Next Article in Journal
Admission Grades as Predictors of Medical Students’ Academic Performance: A Cross-Sectional Study from Saudi Arabia
Next Article in Special Issue
COVID-19 Stress and Teachers Well-Being: The Mediating Role of Sense of Coherence and Resilience
Previous Article in Journal
Effect of Indian Music as an Auditory Stimulus on Physiological Measures of Stress, Anxiety, Cardiovascular and Autonomic Responses in Humans—A Randomized Controlled Trial
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Emotion Dysregulation and Conspiracy Beliefs about COVID-19: The Moderating Role of Critical Social Media Use

1
Department of Neurosciences, Reproductive Sciences and Dentistry, University of Naples Federico II, 80131 Napoli, Italy
2
Intradepartmental Program of Clinical Psychology Federico II, University Hospital, 80131 Napoli, Italy
3
Department of Humanistic Studies, University of Naples Federico II, 80133 Napoli, Italy
4
Department of Humanistic Studies, University of Calabria, 87036 Rende, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2022, 12(10), 1559-1571; https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12100109
Submission received: 1 September 2022 / Revised: 9 October 2022 / Accepted: 19 October 2022 / Published: 20 October 2022
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Mental Health during COVID-19 Pandemic: What Do We Know So Far?)

Abstract

:
As COVID-19 has spread worldwide, conspiracy theories have proliferated rapidly on social media platforms, adversely affecting public health. For this reason, media literacy interventions have been highly recommended, although the impact of critical social media use on the development of COVID-19 conspiracy theories has not yet been empirically studied. Moreover, emotional dysregulation may play another crucial role in the development of such theories, as they are often associated with stress, anxiety, lack of control, and other negative emotions. Therefore, the aim of this study was to test the hypothesis that emotion dysregulation would be positively associated with conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19 and that critical use of social media would attenuate this association. Data from 930 Italian participants (339 men and 591 women) were collected online during the third wave of the COVID-19 outbreak. A moderated model was tested using the PROCESS Macro for SPSS. Results showed that: (1) emotion dysregulation and critical social media use accounted for a significant proportion of the variance in conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19; and (2) critical social media use moderated the effect of emotion dysregulation on conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19. Implications for preventing the spread of conspiracy theories are discussed.

1. Introduction

With the global spread of the Coronavirus Disease 2019 (COVID-19), conspiracy theories have spread rapidly on social media platforms, adversely affecting public health [1]. Conspiracy theories can be defined as attempts to explain significant social and political events in terms of secret plots of powerful and malevolent entities acting together against the collective good [2]. Conspiracy beliefs are prevalent in times of crisis, when people seek ways to cope with uncertainty and make sense of ambiguous and unpredictable events [3]. Since the COVID-19 outbreak dramatically increased anxiety and feelings of worry and uncertainty [4,5,6,7,8], it created the “ideal” conditions for the development of conspiracy theories [9]. In such a scenario, where uncertainty is high and the information system is contradictory and often ambiguous, people may turn to alternative explanations to regain control over their environment and restore a sense of predictability [9,10].
Based on the extensive literature suggesting that negative affective states play an important role in the formation of conspiracy theories [2,11,12], the current study aimed to examine the extent to which interindividual differences in emotion regulation contribute to conspiracy belief. In addition, because conspiracy theories are mainly spread through online social networks, interventions to promote social media literacy have been strongly recommended [1,13]. To date, however, there has been no empirical investigation of the impact of critical social media use on the development of conspiracy beliefs related to the COVID-19 pandemic. Therefore, this study attempts to fill this gap in the literature by also examining the role of critical social media use as a protective factor against the development of such theories.

1.1. COVID-19 Conspiracy Theories and the Role of Social Media Consumption

Immediately after the first news about COVID-19, conspiracy theories emerged on social media, becoming more widespread and persistent [1]. The most popular COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs concern the origin of SARS-CoV-2 and assume that the virus was artificially created in a laboratory as a government biological weapon [14] or that the virus was intentionally spread by the largest pharmaceutical companies for profit [10]. There are also numerous narratives circulating on social media suggesting that governments are hiding the truth about the pandemic and using the emergency to pursue their own interests [15], or that the pandemic is a hoax and the preventive measures imposed by governments are designed to achieve political goals [16].
Recent research has shown that conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19 have detrimental consequences for public health [17,18]. Specifically, COVID-19 conspiracy theories were found to be negatively associated with recommended health behaviors (i.e., social distancing and wearing a face mask) and with intentions to undergo diagnostic or antibody testing and vaccination [19,20,21,22]. Because conspiracy theories can have a negative impact on people’s willingness to comply with government recommendations, there are concerns about their proliferation on social media [23]. For example, the overwhelming amount of unreliable information about COVID-19 disease has become a serious threat to public health, prompting the World Health Organization [24] to call on governments to take action against this “infodemic.” An infodemic is an overabundance of information—some accurate, some unverified and questionable—that can lead to confusion and fatigue, making it difficult for individuals to find trustworthy sources and distinguish accurate information from false and low-quality content [24,25]. In the absence traditional editorial control mechanisms to ensure appropriate content quality, social media has been identified as a major potential vector for the spread of misinformation [26].
In this context, the frequency of social media use as a source of information about the COVID-19 pandemic has been positively associated with greater support for COVID-19 conspiracy theories [23,27,28], but little is known about the specific pathways linking social media news consumption to conspiracy beliefs. As previous research has shown, different types of information could influence media users’ susceptibility to conspiracy theories [29]. A crucial role could be played by media literacy, which is a combination of a person’s knowledge about media, motivations, and needs that arise from consuming new media, and intellectual skills needed to critically analyze and interpret information [30,31]. There is evidence that higher media literacy is associated with lower advocacy of conspiracy theories [32,33,34,35], but the limited existing data focus on a general definition of media literacy and leave out the issue of media literacy in the specific context of social media networks.
As social media platforms have been identified as breeding grounds for conspiracy theories, scholars have emphasized the need to teach social media literacy, and a number of interventions have been developed to empower people to reject fake news and consume and share high-quality information [1,13,36]. Particular attention has been paid to critical media consumption as one of the core components of media literacy, which specifically refers to media consumers’ ability to question the credibility of social media content and to synthesize information from different media channels [37]. There is evidence of the relationship between conspiracy beliefs and constructs closely related to critical social media skills (e.g., media skepticism) [29,38,39]. However, the specific role of critical social media consumption in protecting against the development and maintenance of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs has not yet been empirically investigated.

1.2. Emotion Dysregulation and Conspiracy Beliefs

Following the model proposed by Gratz and Roomer [40], emotion regulation can be conceptualized as a multidimensional construct that includes: (a) awareness, understanding, and acceptance of emotions; (b) the ability to control impulsive behaviors when in distress; (c) the ability to use situationally appropriate strategies to modulate the intensity and duration of emotion in the service of desired goals and situational demands, rather than eliminating or avoiding emotions; and (d) the willingness to experience negative affective states in order to pursue meaningful life activities. Deficits in one or more of these dimensions are understood to indicate difficulties in emotion regulation.
Some authors have speculated that the adoption of conspiracy theories can be viewed as a means of relief or escape from negative emotions. For example, Douglas and colleagues [3] argued that people are drawn to conspiracy theories when important psychological needs are frustrated. According to this theoretical work, conspiracist ideation is driven by epistemic motives—i.e., the need to understand what is happening around us and to find safe and consistent explanations—as well as existential needs for control, security, and making sense of the world when we face threatening situations.
Along these lines, it has been argued that conspiracy theories can be characterized as deeply emotional theories because they are based on negative emotional experiences rather than rational considerations [41]. Consistent with this view, extensive research has shown that conspiracy beliefs are associated with perceived loss of control, stress, anxiety, and threat perceptions [11,42,43,44,45,46]. Experimental studies also support this view [42,47,48]. Although belief in conspiracies is often conceptualized as an attempt to manage negative emotional experiences, available evidence suggests that this method is not effective, as such theories ultimately fail to provide effective relief of aversive emotions and may even foster a negative feedback loop that leads to a decreased sense of autonomy and control, as well as to heightened levels of anxiety, powerlessness, and existential threat [49,50].
Research conducted in the context of the COVID-19 pandemic appears to confirm previous findings on the relationship between negative emotions and belief in conspiracy theories. Specifically, COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs were found to be positively related to greater anxiety and stronger feelings of powerlessness [18,51]. Furthermore, Jutzi and colleagues [52] showed that manipulating threat perceptions of the COVID-19 pandemic led to higher anxiety, which in turn led to greater endorsement of COVID-related conspiracy theories. In a study by Heiss and colleagues [43], higher threat perception was associated with stronger endorsement of conspiracy theories one month later.
While it is known that conspiracy beliefs are associated with stress, anxiety, fear, and other negative emotions, no study to date has examined the role of individual differences in the regulation of such emotions. It is conceivable that the effects attributed to negative emotions on conspiracy theories are due to differences in strategies for responding to and regulating affective experiences. Along these lines, emotion dysregulation has typically been associated with increased reactivity to stressful situations [53,54], and consequently, numerous recent studies have found that individuals who lack adaptive emotion regulation skills were particularly affected by the COVID-19 crisis [55,56,57,58]. It is therefore likely that individuals with high emotion dysregulation are particularly vulnerable to conspiracy theories in a climate of heightened negative affect because they have difficulty processing negative emotional experiences effectively.

1.3. The Current Study

Based on this conceptual and empirical framework, the current study aimed to investigate the role of individual differences in emotion regulation and critical social media use in explaining COVID-19 conspiracy theories in a sample of Italian individuals. To this end, based on the previously highlighted relationship between negative emotions and conspiracy beliefs, we first hypothesized that emotion dysregulation would be positively associated with conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19 (Hypothesis 1). Furthermore, given the well-known role of social media in the spread of conspiracy theories, we expected that critical use of social media would reduce the impact of emotion dysregulation on conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19, and thus moderate this relationship (Hypothesis 2). The hypothesized moderation model is depicted in Figure 1.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Procedures

A quantitative cross-sectional web-based survey was conducted between 27 February and 29 March 2021 during the peak of the third wave of the COVID-19 outbreak in Italy. The survey was posted on the main social media platforms (i.e., Facebook, Telegram, Instagram) and participants were recruited through a snowball sampling recruitment procedure by asking them to forward the link to others. The survey was posted on public pages, in public groups, and in closed groups where opinions and causal beliefs about COVID-19 were explicitly shared. The dissemination of the survey attempted to cover all Italian regions by distributing the invitation link for the questionnaire to online public regional groups with a large number of members.
Participants were informed about the objectives, benefits, and risks of the study, as well as about the researchers. On the first page of the survey, the informed consent form was uploaded, and participants were required to give their consent to participate in the study. To avoid missing data, all questions were mandatory, but participants were free to leave the survey if they wished.
The study was approved by the ethical committee of the University of Naples Federico II (protocol number 9/2021), designed in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and developed in compliance with the EU General Data Protection Regulation.

2.2. Participants

Participants were eligible to take the survey if they: (1) were at least 18 years old; (2) had lived in Italy for at least 10 years; and (3) spoke Italian language. A total of 958 participants answered the questions. Of these, 28 did not meet at least one of the first two inclusion criteria. Thus, the final sample consisted of 930 Italian participants. Overall, 339 of the participants were men and 591 were women. The age of the participants ranged from 18 to 78 years (mean (M) = 36.36, standard deviation (SD) = 12.60), and 60.4% (n = 562) had a high level of education (≥college degree).

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Emotion Dysregulation

The Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale-18 [59] is an 18-item questionnaire assessing emotion dysregulation. Response options ranged from 1 (“almost never”) to 5 (“almost always”), with higher scores indicating greater difficulty in emotion regulation. The scale assesses six dimensions related to emotion regulation (i.e., goals, non-acceptance, impulse, clarity, awareness, and strategies). An example item is “I am confused about how I feel.” For simplicity, we used only the total score in the present study, whose alpha coefficient was 0.89.

2.3.2. Critical Social Media Use

The 8-item “Critical consuming” subscale of the Social Media Literacy Scale [60] was used to assess critical use of social media. Response options ranged from 1 (“strongly disagree”) to 4 (“strongly agree”), with higher scores indicating a more critical social media use. An example item is “I define information as fake news after reading from various sources.” The alpha coefficient in the current study was 0.82.

2.3.3. Conspiracy Beliefs about COVID-19

The 9-item “Conspiracy Belief Scale” subscale of the COVID-19 Causal Belief Questionnaire [61] was used to assess conspiracy belief about COVID-19. Response options ranged from 0 (“never”) to 10 (“always”), with higher scores indicating stronger conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19. An example item is “The coronavirus is a means to distract citizens from something that is more important.” The alpha coefficient in the current study was 0.91.

2.4. Statistical Analyses

Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 26), with the significance level set at 0.05.
First, correlations between variables were calculated using the Pearson’s coefficient.
Second, a hierarchical multiple regression analysis was performed to test the hypothesis that conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19 are a function of emotion dysregulation, and specifically that critical use of social media would moderate the relationship between emotion dysregulation and conspiracy beliefs. In the first step, emotion dysregulation and critical use of social media were included as independent variables. Then, an interaction term between emotion dysregulation and critical social media use was created by centering both variables and including the interaction in step 2 of the regression model to test whether this interaction added explained variance to the model.
Third, to assess the conditional effect of emotion dysregulation on conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19 at different levels of critical social media use (−1 SD, M, +1 SD), the PROCESS Macro for SPSS was used, and Model 1 was applied with 10,000 bias-corrected bootstrap samples [62]. This analysis was controlled for age, gender, and education level.

3. Results

3.1. Descriptive Statistics and Bivariate Correlations

Means, standard deviations, and bivariate correlations between emotional dysregulation, critical social media use, and conspiracy beliefs on COVID-19 are shown in Table 1. The results showed a negative correlation between emotion dysregulation and critical use of social media and between critical use of social media and conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19. In contrast, the results showed a positive correlation between emotion dysregulation and conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19.

3.2. Associations between Emotion Dysregulation, Critical Social Media Use, and Conspiracy Beliefs about COVID-19

Hierarchical multiple regression analysis showed that emotion dysregulation and critical social media use accounted for 25% of the variance in conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19 (R2 = 0.25, F (2, 927) = 160.12, p < 0.001). Specifically, emotional dysregulation increased the likelihood of believing in a conspiracy about COVID-19 (b = 0.47, p < 0.001), whereas critical social media use decreased this likelihood (b = −0.11, p < 0.001). Inclusion of the interaction term between emotion dysregulation and critical social media use slightly increased the explained variance of conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19 (ΔR2 = 0.02, ΔF (1, 926) = 29.05, p < 0.001; b = −0.16, p < 0.001).

3.3. Conditional Effect of Emotion Dysregulation on Conspiracy Beliefs about COVID-19 at Different Levels of Critical Social Media Use

Examination of the interaction plot showed that the effect of emotion dysregulation on conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19 was significant for low (b = 0.13, 95% confidence intervals (CI) 0.11, 0.14, p < 0.001), moderate (b = 0.09, 95% CI 0.08, 0.11, p < 0.001), and high (b = 0.06, 95% C.I. 0.05, 0.08, p < 0.001) levels of critical social media use (Figure 2). This result indicated that critical use of social media may protect individuals with emotion regulation difficulties from the likelihood of developing conspiracy beliefs about the COVID-19. However, this result was particularly true for participants with lower levels of education (b = −0.69, p < 0.001).

4. Discussion

The aim of the present work was to investigate the role of emotion regulation difficulties and social media use in explaining conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19 in a large sample of Italian individuals. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to examine the influence of emotional dysregulation on endorsement of conspiracy beliefs and the role of critical social media consumption in moderating this relationship.
In support of our first hypothesis, we found that participants with emotion regulation difficulties were more likely to report conspiracy beliefs. This finding supports the theory that conspiracy beliefs help control acute stress and ward off negative emotions by providing a sense of order, structure, and predictability [2,63]. Our findings are also consistent with empirical studies showing that psychological stress, anxiety, and other negative emotions play a role in predicting endorsement of conspiracy theories [46,64], even during the current COVID-19 pandemic [43,45,51,52]. According to an extensive literature, fearful situations and threatening events can trigger meaning-making mechanisms that focus on reducing unpleasant emotional experiences and regaining a compensatory sense of control, which has been shown to promote conspiracy theory ideologies [2,63,65]. Specifically, elevated levels of anxiety and psychological distress have been shown to make people more likely to engage in heuristics and cognitive patterns that promote conspiracy thinking, such as perceiving illusory correlations beyond unrelated stimuli and making dispositional inferences about others [12,47,48]. Nevertheless, anxiety appears to be associated with heightened threat awareness and a greater propensity to interpret ambiguous information and circumstances in a threatening manner [66], which in turn promotes belief in conspiracy theories [67]. While previous work has shown a significant relationship between negative emotions and conspiracy theories [45,46], our findings are among the first to show a relationship between conspiracy beliefs and difficulties in emotion regulation. If higher levels of stress and anxiety are inevitable when faced with a global crisis such as the COVID-19 pandemic, the present findings suggest that interindividual differences in the experience of and response to stress may be critical in predicting endorsement of conspiracy theories. Indeed, the ability to flexibly regulate emotions is thought to play an important role in stress management [68]. That is, people who rely on dysfunctional emotion regulation processes to cope with situational challenges are more sensitive to threatening and stressful situations [68,69,70].
Recently, difficulties in emotion regulation were also found to be positively associated with increased anxiety and higher levels of psychological distress during COVID-19 outbreak [71,72,73]. Thus, because of difficulty responding adaptively to emotions, individuals with emotion dysregulation may be particularly at risk for developing faulty cognitive thinking patterns in the context of the current COVID-19 pandemic. This is in line with a consistent literature on judgment and decision making, which suggests that individuals with emotion dysregulation are more likely to use biased information processing when faced with stressful situations, with maladaptive emotion regulation processes being positively related to the need to minimize cognitive effort and avoid risk and uncertainty [40,74,75,76]. Accordingly, empirical evidence suggests that intolerance of uncertainty and the need for cognitive closure (i.e., the need to eliminate ambiguity and obtain an answer quickly) promote conspiracy beliefs in situations where clear explanations are lacking [43,77,78]. This could be due to the explanatory power of these theories, which seem to offer ready and comprehensive solutions that promise to resolve ambiguous events through the construction of patterns and meanings. In this sense, conspiracy theories about COVID-19 might be particularly attractive to people with emotion regulation problems, who may turn to such narratives to manage their anxiety and cope with the other negative emotions triggered by the pandemic. In summary, our findings provide preliminary evidence that difficulties in emotion regulation may contribute in part to the acceptance of conspiratorial explanations. Moreover, our results are innovative in finding a novel predictor of conspiracy belief that could have important implications for interventions.
Regarding our second hypothesis, our results showed the moderating effect of critical social media use on the relationship between emotion dysregulation and conspiracy beliefs. Two main considerations can be made in relation to this finding.
First, frequent use of social media has long been associated with higher risk of anxiety and stress symptoms, and recent evidence suggests a positive association between social media use and greater anxiety during the COVID-19 outbreak [79,80]. In particular, it has been suggested that emotion dysregulation may trigger a greater need for information about the pandemic to make the threat more manageable and predictable, with several studies linking difficulties in emotion regulation to increased online search behavior [81,82,83]. However, relying on social media platforms to obtain data about COVID-19 appears to put users at risk of being overwhelmed by an excessive amount of conflicting and often unverified information, which can lead to confusion and reinforce widespread fears and anxieties [84]; as a result, this can trigger a vicious cycle of anxiety in individuals with emotion regulation difficulties. In addition, fake news and conspiratorial articles often aim to fuel shock, anger, and worry by foregrounding sensationalist and emotionally arousing information [85]. This seems to be a crucial factor in explaining the widespread success of conspiracy theories and their great influence on people’s perceptions and attitudes. Previous research has shown that experiencing more emotion can lead to an increased tendency to believe in fake news, and that people who rely on emotion rather than reason are more likely to believe in false headlines [86].
Second, our findings are also consistent with previous studies linking unregulated social media use with endorsement of conspiracy theories and the strength of such beliefs [23,34,35]. These data are not surprising given that conspiracy theories, like most misinformation, are consumed and reinforced primarily through the most popular social media platforms. For example, Allington and colleagues [23] found a positive association between COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs and the extent to which people rely on social media as a source of information about COVID-19. Similarly, numerous recent studies have shown that conspiracy beliefs are associated with an avoidance of traditional and mainstream media sources [87,88]. However, although social media use is widely and consistently recognized as a major risk factor for advocating and spreading conspiracy theories, this does not mean that social media platforms are inherently harmful [89]. Indeed, it may not be the extent of social media use that matters, but rather the quality of the consumer’s information gathering. In this sense, as the recent literature suggests, passive information consumption may play a crucial role in influencing conspiracy theories, while conversely, greater skepticism and lower levels of blind trust in social media news have been shown to be significant moderators of the relationship between social media news use and conspiracy beliefs [29,39]. In this sense, our findings seem to support theoretical work and intervention models that assume that critical consumption of social media can protect against the development of conspiracy beliefs. Our results are also consistent with some previous research conducted outside of the current COVID-19 pandemic that has shown that the higher the level of media literacy, the lower the likelihood of endorsing conspiracy theories [33]. Our study extends these results to specific COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs. While previous research has demonstrated the protective role of general media literacy in the development and maintenance of conspiracy theories [32,33,34,35], our findings represent a new contribution to the literature because here we considered critical media literacy in the specific context of social media.
Regarding sociodemographic data, our results showed that agreement with coronavirus-specific conspiracy beliefs varied according to educational level, which is consistent with previous studies [90,91]. It is well documented that lower levels of education are associated with lower levels of analytical thinking skills, greater feelings of powerlessness, and greater feelings of disadvantage and distrust of institutions [92]. These are factors known to increase people’s susceptibility to believing conspiracy claims [92]. Our findings indicate that lower educational attainment is a risk factor that increases susceptibility to conspiracy belief and, as such, could be considered when planning intervention programs.
Our findings should be read in light of several limitations. First, this was a cross-sectional study, which does not allow for conclusive inferences about the temporality and causality of relationships among variables. Second, participants were recruited during a specific phase of the COVID-19 outbreak (i.e., the third wave). Both limitations should prompt researchers to conduct longitudinal studies to identify cause-and-effect relationships between variables and to assess the validity of the present model during different waves. In addition, participants were recruited through Facebook and other social media platforms, so, although the sample was large, it cannot be considered representative of the Italian population. Social media websites are often used as a sampling tool, and since they are an important means of spreading misinformation, they can easily reach groups of people inclined to conspiracy theories [93]. Notwithstanding, this sampling strategy limited our results to individuals who were active on social media. Future research should replicate our study with more representative samples of the general population. Finally, critical social media use was measured via a self-report questionnaire that may not reflect participants’ actual abilities to critically consume social media content. Future research would therefore benefit from multi-method designs that combine self-report with other measures to test both perceived and actual abilities.

5. Conclusions

Despite its limitations, this study adds to the growing literature on the antecedents of conspiracy belief by demonstrating the role of emotion dysregulation and critical use of social media. This confirms the usefulness of intervention strategies aimed at promoting healthy critical behavior (e.g., actively examining information and seeking out credible sources of information) in response to the current infodemic. However, social media literacy programs focus mainly on vigilance, i.e., the cognitive ability to critically analyze news, and ignore the crucial role of emotions in decision making [94,95], which may explain why such interventions have had limited success in countering conspiracy theories. Our findings highlight the urgent need for a multimodal approach that not only targets cognitive factors but can also improve emotion regulation. Helping people develop adaptive coping with negative emotions may prove promising in stopping the spread of conspiracy theories.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.S., R.P. (Rosa Pizzo), L.E.P., C.C., R.P. (Renata Pellegrini), F.C., O.D. and B.M.; methodology, C.S. and R.P. (Rosa Pizzo); validation, V.B. and N.M.M.; formal analysis, C.S.; investigation, R.P. (Rosa Pizzo), L.E.P., C.C., R.P. (Renata Pellegrini), F.C., O.D. and B.M.; resources, V.B. and N.M.M.; data curation, C.S. and R.P. (Rosa Pizzo); writing—original draft preparation, C.S., R.P. (Rosa Pizzo), L.E.P., C.C., R.P. (Renata Pellegrini), F.C., O.D. and B.M.; writing—review and editing, V.B. and N.M.M.; supervision, V.B. and N.M.M.; project administration, C.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Ethics Committee of University of Naples Federico II (Blind for peer review) (protocol number: 9/2021; date of approval: 26 February 2021).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Data will be made available upon reasonable request to the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Bavel, J.J.V.; Baicker, K.; Boggio, P.S.; Capraro, V.; Cichocka, A.; Cikara, M.; Willer, R. Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nat. Hum. Behav. 2020, 4, 460–471. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Douglas, K.M.; Uscinski, J.E.; Sutton, R.M.; Cichocka, A.; Nefes, T.; Ang, C.S.; Deravi, F. Understanding conspiracy theories. Polit. Psychol. 2019, 40, 3–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  3. Douglas, K.M.; Sutton, R.M.; Cichocka, A. The psychology of conspiracy theories. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2017, 26, 538–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  4. Bochicchio, V.; Winsler, A.; Pagliaro, S.; Pacilli, M.G.; Dolce, P.; Scandurra, C. Negative affectivity, authoritarianism, and anxiety of infection explain early maladjusted behavior during the COVID-19 outbreak. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 583883. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Gasparro, R.; Scandurra, C.; Maldonato, N.M.; Dolce, P.; Bochicchio, V.; Valletta, A.; Marenzi, G. Perceived job insecurity and depressive symptoms among Italian dentists: The moderating role of fear of COVID-19. Int. J. Environ. Health Res. 2020, 17, 5338. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Maldonato, N.M.; Bottone, M.; Chiodi, A.; Continisio, G.I.; De Falco, R.; Duval, M.; Scandurra, C. A mental health first aid service in an italian university public hospital during the coronavirus disease 2019 outbreak. Sustainability 2020, 12, 4244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Horesh, D.; Brown, A.D. Traumatic stress in the age of COVID-19: A call to close critical gaps and adapt to new realities. Psychol. Trauma Theory Res. Pract. Policy 2020, 12, 331–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Taylor, S.; Landry, C.A.; Paluszek, M.M.; Fergus, T.A.; McKay, D.; Asmundson, G.J. COVID stress syndrome: Concept, structure, and correlates. Depress. Anxiety 2020, 37, 706–714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Douglas, K.M. COVID-19 conspiracy theories. Group Process. Intergr. Relat. 2021, 24, 270–275. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Earnshaw, V.A.; Eaton, L.A.; Kalichman, S.C.; Brousseau, N.M.; Hill, E.C.; Fox, A.B. COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs, health behaviors, and policy support. Transl. Behav. Med. 2020, 10, 850–856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Radnitz, S.; Underwood, P. Is belief in conspiracy theories pathological? A survey experiment on the cognitive roots of extreme suspicion. Br. J. Political Sci. 2017, 47, 113–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Van Prooijen, J.W.; Jostmann, N.B. Belief in conspiracy theories: The influence of uncertainty and perceived morality. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2013, 431, 109–115. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Vraga, E.K.; Tully, M.; Bode, L. Empowering users to respond to misinformation about COVID-19. Media Commun. 2020, 82, 475–479. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Sutton, R.; Douglas, K. Agreeing to disagree: Reports of the popularity of Covid-19 conspiracy theories are greatly exaggerated. Psychol. Med. 2022, 52, 791–793. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Oleksy, T.; Wnuk, A.; Maison, D.; Łyś, A. Content matters. Different predictors and social consequences of general and government-related conspiracy theories on COVID-19. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2021, 168, 110289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Imhoff, R.; Lamberty, P. A bioweapon or a hoax? The link between distinct conspiracy beliefs about the coronavirus disease (COVID-19) outbreak and pandemic behavior. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2020, 11, 1110–1118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Bierwiaczonek, K.; Gundersen, A.B.; Kunst, J.R. The role of conspiracy beliefs for COVID-19 health responses: A meta-analysis. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2022, 46, 101346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Van Mulukom, V.; Pummerer, L.J.; Alper, S.; Bai, H.; Čavojová, V.; Farias, J.; Žeželj, I. Antecedents and consequences of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs: A systematic review. Soc. Sci. Med. 2022, 301, 114912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Bertin, P.; Nera, K.; Delouvée, S. Conspiracy beliefs, rejection of vaccination, and support for hydroxychloroquine: A conceptual replication-extension in the COVID-19 pandemic context. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 565128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Freeman, D.; Waite, F.; Rosebrock, L.; Petit, A.; Causier, C.; East, A.; Jenner, L.; Teale, A.L.; Carr, L.; Mulhall, S.; et al. Coronavirus conspiracy beliefs, mistrust, and compliance with government guidelines in England. Psychol. Med. 2022, 52, 251–263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  21. Romer, D.; Jamieson, K.H. Conspiracy theories as barriers to controlling the spread of COVID-19 in the US. Soc. Sci. Med. 2020, 263, 113356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Pivetti, M.; Melotti, G.; Bonomo, M.; Hakoköngäs, E. Conspiracy beliefs and acceptance of COVID-vaccine: An exploratory study in Italy. Soc. Sci. 2021, 10, 108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Allington, D.; Duffy, B.; Wessely, S.; Dhavan, N.; Rubin, J. Health-protective behaviour, social media usage and conspiracy belief during the COVID-19 public health emergency. Psychol. Med. 2021, 51, 1763–1769. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. World Health Organization (WHO). Infodemic Management: Infodemiology. 2020. Available online: https://www.who.int/teams/risk-communication/infodemic-management/1st-who-infodemiology-conference (accessed on 28 July 2022).
  25. Zarocostas, J. How to fight an infodemic. Lancet 2020, 395, 676. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Jamieson, K.H.; Albarracin, D. The relation between media consumption and misinformation at the outset of the SARS-CoV-2 pandemic in the US. HKS Misinf. Rev. 2020, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Enders, A.M.; Uscinski, J.E.; Seelig, M.I.; Klofstad, C.A.; Wuchty, S.; Funchion, J.R.; Murthi, M.N.; Premaratne, K.; Stoler, J. The Relationship Between Social Media Use and Beliefs in Conspiracy Theories and Misinformation. Political Behav. 2021, 1–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  28. Mari, S.; Gil de Zúñiga, H.; Suerdem, A.; Hanke, K.; Brown, G.; Vilar, R.; Bilewicz, M. Conspiracy theories and institutional trust: Examining the role of uncertainty avoidance and active social media use. Political Psychol. 2022, 43, 277–296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Xiao, X.; Borah, P.; Su, Y. The dangers of blind trust: Examining the interplay among social media news use, misinformation identification, and news trust on conspiracy beliefs. Public Underst. Sci. 2021, 30, 977–992. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Potter, W.J. Theory of Media Literacy: A Cognitive Approach; Sage Publications: Newbury Park, CA, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Livingstone, S. What is media literacy? Intermedia 2004, 32, 18–20. [Google Scholar]
  32. Borah, P.; Austin, E.; Su, Y. Injecting disinfectants to kill the virus: Media literacy, information gathering sources, and the moderating role of political ideology on misperceptions about COVID-19. Mass. Commun. Soc. 2022, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Craft, S.; Ashley, S.; Maksl, A. News media literacy and conspiracy theory endorsement. Commun. Public. 2017, 2, 388–401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Su, Y.; Borah, P.; Xiao, X. Understanding the “infodemic”: Social media news use, homogeneous online discussion, self-perceived media literacy and misperceptions about COVID-19. Online Inf. Rev. 2022, 46, 1353–1372. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Su, Y.; Lee, D.K.L.; Xiao, X. “I enjoy thinking critically, and I’m in control”: Examining the influences of media literacy factors on misperceptions amidst the COVID-19 infodemic. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2022, 128, 10711. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Guess, A.M.; Lerner, M.; Lyons, B.; Montgomery, J.M.; Nyhan, B.; Reifler, J.; Sircar, N. A digital media literacy intervention increases discernment between mainstream and false news in the United States and India. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2020, 117, 15536–15545. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Lee, L.; Chen, D.T.; Li, J.Y.; Lin, T.B. Understanding new media literacy: The development of a measuring instrument. Comput. Sci. Educ. 2015, 85, 84–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Ahadzadeh, A.S.; Ong, F.S.; Wu, S.L. Social media skepticism and belief in conspiracy theories about COVID-19: The moderating role of the dark triad. Curr. Psychol. 2021, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Su, Y.; Lee, D.; Xiao, X.; Li, W.; Shu, W. Who endorses conspiracy theories? A moderated mediation model of Chinese and international social media use, media skepticism, need for cognition, and COVID-19 conspiracy theory endorsement in China. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2021, 120, 106760. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Gratz, K.L.; Roemer, L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2004, 26, 41–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Van Prooijen, J.W.; Douglas, K.M.; De Inocencio, C. Connecting the dots: Illusory pattern perception predicts belief in conspiracies and the supernatural. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 48, 320–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  42. Grzesiak-Feldman, M. The effect of high-anxiety situations on conspiracy thinking. Curr. Psychol. 2013, 32, 100–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Heiss, R.; Gell, S.; Röthlingshöfer, E.; Zoller, C. How threat perceptions relate to learning and conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19: Evidence from a panel study. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2021, 175, 110672. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Moulding, R.; Nix-Carnell, S.; Schnabel, A.; Nedeljkovic, M.; Burnside, E.E.; Lentini, A.F.; Mehzabin, N. Better the devil you know than a world you don’t? Intolerance of uncertainty and worldview explanations for belief in conspiracy theories. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2016, 98, 345–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Stojanov, A.; Halberstadt, J. Does lack of control lead to conspiracy beliefs? A meta-analysis. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2020, 50, 955–968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Swami, V.; Furnham, A.; Smyth, N.; Weis, L.; Lay, A.; Clow, A. Putting the stress on conspiracy theories: Examining associations between psychological stress, anxiety, and belief in conspiracy theories. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2016, 99, 72–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Sullivan, D.; Landau, M.J.; Rothschild, Z.K. An existential function of enemyship: Evidence that people attribute influence to personal and political enemies to compensate for threats to control. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 2010, 98, 434–449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  48. Whitson, J.A.; Galinsky, A.D. Lacking control increases illusory pattern perception. Science 2008, 322, 115–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Liekefett, L.; Christ, O.; Becker, J.C. Can conspiracy beliefs be beneficial? Longitudinal linkages between conspiracy beliefs, anxiety, uncertainty aversion, and existential threat. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 2021, 965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Peitz, L.; Lalot, F.; Douglas, K.; Sutton, R.; Abrams, D. COVID-19 conspiracy theories and compliance with governmental restrictions: The mediating roles of anger, anxiety, and hope. J. Pac. Rim. Psychol. 2021, 15, 6646. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Šrol, J.; Ballová Mikušková, E.; Čavojová, V. When we are worried, what are we thinking? Anxiety, lack of control, and conspiracy beliefs amidst the COVID-19 pandemic. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2021, 35, 720–729. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Jutzi, C.A.; Willardt, R.; Schmid, P.C.; Jonas, E. Between conspiracy beliefs, ingroup bias, and system justification: How people use defense strategies to cope with the threat of COVID-19. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 578586. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Deatherage, S.; Servaty-Seib, H.L.; Aksoz, I. Stress, coping, and internet use of college students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2014, 62, 40–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  54. Reid, R.C.; Bramen, J.E.; Anderson, A.; Cohen, M.S. Mindfulness, emotional dysregulation, impulsivity, and stress proneness among hypersexual patients. J. Clin. Psychol. 2014, 70, 313–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  55. Guerrini Usubini, A.; Cattivelli, R.; Varallo, G.; Castelnuovo, G.; Molinari, E.; Giusti, E.M.; Musetti, A. The relationship between psychological distress during the second wave lockdown of COVID-19 and emotional eating in Italian young adults: The mediating role of emotional dysregulation. J. Pers. Med. 2021, 11, 569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. McAtamney, K.; Mantzios, M.; Egan, H.; Wallis, D.J. Emotional eating during COVID-19 in the United Kingdom: Exploring the roles of alexithymia and emotion dysregulation. Appetite 2021, 161, 105120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  57. Mehak, A.; Miller, A.E.; Trolio, V.; Racine, S.E. ‘Feeling fat’amid the COVID-19 pandemic: Examining the role of emotion dysregulation in the body displacement hypothesis. Eat. Behav. 2022, 44, 101597. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Musetti, A.; Gori, A.; Alessandra, A.; Topino, E.; Terrone, G.; Plazzi, G.; Franceschini, C. The Interplay Between Problematic Online Pornography Use, Psychological Stress, Emotion Dysregulation and Insomnia Symptoms During the COVID-19 Pandemic: A Mediation Analysis. Nat. Sci. Sleep 2022, 14, 83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Victor, S.E.; Klonsky, E.D. Validation of a brief version of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS-18) in five samples. J. Psychopathol. Behav. Assess. 2016, 384, 582–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Syam, H.M.; Nurrahmi, F. ‘I don’t know if it is fake or real news’ how Little Indonesian University students understand social media literacy. J. Komun. Malays. J. Commun. 2020, 36, 92–105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Somma, A.; Gialdi, G.; Frau, C.; Barranca, M.; Fossati, A. COVID-19 pandemic preventive behaviors and causal beliefs among Italian community dwelling adults. J. Health Psychol. 2022, 27, 601–611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach (Methodology in the Social Sciences), 2nd ed.; The Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  63. Van Prooijen, J.W.; Douglas, K.M. Belief in conspiracy theories: Basic principles of an emerging research domain. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 487, 897–908. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. Tomljenovic, H.; Bubic, A.; Erceg, N. It just doesn’t feel right–the relevance of emotions and intuition for parental vaccine conspiracy beliefs and vaccination uptake. Psychol. Health 2020, 35, 538–554. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  65. Van Prooijen, J.W.; Acker, M. The influence of control on belief in conspiracy theories: Conceptual and applied extensions. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2015, 295, 753–761. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Mathews, A.; MacLeod, C. Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety. Cogn. Emot. 2002, 16, 331–354. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Swami, V.; Voracek, M.; Stieger, S.; Tran, U.S.; Furnham, A. Analytic thinking reduces belief in conspiracy theories. Cognition 2014, 133, 572–585. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Martin, R.C.; Dahlen, E.R. Cognitive emotion regulation in the prediction of depression, anxiety, stress, and anger. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2005, 39, 1249–1260. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Troy, A.S.; Mauss, I.B. Resilience in the face of stress: Emotion regulation as a protective factor. In Resilience and Mental Health: Challenges Across the Lifespan; Southwick, S.M., Litz, B.T., Charney, D., Friedman, M.J., Eds.; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2011; pp. 30–44. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Wang, M.; Saudino, K.J. Emotion regulation and stress. J. Adult Dev. 2011, 182, 95–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Brehl, A.K.; Schene, A.; Kohn, N.; Fernández, G. Maladaptive emotion regulation strategies in a vulnerable population predict increased anxiety during the Covid-19 pandemic: A pseudo-prospective study. J. Affect. Disord. Rep. 2021, 4, 100113. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Groarke, J.M.; McGlinchey, E.; McKenna-Plumley, P.E.; Berry, E.; Graham-Wisener, L.; Armour, C. Examining temporal interactions between loneliness and depressive symptoms and the mediating role of emotion regulation difficulties among UK residents during the COVID-19 lockdown: Longitudinal results from the COVID-19 psychological wellbeing study. J. Affect. Disord. 2021, 285, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Panayiotou, G.; Panteli, M.; Leonidou, C. Coping with the invisible enemy: The role of emotion regulation and awareness in quality of life during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Context. Behav. Sci. 2021, 19, 17–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Heilman, R.M.; Crişan, L.G.; Houser, D.; Miclea, M.; Miu, A.C. Emotion regulation and decision making under risk and uncertainty. Emotion 2010, 10, 257–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Panno, A.; Lauriola, M.; Figner, B. Emotion regulation and risk taking: Predicting risky choice in deliberative decision making. Cogn. Emot. 2013, 27, 326–334. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  76. Schudy, A.; Żurek, K.; Wiśniewska, M.; Piejka, A.; Gawȩda, Ł.; Okruszek, Ł. The role of cognitive biases and emotion regulation strategies in risk perception and affective response to COVID-19. Front. Psychiatry 2020, 11, 589973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Larsen, E.M.; Donaldson, K.R.; Liew, M.; Mohanty, A. Conspiratorial thinking during COVID-19: The roles of paranoia, delusion-proneness, and intolerance of uncertainty. Front. Psychiatry 2021, 12, 698147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Marchlewska, M.; Cichocka, A.; Kossowska, M. Addicted to answers: Need for cognitive closure and the endorsement of conspiracy beliefs. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 48, 109–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Garfin, D.R.; Silver, R.C.; Holman, E.A. The novel coronavirus (COVID-2019) outbreak: Amplification of public health consequences by media exposure. Health Psychol. 2020, 39, 355–357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Chao, M.; Xue, D.; Liu, T.; Yang, H.; Hall, B.J. Media use and acute psychological outcomes during COVID-19 outbreak in China. J. Anxiety Disord. 2020, 74, 102248. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Akgül, G.; Atalan Ergin, D. Adolescents’ and parents’ anxiety during COVID-19: Is there a role of cyberchondriasis and emotion regulation through the internet? Curr. Psychol. 2021, 40, 4750–4759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Hussain, Z.; Wegmann, E.; Griffiths, M.D. The association between problematic social networking site use, dark triad traits, and emotion dysregulation. BMC Psychol. 2021, 9, 160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Jungmann, S.M.; Witthöft, M. Health anxiety, cyberchondria, and coping in the current COVID-19 pandemic: Which factors are related to coronavirus anxiety? J. Anxiety Disord. 2020, 73, 102239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Wong, F.H.C.; Liu, T.; Leung, D.K.Y.; Zhang, A.Y.; Au, W.S.H.; Kwok, W.W.; Lum, T.Y.S. Consuming information related to COVID-19 on social media among older adults and its association with anxiety, social trust in information, and COVID-safe behaviors: Cross-sectional telephone survey. J. Med. Internet Res. 2021, 23, e26570. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Pennycook, G.; Rand, D.G. The psychology of fake news. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2021, 25, 388–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  86. Martel, C.; Pennycook, G.; Rand, D.G. Reliance on emotion promotes belief in fake news. Cogn. Res. Princ. Implic. 2020, 5, 47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  87. Boberg, S.; Quandt, T.; Schatto-Eckrodt, T.; Frischlich, L. Pandemic populism: Facebook pages of alternative news media and the corona crisis--A computational content analysis. arXiv 2004, arXiv:2004.02566. [Google Scholar]
  88. De Coninck, D.; Frissen, T.; Matthijs, K.; d’Haenens, L.; Lits, G.; Champagne-Poirier, O.; Généreux, M. Beliefs in conspiracy theories and misinformation about COVID-19: Comparative perspectives on the role of anxiety, depression and exposure to and trust in information sources. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 646394. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Cato, S.; Iida, T.; Ishida, K.; Ito, A.; Katsumata, H.; McElwain, K.M.; Shoji, M. The bright and dark sides of social media usage during the COVID-19 pandemic: Survey evidence from Japan. Int. J. Disaster Risk Reduct. 2021, 54, 102034. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. Georgiou, N.; Delfabbro, P.; Balzan, R. Conspiracy beliefs in the general population: The importance of psychopathology, cognitive style and educational attainment. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2019, 151, 109521. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Georgiou, N.; Delfabbro, P.; Balzan, R. COVID-19-related conspiracy beliefs and their relationship with perceived stress and pre-existing conspiracy beliefs. Pers. Individ. Differ. 2020, 166, 110201. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Van Prooijen, J.W.; Van Dijk, E. When consequence size predicts belief in conspiracy theories: The moderating role of perspective taking. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2014, 55, 63–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Bruns, A.; Harrington, S.; Hurcombe, E. ‘Corona? 5G? or both?’: The dynamics of COVID-19/5G conspiracy theories on Facebook. Media Int. Aust. 2020, 177, 12–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Ali, K.; Li, C.; Muqtadir, S.A. The effects of emotions, individual attitudes towards vaccination, and social endorsements on perceived fake news credibility and sharing motivations. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2022, 134, 107307. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Sivek, S.C. Both facts and feelings: Emotion and news literacy. J. Media Lit. Educ. 2018, 10, 123–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The hypothesized moderation model.
Figure 1. The hypothesized moderation model.
Ejihpe 12 00109 g001
Figure 2. Interaction effect of emotion dysregulation by critical social media use on conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19.
Figure 2. Interaction effect of emotion dysregulation by critical social media use on conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19.
Ejihpe 12 00109 g002
Table 1. Correlations between emotion dysregulation, critical social media use, and conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19.
Table 1. Correlations between emotion dysregulation, critical social media use, and conspiracy beliefs about COVID-19.
123MSD
1. Emotion dysregulation1 40.1312.13
2. Critical social media use−0.21 ***1 2.950.68
3. Conspiracy beliefs 0.49 ***−0.21 ***11.962.57
Notes: M = Mean; SD = Standard Deviation. *** p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Scandurra, C.; Pizzo, R.; Pinto, L.E.; Cafasso, C.; Pellegrini, R.; Cafaggi, F.; D’Anna, O.; Muzii, B.; Bochicchio, V.; Maldonato, N.M. Emotion Dysregulation and Conspiracy Beliefs about COVID-19: The Moderating Role of Critical Social Media Use. Eur. J. Investig. Health Psychol. Educ. 2022, 12, 1559-1571. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12100109

AMA Style

Scandurra C, Pizzo R, Pinto LE, Cafasso C, Pellegrini R, Cafaggi F, D’Anna O, Muzii B, Bochicchio V, Maldonato NM. Emotion Dysregulation and Conspiracy Beliefs about COVID-19: The Moderating Role of Critical Social Media Use. European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education. 2022; 12(10):1559-1571. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12100109

Chicago/Turabian Style

Scandurra, Cristiano, Rosa Pizzo, Luca Emanuel Pinto, Claudia Cafasso, Renata Pellegrini, Federica Cafaggi, Oriana D’Anna, Benedetta Muzii, Vincenzo Bochicchio, and Nelson Mauro Maldonato. 2022. "Emotion Dysregulation and Conspiracy Beliefs about COVID-19: The Moderating Role of Critical Social Media Use" European Journal of Investigation in Health, Psychology and Education 12, no. 10: 1559-1571. https://doi.org/10.3390/ejihpe12100109

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop