Next Article in Journal
Growth Prediction in Orthodontics: ASystematic Review of Past Methods up to Artificial Intelligence
Previous Article in Journal
Effect of Malnutrition on Femoral Cartilage Thickness in Pediatric Patients
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Relationship Between Parental Phubbing and Preschoolers’ Behavioral Problems: The Mediation Role of Mindful Attention Awareness

1
Department of Developmental Psychology and Socialization, University of Padova, Via Venezia, 8, 35131 Padova, PD, Italy
2
Department of Pedagogy, Psychology, Philosophy, University of Cagliari, Via Is Mirrionis 1, 09123 Cagliari, CA, Italy
3
Department of History, Human Sciences and Education, University of Sassari, Via Maurizio Zanfarino 62, 07100 Sassari, SS, Italy
4
Department of Medicine and Surgery, University of Parma, Via Volturno, 39, 43125 Parma, PR, Italy
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2025, 12(8), 1022; https://doi.org/10.3390/children12081022
Submission received: 24 June 2025 / Revised: 29 July 2025 / Accepted: 31 July 2025 / Published: 2 August 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Pediatric Mental Health)

Abstract

Phubbing, a relatively new phenomenon in the field of digital risks, refers to the act of ignoring someone in favor of focusing on a smartphone during face-to-face interactions. Parental phubbing, a specific form of this behavior, is a prevalent negative parenting practice that can affect parent–child relationships and child development. However, the impact of parental phubbing on the emotional and behavioral development of preschool children remains unclear. This study aims to explore the relationship between parental phubbing and preschoolers’ behavioral problems, as well as test whether parents’ mindful attention awareness (MAA) acts as a mediator between them. Method: A questionnaire was administered to 138 Italian parents (mean age = 38.5, SD = 6.2) of 138 kindergarten preschoolers (mean age = 3.9, SD = 1.03). Questionnaires included the Generic Scale of Phubbing (GSP), the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS), and the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Results: Analyses revealed a significant negative correlation between the MAAS and SDQ total scores, a positive correlation between the GSP total score and the SDQ total score, and a negative correlation between the GSP total score and the MAAS total score. The mediation analysis did not show a direct effect of GSP on SDQ, suggesting that parental phubbing did not directly predict children’s behavioral difficulties. Nevertheless, the indirect effect measured by bootstrapping was significant, indicating that parental MAA fully mediated the relationship between parental phubbing and preschoolers’ problematic behaviors. Conclusions: Although further research is needed, parental mindfulness may influence phubbing behaviors in parents providing valuable insights for early interventions aimed at reducing problem behaviors in young children.

1. Introduction

Smartphones have become an essential part of many people’s lives. The widespread use of smartphones for everyday activities has led to the phenomenon of ‘phubbing’, a portmanteau of ‘phone’ and ‘snubbing’, referring to the act of ignoring someone to focus on the smartphone during face-to-face interactions [1,2,3]. Many studies highlight the negative impact of phubbing in workplaces [4], romantic relationships [5], and education [6]. However, the effects of phubbing on young children’s emotional and behavioral issues are less understood. Recently, the occurrence of phubbing within the family setting has gained attention as a significant social concern. Parental phubbing is a prevalent negative parenting practice that affects parent–child relationships and child development [7,8,9]. Studies increasingly examine how parental phubbing undermines emotional connections between parents and children, leading to reduced parental sensitivity, responsiveness [10] and feelings of closeness [11]. Research shows that parents who frequently engage in phubbing tend to exhibit higher levels of smartphone overuse (sometimes addiction) and are more likely to adopt authoritarian parenting styles [12]. These parents often demonstrate fewer positive postures, such as listening and warmth, relying more on authoritarian behaviors, such as commands, threats, and deprivations [13]. These attitudes can result in children’s emotional and behavioral issues [13,14] as well as in increased likelihood of engaging in risky behaviors, which elevates the risk of injury, particularly in younger children [15,16,17,18,19]. Children exposed to parental phubbing have also been found to struggle with delaying gratification, an effect mediated by both the child’s and mother’s positive emotions [20].
Moreover, the association of phubbing with several developmental challenges of children [14,21,22,23,24,25] highlights the need for interventions that address this growing problem.

1.1. Relationship of Parental Phubbing and Mindful Attention Awareness (MAA)

Since parental phubbing reflects a lack of physical and mental engagement during interaction with children [24], it can be interpreted as a parenting behavior characterized by low levels of interpersonal mindfulness during parent–child interactions. Mindfulness is defined as the intentional focus on the present moment with an open, non-judgmental, and non-reactive attitude [26], characterized by curiosity and attentiveness to the here and now [27].
In today’s digital age, in which adults extensively use smartphones in daily life, being engaged simultaneously in both the physical and digital environments via smartphones is often criticized for distracting attention, fostering dependency, and diminishing the quality of in-person interactions [28]. Even when they are not actively used, smartphones’ mere presence can cause distractions [29]. Furthermore, frequent thoughts about one’s smartphone [30] and staring at screens can make conversations less fulfilling [31].
In this context, parental phubbing can be viewed as a negative form of parenting, marked by low interpersonal mindfulness. When parents are engrossed in their smartphones, they may unintentionally neglect their children’s basic needs, leading to feelings of social exclusion and a reduced sense of belonging in those who are phubbed [2,32]. Phubbing limits eye contact and engagement in conversations, causing individuals to become less attentive and experience divided attention [33,34]. Consequently, parents who are less mindful may be more prone to smartphone addiction and phubbing.
Research suggests a negative correlation between mindfulness and smartphone addiction in adults [35,36]. Excessive smartphone use has been linked to reduced focus and productivity [37], while mindfulness can mitigate problematic smartphone use by reducing risks such as nomophobia and boredom [38], improving self-control [39], and enhancing self-regulation [40]. Mindfulness has also been used to treat addictive behaviors, including automatic phone-checking [41,42]. Thus, it can be expected that mindfulness acts as a protective factor against excessive smartphone use in adults, including parents.
Although there are few studies on mindfulness and phubbing, the available research shows an inverse relationship between them [43,44]. When people are less mindful of others and their own actions, they are more likely to engage in phubbing [44].
Several studies have reported a link between mindfulness and phubbing, revealing that relationship satisfaction and perceived relationship quality decrease in adults exposed to their partner’s phubbing behavior. Furthermore, this decrease negatively impacts overall life satisfaction [45]
Therefore, it can be inferred that mindfulness may increase the awareness of their habits, helping individuals regain control over their smartphone use and reduce phubbing behavior, particularly among parents.

1.2. Relationship of Phubbing and Externalizing and Internalizing Problems

In previous studies, several negative outcomes for children who experience parental phubbing are highlighted. For instance, parents who frequently use their phones while interacting with their children report more behavioral problems in their children [14].
Two typical types of behaviors are recognized as significant predictors of the social competence of children: externalizing dysregulated behaviors such as aggression, impulsivity, and control problems and internalizing dysregulated behaviors such as withdrawal, anxiety, and depression [46,47,48].
Previous research [49] has shown that parenting styles affect infants’ and toddlers’ social, emotional, and intellectual development.
Parents who engage in phubbing tend to display negative parenting postures, including neglect, impatience, and a lack of warmth, which can contribute to behavioral issues in their children [13]. Moreover, adolescents whose parents regularly prioritize phone use over a direct interaction with them are more likely to engage in harmful activities like online bullying [50]. These negative consequences are strongly linked to heightened anxiety, poor self-control, and other emotional challenges in both children and adolescents [51].
However, most research on parental phubbing focuses on its effects on adolescents rather than younger children [13,52], leaving this field underexplored. Few exceptions concern the studies aimed to examine how parental phubbing affects children’s behavior [14,23,24,53], making it important to investigate its influence on both internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Internalizing behaviors involve children’s self-isolation and their difficulties with social situations, which can lead to developmental issues [54,55]. Externalizing behaviors refer to intentionally harming others or violating social norms, resulting in negative effects on children’s emotional and physical well-being, and often lead to poor social relationships [56,57]. Understanding how parental phubbing contributes to these issues is vital for addressing its impact on child development.
Indeed, parents who are distracted by their phones during interactions with their children often fail to respond with emotional warmth to children’s needs [14]. In addition, being phubbed by their parents makes children more likely to develop behavioral problems, including aggression and social withdrawal [58]. Furthermore, such distractions can interfere with the development of prosocial behaviors, like helping and sharing, which are necessary to counterbalance problem behaviors [59]. Gender differences can also emerge, with more peer problems in boys and reduced prosocial behavior in girls [60,61].
Since children learn about the world through their interactions with their parents [62], when these interactions are disrupted by technology the consequences can be far-reaching. Parental phubbing not only weakens emotional bonds but may also reduce prosocial behaviors, and increase internalizing and externalizing behaviors [14,53]. Understanding these dynamics highlights the importance of minimizing distractions during parent–child interactions to foster healthier emotional and behavioral development in children.

1.3. Relationship of MAA and Externalizing and Internalizing Problems

According to Duncan et al. [63], mindful parenting consists of attentive listening, non-judgmental acceptance, emotional awareness, self-regulation, and compassion, both for the parent and for the child. Therefore, mindful parenting allegedly has a positive influence on general parenting, parental well-being, child management practices, and parent–child affection, which in turn reduce youth problem outcomes and increase youth positive outcomes [63]. This is also shown in several intervention studies that targeted mindful parenting. Studies provided by Coatsworth et al. [64,65] demonstrates that improvements in mindful parenting positively influence child management practices and parent–youth relationships. A positive impact of mindful parenting on the relationship between parents and children and subsequently on the children’s well-being is also shown by Medeiros et al. [66]. Similarly, in a literature review about mindful parenting and mindfulness interventions aimed at parents, Bröning and Brandt [67] reported positive effects of mindful parenting for the well-being of parents and children through parental self-regulation and positive parent–child-interactions. These favorable effects seem to also carry over to children’s behavior, as demonstrated in a mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) intervention study for parents of children with developmental delays [68]. This study reports that the improved well-being of participating parents is also reflected in less behavior problems from their children. In another mindfulness intervention study with parents of children with developmental disabilities, Singh et al. [69] found positive outcomes for parents and positive behavior change in children, especially concerning aggressive and prosocial behaviors. Apart from improvements in the parent–child-relationship, parental self-regulation, and well-being, mindfulness in parenting can also help to reduce family conflict [70] and improve mother–child communication and adolescent self-disclosure [71], which in turn may decrease internalizing and externalizing problems of children. Indeed, several studies demonstrate a negative relationship between mindful parenting and internalizing and externalizing problems in children and adolescents [67]. In a scoping review of 16 studies concerning mindful parenting training for parents of young children with behavioral problems, Donovan et al. [72] concluded that mindfulness interventions can improve child behavior with a small effect size. This is also confirmed in a systematic review of seven randomized controlled trials by Townshend et al. [73], who found specific evidence that mindful parenting programs can increase positive parenting and reduce preschooler’s externalizing symptoms. This also applies to mindfulness programs for children and adolescents. In a mindfulness intervention study with adolescents affected by different externalizing disorders, Bögels et al. [74] showed that the participants of a mindfulness training reported less internalizing and externalizing complaints after the intervention period compared to the waitlist period before starting the intervention. These symptom improvements were also confirmed by their parents. In another, cross-sectional study including adolescents and their parents, only non-judgmental acceptance of parental functioning as a dimension of mindful parenting is significantly related to adolescent-reported internalizing problems, namely depression and anxiety [75]. This implies that specific domains of mindful parenting might be especially relevant to specific aspects of behavioral problems in children and adolescents. Taken together, mindfulness in parenting may help to reduce internalizing and externalizing problems of children through different mechanisms, including improvements in the parent–child relationship, parent–child communication, and well-being of parents and children.
Another study showed that parental phubbing mediated the link between parenting MAA and children’s externalizing behaviors [76] and executive function [77]. Several studies have reported a link between mindfulness and phubbing, revealing that a high level of mindful parenting is associated with less parental phubbing as reported by parents [76].
In summary, these findings suggest that the direct associations between phubbing and mindfulness vary depending on the context and that mindfulness plays a moderating and mediating role in phubbing-related behavioral pathways.

1.4. The Present Study

However, the existing literature offers limited and sometimes inconsistent evidence regarding the mechanisms through which parental smartphone use—particularly phubbing—is associated with young children’s emotional and behavioral development, especially in relation to parental MAA as a potential protective factor.
The purpose of this study was to explore how parental phubbing behavior and mindfulness impact problematic behavior in preschool-aged children. We hypothesize that (1) parental mindfulness (MAA) will be negatively associated with children’s behavioral problems, and (2) parental phubbing will be positively associated with those problems. Therefore, we also expect to find (3) a negative correlation between parental phubbing and mindfulness. Additionally, this study investigates whether mindfulness in parents serves as a mediator: (4) we propose a mediation model in which parental mindfulness mediates the effect of parental phubbing on behavioral problems in preschoolers. According to this model, parental mindfulness is expected to reduce both phubbing behaviors and behavioral difficulties in children, thus accounting for the indirect pathway between the two.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants

A total of 138 Italian parents (mean age = 38.06, SD = 6.12; 121 mothers and 15 fathers) filled out self-report and parent-report questionnaires concerning a total of 138 children (mean age = 3.9, SD = 1.03; 67 girls and 62 boys, 9 did not disclose gender). A total of 157 parents completed the questionnaire, including instances where both parents responded for the same child. In this case, to avoid the partial duplication of data, 19 parents were excluded from the analysis.
With a sample size of 138 participants, we calculated the post hoc statistical power of the observed correlation between the variable used for the mediation model using a Monte Carlo simulation approach specifically designed for mediation analysis [78]. We conducted the simulation via the online Monte Carlo power calculator for indirect effects, entering the correlations (M-X = −0.35, Y-X = 0.19, Y m = −0.36; M = MAAS; X = GSP; Y = SDQ) and standard deviations (X = 8.63, M = 9.58, Y = 3.63), with a sample size of N = 138. The resulting estimated power was approximately 96%. This exceeds the commonly accepted threshold of 80%, suggesting that the analysis was sufficiently powered. Parents answered questions about their own and their partner’s educational background and profession. The responses concerning education and profession were converted into numbers and averaged between both parents to reflect the total socioeconomic status of the couple. Averaged between partners, most couples completed secondary school (31.17%) or pre-university courses (29.87%) and worked in specialized work or farming (25.81%) or pursued executive professions or office work (23.23%). This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Cagliari (protocol number: 0000611, issued on 04 January 2024) and performed according to the principles expressed in the 1964 Declaration of Helsinki.

2.2. Procedure

We conducted a cross-sectional quantitative study, with non-random convenience sampling of parents. By means of an online survey, we assessed parental phubbing, parental mindfulness, and internalizing and externalizing difficulties of children. The sample was recruited by sending an email to the preschools. The purposes of this research reported in the informed consent were explained in the email. We contacted the kindergarten directors to explain our research intentions and received their support. In addition, we provided instructions to the kindergarten parents and obtained their consent to participate in the study by anonymously filling out the questionnaire.
The survey was administered in Italian via the online Qualtrics platform. The system prevented missing data by requiring participants to complete all items before submission. Completion of the questionnaire took between 10 and 15 min.
Data collection took place between February 2024 and June 2024, and throughout that period, the online survey was permanently accessible.

2.3. Measures

2.3.1. Parental Phubbing

Parental phubbing was assessed using an Italian translation of the Generic Scale of Phubbing (GSP) [79], which consists of 15 items across four dimensions: nomophobia (e.g., I feel anxious if my phone is not nearby; 4 items; α = 0.84); interpersonal conflict (e.g., I have conflicts with others because I am using my phone; 4 items; α = 0.87); self-isolation (e.g., I would rather pay attention to my phone than talk to others; 4 items; α = 0.83); and problem acknowledgement (e.g., I pay attention to my phone for longer than I intend to do so; 3 items; α = 0.82). Participants responded to each item on a 7-point Likert scale (1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = occasionally, 4 = sometimes, 5 = frequently, 6 = usually, 7 = always). Subscale scores were computed, and an overall phubbing score (α = 0.93) was obtained by summing the subscale scores, with higher scores indicating greater levels of phubbing behavior.

2.3.2. Parental Mindful Attention Awareness

Parental mindfulness was assessed using the unidimensional Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS) developed by Brown and Ryan [80], which measures individual differences in the absence of attention and awareness to the present moment. The scale consists of 15 items rated on a 6-point Likert scale (1 = almost always, 2 = very frequently, 3 = somewhat frequently, 4 = somewhat infrequently, 5 = very infrequently, and 6 = almost never), with participants indicating how often they experience states of mindlessness in daily life. An overall mindfulness score is calculated by averaging the responses across all items, with higher scores indicating greater mindfulness. For the present study, we used the Italian items from the adaptation by Veneziani and Voci [81], along with an Italian translation of the original response scale by Brown and Ryan [80]. The translated items have shown good psychometric properties (α = 0.84), a unidimensional factor structure and divergent and convergent validity [81].

2.3.3. Internalizing and Externalizing Difficulties of Children

Children’s internalizing and externalizing difficulties were assessed using the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ), developed by Goodman [82] as a revision of the Rutter parent questionnaire [83], with the addition of items focusing on children’s strengths. The questionnaire consists of 25 items divided into five subscales: conduct problems (e.g., Often has temper tantrums or hot tempers; 5 items; α = 0.84), emotional symptoms (e.g., Often unhappy, down-hearted or tearful; 5 items; α = 0.75), hyperactivity–inattention (e.g., Restless, overactive, cannot stay still for long; 5 items; α = 0.86), peer problems (e.g., Rather solitary, tends to play alone; 5 items; α = 0.68), and prosocial behavior (e.g., Considerate of other people’s feelings; 5 items; α = 0.83) [84]. Items are rated on a 3-point Likert scale (0 = not true, 1 = somewhat true, 2 = certainly true), and subscale scores are computed by summing the items after reverse-coding where necessary.
The total difficulties score (α = 0.88) is derived by summing all subscales except prosocial behavior, yielding a maximum score of 40, with higher scores indicating greater difficulties related to behavior, emotions, and peer relationships [84].
In this study, the Italian adaptation of the SDQ by Marzocchi et al. [85] was used.

2.4. Data Analyses

To test our hypothesis, we first checked the parents’ data for multivariate outliers in the total scores and subscales, using Mahalanobis distances with a threshold of 0.975. After the exclusion of multivariate outliers, we tested the univariate normal distribution of the total scores, using Shapiro–Wilks tests. For descriptive statistics, the means and standard deviations of the GSP, MAAS, and SDQ total scores were calculated. To understand how parental phubbing, parental mindfulness, and internalizing and externalizing difficulties of children are related, we calculated Pearson’s correlations with all the subscales and total scores from the parents’ data while filtering out parent’s age and children’s age as potential confounding variables. Moreover, we calculated Pearson’s correlations between the parents’ and teachers’ SDQ ratings.
Afterwards, we tested a mediation model with the GSP total score as a predictor, the MAAS total score as a mediator, and the SDQ total score as the outcome, using children’s age as a control variable. The mediation analysis was performed using a bootstrapping technique with 1000 resamples. The data analysis was conducted in JASP JASP Team (2024). JASP (Version 0.19.3) [Computer software] [86] and RStudio [Version 2023.03.1 Build 446; R Development Core Team, 2016] [87].
No missing data were present in the final dataset. Partial responses (e.g., incomplete questionnaires) were not recorded by the system and therefore were not included in the dataset.

3. Results

The sample was checked for univariate normal distribution, which could not be confirmed for the GSP total score (W = 0.93, p = <0.001) and neither for the MAAS total score (W = 0.98, p = 0.033) and the SDQ total score (W = 0.97, p = 0.004). Parent-rated GSP, MAAS, and SDQ had average total scores of M = 27.61 (SD = 9.08), M = 4.59 (SD = 0.66), and M = 7.79 (SD = 3.66), respectively. Since Pearson’s correlation is robust in samples larger than 30 [81], it was calculated despite the total scores of GSP, MAAS, and SDQ not being normally distributed.
In relation to our first hypothesis, we examined whether children’s behavioral problems were associated with parental mindfulness, hypothesizing a negative correlation between these two variables. Our analysis revealed a significant negative correlation between the MAAS and SDQ total scores (r = −0.356, p < 0.001) (see Figure 1), supporting our hypothesis. Furthermore, the MAAS total score demonstrated significant negative correlations with SDQ hyperactivity–inattention (r = −0.175, p = 0.044), SDQ conduct problems (r = −0.253, p = 0.003), and SDQ emotional difficulties (r = −0.334, p < 0.001), as well as a significant positive correlation with SDQ prosocial behavior (r = 0.211, p = 0.015).
For our second research question, we investigated whether parental phubbing was related to children’s behavioral difficulties. We hypothesized that greater behavioral problems in children would be associated with higher levels of parental phubbing. Consistent with this hypothesis, correlational analyses revealed a positive correlation between the GSP total score and the SDQ total score (r = 0.187, p = 0.031) (see Figure 2). Additionally, significant correlations were observed between the SDQ subscales and the GSP subscales, specifically between SDQ hyperactivity–inattention and GSP self-isolation (r = 0.233, p = 0.007) and SDQ prosocial behavior and GSP problem acknowledgment (r = −0.195, p = 0.024).
Next, we examined the relationship between parental mindfulness and phubbing, hypothesizing a negative correlation. Our findings showed that the GSP total score was negatively correlated with the MAAS score (r = −0.362, p < 0.001) (see Figure 3).
Lastly, we examined the role of mindfulness as a mediator between parental phubbing and children’s problem behaviors. In order to examine whether parental mindfulness mediated the relationship between parental phubbing and behavioral problems in preschool children, we tested a mediation model with parental mindfulness (MAAS) as the mediator, parental phubbing (GSP) as the predictor, and behavioral difficulties in children (SDQ) as the outcome.
The total effect of parental phubbing on children’s behavioral problems was statistically significant (β = 0.023, SE = 0.010, z = 2.350, p = 0.019, 95% CI [0.001, 0.044]) (see Figure 4), indicating that higher levels of parental phubbing were associated with greater behavioral difficulties in children. However, the direct effect of parental phubbing on behavioral problems was not significant when parental mindfulness was included in the model (β = 0.009, SE = 0.010, z = 0.883, p = 0.377, 95% CI [–0.012, 0.031]).
Conversely, the indirect effect via parental mindfulness was significant: β = 0.014, SE = 0.005, z = 2.997, p = 0.003, 95% CI [0.006, 0.026]. Specifically, greater parental phubbing was associated with lower levels of parental mindfulness (β = −0.041, SE = 0.009, z = −4.489, p < 0.001), which in turn predicted higher levels of behavioral problems in children (β = −0.340, SE = 0.085, z = −4.024, p < 0.001).
Taken together, these findings support the hypothesized mediation model, suggesting that the effect of parental phubbing on children’s behavioral problems operates indirectly through reduced parental mindfulness.

4. Discussion

The rise of phubbing in everyday parent–child interactions has drawn attention from researchers due to its negative impact on child development. Identifying the factors that can reduce parental phubbing is essential for promoting a healthy development in children. This study aims to explore the connection between parental phubbing, parental mindfulness, and preschool children’s behavioral difficulties, as well as determine whether a mindful attitude in parents serves as a mediator in the relationship between parental phubbing and children’s problem behaviors.
Our findings revealed that mindful parents reported fewer behavioral issues in their children. Specifically, parental mindfulness was found to be negatively related to externalizing behaviors, such as hyperactivity and conduct problems, as well as internalizing behaviors like emotional difficulties in children. This aligns with earlier research showing that mindful parenting supports positive psychosocial outcomes in children and adolescents [69,75,88,89,90]. For example, Parent et al. [89] noted that higher levels of mindfulness in parents were linked to lower levels of both internalizing and externalizing issues in their children. Likewise, other studies pointed to a connection between mindful parenting and better adjustment along with fewer externalizing behavioral problems [71,91]. Conversely, a positive relationship was identified between parental mindfulness and prosocial behavior in children. This outcome may stem from the idea that mindful caregivers are more likely to offer the emotional support and security their children require, fostering trust and emotional well-being. As a result, children may become more inclined to form positive, prosocial connections with others [92].
Furthermore, phubbing behavior in parents was found to be positively associated with behavioral children’s problems. These results are in line with other studies that underline the relationship between the interference of technology in parent–child interaction and children’s behavioral difficulties [76], both internalized and externalized [13,76,93,94]. In particular, the self-isolation dimension of phubbing was found to be positively associated with hyperactivity and attention difficulties in children, confirming the results of other studies that support the relationship between parent technoference and child externalizing behavior [14,58,95]. For instance, Carson and Kuzik [95] found that higher parent–child technology interference was associated with lower executive functions (i.e., response inhibition) and self-regulation (i.e., emotional) as well as higher behavioral problems (i.e., externalizing and internalizing). Preschool years are a critical time for children to learn social skills, and if parents are constantly looking down at their phones, they may not be able to provide their children with enough opportunities for social interaction [96]. This may result in children having difficulty understanding and responding to the emotions of others, reducing their level of prosocial interaction with peers [97].
Parental phubbing behavior was also associated with lower levels of mindfulness in parents. This is congruent with previous research that pointed out a negative relationship between phubbing and mindfulness [43,44], highlighting how risk factors for problematic smartphone use can show a reduction as mindful levels increase [36].
Our findings also revealed that parental mindfulness plays a mediating role in the link between parental phubbing and children’s problematic behaviors. This mediation can be understood by recognizing that parents who engage in phubbing behaviors with their children are more likely to exhibit negative parenting practices, such as being less attentive, lacking warmth, and neglecting the child’s basic needs, either consciously or unconsciously [13,32]. Consequently, children may develop maladaptive behaviors in an effort to capture their caregivers’ attention [58]. A mindful attitude in parents may encourage healthier parenting practices, such as actively listening without judgment and responding with thoughtful, compassionate choices instead of automatic reactions [63,91,98,99], which can reduce phubbing behaviors. Furthermore, mindful parents are more aware of the harmful effects that phubbing can have on their child’s development [88,100,101] and are better able to avoid engaging in such behavior. Nevertheless, it is important to note that the results of this study do not advocate for the condemnation of digital technology in the modern age. There is scientific evidence that demonstrates the efficacy of educational apps in supporting preschool learning under adult guidance [102].

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, our findings highlight the potential risks associated with parental phubbing in the emergence of behavioral problems among preschool-aged children, while also highlighting parental mindfulness as both a protective factor against technology-based disruptions in parent–child interactions and a promising target for intervention.
By enhancing present-moment awareness and emotional regulation, mindful parenting may reduce the likelihood of parental phubbing, thereby promoting more responsive and engaged caregiving.
The present study is subject to the limitations inherent in research of this nature. In particular, the cross-sectional design of the study precludes the formulation of conclusions that would be considered causal statements. It is therefore recommended that future studies examine the same relationship through the medium of longitudinal studies.
Moreover, the data pertaining to the measures were collected via self-report questionnaires, a method known to prompt socially desirable responses. It is recommended that future studies concentrate on the ecological observation of phubbing behavior within a domestic context.
Future research should explore longitudinal effects and examine potential mediating variables—such as parental stress, attachment styles, and coping strategies of both children and parents—to better understand the mechanisms at play. Ultimately, integrating mindfulness-based strategies into parenting programs could offer a promising avenue for strengthening parent–child relationships and supporting positive developmental outcomes across early childhood. Although further research is needed, understanding how parental mindfulness influences phubbing can provide valuable insights for early interventions aimed at reducing problem behaviors in young children. Therefore, addressing this early on may benefit both parents and children, fostering healthier family dynamics and child development.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, D.L. and M.P.P.; methodology, A.P. (Antonio Puligheddu) and A.P. (Annamaria Porru); formal analysis, A.P. (Antonio Puligheddu) and A.P. (Annamaria Porru).; investigation, A.P. (Antonio Puligheddu) and A.P. (Annamaria Porru); data curation, A.P. (Antonio Puligheddu) and A.P. (Annamaria Porru); writing—original draft preparation, A.P. (Antonio Puligheddu), A.P. (Annamaria Porru), M.L.M. and A.S.; writing—review and editing, S.C., D.R., C.C., M.P.P. and D.L.; visualization, A.P. (Antonio Puligheddu) and A.P. (Annamaria Porru). All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was supported by the PRIN 2022 program by the Italian Ministry of University and Research (MUR), funded by the European Union—NextGenerationEU Prot. 2022E9MLS5 to D.L. and M.P.P.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was approved by the Ethics Committee at the University of Cagliari (document 0000611 issued on 04 January 2024).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Datasets generated and/or analyzed during the study are available from the first author on reasonable request.

Acknowledgments

The authors are thankful to the preschools who contributed to the sample recruitment. We wish to thank Leonie Hans for their contribution in recruiting participants and data collection.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest. The funders had no role in the design of the study; in the collection, analyses, or interpretation of data; in the writing of the manuscript; or in the decision to publish the results.

References

  1. Chotpitayasunondh, V.; Douglas, K.M. How “Phubbing” Becomes the Norm: The Antecedents and Consequences of Snubbing via Smartphone. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016, 63, 9–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. Chotpitayasunondh, V.; Douglas, K.M. The Effects of “Phubbing” on Social Interaction. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 48, 304–316. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Karadaǧ, E.; Tosuntaş, Ş.B.; Erzen, E.; Duru, P.; Bostan, N.; Şahin, B.M.; Çulha, I.; Babadaǧ, B. Determinants of Phubbing, Which Is the Sum of Many Virtual Addictions: A Structural Equation Model. J. Behav. Addict. 2015, 4, 60–74. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Roberts, J.A.; David, M.E. Put down Your Phone and Listen to Me: How Boss Phubbing Undermines the Psychological Conditions Necessary for Employee Engagement. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 75, 206–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Roberts, J.A.; David, M.E. My Life Has Become a Major Distraction from My Cell Phone: Partner Phubbing and Relationship Satisfaction among Romantic Partners. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016, 54, 134–141. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Ugur, N.G.; Koc, T. Time for Digital Detox: Misuse of Mobile Technology and Phubbing. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2015, 195, 1022–1031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Davey, S.; Davey, A.; Raghav, S.K.; Singh, J.V.; Singh, N.; Blachnio, A.; Przepiórkaa, A. Predictors and Consequences of “Phubbing” among Adolescents and Youth in India: An Impact Evaluation Study. J. Fam. Community Med. 2018, 25, 35–42. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Stockdale, L.A.; Coyne, S.M.; Padilla-Walker, L.M. Parent and Child Technoference and Socioemotional Behavioral Outcomes: A Nationally Representative Study of 10- to 20-Year-Old Adolescents. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 88, 219–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. McDaniel, B.T.; Galovan, A.M.; Cravens, J.D.; Drouin, M. “Technoference” and Implications for Mothers’ and Fathers’ Couple and Coparenting Relationship Quality. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 80, 303–313. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Braune-Krickau, K.; Schneebeli, L.; Pehlke-Milde, J.; Gemperle, M.; Koch, R.; von Wyl, A. Smartphones in the Nursery: Parental Smartphone Use and Parental Sensitivity and Responsiveness within Parent–Child Interaction in Early Childhood (0–5 Years): A Scoping Review. Infant Ment. Health. J. 2021, 42, 161–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Kushlev, K.; Dunn, E.W. Smartphones Distract Parents from Cultivating Feelings of Connection When Spending Time with Their Children. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 2019, 36, 1619–1639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Li, J.; Jiang, Y.; Xiao, B.; Wang, J.; Zhang, Q.; Zhang, W.; Li, Y. Validation of a Revised Parental Phubbing Scale for Parents of Young Children in China. Early Child. Dev. Care. 2024, 194, 167–182. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Wang, X.; Qiao, Y.; Li, W.; Lei, L. Parental Phubbing and Children’s Social Withdrawal and Aggression: A Moderated Mediation Model of Parenting Behaviors and Parents’ Gender. J. Interpers. Violence 2022, 37, NP19395–NP19419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. McDaniel, B.T.; Radesky, J.S. Technoference: Parent Distraction with Technology and Associations with Child Behavior Problems. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 100–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Boles, R.E.; Roberts, M.C. Supervising Children during Parental Distractions. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 2008, 33, 833–841. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Moran, K. Watching Parents, Watching Kids: Water Safety Supervision of Young Children at the Beach. Int. J. Aquat. Res. Educ. 2010, 4, 6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Palsson, C. That Smarts!: Smartphones and Child Injuries. 2014. Available online: https://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/document?repid=rep1&type=pdf&doi=3c11291c64570e73e9fb7c1b94c0d5d206a513be (accessed on 23 June 2025).
  18. Steiner-Adair, C.; Barker, T.H. The Big Disconnect—Procting Childhood and Family Relationships in the Digital Age; HarperCollins Publishers: New York, NY, USA, 2013; 384p. [Google Scholar]
  19. Kildare, C.A.; Middlemiss, W. Impact of Parents Mobile Device Use on Parent-Child Interaction: A Literature Review. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2017, 75, 579–593. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Błachnio, A.; Przepiórka, A.; Gorbaniuk, O.; Kot, P.; Chmielik, M.; Sobol, M. Look at Me, I Am Here! A Diary Study on Parental Phubbing and Children’s Delay of Gratification. Annu. Rev. CyberTherapy Telemed. 2023, 21, 104–110. [Google Scholar]
  21. Bitar, Z.; Hallit, S.; Khansa, W.; Obeid, S. Phubbing and Temperaments among Young Lebanese Adults: The Mediating Effect of Self-Esteem and Emotional Intelligence. BMC Psychol. 2021, 9, 87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Hong, W.; Liu, R.D.; Ding, Y.; Oei, T.P.; Zhen, R.; Jiang, S. Parents’ Phubbing and Problematic Mobile Phone Use: The Roles of the Parent-Child Relationship and Children’s Self-Esteem. Cyberpsychology Behav. Soc. Netw. 2019, 22, 749. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. McDaniel, B.T. Parent Distraction with Phones, Reasons for Use, and Impacts on Parenting and Child Outcomes: A Review of the Emerging Research. Hum. Behav. Emerg. Technol. 2019, 1, 72–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. McDaniel, B.T.; Coyne, S.M. Technology Interference in the Parenting of Young Children: Implications for Mothers’ Perceptions of Coparenting. Soc. Sci. J. 2016, 53, 435–443. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Xie, X.; Chen, W.; Zhu, X.; He, D. Parents’ Phubbing Increases Adolescents’ Mobile Phone Addiction: Roles of Parent-Child Attachment, Deviant Peers, and Gender. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2019, 105, 104426. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Kabat-Zinn, J. Mindfulness. Mindfulness 2015, 6, 1481–1483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Bishop, S.R.; Lau, M.; Shapiro, S.; Carlson, L.; Anderson, N.D.; Carmody, J.; Segal, Z.V.; Abbey, S.; Speca, M.; Velting, D.; et al. Mindfulness: A Proposed Operational Definition. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract. 2004, 11, 230–241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Anshari, M.; Almunawar, M.N.; Shahrill, M.; Wicaksono, D.K.; Huda, M. Smartphones Usage in the Classrooms: Learning Aid or Interference? Educ. Inf. Technol. 2017, 22, 3063–3079. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Przybylski, A.K.; Weinstein, N. Can You Connect with Me Now? How the Presence of Mobile Communication Technology Influences Face-to-Face Conversation Quality. J. Soc. Pers. Relat. 2013, 30, 237–246. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Tanil, C.T.; Yong, M.H. Mobile Phones: The Effect of Its Presence on Learning and Memory. PLoS ONE 2020, 15, e0219233. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Vanden Abeele, M.M.P.; Antheunis, M.L.; Schouten, A.P. The Effect of Mobile Messaging during a Conversation on Impression Formation and Interaction Quality. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2016, 62, 562–569. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Shao, L.; Dong, Y.; Feng, J.X.; Zhang, D.H. The Relationship between Parent Phubbing and Aggression among Adolescents: The Role of Ostracism and Interpersonal Sensitivity. Psychology 2020, 8, 513–520. [Google Scholar]
  33. Karadağ, E.; Tosuntaş, Ş.B.; Erzen, E.; Duru, P.; Bostan, N.; Mızrak Şahin, B.; Çulha, İ.; Babadağ, B. The Virtual World’s Current Addiction: Phubbing. Addicta Turk. J. Addict. 2016, 3, 250–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Dwyer, R.J.; Kushlev, K.; Dunn, E.W. Smartphone Use Undermines Enjoyment of Face-to-Face Social Interactions. J. Exp. Soc. Psychol. 2018, 78, 233–239. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Arpaci, I. Relationships Between Early Maladaptive Schemas and Smartphone Addiction: The Moderating Role of Mindfulness. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2021, 19, 778–792. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Regan, T.; Harris, B.; Van Loon, M.; Nanavaty, N.; Schueler, J.; Engler, S.; Fields, S.A. Does Mindfulness Reduce the Effects of Risk Factors for Problematic Smartphone Use? Comparing Frequency of Use versus Self-Reported Addiction. Addict. Behav. 2020, 108, 106435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Montag, C.; Walla, P. Carpe Diem Instead of Losing Your Social Mind: Beyond Digital Addiction and Why We All Suffer from Digital Overuse. Cogent Psychol. 2016, 3, 1157281. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Liu, Q.Q.; Zhang, D.J.; Yang, X.J.; Zhang, C.Y.; Fan, C.Y.; Zhou, Z.K. Perceived Stress and Mobile Phone Addiction in Chinese Adolescents: A Moderated Mediation Model. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 87, 247–253. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Song, W.J.; Park, J.W. The Influence of Stress on Internet Addiction: Mediating Effects of Self-Control and Mindfulness. Int. J. Ment. Health Addict. 2019, 17, 1063–1075. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Hölzel, B.K.; Carmody, J.; Vangel, M.; Congleton, C.; Yerramsetti, S.M.; Gard, T.; Lazar, S.W. Mindfulness Practice Leads to Increases in Regional Brain Gray Matter Density. Psychiatry Res. Neuroimaging 2011, 191, 36–43. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Oulasvirta, A.; Rattenbury, T.; Ma, L.; Raita, E. Habits Make Smartphone Use More Pervasive. Pers. Ubiquitous Comput. 2012, 16, 105–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Van Deursen, A.J.A.M.; Bolle, C.L.; Hegner, S.M.; Kommers, P.A.M. Modeling Habitual and Addictive Smartphone Behavior: The Role of Smartphone Usage Types, Emotional Intelligence, Social Stress, Self-Regulation, Age, and Gender. Comput. Hum. Behav. 2015, 45, 411–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Bradford, B. No Time to Think: The Impact of Smartphone Technology on Mindfulness and Reflection. Available online: https://www.academia.edu/36251280/No_time_to_think_The_impact_of_smartphone_technology_on_mindfulness_and_reflection (accessed on 18 June 2025).
  44. Calvete, E.; Gámez-Guadix, M.; Cortazar, N. Mindfulness Facets and Problematic Internet Use: A Six-Month Longitudinal Study. Addict. Behav. 2017, 72, 57–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  45. Faruk, C. The Relationship Between Partner Phubbing and Life Satisfaction: The Mediating Role of Relationship Satisfaction and Perceived Romantic Relationship Quality. Psychol. Rep. 2023, 126, 303–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Burt, K.B.; Obradović, J.; Long, J.D.; Masten, A.S. The Interplay of Social Competence and Psychopathology over 20 Years: Testing Transactional and Cascade Models. Child Dev. 2008, 79, 359–374. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Achenbach, T.M.; Rescorla, L. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms & Profiles: An Integrated System of Multi-Informant Assessment; ASEBA: Burlington, VT, USA, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  48. Achenbach, T.M. The Achenbach System of Empirically Based Assessment (ASEBA): Development, Findings, Theory, and Applications; University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families: Burlington, VT, USA, 2009; ISBN 1932975136. [Google Scholar]
  49. Baumrind, D. The Influence of Parenting Style on Adolescent Competence and Substance Use. J. Early Adolesc. 1991, 11, 56–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Wang, X.; Wang, W.; Qiao, Y.; Gao, L.; Yang, J.; Wang, P. Parental Phubbing and Adolescents’ Cyberbullying Perpetration: A Moderated Mediation Model of Moral Disengagement and Online Disinhibition. J. Interpers. Violence 2022, 37, NP5344–NP5366. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Solecki, S. The Phubbing Phenomenon: The Impact on Parent-Child Relationships. J. Pediatr. Nurs. 2022, 62, 211–214. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  52. Wang, X.; Gao, L.; Yang, J.; Zhao, F.; Wang, P. Parental Phubbing and Adolescents’ Depressive Symptoms: Self-Esteem and Perceived Social Support as Moderators. J. Youth Adolesc. 2020, 49, 427–437. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. McDaniel, B.T.; Radesky, J.S. Technoference: Longitudinal Associations between Parent Technology Use, Parenting Stress, and Child Behavior Problems. Pediatr. Res. 2018, 84, 210–218. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Rubin, K.H.; Coplan, R.J.; Bowker, J.C. Social Withdrawal in Childhood. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2009, 60, 141–171. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Zarra-Nezhad, M.; Kiuru, N.; Aunola, K.; Zarra-Nezhad, M.; Ahonen, T.; Poikkeus, A.M.; Lerkkanen, M.K.; Nurmi, J.E. Social Withdrawal in Children Moderates the Association between Parenting Styles and the Children’s Own Socioemotional Development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2014, 55, 1260–1269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Liu, J. Childhood Externalizing Behavior: Theory and Implications. J. Child Adolesc. Psychiatr. Nurs. 2004, 17, 93–103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Bongers, I.L.; Koot, H.M.; Van Der Ende, J.; Verhulst, F.C. Predicting Young Adult Social Functioning from Developmental Trajectories of Externalizing Behaviour. Psychol. Med. 2008, 38, 989–999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Radesky, J.S.; Silverstein, M.; Zuckerman, B.; Christakis, D.A. Infant Self-Regulation and Early Childhood Media Exposure. Pediatrics 2014, 133, e1172–e1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Eisenberg, N.; Spinrad, T.L.; Knafo-Noam, A. Prosocial Development. In Handbook of Child Psychology and Developmental Science. Vol. 3: Socioemotional Processes; Lamb, M.E., Ed.; Wiley: Hoboken, NJ, USA, 2015; pp. 610–656. [Google Scholar]
  60. Mieloo, C.; Raat, H.; van Oort, F.; Bevaart, F.; Vogel, I.; Donker, M.; Jansen, W. Validity and Reliability of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire in 5–6 Year Olds: Differences by Gender or by Parental Education? PLoS ONE 2012, 7, e36805. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  61. Maurice-Stam, H.; Haverman, L.; Splinter, A.; van Oers, H.A.; Schepers, S.A.; Grootenhuis, M.A. Dutch Norms for the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)—Parent Form for Children Aged 2–18 years. Health Qual. Life Outcomes 2018, 16, 123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Daniel, E.; Madigan, S.; Jenkins, J. Paternal and Maternal Warmth and the Development of Prosociality among Preschoolers. J. Fam. Psychol. 2016, 30, 114–124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Duncan, L.G.; Coatsworth, J.D.; Greenberg, M.T. A Model of Mindful Parenting: Implications for Parent-Child Relationships and Prevention Research. Clin. Child Fam. Psychol. Rev. 2009, 12, 255–270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Coatsworth, J.D.; Duncan, L.G.; Greenberg, M.T.; Nix, R.L. Changing Parent’s Mindfulness, Child Management Skills and Relationship Quality with Their Youth: Results from a Randomized Pilot Intervention Trial. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2010, 19, 203–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Coatsworth, J.D.; Duncan, L.G.; Nix, R.L.; Greenberg, M.T.; Gayles, J.G.; Bamberger, K.T.; Berrena, E.; Demi, M.A. Integrating Mindfulness with Parent Training: Effects of the Mindfulness-Enhanced Strengthening Families Program. Dev. Psychol. 2015, 51, 23–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Medeiros, C.; Gouveia, M.J.; Canavarro, M.C.; Moreira, H. The Indirect Effect of the Mindful Parenting of Mothers and Fathers on the Child’s Perceived Well-Being Through the Child’s Attachment to Parents. Mindfulness 2016, 7, 916–927. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Bröning, S.; Brandt, M. ”Mindful Parenting”—Achtsamkeit in Der Eltern-Kind-Beziehung. Z Kinder Jugendpsychiatr Psychother 2022, 50, 395–406. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Neece, C.L.; Chan, N.; Klein, K.; Roberts, L.; Fenning, R.M. Mindfulness-Based Stress Reduction for Parents of Children with Developmental Delays: Understanding the Experiences of Latino Families. Mindfulness 2019, 10, 1017–1030. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Singh, N.N.; Lancioni, G.E.; Winton, A.S.W.; Fisher, B.C.; Wahler, R.G.; McAleavey, K.; Singh, J.; Sabaawi, M. Mindful Parenting Decreases Aggression, Noncompliance, and Self-Injury in Children with Autism. J. Emot. Behav. Disord. 2006, 14, 169–177. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Park, Y.R.; Nix, R.L.; Duncan, L.G.; Coatsworth, J.D.; Greenberg, M.T. Unfolding Relations among Mindful Parenting, Recurrent Conflict, and Adolescents’ Externalizing and Internalizing Problems. Fam. Process 2020, 59, 1690–1705. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Yang, W.; Deng, J.; Wang, Y. The Association Between Mindful Parenting and Adolescent Internalizing and Externalizing Problems: The Role of Mother–Child Communication. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2022, 53, 776–785. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Donovan, M.O.; Pickard, J.A.; Herbert, J.S.; Barkus, E. Mindful Parent Training for Parents of Children Aged 3–12 Years with Behavioral Problems: A Scoping Review. Mindfulness 2022, 13, 801–820. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Townshend, K.; Jordan, Z.; Stephenson, M.; Tsey, K. The Effectiveness of Mindful Parenting Programs in Promoting Parents’ and Children’s Wellbeing: A Systematic Review. JBI Database Syst. Rev. Implement. Rep. 2016, 14, 139–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Bögels, S.; Hoogstad, B.; Van Dun, L.; De Schutter, S.; Restifo, K. Mindfulness Training for Adolescents with Externalizing Disorders and Their Parents. Behav. Cogn. Psychother. 2008, 36, 193–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  75. Geurtzen, N.; Scholte, R.H.J.; Engels, R.C.M.E.; Tak, Y.R.; van Zundert, R.M.P. Association Between Mindful Parenting and Adolescents’ Internalizing Problems: Non-Judgmental Acceptance of Parenting as Core Element. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2015, 24, 1117–1128. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Zou, Z.; Zeng, X. The association of mindful parenting with preschool children’s problem behaviors: Parental phubbing as a mediator. Psychologia 2024, 66, 125–136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Huang, J.; Chen, X.; Wu, D.; Liu, R.; Xie, S.; Chi, X. Mindful Parenting and Preschoolers’ Digital Overuse: Exploring the Roles of Executive Function and Gender Differences. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2025, 176, 108440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Schoemann, A.M.; Boulton, A.J.; Short, S.D. Determining Power and Sample Size for Simple and Complex Mediation Models. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 2017, 8, 379–386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Chotpitayasunondh, V.; Douglas, K.M. Measuring Phone Snubbing Behavior: Development and Validation of the Generic Scale of Phubbing (GSP) and the Generic Scale of Being Phubbed (GSBP). Comput. Hum. Behav. 2018, 88, 5–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Brown, K.W.; Ryan, R.M. Mindful Attention Awereness Scale. J. Personal. Soc. Psychol. 2003, 84, 822–848. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  81. Veneziani, C.A.; Voci, A. The Italian Adaptation of the Mindful Awareness Attention Scale and Its Relation with Individual Differences and Quality of Life Indexes. Mindfulness 2015, 6, 373–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Goodman, R. A Modified Version of the Rutter Parent Questionnaire Including Extra Items on Children’s Strengths: A Research Note. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1994, 35, 1483–1494. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Rutter, M. A children’s behaviour questionnaire for completion by teachers: Preliminary findings. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 1967, 8, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Tobia, V.; Gabriele, M.A.; Marzocchi, G.M. The Italian Version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ)-Teacher: Psychometric Properties. J. Psychoeduc. Assess. 2013, 31, 493–505. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Marzocchi, G.M.; Pietro, M.D.; Vio, C.; Bassi, E.; Filoramo, G.; Salmaso, A. Il Questionario SDQ. Strengtht and Difficulties Questionnaire: Uno Strumento per Valutare Difficoltà Comportamentali Ed Emotive in Età Evolutiva. Difficoltà Apprendimento 2003, 8, 483–493. [Google Scholar]
  86. JASP Team. JASP(Version 0.19.3) 2024; JASP Team: Amsterdam, The Netherlands, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  87. R Development Core Team. RStudio (Version 2023.03.1) 2024; R Development Core Team: Boston, MA, USA, 2024. [Google Scholar]
  88. Bögels, S.M.; Lehtonen, A.; Restifo, K. Mindful Parenting in Mental Health Care. Mindfulness 2010, 1, 107–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Parent, J.; McKee, L.G.; Anton, M.; Gonzalez, M.; Jones, D.J.; Forehand, R. Mindfulness in Parenting and Coparenting. Mindfulness 2016, 7, 504–513. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  90. Van der Oord, S.; Bögels, S.M.; Peijnenburg, D. The Effectiveness of Mindfulness Training for Children with ADHD and Mindful Parenting for Their Parents. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2012, 21, 139–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  91. Turpyn, C.C.; Chaplin, T.M. Mindful Parenting and Parents’ Emotion Expression: Effects on Adolescent Risk Behaviors. Mindfulness 2016, 7, 246–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  92. Saral, B.; Acar, I.H. Preschool Children’s Social Competence: The Roles of Parent–Child, Parent–Parent, and Teacher–Child Relationships. Eur. Early Child. Educ. Res. J. 2021, 29, 856–876. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  93. Lv, H.; Ye, W.; Chen, S.; Zhang, H.; Wang, R. The Effect of Mother Phubbing on Young Children’s Emotional and Behavioral Problems: A Moderated Mediation Model of Mother–Child Attachment and Parenting Stress. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2022, 19, 16911. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Sundqvist, A.; Heimann, M.; Koch, F.S. Relationship between Family Technoference and Behavior Problems in Children Aged 4–5 Years. Cyberpsychol. Behav. Soc. Netw. 2020, 23, 371–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  95. Carson, V.; Kuzik, N. The Association between Parent–Child Technology Interference and Cognitive and Social–Emotional Development in Preschool-Aged Children. Child Care Health Dev. 2021, 47, 477–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Niu, G.; Yao, L.; Wu, L.; Tian, Y.; Xu, L.; Sun, X. Parental Phubbing and Adolescent Problematic Mobile Phone Use: The Role of Parent-Child Relationship and Self-Control. Child. Youth Serv. Rev. 2020, 116, 105247. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Xu, C.; Xie, X. Put down the Phone and Accompany Me: How Parental Phubbing Undermines Prosocial Behavior of Early Adolescents. Child Youth Serv. Rev. 2023, 149, 106958. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  98. Kakhki, Z.B.; Mashhadi, A.; Yazdi, S.A.A.; Saleh, S. The Effect of Mindful Parenting Training on Parent–Child Interactions, Parenting Stress, and Cognitive Emotion Regulation in Mothers of Preschool Children. J. Child Fam. Stud. 2022, 31, 3113–3124. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Lippold, M.A.; Duncan, L.G.; Coatsworth, J.D.; Nix, R.L.; Greenberg, M.T. Understanding How Mindful Parenting May Be Linked to Mother–Adolescent Communication. J. Youth Adolesc. 2015, 44, 1663–1673. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  100. Dumas, J.E. Mindfulness-Based Parent Training: Strategies to Lessen the Grip of Automaticity in Families with Disruptive Children. J. Clin. Child Adolesc. Psychol. 2005, 34, 779–791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  101. Potharst, E.S.; Leyland, A.; Colonnesi, C.; Veringa, I.K.; Salvadori, E.A.; Jakschik, M.; Bögels, S.M.; Zeegers, M.A.J. Does Mothers’ Self-Reported Mindful Parenting Relate to the Observed Quality of Parenting Behavior and Mother-Child Interaction? Mindfulness 2021, 12, 344–356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  102. Perini, N.; Porru, A.; Moeller, K.; Jay, T.; Sella, F. Number Express: A Digital Game to Improve Early Numeracy. In Proceedings of the 17th European Conference on Games Based Learning, ECGBL 2023, Enschede, The Netherlands, 5–6 October 2023. [Google Scholar]
Figure 1. Correlation analysis between MAAS (Mindful Attention Awareness Scale) and SDQ (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire). The figure displays Pearson’s correlation coefficients between SDQ subscales (hyperactivity–inattention, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, prosocial behavior, peer problems, and total difficulties) and parental mindfulness (MAAS). Warmer shades represent negative correlations, whereas cooler shades indicate positive correlations. Asterisks denote significance levels (* = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001).
Figure 1. Correlation analysis between MAAS (Mindful Attention Awareness Scale) and SDQ (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire). The figure displays Pearson’s correlation coefficients between SDQ subscales (hyperactivity–inattention, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, prosocial behavior, peer problems, and total difficulties) and parental mindfulness (MAAS). Warmer shades represent negative correlations, whereas cooler shades indicate positive correlations. Asterisks denote significance levels (* = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001).
Children 12 01022 g001
Figure 2. Correlation analysis between GSP (Generic Scale of Phubbing) and SDQ (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire). The figure displays Pearson’s correlation coefficients between SDQ subscales (hyperactivity–inattention, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, prosocial behavior, peer problems, and total difficulties) and the components of the Generic Scale of Phubbing (GSP; nomophobia, interpersonal conflict, self-isolation, problem acknowledgement, and total score). Warmer shades represent negative correlations, whereas cooler shades indicate positive correlations. The intensity of the color corresponds to the strength of the association. Asterisks denote significance levels (* = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001).
Figure 2. Correlation analysis between GSP (Generic Scale of Phubbing) and SDQ (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire). The figure displays Pearson’s correlation coefficients between SDQ subscales (hyperactivity–inattention, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, prosocial behavior, peer problems, and total difficulties) and the components of the Generic Scale of Phubbing (GSP; nomophobia, interpersonal conflict, self-isolation, problem acknowledgement, and total score). Warmer shades represent negative correlations, whereas cooler shades indicate positive correlations. The intensity of the color corresponds to the strength of the association. Asterisks denote significance levels (* = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001).
Children 12 01022 g002
Figure 3. Correlation analysis between GSP (Generic Scale of Phubbing) and MAAS (Mindful Attention Awareness Scale). The figure displays pairwise Pearson’s correlation coefficients among SDQ subscales (hyperactivity–inattention, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, prosocial behavior, peer problems, and total difficulties), the components of the Generic Scale of Phubbing (GSP: nomophobia, interpersonal conflict, self-isolation, problem acknowledgement, and total score), and parental mindfulness (MAAS). Cooler shades (blue) indicate positive correlations, whereas warmer shades (red) represent negative correlations. The intensity of the color reflects the magnitude of the correlation. Asterisks denote significance levels (** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001).
Figure 3. Correlation analysis between GSP (Generic Scale of Phubbing) and MAAS (Mindful Attention Awareness Scale). The figure displays pairwise Pearson’s correlation coefficients among SDQ subscales (hyperactivity–inattention, conduct problems, emotional symptoms, prosocial behavior, peer problems, and total difficulties), the components of the Generic Scale of Phubbing (GSP: nomophobia, interpersonal conflict, self-isolation, problem acknowledgement, and total score), and parental mindfulness (MAAS). Cooler shades (blue) indicate positive correlations, whereas warmer shades (red) represent negative correlations. The intensity of the color reflects the magnitude of the correlation. Asterisks denote significance levels (** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001).
Children 12 01022 g003
Figure 4. Mediation analysis between SDQ (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire), MAAS (Mindful Attention Awareness Scale) and GSP (Generic Scale of Phubbing). Arrows represent standardized path coefficients, with curved arrows indicating explained variance (R2) for each endogenous variable.
Figure 4. Mediation analysis between SDQ (Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire), MAAS (Mindful Attention Awareness Scale) and GSP (Generic Scale of Phubbing). Arrows represent standardized path coefficients, with curved arrows indicating explained variance (R2) for each endogenous variable.
Children 12 01022 g004
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Puligheddu, A.; Porru, A.; Spano, A.; Cataudella, S.; Mascia, M.L.; Rollo, D.; Cabras, C.; Penna, M.P.; Lucangeli, D. The Relationship Between Parental Phubbing and Preschoolers’ Behavioral Problems: The Mediation Role of Mindful Attention Awareness. Children 2025, 12, 1022. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12081022

AMA Style

Puligheddu A, Porru A, Spano A, Cataudella S, Mascia ML, Rollo D, Cabras C, Penna MP, Lucangeli D. The Relationship Between Parental Phubbing and Preschoolers’ Behavioral Problems: The Mediation Role of Mindful Attention Awareness. Children. 2025; 12(8):1022. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12081022

Chicago/Turabian Style

Puligheddu, Antonio, Annamaria Porru, Andrea Spano, Stefania Cataudella, Maria Lidia Mascia, Dolores Rollo, Cristina Cabras, Maria Pietronilla Penna, and Daniela Lucangeli. 2025. "The Relationship Between Parental Phubbing and Preschoolers’ Behavioral Problems: The Mediation Role of Mindful Attention Awareness" Children 12, no. 8: 1022. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12081022

APA Style

Puligheddu, A., Porru, A., Spano, A., Cataudella, S., Mascia, M. L., Rollo, D., Cabras, C., Penna, M. P., & Lucangeli, D. (2025). The Relationship Between Parental Phubbing and Preschoolers’ Behavioral Problems: The Mediation Role of Mindful Attention Awareness. Children, 12(8), 1022. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12081022

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop