Next Article in Journal
Risk Factors for Postoperative Pulmonary Compromise in a Pediatric Population: A Retrospective Review of a Single Institution Cohort
Previous Article in Journal
Age-Dependent Burst Suppression During Anesthesia in Young Children with Congenital Heart Disease: The Impact of Anesthetic Depth
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

The Potential Mediating Role of Good Mental Health on the Relationship Between Low Physical Activity and High Screen Time with Executive Functions in Chilean Children and Adolescents

by
Felipe Caamaño-Navarrete
1,
Carlos Arriagada-Hernández
1,
Roberto Lagos-Hernández
1,
Gerardo Fuentes-Vilugrón
1,
Lorena Jara-Tomckowiack
2,
Eduardo Sandoval-Obando
3,
Guido Contreras-Díaz
4,
Daniel Jerez-Mayorga
5,6,
Claudio Hernández-Mosqueira
7 and
Pedro Delgado-Floody
8,*
1
Physical Education Career, Universidad Autónoma de Chile, Temuco 4780000, Chile
2
Facultad de Educación, Universidad Católica de Temuco, Temuco 4780000, Chile
3
Escuela de Psicología, Instituto Iberoamericano Para el Desarrollo Sostenible, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales y Humanas, Universidad Autónoma de Chile, Temuco 4800916, Chile
4
Escuela de Kinesiología, Facultad de Ciencias de la Rehabilitación y Calidad de Vida, Universidad San Sebastián, Lago Panguipulli 1390, Puerto Montt 5501842, Chile
5
Department of Physical Education and Sports, Faculty of Sport Sciences, Sport and Health University Research Institute (iMUDS), University of Granada, 18012 Granada, Spain
6
Exercise and Rehabilitation Sciences Institute, Faculty of Rehabilitation Sciences, Universidad Andres Bello, Santiago 7591538, Chile
7
Departamento de Ciencias de la Educación, Universidad del Bio-Bio, Chillán 3800708, Chile
8
Department of Physical Education, Sport and Recreation, Universidad de La Frontera, Temuco 4811230, Chile
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Children 2025, 12(10), 1402; https://doi.org/10.3390/children12101402
Submission received: 29 August 2025 / Revised: 8 October 2025 / Accepted: 14 October 2025 / Published: 17 October 2025
(This article belongs to the Section Pediatric Mental Health)

Abstract

Highlights

What are the main findings?
  • Good mental health partially mediates the link between an unhealthy lifestyle and executive functions.
  • Low physical activity and high screen time were inversely related to attention, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and working memory.
What is the implication of the main finding?
  • Promoting active lifestyles and mental well-being from early ages may enhance cognitive development.
  • Schools and families are key in integrated actions for physical activity, mental health, and screen use.

Abstract

Background: Childhood and adolescence are increasingly recognized as life stages that pose specific challenges for treating and promoting mental health and cognitive development. Objective: The objective of the present study was to determine the potential mediating role of good mental health in the association between an unhealthy lifestyle (i.e., low physical activity (PA) and high screen time (ST)) with executive functions (EFs) (i.e., attention, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and working memory) in children and adolescents. Methods: A cross-sectional investigation with 625 students aged 10–17 years participated. The Krece Plus questionnaire (lifestyle, PA, and ST), Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS-21, metal health), and CogniFit (EFs) were used in the present study. Results: Good mental health presented a partial mediating role in the relationship between a bad lifestyle and EFs. Likewise, a bad lifestyle was linked inversely to attention (β −37.45, p = 0.002), the executive function of cognitive flexibility (β −85.91, p < 0.001), inhibition (β −60.16, p < 0.001), and working memory (β −75.73, p < 0.001). Conclusions: Good mental health acts as a relevant mediator in child and adolescent cognitive development. These results reinforce the need to promote active and healthy lifestyles, as well as strategies that promote psychological wellbeing from an early age. Schools and families play an important role as protective agents and promoters of integral development; it is therefore recommended to implement intervention programmes that strategically address the physical activity, mental health, and digital habits of this population.

1. Introduction

Childhood and adolescence are increasingly recognized as life stages that pose specific challenges for treating and promoting mental health and cognitive development. The scientific literature has explicitly highlighted the importance of mental health in the human being [1]. Likewise, childhood and adolescence are critical periods for the development of psychological wellbeing [2]. Establishing good levels at an early age is therefore critical due to the implications for mental wellbeing in adulthood [3], and so, estimations and analyses of mental health in schools have garnered increased interest [4]. The evidence highlights that young people’s mental health is a global public health challenge (i.e., problems affect 10–20 per cent of children and adolescents worldwide) [5]. For example, since the mid-twentieth century, mental illness has positioned itself as one of the most relevant burdens for the health systems of countries, particularly among adolescents [6].
Mental health components could have negative effects on cognitive, emotional, and social development [7]. The stress faced by the population in this age group has an impact on child and youth wellbeing [8]. Research has estimated a high prevalence of mental disorders with an average onset before the age of 14 [9]. In Chile, high rates of anxiety, depression, and stress have been identified in this population; these have been associated with multifactorial situations [10]. Mental health not only influences individual wellbeing but also academic performance [11] and social relations, highlighting its relevance as a key component of human development [12], which includes, among other capacities, executive functions [13] such as attention, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and working memory [14], these being essential skills for learning, emotional regulation, and decision-making. In this study, we adopt a two-dimensional model of mental health, which recognizes both the absence of negative emotional states (depression, anxiety, and stress) and the presence of positive wellbeing. This approach was chosen because it provides a more comprehensive and consistent framework for understanding child and adolescent development.
EFs have been defined as higher-order cognitive skills oriented towards the achievement of objectives (intentional behaviour) based on the performance of complex tasks [15,16]. They allow a person to regulate their attention span, keep objectives and information in mind, avoid automated responses, and reflect on the consequences of different behaviours [17]. In addition, they are vital mental processes in novel situations that require the rapid and flexible adjustment of behaviour in response to changing environmental demands [18]. There is consensus in establishing the three dimensions related to EFs: inhibition and control of interference (i.e., attention and concentration), working memory, and cognitive flexibility [19]. In addition, it has been established that developing EFs could have beneficial effects on different dimensions of human beings’ lives [13]. High cognitive development during early stages has been associated with improved mental health in adulthood [20].
Traditionally, the scientific literature has reported that a healthy lifestyle is positively related to variables associated with health and a decrease in the risk of death [21]. However, several studies have broadened the view, generating strong evidence that a healthy lifestyle is positively associated with EFs and mental health [22,23]. Consistent with this, it has been indicated that regular participation in physical activity (PA) programmes is positively related to working memory, inhibition, problem solving, and cognitive skills [24]. On the other hand, one study indicated that high screen time (ST) had a negative impact on cognition [25]. Therefore, the components of a healthy lifestyle must be diagnosed according to their relationship with different variables of interest for the school context. In line with the above, it has been indicated that improving one’s lifestyle could be an effective tool to positively impact mental health and EFs [26,27].
Studies have shown that children’s PA favourably affects mental health in youth [28,29] and acts as a mediator that promotes wellbeing and generates a decrease in anxiety [30], suggesting that interventions of this nature could have positive effects, including on symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [31]. Despite this background, the impact of PA and its specific role as a mediator in mental health and lifestyle has not been systematically explored in Chile [32]. In relation to an unhealthy lifestyle, characterized by low PA and high ST [33], PA has been considered a critical aspect of mental health in child and youth populations [34]. Although some of the instruments applied have been previously validated in Chile, it is important to consider that cultural, educational, and socioeconomic factors in this context may generate distinctive patterns that differ from international evidence. Therefore, examining these associations locally provides a more nuanced understanding of child and adolescent development in Chile. In Chile, data reveal that active adolescents report positive wellbeing [27]; although the harmful effect of excessive screen use [35] is not necessarily compensated by PA [32], it is positively associated with working memory and cognitive flexibility and a better quality of life [27], which is also related to eating habits [36]. Evidence indicates that participation in team sports favours EFs [37] and that vigorous PA and good sleep quality improve working memory and cognitive flexibility [38]. Thus, these physical skills are associated with better cognition and represent mediators directly related to executive functions [39], while their absence is related to consequences for sleep, quality of life, and cognitive development. In this study, we conceptualize mental health as an affective–emotional state (levels of depression symptoms, anxiety, and stress) that can either facilitate or hinder the optimal functioning of EFs. We therefore adopt a mediating (not moderating) framework: lifestyle may influence EFs performance indirectly through its impact on mental health rather than changing the strength of the lifestyle–EF association across levels of mental health.
This study contributes to the growing body of scientific evidence that supports the design of interventions aimed at improving mental health and EFs in students, as well as how good mental health can mediate the link between lifestyle and cognition. The mediation of mental health and its link with lifestyle and EFs have been explored in international contexts [40], suggesting that PA could have favourable effects, interceding against EFs [41], with a favourable influence on academic performance, self-efficacy, and emotional regulation. Therefore, observing the potential mediating role in mental health in association with lifestyle and EFs, based on Chilean children and adolescents, constitutes a substantive contribution to the understanding of this phenomenon. Given the cross-sectional nature of the research, the study hypothesizes that an unhealthy lifestyle is negatively associated with EFs, and better mental health mediates these associations. Considering this background, the aim of the present study was to determine the potential mediating role of good mental health in the association between an unhealthy lifestyle (i.e., low physical activity and high screen time) with executive functions (i.e., attention, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and working memory) in children and adolescents.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Participants and Study Design

The present study employed a quantitative, cross-sectional, and descriptive-associative design. A total of 625 Chilean youth aged 10 to 17 years (mean age 13.68 ± 1.65 years) from various schools in Temuco, Chile, participated in the present investigation. A total of twenty-three students were excluded. The sample was intentional and non-probabilistic. In this regard, the main limitation of results from non-probabilistic data is their uncertain and limited generalizability to the population of interest [42]
The sample size was calculated considering the following factors: (1) enrolment of students in educational institutions appropriate for their age group (10–17 years); (2) a significance level of 5 percent; (3) an absolute precision of 5 percent; (4) a statistical power of 95 percent; (5) the statistical test (t-test); (6) the number of measurements (x1); and (7) an effect size of 0.2. Based on these parameters, and accounting for an expected response rate of 80 percent, a sample size of 400 participants aged 10 to 17 years was determined.
The inclusion criteria were as follows: (i) subjects had to be enrolled in school and (ii) be aged between 10 and 17 years. The exclusion criteria included: (i) any medical contraindications that would prevent normal performance in the assessments and (ii) absence during the evaluations.
The study adhered to the principles outlined in the 2013 Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Universidad Autónoma de Chile (approval number: CEC 13-25). Participation in the research required signed consent from the students themselves, as well as informed consent from their parents.

2.2. Main Outcomes

2.2.1. Lifestyle

PA patterns were determined with the Krece Plus test [43]. Krece Plus is a quick questionnaire that classifies lifestyle based on the average number of hours spent watching television or playing video games per day (ST) and the hours of PA after school per week. The classification is made according to the number of hours spent on each item. The total points are added, and the person’s lifestyle is classified as good (men ≥ 9, women ≥ 8), regular (men 6–8, women 5–7), or bad (men ≤ 5, women ≤ 4), according to the lifestyle score. The Krece Plus instrument has been used previously with Chilean students [44] and has shown moderate internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha 0.79) [45].

2.2.2. Executive Function

To evaluate EFs, including inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility, and attention, the CogniFit neurocognitive assessment battery (San Francisco, CA, USA) was used [46]. This 40 min assessment provides both a general cognitive score and specific scores for EFs. The CogniFit battery has been reported to exhibit good reliability and has been successfully used with school-aged subjects [47]. Moreover, CogniFit has been used previously with Chilean schoolchildren [36]. Cognifit© is used individually to measure the cognitive functioning of all participants. This cognitive profile has demonstrated high reliability, consistency, and stability in previous studies [48]. CogniFit has shown good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha 0.85–0.88) [49].

2.2.3. Mental Health

The abbreviated version of the Depression Anxiety Stress Scale (DASS-21) was used to determine mental health problems [50]. This three-dimensional self-report scale was designed to evaluate the presence and intensity of emotional states or symptoms of depression, anxiety, and stress [51,52,53]. The questionnaire is composed of 21 questions divided into three subscales: depression (feelings of dysphoria, hopelessness, devaluation of life, self-hatred, lack of interest, and anhedonia); anxiety (evaluates experiences of physiological arousal, situational anxiety, and general anxiety); and stress (evaluates levels of irritability, feelings of overwhelm, problems relaxing, and difficulty controlling excessive thinking). Questions are scored on a scale from 0 (“It doesn’t describe anything that happened to me or that I felt during the week”) to 3 points (“Yes, this happened to me a lot, or most of the time”) according to the degree of intensity in the last week. The sum of the questions is therefore within the range of 0 to 21 points. Lower scores corresponded to reduced symptoms. A score > 5 points was the cut-off point for depression and stress symptoms. A score > 4 points was the cut-off point for anxiety symptoms [54]. This instrument has the advantages of being a self-report scale, brief, easy to administer and answer, and easy to interpret [55]. These questionnaires have been used with Chilean students [56] and presented adequate psychometric properties [55]. DASS-21 has shown good internal consistency (Cronbach’s alpha > 0.80) in adolescent subjects [57].
The research team responsible for evaluating the adolescent participants was trained in the study protocols and assessment procedures prior to data collection. Subjects completed the questionnaires and evaluations individually under the supervision of researchers who were available to address any questions or concerns.

2.3. Statistical Analysis

The data are presented as means and standard deviations (SD). Normality assumptions and homoscedasticity were analyzed using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test and the Levene test, respectively. t-test was used to identify differences between sexes (girls vs. boys); to compare differences in lifestyle, analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used; meanwhile, the Bonferroni post hoc test was applied to test differences among groups according to lifestyle (good vs. regular vs. bad lifestyle). Overall, the alpha level was set at p < 0.05 for statistical significance.
To examine the association between lifestyle and EFs, lineal regression models were applied and adjusted for age (Model 2) and sex (Model 3). The results are reported as Unstandardized Coefficients of beta coefficients (β) and 95% confidence interval [CI]). In addition, the Standardized Coefficients of beta and standard error (SE) were added.
Regression analyses were performed to verify the effect of the mediating variable good mental health (including anxiety, stress, and depression symptoms) (M), considering bad lifestyle (i.e., low PA and high ST) as the independent variable (X) and EFs as the dependent variables (Y). Within the analysis, the total effect (c), direct effect (c′), and indirect effect (a*b, IE) were calculated for the samples, as well as the 95 percent CI using the macro/interface PROCESS v. 3.3 for SPSS v. 23 and the bootstrapping method with a resampling rate of 5000 [58]. The percentage of mediation was estimated as a proportion between the direct and the total effect 1 − (c′/c). All the statistical analyses were performed using SPSS statistical software version 23.0 (SPSSTM Inc., Chicago, USA). The alpha level was set at p < 0.05 for statistical significance.

3. Results

Table 1 shows the comparison of study variables according to sex. Boys reported higher levels of PA than girls (p < 0.001). The girls presented worse symptoms (p < 0.001) in anxiety, depression, and stress. Compared with girls, boys reported higher scores in cognitive flexibility (p < 0.001).
Table 2 presents the results for the frequency of mental health problems according to sex. There were significant differences in mental health variables according to sex. The lifestyle categories presented no significant differences (p > 0.05).
Table 3 shows the association between EFs and a bad lifestyle. Bad lifestyle was inversely linked to attention (β −37.45, p = 0.002); when the association was adjusted by sex and age, the significant association remained (Model 3). The function executive of cognitive flexibility in a model adjusted by sex and age (Model 3) reported an inverse association with bad lifestyle (β −85.91, p < 0.001). The inhibition variable presented an inverse association with bad lifestyle, adjusted for sex and age (β −60.16, p < 0.001, Model 3). Finally, working memory was inversely linked to bad lifestyle (β −75.73, p < 0.001).
The mediation analysis results for the total sample (n = 625 children and adolescents) are shown in Figure 1. Good mental health emerged as a mediating variable in the relationship between a bad lifestyle and EFs. In the first regression step (a), a bad lifestyle was inversely related to mental health. In the second step (c’), the regression coefficient of a bad lifestyle in attention was also significant. In the third step, the potential mediator of mental health was positively related to the dependent variable (b); when both bad lifestyle and mental health were included in the model (c), the regression coefficient remained statistically significant (p < 0.001). Finally, the indirect effect confirmed that mental health was a partial mediator of attention (indirect effect −1.61; SE 1.34; 95% CI −4.71–0.48; and Med 4.5%) (see Figure 1A). Similarly, the potential mediator of mental health was positively related to the dependent variables (b) of cognitive flexibility (Figure 1B) and to working memory (Figure 1D). Finally, the indirect effect confirmed that mental health was a partial mediator of cognitive flexibility (indirect effect −1.92; SE 1.93; 95% CI −6.40–1.18; and Med 3%) and working memory (indirect effect −1.027; SE 1.45; 95% CI −4.44–1.45; and Med 3.3%). Mental health presented no mediation in the association between a bad lifestyle and the EF of flexibility.

4. Discussion

The objective of this study was to determine the potential mediating role in the association between an unhealthy lifestyle (i.e., low PA and high ST) and EFs (i.e., attention, inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and working memory) in children and adolescents. The main results of this study were as follows: (i) mental health presented as a mediator role in the relationship between a bad lifestyle (i.e., high ST and low PA) and EFs; (ii) bad lifestyle was inversely associated with all EFs; and (iii) girls reported lower levels of cognitive flexibility and lower mental health than boys.
In the present study, mental health presented a mediator role in the relationship between an unhealthy lifestyle (i.e., high ST and low PA) with the EFs of (i) attention, (ii) cognitive flexibility, and (iii) working memory. Although we regard executive function as an overarching concept, its various elements react in distinct ways to lifestyle factors and mental health influences. Aspects like attention and working memory seem more immediately affected by stress and the management of emotions, which accounts for the anticipated more pronounced mediating role of mental health in those specific areas. Another study, which had the objective of determining the potential mediating role of subjective wellbeing (i.e., health-related quality of life, HRQoL) in the association between ST and EFs, showed that HRQoL partially mediated the relationship between ST and EFs in schoolchildren, including attention (5%), inhibition (3.18%), working memory (3.82%), and cognitive flexibility (5.3%) [27]. This finding is supported by recent research such as that by Caamaño-Navarrete et al. [36], in which it was observed that an unhealthy lifestyle was inversely associated with negative mental health in Chilean schoolchildren. Halse et al. [59] found, in a longitudinal analysis, that poorer mental health (i.e., increased depression, anxiety, ADHD, and ODD/CD) may negatively impact executive functioning among Norwegian children. A systematic review with metanalysis highlighted that adolescents with mental health issues such as depression showed lower performance on cognitive tasks [60]. Research has proven that a better childhood cognitive ability is linked to fewer mental health problems (i.e., symptoms of anxiety and depression) in adulthood in women from the British 1946 birth cohort [61]. It has been shown that depressive symptoms mediate the association between a healthy lifestyle and cognitive function [62]. Another study found that students with a better cognitive ability had higher levels of mental health (i.e., higher levels of global self-esteem), fewer behaviour problems, and reduced hyperactivity and inattention problems [63]. The fact that mental health and EFs have a complex interactive relationship was obtained from exhaustive studies [61]. These findings underline that mental health is an important factor to consider in understanding the relationship between lifestyle and EFs; therefore, improving mental health could buffer the negative effects of low PA and high ST on the cognition of children and youths.
Likewise, an unhealthy lifestyle was inversely associated with all EFs: (i) attention, (ii) cognitive flexibility, (iii) inhibition, and (iii) working memory. This aligns with prior research, such as the study on lifestyle and cognition among Chilean schoolchildren that found that children’s lifestyles (i.e., high ST and low MD adherence) were negatively associated with EFs [44]. Complementary to the above, previous evidence has shown that a bad lifestyle could have a negative effect on attention [64]. Another study has shown that high ST contributes to adverse cognitive, EF, and behaviour outcomes [65]. Similarly, a recent study indicated that excessive ST was negatively related to EFs in children [66]. Furthermore, evidence indicates that ST has adverse consequences on brain structure and intelligence [67]. Similarly, the study by Cadenas-Sánchez et al. [68] highlighted that sedentary time was negatively linked to the shape of subcortical brain structures, which in turn was related to intelligence in schoolchildren. Complementary to the above, after a two-year follow-up study, Chen et al. [69] suggested that a negative long-term impact of increased daily ST on children’s neuropsychological development. The study conducted by Lewin et al. [38] on schoolchildren demonstrated that ST was linked to reduced brain activation during an inhibitory control task. Complementary to the above, Jirout et al. [70] reported that modifiable lifestyle factors such as diet quality, PA, and sleep patterns are important for learning skills in school. Indeed, good food habits and higher PA are good predictors of EFs despite the inverse link between obesity and EFs among Malaysian adolescents [71].
Additionally, another study highlighted that a lifestyle characterized by a lack of PA could have detrimental effects on cognitive functioning [72]. The study conducted by Daniele et al. [73] indicated that physical inactivity progressively led to reduced peripheral and brain vasculature. Meanwhile, the study by McMath et al. [74] reported that meeting guidelines for PA and ST was related to greater EFs. Our findings are supported by research by Zeng et al. [75], in which it was observed that low ST and high PA were positively associated with the development of EFs in children. The systematic review and metanalysis by Álvarez-Bueno et al. [76] demonstrated that PA benefits several domains of cognition and metacognition in youths. Moreover, it has been indicated that PA in youth is positively related with cognitive benefits [77]. This aligns with prior research highlighting the crucial role of PA interventions and programmes on EFs at the school stage [75]. Several researchers have affirmed that PA can influence brain function through the modulatory effects of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) and hormones and metabolites of the muscle–brain axis such as irisin, lactate, cathepsin B, kynurenine, and insulin-like growth factor-1 (IGF-1) [78,79], and school is among the most relevant factors to promote PA in children.
Girls reported lower levels of cognitive flexibility. Another study [80] also reported sex differences in EFs (i.e., inhibition task). Additionally, a study carried out by Gaillard et al. [81] claimed that there is evidence for sex differences in the neural networks underlying all tasks of executive control. Another study among children reported sex differences in neutral reaction time (RT) in a flanker task and go accuracy in the go/no-go task [82]. A metanalysis also provided evidence for gender-specific networks in working memory, whereby females consistently activate more limbic (e.g., amygdala and hippocampus) and prefrontal structures (e.g., right inferior frontal gyrus) [83]. On the other hand, a systematic review and metanalysis reported that males and females did not differ in performance in any of the three EF domains [84].
Moreover, we found that girls presented lower mental health than boys. Sex differences in adolescents’ mental health problems have been extensively reported. A longitudinal study reported that young people, particularly girls, are at increased risk of mental health problems between the ages of 11 and 14 [85]. A study on mental health in adolescents demonstrated that girls had higher levels of anxiety and depression symptomatology than boys [86]. Furthermore, evidence indicates that there appear to be sex and gender differences in mental health prevalence and risk factors [87]. In addition, it has been shown that mental health, cognitive development, and social background are linked to educational success; therefore, promoting positive mental health can improve children’s learning outcomes [88].
The findings of this study have significant implications for the development of targeted interventions aimed at promoting healthy lifestyles among Chilean students. Educational programmes focusing on PA are crucial. Furthermore, schools should consider implementing policies that support healthy behaviours, such as providing access to affordable and nutritious meals, creating opportunities for PA, and promoting awareness of the importance of healthy lifestyle. Likewise, scientific evidence highlights the importance of stimulating the development of healthy habits in the school context. A systematic review highlighted the importance of having a healthy lifestyle due to the benefits it provides at the mental level and for subjective wellbeing [89]. Therefore, it is important to continue researching the relationship between lifestyle and different variables of importance in the school context such as EFs and mental health. In a future study, we plan to implement short, high-intensity physical activity breaks in classrooms to determine their impact on high-intensity, short-duration cognitive and mental health variables.
This study has some limitations that should be considered when interpreting the results. Firstly, the cross-sectional design of this study prevents establishing causal relationships between lifestyle, mental health, and EFs, limiting inferences to correlational associations. Secondly, a non-probabilistic and intentional sample was used, which restricts the generalization of the findings to other child and adolescent populations. Thirdly, another limitation is the absence of contextual variables, such as the socioeconomic level or family environment. Future research should also consider incorporating objective measures of PA and ST.

5. Conclusions

In conclusion, this study highlights that an unhealthy lifestyle, characterized by unhealthy lifestyle habits such as low physical activity and high screen time, is inversely associated with the executive functions of Chilean children and adolescents, especially in attention, cognitive flexibility, and working memory. In addition, good mental health acts as a relevant mediator in child and adolescent cognitive development. These results reinforce the need to promote an active and healthy lifestyle along with strategies that promote psychological wellbeing from an early age. Schools and families play an important role as protective agents and promoters of integral development, so it is recommended to implement intervention programmes that strategically address the physical activity, mental health, and digital habits of this population.

Author Contributions

F.C.-N., P.D.-F., C.A.-H. and L.J.-T. contributed to the conception, organization, and oversight of this study, drafting of the analysis plan, writing of the original manuscript draft, and final approval of the version to be published. R.L.-H., G.F.-V., D.J.-M., E.S.-O., C.H.-M. and G.C.-D. contributed to critical manuscript revision and final approval of the version to be published. F.C.-N., P.D.-F. and C.A.-H. contributed to data analysis and interpretation, critical manuscript revision, and final approval of the version to be published. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the ANID FONDECYT Postdoctoral project Folio: 3250451, titled “Proyecto Feliz-Mente Araucanía. Análisis de los beneficios de un modelo de intervención de breaks cortos y activos de actividad física de alta intensidad sobre las funciones ejecutivas y la salud mental en el contexto escolar”.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The investigation complied with the 2013 Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by an Ethics Committee of the Universidad Autónoma de Chile (N° CEC 13-25 Act, 30 April 2025).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in this study.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in this study are included in the article. Further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

  1. Gregory, T.; Engelhardt, D.; Lewkowicz, A.; Luddy, S.; Guhn, M.; Gadermann, A.; Schonert-Reichl, K.; Brinkman, S. Validity of the middle years development instrument for population monitoring of student wellbeing in Australian school children. Child Indic. Res. 2019, 12, 873–899. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  2. McDool, E.; Powell, P.; Roberts, J.; Taylor, K. The internet and children’s psychological wellbeing. J. Health Econ. 2020, 69, 102274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Tomyn, A.J.; Cummins, R.A. The Subjective Wellbeing of High-School Students: Validating the Personal Wellbeing Index—School Children. Soc. Indic. Res. 2011, 101, 405–418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. O’Farrell, P.; Wilson, C.; Shiel, G. Teachers’ perceptions of the barriers to assessment of mental health in schools with implications for educational policy: A systematic review. Br. J. Educ. Psychol. 2023, 93, 262–282. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Kieling, C.; Baker-Henningham, H.; Belfer, M.; Conti, G.; Ertem, I.; Omigbodun, O.; Rohde, L.A.; Srinath, S.; Ulkuer, N.; Rahman, A. Child and adolescent mental health worldwide: Evidence for action. Lancet 2011, 378, 1515–1525. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. McGorry, P.; Gunasiri, H.; Mei, C.; Rice, S.; Gao, C.X. The youth mental health crisis: Analysis and solutions. Front. Psychiatry 2025, 15, 1517533. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Jürges, H.; Schwarz, A.; Cahan, S.; Abdeen, Z. Child mental health and cognitive development: Evidence from the West Bank. Empirica 2019, 46, 423–442. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Patton, G.C.; Sawyer, S.M.; Santelli, J.S.; Ross, D.A.; Afifi, R.; Allen, N.B.; Arora, M.; Azzopardi, P.; Baldwin, W.; Bonell, C. Our future: A Lancet commission on adolescent health and wellbeing. Lancet 2016, 387, 2423–2478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Merikangas, K.R.; He, J.P.; Burstein, M.; Swanson, S.A.; Avenevoli, S.; Cui, L.; Benjet, C.; Georgiades, K.; Swendsen, J. Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in U.S. adolescents: Results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication—Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A). J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2010, 49, 980–989. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Diaz, A.M.; Flores, C.P.; Huerta, I.M.; Tardone, N.D.; Pizarro, C.A.; Cerda, D.A.; Estay, J.A. Factores relacionados a la depresión durante la adolescencia: Una revisión integrativa. Horizonte de enfermería 2023, 34, 321–358. [Google Scholar]
  11. Murphy, J.M.; Guzmán, J.; McCarthy, A.E.; Squicciarini, A.M.; George, M.; Canenguez, K.M.; Dunn, E.C.; Baer, L.; Simonsohn, A.; Smoller, J.W. Mental health predicts better academic outcomes: A longitudinal study of elementary school students in Chile. Child Psychiatry Hum. Dev. 2015, 46, 245–256. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. de la Barra, F.; Irarrazaval, M.; Valdes, A.; Soto-Brandt, G. Evolving child and adolescent mental health and development programs in Chile. Rev. Panam. Salud Pública 2019, 43, e33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2013, 64, 135–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Cortés Pascual, A.; Moyano Muñoz, N.; Quílez Robres, A. The relationship between executive functions and academic performance in primary education: Review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 449759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  15. Zhan, Z.; Ai, J.; Ren, F.; Li, L.; Chu, C.-H.; Chang, Y.-K. Cardiorespiratory fitness, age, and multiple aspects of executive function among preadolescent children. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 1198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  16. Etnier, J.L.; Chang, Y.-K. The effect of physical activity on executive function: A brief commentary on definitions, measurement issues, and the current state of the literature. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol. 2009, 31, 469–483. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Zelazo, P.D.; Blair, C.B.; Willoughby, M.T. Executive Function: Implications for Education. NCER 2017–2000. National Center for Education Research 2016. Available online: https://ies.ed.gov/ncer/2025/01/executive-function-implications-education (accessed on 18 May 2025).
  18. Huizinga, M.; Baeyens, D.; Burack, J.A. Executive function and education. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 1357. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. St Clair-Thompson, H.L.; Gathercole, S.E. Executive functions and achievements in school: Shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 2006, 59, 745–759. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Feinstein, L.; Bynner, J. The importance of cognitive development in middle childhood for adulthood socioeconomic status, mental health, and problem behavior. Child Dev. 2004, 75, 1329–1339. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Tamakoshi, A.; Tamakoshi, K.; Lin, Y.; Yagyu, K.; Kikuchi, S.; Group, J.S. Healthy lifestyle and preventable death: Findings from the Japan Collaborative Cohort (JACC) Study. Prev. Med. 2009, 48, 486–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Pilato, I.B.; Beezhold, B.; Radnitz, C. Diet and lifestyle factors associated with cognitive performance in college students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2020, 70, 2230–2236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Jao, N.C.; Robinson, L.D.; Kelly, P.J.; Ciecierski, C.C.; Hitsman, B. Unhealthy behavior clustering and mental health status in United States college students. J. Am. Coll. Health 2019, 67, 790–800. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Sweet, M. The Relationship Between Executive Functions and Physical Activity in University Students. Senior Thesis, Honors College, Baylor University, Waco, TX, USA, 2016. [Google Scholar]
  25. Choudhary, A.K.; Loganathan, S.; Maheshkumar, K. A Sedentary Lifestyle and Cognitive Function. Chall. Dis. Health Res. 2021, 6, 52–62. [Google Scholar]
  26. Melnyk, B.; Kelly, S.; Jacobson, D.; Arcoleo, K.; Shaibi, G. Improving physical activity, mental health outcomes, and academic retention in college students with Freshman 5 to thrive: COPE/Healthy lifestyles. J. Am. Assoc. Nurse Pract. 2014, 26, 314–322. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  27. Caamaño-Navarrete, F.; Arriagada-Hernández, C.; Jara-Tomckowiack, L.; Hernandez-Martinez, J.; Valdés-Badilla, P.; Contreras-Díaz, G.; del-Cuerpo, I.; Delgado-Floody, P. Association Between Screen Time and Lifestyle Parameters with Executive Functions in Chilean Children and Adolescents: Potential Mediating Role of Health-Related Quality of Life. Children 2025, 12, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Rodriguez-Ayllon, M.; Cadenas-Sánchez, C.; Estévez-López, F.; Muñoz, N.E.; Mora-Gonzalez, J.; Migueles, J.H.; Molina-García, P.; Henriksson, H.; Mena-Molina, A.; Martínez-Vizcaíno, V. Role of physical activity and sedentary behavior in the mental health of preschoolers, children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2019, 49, 1383–1410. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Gnazzo, M.; Baldini, V.; Carotenuto, M.; Pisanò, G.; Messina, G.; Moscatelli, F.; Ruberto, M. Impact of Absence Seizures on Physical Activity Levels in Children: A Cross-Sectional Study. Children 2025, 12, 791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Li, Y.; Xia, X.; Meng, F.; Zhang, C. The association of physical fitness with mental health in children: A serial multiple mediation model. Curr. Psychol. 2022, 41, 7280–7289. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Ganjeh, P.; Meyer, T.; Hagmayer, Y.; Kuhnert, R.; Ravens-Sieberer, U.; von Steinbuechel, N.; Rothenberger, A.; Becker, A. Physical Activity Improves Mental Health in Children and Adolescents Irrespective of the Diagnosis of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)—A Multi-Wave Analysis Using Data from the KiGGS Study. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 2207. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. de Bruijn, A.G.M.; Meijer, A.; Königs, M.; Oosterlaan, J.; Smith, J.; Hartman, E. The mediating role of neurocognitive functions in the relation between physical competencies and academic achievement of primary school children. Psychol. Sport Exerc. 2023, 66, 102390. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  33. Likhitweerawong, N.; Boonchooduang, N.; Khorana, J.; Phinyo, P.; Patumanond, J.; Louthrenoo, O. Executive dysfunction as a possible mediator for the association between excessive screen time and problematic behaviors in preschoolers. PLoS ONE 2024, 19, e0298189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Xiang, H.; Lin, L.; Chen, W.; Li, C.; Liu, X.; Li, J.; Ren, Y.; Guo, V.Y. Associations of excessive screen time and early screen exposure with health-related quality of life and behavioral problems among children attending preschools. BMC Public Health 2022, 22, 2440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Zapata-Lamana, R.; Ibarra-Mora, J.; Henriquez-Beltrán, M.; Sepúlveda-Martin, S.; Martínez-González, L.; Cigarroa, I. Aumento de horas de pantalla se asocia con un bajo rendimiento escolar. Andes Pediatrica 2021, 92, 565–575. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Caamaño-Navarrete, F.; Del-Cuerpo, I.; Arriagada-Hernández, C.; Cresp-Barria, M.; Hernández-Mosqueira, C.; Contreras-Díaz, G.; Valdés-Badilla, P.; Jerez-Mayorga, D.; Delgado-Floody, P. Association between food habits with mental health and executive function in chilean children and adolescents. Children 2025, 12, 268. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Yang, L.; Corpeleijn, E.; Hartman, E. Daily physical activity, sports participation, and executive function in children. JAMA Netw. Open 2024, 7, e2449879. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Liang, X.; Li, R.; Wong, S.H.; Sum, R.K.; Wang, P.; Yang, B.; Sit, C.H. Physical activity and executive function in children with ADHD: The mediating role of sleep. Front. Pediatr. 2022, 9, 775589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  39. Veraksa, A.; Tvardovskaya, A.; Gavrilova, M.; Yakupova, V.; Musálek, M. Associations between executive functions and physical fitness in preschool children. Front. Psychol. 2021, 12, 674746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Zhao, G.; Sun, K.; Fu, J.; Li, Z.; Liu, D.; Tian, X.; Yang, J.; Zhang, Q. Impact of physical activity on executive functions: A moderated mediation model. Front. Psychol. 2024, 14, 1226667. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Benzing, V.; Chang, Y.-K.; Schmidt, M. Acute physical activity enhances executive functions in children with ADHD. Sci. Rep. 2018, 8, 12382. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Howarth, A.; Cooke, S.J.; Nguyen, V.M.; Hunt, L.M. Non-probabilistic surveys and sampling in the human dimensions of fisheries. Rev. Fish Biol. Fish. 2024, 34, 597–622. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Majem, L.S.; Barba, L.R.; Bartrina, J.A.; Rodrigo, C.P.; Santana, P.S.; Quintana, L.P. Obesidad infantil y juvenil en España. Resultados del Estudio enKid (1998–2000). Med. Clin. 2003, 121, 725–732. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  44. Caamaño-Navarrete, F.; Latorre-Román, P.Á.; Párraga-Montilla, J.; Jerez-Mayorga, D.; Delgado-Floody, P. Selective attention and concentration are related to lifestyle in chilean schoolchildren. Children 2021, 8, 856. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Carrillo, H.A.; Ramírez-Vélez, R. Adherencia a la dieta mediterránea en una población escolar colombiana: Evaluación de las propiedades psicométricas del cuestionario KIDMED. Nutr. Hosp. 2020, 37, 73–79. [Google Scholar]
  46. Tapia, J.L.; Rocabado, F.; Duñabeitia, J.A. Cognitive estimation of speed, movement and time across the lifespan. J. Integr. Neurosci. 2022, 21, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Reina-Reina, C.; Conesa, P.J.; Duñabeitia, J.A. Impact of a cognitive stimulation program on the reading comprehension of children in primary education. Front. Psychol. 2023, 13, 985790. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Caamaño-Navarrete, F.; Arriagada-Hernández, C.; Fuentes-Vilugrón, G.; Jara-Tomckowiack, L.; Levin-Catrilao, A.; del Val Martín, P.; Muñoz-Troncoso, F.; Delgado-Floody, P. Healthy Lifestyle Related to Executive Functions in Chilean University Students: A Pilot Study. Healthcare 2024, 12, 1022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Jowik-Krzemińska, K.; Dylewska, D.; Pawlińska-Maćkowiak, A.; Słopień, A.; Tyszkiewicz-Nwafor, M. Cognitive Functions in Adolescent Girls with Anorexia Nervosa during Nutritional Rehabilitation. Nutrients 2024, 16, 3435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Chen, I.-H.; Chen, C.-Y.; Liao, X.-l.; Chen, X.-m.; Zheng, X.; Tsai, Y.-C.; Lin, C.-Y.; Griffiths, M.D.; Pakpour, A.H. Psychometric properties of the Depression, Anxiety, and Stress Scale (DASS-21) among different Chinese populations: A cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis. Acta Psychol. 2023, 240, 104042. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Mella, F.R.; Vinet, E.V.; Muñoz, A.M.A. Escalas de depresión, ansiedad y estrés (DASS-21): Adaptación y propiedades psicométricas en estudiantes secundarios de Temuco. Rev. Argent. Clín. Psicol. 2014, 23, 179–190. [Google Scholar]
  52. Román, F.; Santibáñez, P.; Vinet, E.V. Uso de las Escalas de Depresión Ansiedad Estrés (DASS-21) como instrumento de tamizaje en jóvenes con problemas clínicos. Acta Investig. Psicol. 2016, 6, 2325–2336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Vaughan, R.S.; Edwards, E.J.; MacIntyre, T.E. Mental health measurement in a post Covid-19 world: Psychometric properties and invariance of the DASS-21 in athletes and non-athletes. Front. Psychol. 2020, 11, 590559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Morales, G.; Balboa-Castillo, T.; Fernández-Rodríguez, R.; Garrido-Miguel, M.; Guidoni, C.M.; Sirtoli, R.; Mesas, A.E.; Rodrigues, R. Adherence to the Mediterranean diet and depression, anxiety, and stress symptoms in Chilean university students: A cross-sectional study. Cad. Saúde Pública 2023, 39, e00206722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Antúnez, Z.; Vinet, E.V. Escalas de depresión, ansiedad y estrés (DASS-21): Validación de la versión abreviada en estudiantes universitarios chilenos. Ter. Psicológica 2012, 30, 49–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Lagos San Martín, N.; Ossa Cornejo, C.; Palma Luengo, M.; Arriagada Allaire, C. Autopercepción de desarrollo emocional de los estudiantes secundarios de la región de Ñuble, Chile. Rev. Estud. Exp. Educ. 2020, 19, 17–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Contreras-Mendoza, I.; Olivas-Ugarte, L.O.; De La Cruz-Valdiviano, C. Escalas abreviadas de Depresión, Ansiedad y Estrés (DASS-21): Validez, fiabilidad y equidad en adolescentes peruanos. Rev. Psicol. Clin. Ninos Adolesc. 2021, 8, 24–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Preacher, K.; Hayes, A. SPSS and SAS procedures for estimating indirect effects in simple mediation models. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 2004, 36, 717–731. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Halse, M.; Steinsbekk, S.; Hammar, Å.; Wichstrøm, L. Longitudinal relations between impaired executive function and symptoms of psychiatric disorders in childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2022, 63, 1574–1582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Schumacher, A.; Campisi, S.C.; Khalfan, A.F.; Merriman, K.; Williams, T.S.; Korczak, D.J. Cognitive functioning in children and adolescents with depression: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur. Neuropsychopharmacol. 2024, 79, 49–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Hatch, S.L.; Jones, P.B.; Kuh, D.; Hardy, R.; Wadsworth, M.E.J.; Richards, M. Childhood cognitive ability and adult mental health in the British 1946 birth cohort. Soc. Sci. Med. 2007, 64, 2285–2296. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Xian, G.; Chai, Y.; Gong, Y.; He, W.; Ma, C.; Zhang, X.; Zhang, J.; Ma, Y. The relationship between healthy lifestyles and cognitive function in Chinese older adults: The mediating effect of depressive symptoms. BMC Geriatr. 2024, 24, 299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Lavrijsen, J.; Verschueren, K. High Cognitive Ability and Mental Health: Findings from a Large Community Sample of Adolescents. J. Intell. 2023, 11, 38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  64. McCormick, C.R. Lifestyle factors and their impact on the networks of attention. Appl. Cogn. Psychol. 2022, 36, 135–153. [Google Scholar]
  65. Vohr, B.R.; McGowan, E.C.; Bann, C.; Das, A.; Higgins, R.; Hintz, S.; Ambalavanan, N.; Carlo, W.A.; Collins, M.V.; Cosby, S.S. Association of high screen-time use with school-age cognitive, executive function, and behavior outcomes in extremely preterm children. JAMA Pediatr. 2021, 175, 1025–1034. [Google Scholar]
  66. Lakicevic, N.; Manojlovic, M.; Chichinina, E.; Drid, P.; Zinchenko, Y. Screen time exposure and executive functions in preschool children. Sci. Rep. 2025, 15, 1839. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  67. Zavala-Crichton, J.P.; Esteban-Cornejo, I.; Solis-Urra, P.; Mora-Gonzalez, J.; Cadenas-Sanchez, C.; Rodriguez-Ayllon, M.; Migueles, J.H.; Molina-Garcia, P.; Verdejo-Roman, J.; Kramer, A.F. Association of sedentary behavior with brain structure and intelligence in children with overweight or obesity: The ActiveBrains project. J. Clin. Med. 2020, 9, 1101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Cadenas-Sanchez, C.; Migueles, J.H.; Verdejo-Román, J.; Erickson, K.I.; Esteban-Cornejo, I.; Catena, A.; Ortega, F.B. Physical activity, sedentary time, and fitness in relation to brain shapes in children with overweight/obesity: Links to intelligence. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports 2023, 33, 319–330. [Google Scholar]
  69. Chen, Y.-Y.; Yim, H.; Lee, T.-H. Negative impact of daily screen use on inhibitory control network in preadolescence: A two-year follow-up study. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2023, 60, 101218. [Google Scholar]
  70. Jirout, J.; LoCasale-Crouch, J.; Turnbull, K.; Gu, Y.; Cubides, M.; Garzione, S.; Evans, T.M.; Weltman, A.L.; Kranz, S. How lifestyle factors affect cognitive and executive function and the ability to learn in children. Nutrients 2019, 11, 1953. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Tee, J.Y.H.; Gan, W.Y.; Tan, K.-A.; Chin, Y.S. Obesity and unhealthy lifestyle associated with poor executive function among Malaysian adolescents. PLoS ONE 2018, 13, e0195934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  72. Lipnicki, D.M.; Gunga, H.-C. Physical inactivity and cognitive functioning: Results from bed rest studies. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol. 2009, 105, 27–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Daniele, A.; Lucas, S.J.; Rendeiro, C. Detrimental effects of physical inactivity on peripheral and brain vasculature in humans: Insights into mechanisms, long-term health consequences and protective strategies. Front. Physiol. 2022, 13, 998380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. McMath, A.L.; Iwinski, S.; Shen, S.; Bost, K.F.; Donovan, S.M.; Khan, N.A. Adherence to screen time and physical activity guidelines is associated with executive function in US toddlers participating in the STRONG Kids 2 birth cohort study. J. Pediatr. 2023, 252, 22–30.e6. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  75. Zeng, X.; Cai, L.; Wong, S.H.-S.; Lai, L.; Lv, Y.; Tan, W.; Jing, J.; Chen, Y. Association of sedentary time and physical activity with executive function among children. Acad. Pediatr. 2021, 21, 63–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Álvarez-Bueno, C.; Pesce, C.; Cavero-Redondo, I.; Sánchez-López, M.; Martínez-Hortelano, J.A.; Martínez-Vizcaíno, V. The Effect of Physical Activity Interventions on Children’s Cognition and Metacognition: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Am. Acad. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2017, 56, 729–738. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Reyes-Amigo, T.; Bezerra, A.; Gomez-Mazorra, M.; Boppre, G.; Martins, C.; Carrasco-Beltran, H.; Cordero-Roldan, E.; Mota, J. Effects of high-intensity interval training on executive functions in children and adolescents: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. Phys. Act. Rev. 2022, 10, 77–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  78. Dadkhah, M.; Saadat, M.; Ghorbanpour, A.M.; Moradikor, N. Experimental and clinical evidence of physical exercise on BDNF and cognitive function: A comprehensive review from molecular basis to therapy. Brain Behav. Immun. Integr. 2023, 3, 100017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  79. Nay, K.; Smiles, W.J.; Kaiser, J.; McAloon, L.M.; Loh, K.; Galic, S.; Oakhill, J.S.; Gundlach, A.L.; Scott, J.W. Molecular mechanisms underlying the beneficial effects of exercise on brain function and neurological disorders. Int. J. Mol. Sci. 2021, 22, 4052. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  80. Ribeiro, F.; Cavaglia, R.; Rato, J.R. Sex differences in response inhibition in young children. Cogn. Dev. 2021, 58, 101047. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  81. Gaillard, A.; Fehring, D.J.; Rossell, S.L. Sex differences in executive control: A systematic review of functional neuroimaging studies. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2021, 53, 2592–2611. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Sadeghi, S.; Shalani, B.; Nejati, V. Sex and age-related differences in inhibitory control in typically developing children. Early Child Dev. Care 2022, 192, 292–301. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Hill, A.C.; Laird, A.R.; Robinson, J.L. Gender differences in working memory networks: A BrainMap meta-analysis. Biol. Psychol. 2014, 102, 18–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  84. Gaillard, A.; Fehring, D.J.; Rossell, S.L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of behavioural sex differences in executive control. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2021, 53, 519–542. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  85. Yoon, Y.; Eisenstadt, M.; Lereya, S.T.; Deighton, J. Gender difference in the change of adolescents’ mental health and subjective wellbeing trajectories. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry 2023, 32, 1569–1578. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  86. Iannattone, S.; Mignemi, G.; Pivetta, E.; Gatta, M.; Sica, C.; Cardi, V.; Canale, N.; Spoto, A.; Bottesi, G. Are anxiety, depression, and stress distinguishable in Italian adolescents? an examination through the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales-21. PLoS ONE 2024, 19, e0299229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Boyd, A.; Van de Velde, S.; Vilagut, G.; De Graaf, R.; Florescu, S.; Alonso, J.; Kovess-Masfety, V.; Investigators, E.-W. Gender differences in mental disorders and suicidality in Europe: Results from a large cross-sectional population-based study. J. Affect. Disord. 2015, 173, 245–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Gómez, G.; Rivas, M.; Giaconi, V.; Martínez, C.; Burrone, M.S. Understanding the influence of children’s mental health, cognitive development, and environmental factors on learning outcomes in Chile. Humanit. Soc. Sci. Commun. 2024, 11, 1419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Dale, H.; Brassington, L.; King, K. The impact of healthy lifestyle interventions on mental health and wellbeing: A systematic review. Ment. Health Rev. J. 2014, 19, 1–26. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
Figure 1. The mediation analysis for the total sample. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.001, *** p < 0.0001.
Figure 1. The mediation analysis for the total sample. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.001, *** p < 0.0001.
Children 12 01402 g001
Table 1. Characteristics of the study sample and comparison by sex.
Table 1. Characteristics of the study sample and comparison by sex.
Boys (n = 306)Girls (n = 319)Total (n = 625)p-ValueF-Value
Age (y)13.20 ± 1.7513.23 ± 1.9113.22 ± 1.830.8590.03
Screen time (h/day)3.40 ± 1.623.041 ± 1.443.22 ± 1.540.3008.72
Physical activity (h/week)2.12 ± 1.171.73 ± 1.101.92 ± 1.15p < 0.00117.88
Lifestyle3.80 ± 1.903.75 ± 1.813.78 ± 1.850.7360.11
Executive functions
Attention (score)435.59 ± 148.64405.42 ± 155.72419.80 ± 153.000.0175.68
Cognitive flexibility (score)385.48 ± 245.60330.18 ± 240.03356.59 ± 244.070.0067.51
Inhibition284.35 ± 222.88301.06 ± 243.98293.08 ± 234.090.3910.74
Working Memory(score)208.48 ± 211.79195.66 ± 195.46201.78 ± 203.340.4490.57
Mental health
Anxiety6.32 ± 4.999.37 ± 5.817.88 ± 5.63p < 0.00149.57
Depression6.70 ± 4.929.27 ± 5.778.01 ± 5.52p < 0.00135.94
Stress8.40 ± 4.8610.80 ± 5.289.62 ± 5.21p < 0.00134.90
The values are presented as mean ± SD; a p-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Table 2. Frequency of mental health problems according to sex.
Table 2. Frequency of mental health problems according to sex.
Boys Girls Total
n%n%n%
AnxietyAbsence11336.9%6319.7%17628.2%p < 0.001
Mild258.2%123.8%375.9%
Moderate5116.7%5617.6%10717.1%
Severe3712.1%3511.0%7211.5%
Extremely severe8026.1%15348.0%23337.3%
Total306100.0%319100.0%625100.0%
DepressionAbsence12039.2%7623.8%19631.4%p < 0.001
Mild5016.3%3310.3%8313.3%
Moderate6420.9%8025.1%14423.0%
Severe3812.4%5216.3%9014.4%
Extremely severe3411.1%7824.5%11217.9%
Total306100.0%319100.0%625100.0%
StressAbsence13845.1%9128.5%22936.6%p < 0.001
Mild4514.7%319.7%7612.2%
Moderate5417.6%6319.7%11718.7%
Severe5417.6%8426.3%13822.1%
Extremely severe154.9%5015.7%6510.4%
Total306100.0%319100.0%625100.0%
LifestyleBad lifestyle19365.2%20866.7%40166.0%p = 0.434
Regular lifestyle7826.4%8627.6%16427.0%
Good lifestyle258.4%185.8%437.1%
Total296100.0%312100.0%608100.0%
Data are presented as n and proportions (%). A p-value of less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Table 3. Association between negative lifestyle with executive functions.
Table 3. Association between negative lifestyle with executive functions.
β95% CIBetaSEp-Value
Model Attention
Negative Lifestyle1−37.45−61.12−13.78−0.1312.050.002
2−37.74−61.29−14.19−0.1311.990.002
3−44.23−67.93−20.54−0.1512.06p < 0.001
Cognitive Flexibility
Negative Lifestyle1−72.23−109.72−34.75−0.1619.08p < 0.001
2−72.79−110.05−35.54−0.1618.97p < 0.001
3−85.91−123.03−48.78−0.1918.90p < 0.001
Inhibition
Negative Lifestyle1−60.32−96.18−24.45−0.1418.260.001
2−60.16−96.04−24.28−0.1418.270.001
3−65.77−102.26−29.29−0.1518.57p < 0.001
Working Memory
Negative Lifestyle1−61.98−93.19−30.77−0.1615.89p < 0.001
2−62.15−93.36−30.94−0.1615.89p < 0.001
3−75.73−106.61−44.85−0.2015.72p < 0.001
Model non-adjusted (Model 1); model adjusted by age (Model 2); and model adjusted by age and sex (Model 3). p-value < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Caamaño-Navarrete, F.; Arriagada-Hernández, C.; Lagos-Hernández, R.; Fuentes-Vilugrón, G.; Jara-Tomckowiack, L.; Sandoval-Obando, E.; Contreras-Díaz, G.; Jerez-Mayorga, D.; Hernández-Mosqueira, C.; Delgado-Floody, P. The Potential Mediating Role of Good Mental Health on the Relationship Between Low Physical Activity and High Screen Time with Executive Functions in Chilean Children and Adolescents. Children 2025, 12, 1402. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12101402

AMA Style

Caamaño-Navarrete F, Arriagada-Hernández C, Lagos-Hernández R, Fuentes-Vilugrón G, Jara-Tomckowiack L, Sandoval-Obando E, Contreras-Díaz G, Jerez-Mayorga D, Hernández-Mosqueira C, Delgado-Floody P. The Potential Mediating Role of Good Mental Health on the Relationship Between Low Physical Activity and High Screen Time with Executive Functions in Chilean Children and Adolescents. Children. 2025; 12(10):1402. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12101402

Chicago/Turabian Style

Caamaño-Navarrete, Felipe, Carlos Arriagada-Hernández, Roberto Lagos-Hernández, Gerardo Fuentes-Vilugrón, Lorena Jara-Tomckowiack, Eduardo Sandoval-Obando, Guido Contreras-Díaz, Daniel Jerez-Mayorga, Claudio Hernández-Mosqueira, and Pedro Delgado-Floody. 2025. "The Potential Mediating Role of Good Mental Health on the Relationship Between Low Physical Activity and High Screen Time with Executive Functions in Chilean Children and Adolescents" Children 12, no. 10: 1402. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12101402

APA Style

Caamaño-Navarrete, F., Arriagada-Hernández, C., Lagos-Hernández, R., Fuentes-Vilugrón, G., Jara-Tomckowiack, L., Sandoval-Obando, E., Contreras-Díaz, G., Jerez-Mayorga, D., Hernández-Mosqueira, C., & Delgado-Floody, P. (2025). The Potential Mediating Role of Good Mental Health on the Relationship Between Low Physical Activity and High Screen Time with Executive Functions in Chilean Children and Adolescents. Children, 12(10), 1402. https://doi.org/10.3390/children12101402

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop