatkul, W. Effects of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Visual Cues on Consumer Emotion and Purchase Intent: A Case of Ready-to-Eat Salad. Foods 2020, 9, 396. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040396

AMA Style

Chonpracha P, Ardoin R, Gao Y, Waimaleongora-ek P, Tuuri G, Prinyawiwatkul W. Effects of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Visual Cues on Consumer Emotion and Purchase Intent: A Case of Ready-to-Eat Salad. Foods. 2020; 9(4):396. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040396

Chicago/Turabian Style

Chonpracha, Pitchayapat, Ryan Ardoin, Yupeng Gao, Pamarin Waimaleongora-ek, Georgianna Tuuri, and Witoon Prinyawiwatkul. 2020. "Effects of Intrinsic and Extrinsic Visual Cues on Consumer Emotion and Purchase Intent: A Case of Ready-to-Eat Salad" Foods 9, no. 4: 396. https://doi.org/10.3390/foods9040396

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop