Deaf Children as ‘English Learners’: The Psycholinguistic Turn in Deaf Education
Abstract
:1. Introduction
2. Why Deaf Children Are Compared to ‘English Learners’
2.1. Deaf Children Whose Home Language Is American Sign Language
2.2. Deaf Children Whose Home Language Is Neither English Nor ASL
2.3. Deaf Children Whose Home Language Is Spoken English
3. The Psycholinguistic Turn in Deaf Education
3.1. The Critical Period Hypothesis
Late L1 learners exhibit initial rapid learning of lexical items in different grammatical categories and subsequent word combinations that are reminiscent of the acquisition of young child language learners, but at a faster pace. At the same time, however, accumulating evidence suggests that two major characteristics of language acquisition begun for the first time at age 12 or older are, first, rapid initial language acquisition, and second, a subsequent protracted period of limited language development, despite rich linguistic environments and language instruction. The language development of adolescent late L1 learners does not progress to complex morphosyntactic structures, but remains limited to simple structures.[78] (p. 896)
3.2. The Bilingual Paradox
From a 21st century vantage point, it is difficult to recall that, at least through the 1950s, knowledge of a first language was believed to be an obstacle to the acquisition of a second language, rather than a help. Habitual first-language language patterns interfered, rather than helped, in the formation of new habits. The picture, rather, is that post-childhood language acquisition cannot proceed as in childhood, but that an existing L1 can provide a kind of scaffold on which to build L2 knowledge. In Chomsky’s metaphor, adults approach a foreign language as “something strange.” Building on this metaphor, we might say that learning one language makes learning the second less strange.[67] (p. 914)
3.3. Cognitive Effects of Language Deprivation
3.4. Social-Emotional Effects of Language Deprivation
3.5. Ought Every Deaf Child Learn to Sign?
(1) Medical education must be updated and include linguistic considerations. Medical professionals should be trained in recent research about language acquisition, particularly with respect to the issues of linguistic deprivation for those children at risk, primarily deaf children. Medical schools, nursing schools, and schools of public health should include this information in their curriculum.(2) Delivery of medical care to deaf children should be coordinated across the relevant health professionals, including audiologists, psychologists, surgeons, and rehabilitation teams. These teams should stay in constant contact with and respond to input from parents, sign language teachers, and classroom teachers. This way, the risk of linguistic deprivation can be caught early and responded to appropriately.(3) Advice from medical professionals must be accurate and adequate. Parents of deaf newborns and newly deafened small children should be advised to teach their child sign language, regardless of whether the child also uses hearing aids or a CI. This means the entire family should learn sign language; and since the biological health of the language mechanism is at stake, this is properly a medical matter, so it is the medical profession’s responsibility to tell the parents this.[59] (pp. 36–37)
4. Literacy Acquisition in a Bimodal Bi/Multilingual Context
4.1. Sign Language as a Foundation for Literacy
4.2. The Value of Shared Attention
4.3. The Critical Role of Linguistic Segmentation
The crucial link for early reading success is not between segmental sounds and print. Instead the human brain’s capacity to segment, categorize, and discern linguistic patterning makes possible the capacity to segment all languages. This biological process includes the segmentation of languages on the hands in signed languages. Exposure to natural sign language in early life equally affords the child’s discovery of silent segmental units in visual sign phonology (VSP) that can also facilitate segmental decoding of print.[143] (p. 1)
4.4. Qualitative Similarity or Qualitative Dissimilarity?
4.5. Bimodal Bi/Multilingual Reading Instruction
4.6. Bimodal Bi/Multilingual Writing Instruction
5. Questions for Future Research
5.1. What Is the Nature of the ‘Bridge’ from Sign to Print?
5.2. Is the Bimodal Bilingual Approach Effective?
5.3. Who Are Deaf Bimodal Bi/Multicultural Learners?
5.4. How Can Hearing Parents Become Proficient Signers?
For families who choose not to risk language deprivation by exposing their child to a sign language, it is critical to evaluate whether the professionals working with the family are equipped to offer support in sign language acquisition. Do they have native or near-native proficiency in the signed language used in the region? Do they have training and expertise to support sign language acquisition (i.e., deep understanding of the linguistic structures of the relevant signed language and the methods for evaluating and promoting acquisition of these structures)? If not, are they able to refer the family to people who have such expertise? If they are hearing, do they know and seek guidance from DHH adults who have lived experience of being DHH?(pp. 17–18)
5.5. How Can Deaf Ontologies and Epistemologies Improve Deaf Education?
Aspects of the Deaf episteme, not caused by deafness but by Deafhood, have a positive impact on how deaf individuals learn, resist audism, stay healthy, and navigate the world.[196] (p. 486)
The vast knowledge generated by the collective experience of deaf people, all of whom have varying degrees of hearing and speaking capabilities, has the potential to provide the truth needed to achieve improved educational success for all deaf children.[1] (p. 476)
Studies of the everyday interactions within Deaf families can provide insight into the learning potential of deaf children when they have the same advantages at birth as hearing, speaking children, that is, when caregivers have the linguistic and cultural knowledge to fulfill the visual language needs of their deaf infant from the moment they are born.[136] (p. 447)
“A repertoire of teacher knowledge, skills, and tools that primarily originate in the Deaf community can infuse and enrich educational practice with the outcome of life-long learning, equity and social justice”.[125] (p. 8)
6. Concluding Thoughts
Efforts on behalf of the US government and local educational agencies to improve outcomes for ELs are well documented. However, one type of student population has over the years not been included in policy discussions and therefore remains unaffected by the reforms and changes in the educational infrastructure that have otherwise—more or less positively—affected lives of school-aged ELs. These are users of American Sign Language (ASL) who are learning English. Literature has suggested that over 500,000 individuals use ASL as their L1 in the US, yet no data are currently available at the state or federal level that identifies children who fit such a profile as ELs.(p. 100)
Author Contributions
Funding
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Holcomb, T.K. Deaf Epistemology: The Deaf Way of Knowing. Am. Ann. Deaf 2010, 154, 471–478. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- García, O. Emergent Bilinguals and TESOL: What’s in a Name? Tesol Q. 2009, 43, 322–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gallaudet Research Institute. 2013–14 Regional and National Summary; Gallaudet Research Institute: Washington, DC, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Knoors, H.; Marschark, M. Language planning for the 21st century: Revisiting bilingual language policy for deaf children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2012, 17, 291–305. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Sandler, W.; Lillo-Martin, D. Sign Language and Linguistic Universals; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Benetti, S.; van Ackeren, M.J.; Rabini, G.; Zonca, J.; Foa, V.; Baruffaldi, F.; Rezk, M.; Pavani, F.; Rossion, B.; Collignon, O. Functional selectivity for face processing in the temporal voice area of early deaf individuals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2017, 114, E6437–E6446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Petitto, L.-A. The Impact of Minimal Language Experience on Children During Sensitive Periods of Brain and Early Language Development: Myths Debunked and New Policy Implications. In The Science of Learning; Kuhl, P., Ed.; OECD: Paris, France, in press.
- García, O. Education, Multilingualism and Translanguaging in the 21st Century. In Multilingual Education for Social Justice: Globalising the L.; Mohanty, A., Panda, M., Phillipson, R., Skutnabb-Kangas, T., Eds.; Orient Blackswan: New Delhi, India, 2009; pp. 128–145. [Google Scholar]
- Herbert, M.; Pires, A. Bilingualism and bimodal code-blending among deaf ASL-English bilinguals. Proc. Linguist. Soc. Am. 2017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berent, G.P. Sign Language—Spoken Language Bilingualism: Code Mixing and Mode Mixing by ASL-English Bilinguals. In The Handbook of Bilingualism; Blackwell: Malden, MA, USA, 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Costa, A.; Sebastián-Gallés, N. How does the bilingual experience sculpt the brain? Neuroscience 2014, 15, 336–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bialystok, E.; Craik, F.I.M.; Green, D.W.; Gollan, T.H. Bilingual minds. Psychol. Sci. Public Interest 2009, 10, 89–129. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jasińska, K.K.; Petitto, L.-A. How age of bilingual exposure can change the neural systems for language in the developing brain: A functional near infrared spectroscopy investigation of syntactic processing in monolingual and bilingual children. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2013, 6, 87–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Kushalnagar, P.; Hannay, H.J.; Hernandez, A.E. Bilingualism and Attention: A Study of Balanced and Unbalanced Bilingual Deaf Users of American Sign Language and English. J. Deaf. Stud. Deaf. Educ. 2010, 15, 263–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Klatter-Folmer, J.; van Hout, R.; Kolen, E.; Verhoeven, L. Language Development in Deaf Children’s Interactions With Deaf and Hearing Adults: A Dutch Longitudinal Study. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2005, 11, 238–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pizzo, L. d/Deaf and Hard of Hearing Multilingual Learners: The Development of Communication and Language. Am. Ann. Deaf 2016, 161, 17–32. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gallaudet Research Institute. 1999–2000 Regional and National Summary; Gallaudet Research Institute: Washington, DC, USA, 2001. [Google Scholar]
- Compton, S. American Sign Language as a heritage language. In Handbook of Heritage, Community, and Native American Languages in the United States: Research, Policy, and Educational Practice; Wiley, T.G., Kreeft Peyton, J., Christian, D., Moore, S.C., Liu, N., Eds.; Routledge: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 272–283. [Google Scholar]
- Paul, P.V. d/Deaf and Hard of Hearing Learners: DML, DLL, ELL, EL, ESL, …, or Culturally and Linguistically Diverse. Am. Ann. Deaf 2016, 161, 3–7. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Gerner de Garcia, B.A. Literacy for Latino Deaf and Hard of Hearing English Language Learners: Building the Knowledge Base. In Proceedings of the First Wednesdays Research Seminar Series, Gallaudet University, Washington, DC, USA, 7 April 2004. [Google Scholar]
- Gerner de Garcia, B.A. ESL Applications for Hispanic Deaf Students. Biling. Res. J. 1995, 19, 453–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grant, J. Hearing-Impaired Children from Mexican-American Homes. Volta Rev. 1993, 95, 131–135. [Google Scholar]
- Sacks, C.; Shay, S.; Repplinger, L.; Leffel, K.R.; Sapolich, S.G.; Suskind, E.; Tannenbaum, S.; Suskind, D. Pilot testing of a parent-directed intervention (Project ASPIRE) for underserved children who are deaf or hard of hearing. Child Lang. Teach. Ther. 2014, 30, 91–102. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Q.; Andrews, J.F.; Liu, H.T.; Liu, C.J. Case Studies of Multilingual/Multicultural Asian Deaf Adults: Strategies for Success. Am. Ann. Deaf 2016, 161, 67–88. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Baker, S.; Scott, J.A. Sociocutural and Academic Considerations for School-Age d/Deaf and Hard of Hearing Multlingual Learners: A Case Study of a Deaf Latina. Am. Ann. Deaf 2016, 161, 43–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Cannon, J.E.; Guardino, C.; Gallimore, E. A New Kind of Heterogeneity: What We Can Learn from d/Deaf and Hard of Hearing Multilingual Learners. Am. Ann. Deaf 2016, 161, 8–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lucas, T.; Villegas, A.M. Preparing Linguistically Responsive Teachers: Laying the Foundation in Preservice Teacher Education. Theory Pract. 2013, 52, 98–109. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cannon, J.E.; Guardino, C. Literacy Strategies for Deaf/Hard-of-Hearing English Language Learners: Where Do We Begin? Deaf. Educ. Int. 2012, 14, 78–99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cannon, J.E.; Fredrick, L.D.; Easterbrooks, S.R. Vocabulary instruction through books read in American sign language for English-language learners with hearing loss. Commun. Disord. Q. 2010, 31, 98–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Guardino, C.; Cannon, J.E.; Eberst, K. Building the Evidence-Base of Effective Reading Strategies to Use With Deaf English-Language Learners. Commun. Disord. Q. 2014, 35, 59–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bunta, F.; Douglas, M.; Dickson, H.; Cantu, A.; Wickesberg, J.; Gifford, R.H. Dual language versus English-only support for bilingual children with hearing loss who use cochlear implants and hearing aids. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2016, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bunta, F.; Douglas, M. The Effects of Dual-Language Support on the Language Skills of Bilingual Children With Hearing Loss Who Use Listening Devices Relative to Their Monolingual Peers. Lang. Speech. Hear. Serv. Sch. 2013, 44, 281–290. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hall, M.L.; Hall, W.C.; Caselli, N.K. Deaf children need language, not (just) speech. First Lang. 2019. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- American Cochlear Implant Alliance. Position Paper: Supporting Parent Choice for Children Who Are Deaf and Hard of Hearing; American Cochlear Implant Alliance: McLEAN, VA, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Hart, B.; Risley, T.R. The Early Catastrophe: The 30 Million Word Gap by Age 3. Am. Educ. 1995, 27, 4–9. [Google Scholar]
- John, L.K.; Loewenstein, G.; Prelec, D. Beyond the 30-Million-Word Gap: Children’s Conversational Exposure Is Associated With Language-Related Brain Function. Psychol. Sci. 2018, 29, 700–710. [Google Scholar]
- Hoff-Ginsberg, E. Function and structure in maternal speech: Their relation to the child’s development of syntax. Dev. Psychol. 1986, 22, 155–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hoff, E.; Naigles, L. How children use input to acquire a lexicon. Child Dev. 2002, 73, 418–433. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Adamson, L.B.; Bakeman, R.; Owen, M.T.; Golinkoff, R.M.; Pace, A.; Yust, P.K.S.; Suma, K. The Contribution of Early Communication Quality to Low-Income Children’s Language Success. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 26, 1071–1083. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rowe, M. A Longitudinal Investigation of the Role of Quantity and Quality of Child-Directed Speech in Vocabulary Development. Child Dev. 2012, 83, 1762–1774. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Vohr, B.R.; Topol, D.; Watson, V.; St Pierre, L.; Tucker, R. The importance of language in the home for school-age children with permanent hearing loss. Acta Paediatr. 2014, 103, 62–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- VanDam, M.; Ambrose, S.E.; Moeller, M.P. Quantity of Parental Language in the Home Environments of Hard-of-Hearing 2-Year-Olds. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2012, 17, 402–420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ambrose, S.E.; VanDam, M.; Moeller, M.P. Linguistic Input, Electronic Media, and Communication Outcomes of Toddlers with Hearing Loss. Ear Hear. 2014, 35, 139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Aragon, M.; Yoshinaga-Itano, C. Using Language ENvironment Analysis to Improve Outcomes for Children Who Are Deaf or Hard of Hearing. Semin. Speech Lang. 2012, 33, 340–353. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Rufsvold, R.; Wang, Y.; Hartman, M.C.; Arora, S.B.; Smolen, E.R. The Impact of Language Input on Deaf and Hard of Hearing Preschool Children Who Use Listening and Spoken Language. Am. Ann. Deaf 2018, 163, 35–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ambrose, S.E.; Walker, E.A.; Unflat-Berry, L.M.; Oleson, J.J.; Moeller, M.P. Quantity and Quality of Caregivers’ Linguistic Input to 18-Month and 3-Year-Old Children Who Are Hard of Hearing. Ear Hear. 2015, 36, 48S–59S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Tomblin, J.B.; Oleson, J.; Ambrose, S.E.; Walker, E.A.; Moeller, M.P. Early Literacy Predictors and Second-Grade Outcomes in Children Who Are Hard of Hearing. Child Dev. 2018. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Henner, J.; Caldwell-Harris, C.L.; Novogrodsky, R.; Hoffmeister, R.J. American sign language syntax and analogical reasoning skills are influenced by early acquisition and age of entry to signing schools for the deaf. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Novogrodsky, R.; Henner, J.; Caldwell-Harris, C.L.; Hoffmeister, R.J. The Development of Sensitivity to Grammatical Violations in American Sign Language: Native Versus Nonnative Signers. Lang. Learn. 2017, 67, 791–818. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Farran, L.K.; Lederberg, A.R.; Jackson, L.A. Maternal input and lexical development: The case of deaf pre-schoolers. Int. J. Lang. Commun. Disord. 2009, 44, 145–163. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Connor, C.M.; Craig, H.K.; Raudenbush, S.W.; Heavner, K.; Zwolan, T.A. The Age at Which Young Deaf Children Receive Cochlear Implants and Their Vocabulary and Speech-Production Growth: Is There an Added Value for Early Implantation? Ear Hear. 2006, 27, 628–644. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Niparko, J.K.; Tobey, E.A.; Thal, D.J.; Eisenberg, L.S.; Wang, N.-Y.; Quittner, A.L. Spoken language development in children following cochlear implantation. JAMA 2010, 303, 1498–1506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Fagan, M.K.; Pisoni, D.B. Hearing Experience and Receptive Vocabulary Development in Deaf Children With Cochlear Implants. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2010, 15, 149–161. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Anderson, I.; Weichbold, V.; D’Haese, P.S.; Szuchnik, J.; Quevedo, M.S.; Martin, J.; Dieler, W.S.; Phillips, L. Cochlear implantation in children under the age of two—What do the outcomes show us? Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2004, 68, 425–431. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Valencia, D.M.; Rimell, F.L.; Friedman, B.J.; Oblander, M.R.; Helmbrecht, J. Cochlear implantation in infants less than 12 months of age. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2008, 72, 767–773. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mauldin, L. Don’t look at it as a miracle cure: Contested notions of success and failure in family narratives of pediatric cochlear implantation. Soc. Sci. Med. 2019, 228, 117–125. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayer, C.; Trezek, B.J. Literacy outcomes in deaf students with cochlear implants: Current state of the knowledge. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2018, 23, 1–16. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pisoni, S.; Conway, C.; Kronenberger, W.G.; Horn, D.; Karpicke, J.; Henning, S. Efficacy and Effectiveness of Cochlear Impants in Deaf Children. In Deaf Cognition: Foundations and Outcomes; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2008; pp. 52–101. [Google Scholar]
- Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Rathmann, C. Ensuring language acquisition for deaf children: What linguists can do. Language 2014, 90, e31–e52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Koulidobrova, E.; Kuntze, M.; Dostal, H.M. If you use ASL, should you study ESL? Limitations of a modality-b(i)ased policy. Language 2018, 94, 99–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hall, W.C. What You Don’t Know Can Hurt You: The Risk of Language Deprivation by Impairing Sign Language Development in Deaf Children. Matern. Child Health J. 2017, 21, 961–965. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, R.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Rathmann, C.; Smith, S.R. The right to language. J. Law Med. Ethics 2013, 41, 872–884. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hermann-Shores, P. Enabling Pedagogy and Adragony for 21st-Century Sign Language Users and Learners. Am. Ann. Deaf 2017, 162, 360–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Grosjean, F. The Bilingual’s Language Modes. In One Mind, Two Languages: Bilingual Language Processing; Wiley-Blackwell: Malden, MA, USA, 2001; pp. 37–66. [Google Scholar]
- United Nations Division for Social Policy and Development Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities (CRPD). Available online: https://www.un.org/development/desa/disabilities/convention-on-the-rights-of-persons-with-disabilities/convention-on-the-rights-of-persons-with-disabilities-2.html (accessed on 5 January 2019).
- Lenneberg, E. Biological Foundations of Language; Wiley: New York, NY, USA, 1967. [Google Scholar]
- Bley-Vroman, R. Language as something strange. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 21, 913–914. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayberry, R.I.; Kluender, R. Rethinking the critical period for language: New insights into an old question from American Sign Language. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 21, 886–905. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Ferjan Ramirez, N.; Leonard, M.K.; Davenport, T.S.; Torres, C.; Halgren, E.; Mayberry, R.I. Neural Language Processing in Adolescent First-Language Learners: Longitudinal Case Studies in American Sign Language. Cereb. Cortex 2014, 26, 1015–1026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Mayberry, R.I.; Fischer, S.D. Looking through phonological shape to lexical meaning: The bottleneck of nonnative sign language processing. Mem. Cogn. 1989, 17, 740–754. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayberry, R.I.; Eichen, E.B. The long-lasting advantage of learning sign language in childhood: Another look at the critical period for language acquisition. J. Mem. Lang. 1991, 30, 486–512. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayberry, R.I. First-Language Acquisition After Childhood Differs From Second-Language Acquisition. J. Speechlang. Hear. Res. 1993, 36, 1258–1270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayberry, R.I.; Lock, E. Age constraints on first versus second language acquisition: Evidence for linguistic plasticity and epigenesis. Brain Lang. 2003, 87, 369–384. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Boudreault, P.; Mayberry, R.I. Grammatical processing in American Sign Language: Age of first-language acquisition effects in relation to syntactic structure. Lang. Cogn. Process. 2006, 21, 608–635. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Berk, S.; Lillo-Martin, D. The two-word stage: Motivated by linguistic or cognitive constraints? Cogn. Psychol. 2012, 65, 118–140. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Morford, J.P.; Carlson, M.L. Sign Perception and Recognition in Non-Native Signers of ASL. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2012, 7, 149–168. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Woll, B. The consequences of very late exposure to BSL as an L1. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 21, 936–937. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Curtiss, S. Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern—Day “Wild Child”; Academic Press: New York, NY, USA, 1977. [Google Scholar]
- Ferjan Ramirez, N.; Leonard, M.K.; Torres, C.; Hatrak, M.; Halgren, E.; Mayberry, R.I. Neural Language Processing in Adolescent First-Language Learners. Cereb. Cortex 2013, 24, 2772–2783. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mayberry, R.I.; Davenport, T.S.; Roth, A.; Halgren, E. Neurolinguistic Processing When the Brain Matures Without Language. Cortex 2018, 99, 390–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mayberry, R.I.; Chen, J.-K.; Witcher, P.; Klein, D. Age of acquisition effects on the functional organization of language in the adult brain. Brain Lang. 2011, 119, 16–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Petitto, L.-A.; Katerelos, M.; Levy, B.G.; Gauna, K.; Tetreault, K.; Ferraro, V. Bilingual signed and spoken language acquisition from birth: Implications for the mechanisms underlying early bilingual language acquisition. J. Child Lang. 2001, 28, 453–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Geers, A.E.; Mitchell, C.M.; Warner-Czyz, A.; Wang, N.-Y.; Eisenberg, L.S. Early Sign Language Exposure and Cochlear Implantation Benefits. Pediatrics 2017, 140, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Yoshinaga-Itano, C.; Baca, R.L.; Sedey, A.L. Describing the Trajectory of Language Development in the Presence of Severe-to-Profound Hearing Loss. Otol. Neurotol. 2010, 31, 1268–1274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Tang, G.; Lam, S.; Yiu, K.C. Language Development of Deaf Children in a Sign Bilingual and Co-enrollment Environment. In Bilingualism and Bilingual Deaf Education; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2014; pp. 313–341. [Google Scholar]
- Rönnberg, J. Working Memory, Neuroscience, and Language: Evidence from Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Individuals. In The Oxford Handbook of Deaf Studies, Language, and Education, 2nd ed.; Oxford University Press: London, UK, 2011; Volume 1. [Google Scholar]
- Marshall, C.; Jones, A.C.; Denmark, T.; Mason, K.; Atkinson, J.; Botting, N.; Morgan, G. Deaf children’s non-verbal working memory is impacted by their language experience. Front. Psychol. 2015, 6, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hall, M.L.; Eigsti, I.M.; Bortfeld, H.; Lillo-Martin, D. Auditory deprivation does not impair executive function, but language deprivation might: Evidence from a parent-report measure in deaf native signing children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2016, 22, 9–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Figueras, B.; Edwards, L.; Langdon, D. Executive function and language in deaf children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2008, 13, 362–377. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Botting, N.; Jones, A.C.; Marshall, C.; Denmark, T.; Atkinson, J.; Morgan, G. Nonverbal Executive Function is Mediated by Language: A Study of Deaf and Hearing Children. Child Dev. 2017, 88, 1689–1700. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Jones, A.C.; Gutierrez, R.; Ludlow, A.K. Confronting the language barrier: Theory of mind in deaf children. J. Commun. Disord. 2015, 56, 47–58. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Walker, E.A.; Ambrose, S.E.; Oleson, J.; Moeller, M.P. False Belief Development in Children Who Are Hard of Hearing Compared with Peers With Normal Hearing. J. Speechlang. Hear. Res. 2017, 60, 3487–3506. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Premack, D.; Woodruff, G. Does the chimpanzee have a theory of mind? Behav. Brain Sci. 1978, 1, 515–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Courtin, C. The impact of sign language on the cognitive development of deaf children: The case of theories of mind. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2000, 5, 266–276. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Courtin, C.; Melot, A.-M. Metacognitive development of deaf children: Lessons from the appearance-reality and false belief tasks. Dev. Sci. 2005, 8, 16–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schick, B.; De Villiers, P.; Hoffmeister, R.J. Language and Theory of Mind: A Study of Deaf Children. Child Dev. 2007, 78, 376–396. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Courtin, C. A Critical Period for the Acquisition of a Theory of Mind? In Deaf Around the World; Oxford University Press: New York, NY, USA, 2010; pp. 184–193. [Google Scholar]
- Tomasuolo, E.; Valeri, G.; Di Renzo, A.; Pasqualetti, P.; Volterra, V. Deaf children attending different school environments: Sign language abilities and theory of mind. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2013, 18, 12–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Morgan, G.; Kegl, J. Nicaraguan Sign Language and Theory of Mind: The issue of critical periods and abilities. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2006, 47, 811–819. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Remmel, E.; Peters, K. Theory of mind and language in children with cochlear implants. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2009, 14, 218–236. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Allen, T.E.; Letteri, A.; Choi, S.H.; Dang, D. Early Visual Language Exposure and Emergent Literacy in Preschool Deaf Children: Findings from a National Longitudinal Study. Am. Ann. Deaf 2014, 159, 346–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Chapman, M.; Dammeyer, J. The significance of deaf identity for psychological well-being. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2016, 22, 187–194. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Hall, W.C.; Levin, L.L.; Anderson, M.L. Language deprivation syndrome: A possible neurodevelopmental disorder with sociocultural origins. Soc. Psychiatry Psychiatr. Epidemiol. 2017, 52, 761–776. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bauman, H.-D.; Murray, J. Deaf Gain: Raising the Stakes for Human Diversity; Uninersity of Minnesota Press: Minneapolis, MN, USA, 2014. [Google Scholar]
- Humphries, T. Our Time: The Legacy of the Twentieth Century. Sign Lang. Stud. 2014, 15, 57–73. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lane, H. The Mask of Benevolence: Disabling the Deaf Community; Dawn Sign Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1992. [Google Scholar]
- Komesaroff, L.R. Disabling Pedagogy: Power, Politics, and Deaf Education; Gallaudet University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- LEAD-K. LEAD-K—Deaf Focus. Available online: https://www.deaffocus.org/lead-k/ (accessed on 15 February 2019).
- Lillo-Martin, D. Differences and similarities between late first-language and second-language learning. Biling. Lang. Cogn. 2018, 21, 924–925. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Smith, J.; Wolfe, J. Should All Deaf Children Learn Sign Language? Hear. J. 2016, 69, 18–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Spellun, A.; Kushalnagar, P. Sign Language for Deaf Infants: A Key Intervention for a Developmental Emergency. Clin. Pediatr. 2018, 57, 1613–1615. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Mathur, G.; Rathmann, C.; Smith, S.R. Cochlear Implants and the Right to Language: Ethical Considerations, the Ideal Situation, and Practical Measures Toward Reaching the Ideal. Cochlear Implant Research Updates; IntechOpen: London, UK, 2012; pp. 193–212. [Google Scholar]
- Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Pollard, R.; Rathmann, C.; Smith, S.R. What Medical Education can do to Ensure Robust Language Development in Deaf Children. Med. Sci. Educ. 2014, 24, 409–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Napoli, D.J.; Mellon, N.K.; Niparko, J.K.; Rathmann, C.; Mathur, G.; Humphries, T.; Handley, T.; Scambler, S.; Lantos, J.D. Should All Deaf Children Learn Sign Language? Pediatrics 2015, 136, 170–176. [Google Scholar] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Napoli, D.J.; Rathmann, C.; Mathur, G.; Smith, S.R. Support for Parents of Deaf Children: Common Questions and Informed, Evidence-based Answers. Int. J. Pediatr. Otorhinolaryngol. 2019, 118, 134–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Rathmann, C.; Smith, S.R. Language Choices for Deaf Infants. Clin. Pediatr. 2015, 55, 513–517. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Rathmann, C.; Smith, S.R. Avoiding Linguistic Neglect of Deaf Children. Soc. Serv. Rev. 2016, 90, 589–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C. Bilingualism: A Pearl to Overcome Certain Perils of Cochlear Implants. J. Med. Speech Lang. Pathol. 2014, 21, 107. [Google Scholar]
- Humphries, T.; Kushalnagar, P.; Mathur, G.; Napoli, D.J.; Padden, C.; Rathmann, C.; Smith, S. Discourses of prejudice in the professions: The case of sign languages. J. Med. Ethics 2017, 43, 648–652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Piñar, P.; Dussias, P.E.; Morford, J.P. Deaf readers as bilinguals: An examination of deaf readers’ print comprehension in light of current advances in bilingualism and second language processing. Lang. Linguist. Compass 2011, 5, 691–704. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Freel, B.L.; Clark, M.D.; Anderson, M.L.; Gilbert, G.L.; Musyoka, M.M.; Hauser, P.C. Deaf Individuals’ Bilingual Abilities: American Sign Language Proficiency, Reading Skills, and Family Characteristics. Psychology 2011, 2, 18–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chamberlain, C.; Mayberry, R.I. American Sign Language syntactic and narrative comprehension in skilled and less skilled readers: Bilingual and bimodal evidence for the linguistic basis of reading. Appl. Psycholinguist. 2008, 29, 367–388. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Scott, J.A.; Hoffmeister, R.J. American sign language and academic English: Factors influencing the reading of bilingual secondary school deaf and hard of hearing students. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2017, 22, 59–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Dammeyer, J. Literacy Skills among Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students and Students with Cochlear Implants in Bilingual/Bicultural Education. Deaf. Educ. Int. 2014, 16, 108–119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Humphries, T. Schooling in American Sign Language: A paradigm shift from a deficit model to a bilingual model in deaf education. Berkeley Rev. Educ. 2013, 4, 7–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Allen, T.E. ASL Skills, Fingerspelling Ability, Home Communication Context and Early Alphabetic Knowledge of Preschool-Aged Deaf Children. Sign Lang. Stud. 2015, 15, 233–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Padden, C.; Ramsey, C. ASL and reading ability in deaf children. In Language Acquisition by Eye; Lawrence Erlbaum Associates: Mahwah, NJ, USA, 2012; pp. 165–189. [Google Scholar]
- Harris, M.; Beech, J.R. Implicit Phonological Awareness and Early Reading Development in Prelingually Deaf Children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 1998, 3, 205–216. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hermans, D.; Ormel, E.; Knoors, H. On the relation between the signing and reading skills of deaf bilinguals. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 2010, 13, 187–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Strong, M.; Prinz, P.M. A Study of the Relationship Between American Sign Language and English Literacy. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 1997, 2, 37–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- Hrastinski, I.; Wilbur, R.B. Academic achievement of deaf and hard-of-hearing students in an ASL/English bilingual program. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2016, 21, 156–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jasińska, K.K.; Petitto, L.-A. Age of Bilingual Exposure Is Related to the Contribution of Phonological and Semantic Knowledge to Successful Reading Development. Child Dev. 2018, 89, 310–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jasińska, K.K.; Berens, M.S.; Kovelman, I.; Petitto, L.-A. Bilingualism yields language-specific plasticity in left hemisphere’s circuitry for learning to read in young children. Neuropsychologia 2017, 98, 34–45. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Jasińska, K.K.; Petitto, L.-A. Development of neural systems for reading in the monolingual and bilingual brain: New insights from functional near infrared spectroscopy neuroimaging. Dev. Neuropsychol. 2014, 39, 421–439. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Lieberman, A.M.; Hatrak, M.; Mayberry, R.I. Learning to Look for Language: Development of Joint Attention in Young Deaf Children. Lang. Learn. Dev. 2014, 10, 19–35. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bailes, C.N.; Erting, C.J.; Erting, L.C.; Thumann-Prezioso, C. Language and Literacy Acquisition through Parental Mediation in American Sign Language. Sign Lang. Stud. 2009, 9, 417–456. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sparks, R.L.; Humbach, N.; Patton, J.; Gaschow, L. Sub-components of second-language aptitude and second-language proficiency. Mod. Lang. J. 2011, 95, 253–273. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wang, Y.; Silvestri, J.A.; Jahromi, L.B. Selected Factors in Reading Comprehension for Deaf and Hearing Adults: Phonological Skills and Metacognition. Am. Ann. Deaf 2018, 162, 445–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Harris, M.; Terlektsi, E.; Kyle, F.E. Concurrent and longitudinal predictors of reading for deaf and hearing children in primary school. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2017, 22, 233–242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Webb, M.-Y.; Lederberg, A.R.; Branum-Martin, L.; Connor, C.M. Evaluating the structure of early english literacy skills in deaf and hard-of-hearing children. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2015, 20, 343–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayberry, R.I.; del Giudice, A.A.; Lieberman, A.M. Reading Achievement in Relation to Phonological Coding and Awareness in Deaf Readers: A Meta-analysis. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2011, 16, 164–188. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Clark, M.D.; Hauser, P.C.; Miller, P.; Kargin, T.; Rathmann, C.; Guldenoglu, B.; Kubus, O.; Spurgeon, E.; Israel, E. The Importance of Early Sign Language Acquisition for Deaf Readers. Read. Writ. Q. 2016, 32, 127–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Petitto, L.-A.; Langdon, C.; Stone, A.; Andriola, D.; Kartheiser, G.; Cochran, C. Visual sign phonology: Insights into human reading and language from a natural soundless phonology. WIREs Cogn. Sci. 2016, 7, 366–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Morford, J.P.; Wilkinson, E.; Villwock, A.; Piñar, P.; Kroll, J.F. When deaf signers read English: Do written words activate their sign translations? Cognition 2011, 118, 286–292. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
- McQuarrie, L.; Abbott, M. Bilingual Deaf Students’ Phonological Awareness in ASL and Reading Skills in English. Sign Lang. Stud. 2013, 14, 80–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silvestri, J.A.; Wang, Y. A Grounded Theory of Effective Reading by Profoundly Deaf Adults. Am. Ann. Deaf 2018, 162, 419–444. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Kovelman, I.; Ip, K.; Marks, R.; Tardif, T. Morphological Awareness Literacy Task in Chinese Engages Left Temporal Regions: An fMRI study. In Proceedings of the American Educational Research Association, Toronto, ON, Canada, 7 April 2019. [Google Scholar]
- Paul, P.V.; Lee, C. The Qualitative Similarity Hypothesis. Am. Ann. Deaf 2010, 154, 456–462. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Mounty, J.L.; Pucci, C.T.; Harmon, K.C. How deaf American sign language/English bilingual children become proficient readers: An emic perspective. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2014, 19, 333–346. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bagga-Gupta, S. Explorations in bilingual instructional interaction: A sociocultural perspective on literacy. Learn. Instr. 2002, 12, 557–587. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Evans, C.J. Literacy Development in Deaf Students: Case Studies in Bilingual Teaching and Learning. Am. Ann. Deaf 2004, 149, 17–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Howerton-Fox, A. Teacher Language Awareness in a Swedish Bilingual School for the Deaf: Two Portraits of Grammar Knowledge in Practice. Ph.D. Thesis, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA, February 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Andrews, J.F.; Rusher, M. Codeswitching Techniques: Evidence-Based Instructional Practices for the ASL/English Bilingual Classroom. Am. Ann. Deaf 2017, 155, 407–424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nicolarakis, O. Fingerspelling as Route to Phonology. In Proceedings of the American Educational Research Association, New York, NY, USA, 16 April 2018. [Google Scholar]
- Sehyr, Z.S.; Petrich, J.A.F.; Emmorey, K. Fingerspelled and printed words are recoded into a speech-based code in short-term memory. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2017, 22, 72–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stone, A.; Kartheiser, G.; Hauser, P.C.; Petitto, L.-A.; Allen, T.E. Fingerspelling as a Novel Gateway into Reading Fluency in Deaf Bilinguals. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0139610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Humphries, T.; MacDougal, F. “Chaining” and other links: Making connections between American Sign Language and English in Two Types of School Settings. Vis. Anthropol. Rev. 2000, 15, 84–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kelly, A. Fingerspelling interaction: A set of deaf parents and their deaf daughter. In Sociolinguistics in Deaf Communities; Gallaudet University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1995; pp. 62–73. [Google Scholar]
- Cummins, J. The relationship between ASL proficiency and English academic development: A review of the research. In Proceedings of the Workshop Challenges, Opportunities, and Choices in Educating Minority Group Students, Hedmar University College, Elverum, Norway, 20 November 2006. [Google Scholar]
- Ellis, R. Introduction: Investigating Form-Focused Instruction. Lang. Learn. 2001, 51, 1–46. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lyster, R. Research on form-focused instruction in immersion classrooms: Implications for theory and practice. J. Fr. Lang. Stud. 2004, 14, 321–341. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Silvestri, J.A. An Analysis of the Reading Strategies Used by Deaf and Hearing Adults: Similarities and Differences in Phonological Processing and Metacognition. Ph.D. Thesis, Columbia University, New York, NY, USA, May 2016. [Google Scholar]
- Schleper, D.R. Principles for Reading to Deaf Children; Gallaudet University Press: Washington, DC, USA, 1996. [Google Scholar]
- Wolsey, J.-L.A.; Clark, M.D.; Andrews, J.F. ASL and English bilingual Shared Book Reading: An exploratory intervention for signing deaf children. Biling. Res. J. 2018, 41, 221–237. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolbers, K.A. Using Balanced and Interactive Writing Instruction to Improve the Higher Order and Lower Order Writing Skills of Deaf Students. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2008, 13, 257–277. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dostal, H.M.; Wolbers, K.A.; Kilpatrick, J. Differentiating writing instruction for students who are deaf and hard of hearing. Writ. Pedagog. 2016, in press. [Google Scholar]
- Bowers, L.M.; Dostal, H.M.; Wolbers, K.A.; Graham, S.C. The Assessment of Written Phrasal Constructs and Grammar of Deaf and Hard of Hearing Students with Varying Expressive Language Abilities. Educ. Res. Int. 2018, 2018, 2139626. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolbers, K.A.; Dostal, H.; Graham, S.; Branum-Martin, L.; Kilpatrick, J.; Saulsburry, R. Strategic and Interactive Writing Instruction: An Efficacy Study in Grades 3–5. J. Educ. Dev. Psychol. 2018, 8, 99. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dostal, H.M.; Wolbers, K.A. Examining Student Writing Proficiencies Across Genres: Results of an Intervention Study. Deaf. Educ. Int. 2016, 18, 159–169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolbers, K.A.; Dostal, H.M.; Skerrit, P.; Stephenson, B. The impact of three years of professional development on knowledge and implementation. J. Educ. Res. 2017, 110, 61–71. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolbers, K.A.; Dostal, H.M.; Graham, S.; Cihak, D.; Kilpatrick, J.R. Rachel Saulsburry The writing performance of elementary students receiving strategic and interactive writing instruction. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2015, 20, 385–398. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Bowers, L.M.; Dostal, H.; McCarthy, J.H.; Schwarz, I.; Wolbers, K.A. An Analysis of Deaf Students’ Spelling Skills During a Year-Long Instructional Writing Approach. Commun. Disord. Q. 2016, 37, 160–170. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Dostal, H.M.; Bowers, L.; Wolbers, K.A.; Gabriel, R. “We Are Authors”: A Qualitative Analysis of Deaf Students’ Writing During One Year of Strategic and Interactive Writing Instruction (SIWI). Rev. Disabil. Stud. Int. J. 2015, 11, 1–19. [Google Scholar]
- Wolbers, K.A.; Dostal, H.M.; Bowers, L.M. “I Was Born Full Deaf.” Written Language Outcomes After 1 Year of Strategic and Interactive Writing Instruction. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2012, 17, 19–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
- Wolbers, K.A. Strategic and Interactive Writing Instruction (SIWI): Apprenticing Deaf Students in the Construction of English Text. ITL Int. J. Appl. Linguist. 2008, 156, 299–326. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Menéndez, B. Cross-modal bilingualism: Language contact as evidence of linguistic transfer in sign bilingual education. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 2010, 13, 201–223. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wolbers, K.A.; Graham, S.C.; Dostal, H.M.; Bowers, L.M. A description of ASL features in writing. Ampersand 2014, 1, 19–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
- Wolbers, K.A.; Bowers, L.M.; Dostal, H.M.; Graham, S.C. Deaf writers’ application of American Sign Language knowledge to English. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 2014, 17, 410–428. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Banner, A.; Wang, Y. An analysis of the reading strategies used by adult and student deaf readers. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2011, 16, 2–23. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Bélanger, N.N.; Rayner, K. What Eye Movements Reveal about Deaf Readers. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2015, 24, 220–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Cummins, J. Linguistic Interdependence and the Educational Development of Bilingual Children. Rev. Educ. Res. 1979, 49, 222–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Grushkin, D.A. Written Sign Languages: What For? What Form? Am. Ann. Deaf 2017, 161, 509–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Moores, D.F. Writing Signed Languages: What For? What Form? A Response. Am. Ann. Deaf 2017, 161, 537–539. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Mayer, C. Written Forms of Signed languages: A Route to Literacy for Deaf Learners? Am. Ann. Deaf 2017, 161, 552–559. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Paul, P.V. Thoughts About a Possible Bridge From ASL to English Literacy. Am. Ann. Deaf 2017, 161, 505–508. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- DeLana, M.; Gentry, M.A.; Andrews, J.F. The Efficacy of ASL/English Bilingual Education: Considering Public Schools. Am. Ann. Deaf 2007, 152, 73–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Svartholm, K. 35 Years of Bilingual Deaf Education-and Then? Educ. Rev. 2014, 2, 33–50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- SPM. Årsredovisning—Specialskole Myndigheten [Yearly Reports—National Agency for Special Schools for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing]; SPM: Örebro, Sweden, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- Svartholm, K. Bilingual education for deaf children in Sweden. Int. J. Biling. Educ. Biling. 2010, 13, 159–174. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Rydberg, E.; Gellerstedt, L.C.; Danermark, B. Toward an equal level of educational attainment between deaf and hearing people in Sweden? J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2009, 14, 312–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- SPM. Årsredovisning—Specialskole Myndigheten [Yearly Reports—National Agency for Special Schools for the Deaf and Hard of Hearing]; SPM: Stockholm, Sweden, 2013. [Google Scholar]
- Dammeyer, J.; Marschark, M. Level of educational attainment among deaf adults who attended bilingual-bicultural programs. J. Deaf Stud. Deaf Educ. 2016, 21, 394–402. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Nover, S.; Andrews, J.; Baker, S.; Everhart, V.; Bradford, M. Critical Pedagogy in Deaf Education: Bilingual Methodology and Staff Development. Final Year Impact Report, USDLC Star Schools Project; New Mexico School for the Deaf: Santa Fe, New Mexico, 2002. [Google Scholar]
- Andrews, J.F.; Ferguson, C.; Roberts, S.; Hodges, P. What’s Up, Billy Jo? Deaf Children and Bilingual-Bicultural Instruction in East-Central Texas. Am. Ann. Deaf 1997, 142, 16–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- NCELA. English Learner Toolkit for State and Local Education Agencies; Synergy Enterprises, Inc.: Silver Spring, MD, USA, 2015.
- Hauser, P.C.; O’Hearn, A.; McKee, M.; Steider, A.; Thew, D. Deaf Epistemology: Deafhood and Deafness. Am. Ann. Deaf 2010, 154, 486–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Singleton, J.L.; Supalla, S.J.; Litchfield, S.; Schley, S. From Sign to Word: Considering Modality Constraints in ASL/English Bilingual Education. Top. Lang. Disord. 1998, 18, 16–29. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Komesaroff, L.R.; McLean, M.A. Being There is Not Enough: Inclusion is both Deaf and Hearing. Deaf. Educ. Int. 2006, 8, 88–100. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Skyer, M.E. Deaf Gains in Education. In Proceedings of the National Technical Institute for the Deaf Conference, Rochester, NY, USA, 18 April 2018. [Google Scholar]
© 2019 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Howerton-Fox, A.; Falk, J.L. Deaf Children as ‘English Learners’: The Psycholinguistic Turn in Deaf Education. Educ. Sci. 2019, 9, 133. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci9020133
Howerton-Fox A, Falk JL. Deaf Children as ‘English Learners’: The Psycholinguistic Turn in Deaf Education. Education Sciences. 2019; 9(2):133. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci9020133
Chicago/Turabian StyleHowerton-Fox, Amanda, and Jodi L. Falk. 2019. "Deaf Children as ‘English Learners’: The Psycholinguistic Turn in Deaf Education" Education Sciences 9, no. 2: 133. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci9020133
APA StyleHowerton-Fox, A., & Falk, J. L. (2019). Deaf Children as ‘English Learners’: The Psycholinguistic Turn in Deaf Education. Education Sciences, 9(2), 133. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci9020133