As Symbol as That: Inconsistencies in Symbol Systems of Alleles in Textbooks, and Students’ Justifications for Them
Abstract
:1. Introduction
- What are the symbol systems used in genetics textbooks and the justifications provided for their use?
- How do students justify the use of different symbol systems when solving genetics problems?
2. Materials and Methods
2.1. Mapping Genetics Textbooks
2.2. Interviewing Students
2.2.1. Study Context
2.2.2. Data Sources
- (a)
- Does one of the solutions appear to be better or more correct to you? Which one and why?
- (b)
- Why was the same letter (D) used to symbolize both phenomena?
- (c)
- Do you prefer using one of the symbols over the other? Which one and why?
- (d)
- If both symbols symbolize the same thing (same allele), why use different symbols?
2.2.3. Data Analysis
3. Results
3.1. Symbol Systems in Genetics Textbooks
“The allele for the production of a large amount of the tyrosine transporter protein will be represented as B and the allele for the production of a small amount of the tyrosine transporter protein will be represented as b. Meaning, that for the gene for the tyrosine transporter protein there are two types of alleles: allele B and allele b.”
“The allele for the production of enzyme A is customarily represented as IA.”
“the ABO group is determined by the alleles A,B and O. A and B alleles are both dominant over the O allele.”
“humans’ blood type is co-defined by 3 different alleles: A, B and O. A and B are co-dominant and rule over O.”
“It is customary to represent dominant alleles by upper case Latin letters and recessive alleles by lower case Latin letters.”
3.2. Students’ Justifications for the Use of Different Symbol Systems
Ben: “It would just complicate things if we used more than one letter for the same gene.”
Gali: “No, [I would use] the same letter, because they told me it’s the same gene that affects both traits, so I would mark it, like, with the same letter.”
3.2.1. Type 1: Students Relying on Arguments Deeply Embedded in the Genetic Context
“Because in the first solution, if the two alleles are co-dominant then D1 and D2 together will create something new and different, between a functional and non-functional leg, and it doesn’t say that it’s possible that something like this will happen. So, it’s much more logical that they’re just dominant and recessive.”
Chen: “…and I think solution two is more logical because right here you can see that the dominant is a functional leg, now that means that these two [Punnet squares of solution b] are correct, that the dominant affects the recessive.”
Abigail: “What I mentioned, in the beginning, that here it refers to this in the heredity of the recessive or dominant gene, and then it’s more suitable to write it using a lowercase d and an uppercase D. Here it’s co-dominance, so D1 and D2 is more suitable.”
“Heredity of dominant and recessive, so, according to how they taught us in the classroom, then you write it with a capital letter and a lowercase letter, although in reality, that [solution a] comes out right too.”
“Because I think that as long as it’s the same letter and we know it’s the same gene, um, but it’s more accurate when it’s written like that. It explains to us the modes of inheritance by which it is transferred.”
“Because here they work as dominant–recessive and they’re not co-dominant. They’re not creating anything new. So the same alleles will be marked differently and they will create two different traits. And in one trait they will be marked as co-dominant and in the other, they will be marked dominant–recessive.”
“Yes, it could, but I just don’t think it’s efficient to represent it like this because they’re simply not co-dominant.”
“I think that strengthens the argument [to use one convention only] but it just doesn’t make sense that they are dominant.”
3.2.2. Type 2: Students Relying on Non-Genetic Arguments
“It’s easier for me to label it this way (Aa), unless the direction was to label it with this [A1A2], um, I forgot how you call that capital letter with the number.”
“Yes, we had a test. Not on the test…actually, she [the teacher] didn’t say and still I wrote… like, I don’t really remember why I wrote it. It was like, in the question… I don’t really remember… because the heading of the test had this word that I don’t remember.”
“Yes, these things, uh, with 1 and 2? So, in class I had to do a project on genetic illnesses and we did this comparison and that’s how it showed up on the internet.”
“You do this when you talk about, um, there’s like no separation in this but somehow when she [the teacher] writes it on the board then sometimes she does this and sometimes that. So there’s some kind of separation but in reality, it should say the same thing.”
“The ratio between the big eye and the little eye is different than that between the normal and abnormal leg.”
“That probably won’t be accurate, because there must be a reason for putting numbers in addition to the two lowercase d’s.”
“It would be more convenient, but there must be a reason they didn’t do that. I want to believe that there’s a reason.”
“clearer to the eye, because here [D1D2] you see two D’s and you need to look at the number, and here [Dd] you see the difference.”
“I think it makes it easier for me because it’s still as if there’s a difference between them but it’s still as if they are the same with a difference between them.”
“Truthfully, D1 and D2 confuses me, like you saw. I prefer using capital D and lowercase d...Less numbers.”
“It really shows me the difference.”
“…for the simple reason that I won’t have those misguided associations about dominant and non-dominant. That’s how I got used to thinking.”
“It’s not that the numbers are confusing, it’s just that from the beginning, in class, we learned that the capital D is dominant and the lowercase d is recessive. It’s easier for me to see that it’s capital and lowercase.”
Enav: “The letters, I’m more used to working with them. In class we use the capital, lowercase letters. Not that it’s critical.”
Hila: “I think I would choose solution B (with the Aa system) because it’s much more familiar to me.”
“We’re talking about eyes here and legs here, so it was easier when it’s different symbols.”
“It’s much more logical for me when there’s a separation between the, um, that if you talk about one thing it has a letter and if you talk about something else it has a different letter.”
“So maybe it can explain this choice, because they have the same strength, so it’s the same capital letter, but what distinguishes them is the 1 and 2.”
4. Discussion
Author Contributions
Funding
Acknowledgments
Conflicts of Interest
References
- Gilbert, J.K.; Justi, R. The Contribution of Visualisation to Modelling-Based Teaching. In Modelling-Based Teaching in Science Education; Springer International Publishing: Cham, Switzerland, 2016; pp. 121–148. [Google Scholar]
- Duval, R. A Cognitive Analysis of Problems of Comprehension in a Learning of Mathematics. Educ. Stud. Math. 2006, 61, 103–131. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Justi, R.; Gilbert, J.K.; Ferreira, P.F.M. The Application of a ‘Model of Modelling’ to Illustrate the Importance of Metavisualisation in Respect of the Three Types of Representation. In Multiple Representations in Chemical Education; Models and Modeling in Science Education; Gilbert, J.K., Treagust, D., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2009; Volume 4, pp. 285–307. [Google Scholar]
- Kozma, R.; Russell, J. Students Becoming Chemists: Developing Representational Competence. In Visualization in Science Education; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2005; pp. 121–145. [Google Scholar]
- Keig, P.F.; Rubba, P.A. Translation of Representations of the Structure of Matter and Its Relationship to Reasoning, Gender, Spatial Reasoning, and Specific Prior Knowledge. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1993, 30, 883–903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Kozma, R.; Russell, J. Multimedia and Understanding: Expert and Novice Responses to Different Representations of Chemical Phenomena. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1997, 34, 949–968. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Van Mil, M.H.W.; Boerwinkel, D.J.; Waarlo, A.J. Modelling Molecular Mechanisms: A Framework of Scientific Reasoning to Construct Molecular-Level Explanations for Cellular Behaviour. Sci. Educ. 2013, 22, 93–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schönborn, K.J.; Anderson, T.R.; Grayson, D.J. Student Difficulties with the Interpretation of a Textbook Diagram of Immunoglobulin G (IgG). Biochem. Mol. Biol. Educ. 2002, 30, 93–97. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gilbert, J.K. Visualization: A Metacognitive Skill in Science and Science Education. In Visualization in Science Education; Gilbert, J.K., Ed.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2005; pp. 9–27. [Google Scholar]
- Johnstone, A.H. Why Is Science Difficult to Learn? Things Are Seldom What They Seem. J. Comput. Assist. Learn. 1991, 7, 75–83. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marbach-Ad, G.; Stavy, R. Students’ Cellular and Molecular Explanations of Genetic Phenomena. J. Biol. Educ. 2000, 34, 200–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ainsworth, S. The Educational Value of Multiple-Representations When Learning Complex Scientific Concepts. In Visualization: Theory and Practice in Science Education; Gilbert, J.K., Reiner, M., Nakhleh, M., Eds.; Springer: Dordrecht, The Netherlands, 2008; pp. 191–208. [Google Scholar]
- Peirce, C.S. Prolegomena to an Apology for Pragmaticism. Monist 1906, 16, 492–546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schnotz, W. Towards an Integrated View of Learning from Text and Visual Displays. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 2002, 14, 101–120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schnotz, W.; Bannert, M. Construction and Interference in Learning from Multiple Representation. Learn. Instr. 2003, 13, 141–156. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Markovits, Z.; Sowder, J.T. Students’ Understanding of the Relationship between Fractions and Decimals. Focus Learn. Probl. Math. 1991, 13, 3–11. [Google Scholar]
- Vamvakoussi, X.; Vosniadou, S. How Many Decimals Are There between Two Fractions? Aspects of Secondary School Students’ Understanding of Rational Numbers and Their Notation. Cognit. Instr. 2010, 28, 181–209. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Marbach-Ad, G. Attempting to Break the Code in Student Comprehension of Genetic Concepts. J. Biol. Educ. 2001, 35, 183–189. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncan, R.G.; Reiser, B.J. Reasoning across Ontologically Distinct Levels: Students’ Understandings of Molecular Genetics. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 2007, 44, 938–959. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Haskel-Ittah, M.; Yarden, A. Toward Bridging the Mechanistic Gap Between Genes and Traits by Emphasizing the Role of Proteins in a Computational Environment. Sci. Educ. 2017, 26, 1143–1160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Stewart, J.; Van Kirk, J. Understanding and Problem-Solving in Classical Genetics. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 1990, 12, 575–588. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wynne, C.F.; Stewart, J.; Passmore, C. High School Students’ Use of Meiosis When Solving Genetics Problems. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2001, 23, 501–515. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gericke, N.M.; Hagberg, M. Conceptual Incoherence as a Result of the Use of Multiple Historical Models in School Textbooks. Res. Sci. Educ. 2010, 40, 605–623. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wood-Robinson, C. Young People’s Ideas about Inheritance and Evolution. Stud. Sci. Educ. 1994, 24, 29–47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Pashley, M. A-Level Students: Their Problems with Gene and Allele. J. Biol. Educ. 1994, 28, 120–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Lewis, J.; Leach, J.; Wood-Robinson, C. All in the Genes?—Young People’s Understanding of the Nature of Genes. J. Biol. Educ. 2000, 34, 74–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Wood-Robinson, C.; Lewis, J.; Leach, J. Young People’s Understanding of the Nature of Genetic Information in the Cells of an Organism. J. Biol. Educ. 2000, 35, 29–36. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Duncan, R.G.; Castro-Faix, M.; Choi, J. Informing A Learning Progression in Genetics: Which Should Be Taught First, Mendelian Inheritance or the Central Dogma of Molecular Biology. Int. J. Sci. Math. Educ. 2016, 14, 445–472. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Digisi, L.L.; Willett, J.B. What High School Biology Teachers Say about Their Textbook Use: A Descriptive Study. J. Res. Sci. Teach. 1995, 32, 123–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Justi, R.; Gilbert, J.K. Modelling, Teachers’ Views on the Nature of Modelling, and Implications for the Education of Modellers. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2002, 24, 369–387. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Schönborn, K.J.; Anderson, T.R. A Model of Factors Determining Students’ Ability to Interpret External Representations in Biochemistry. Int. J. Sci. Educ. 2009, 31, 193–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Chi, M.T.H.; Feltovich, P.J.; Glaser, R. Categorization and Representation of Physics Problems by Experts and Novices. Cognit. Sci. 1981, 5, 121–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Hmelo-Silver, C.E.; Marathe, S.; Liu, L. Fish Swim, Rocks Sit, and Lungs Breathe: Expert-Novice Understanding of Complex Systems. J. Learn. Sci. 2007, 16, 307–331. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Thörne, K.; Gericke, N.; Hagberg, M. Linguistic Challenges in Mendelian Genetics: Teachers’ Talk in Action. Sci. Educ. 2013, 97, 695–722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Ainsworth, S. DeFT: A Conceptual Framework for Considering Learning with Multiple Representations. Learn. Instr. 2006, 16, 183–198. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Adadan, E. Using Multiple Representations to Promote Grade 11 Students’ Scientific Understanding of the Particle Theory of Matter. Res. Sci. Educ. 2013, 43, 1079–1105. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Goldman, S.R. Learning in Complex Domains: When and Why Do Multiple Representations Help? Learn. Instr. 2003, 13, 239–244. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Gericke, N.; Hagberg, M.; Doris, J. Upper Secondary Students’ Understanding of the Use of Multiple Models in Biology Textbooks—The Importance of Conceptual Variation and Incommensurability. Res. Sci. Edu. 2012, 43, 755–780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
- Sherman, F. Getting Started with Yeast. Methods Enzymol. 1991, 194, 3–21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Textbook | Symbol System | Context |
---|---|---|
A1 | Uppercase/lowercase (A, a) | Dominant/recessive |
One letter accompanied by another letter (IA, IB, i) | Blood types as an example of multiple alleles | |
Two distinct letters (A, S) | Symbols for hemoglobin (in relation to sickle cell anemia) | |
A2 | Uppercase/lowercase (A, a) | Dominant/recessive |
One letter accompanied by a number (A1, A2) | Incomplete dominance | |
One letter accompanied by another letter (IA, IB, i) | Blood types | |
A3 | Uppercase/lowercase (A, a) | Dominant/recessive |
One letter accompanied by a number (A1, A2) | Incomplete dominance | |
One letter accompanied by another letter (IA, IB, i) | Blood types as an example for multiple alleles | |
B1 | Uppercase/lowercase (A, a) | Dominant/recessive |
Three distinct letters (A/B/O) | Blood type | |
B2 | Uppercase/lowercase (A, a) | Dominant/recessive |
Three distinct letters (A/B/O) | Blood type as an example of co-dominance | |
B3 | Uppercase/lowercase (A, a) | Dominant/recessive |
One letter with/without a tag (A/A’) | Co-dominance | |
B4 | Uppercase/lowercase (A, a) | Dominant/recessive |
Three distinct letters (A/B/O) | Blood type as an example of multiple alleles | |
C1 | Uppercase/lowercase (A, a) | Dominant/recessive |
Three distinct letters (A/B/O) | Blood type as an example of multiple alleles | |
One letter with/without a tag (A/A’) | Incomplete dominance | |
X for a chromosome and another letter for the allele (XA/X) | Sex linkage |
© 2018 by the authors. Licensee MDPI, Basel, Switzerland. This article is an open access article distributed under the terms and conditions of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/).
Share and Cite
Livni-Alcasid, G.A.; Haskel-Ittah, M.; Yarden, A. As Symbol as That: Inconsistencies in Symbol Systems of Alleles in Textbooks, and Students’ Justifications for Them. Educ. Sci. 2018, 8, 110. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci8030110
Livni-Alcasid GA, Haskel-Ittah M, Yarden A. As Symbol as That: Inconsistencies in Symbol Systems of Alleles in Textbooks, and Students’ Justifications for Them. Education Sciences. 2018; 8(3):110. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci8030110
Chicago/Turabian StyleLivni-Alcasid, Gur A., Michal Haskel-Ittah, and Anat Yarden. 2018. "As Symbol as That: Inconsistencies in Symbol Systems of Alleles in Textbooks, and Students’ Justifications for Them" Education Sciences 8, no. 3: 110. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci8030110
APA StyleLivni-Alcasid, G. A., Haskel-Ittah, M., & Yarden, A. (2018). As Symbol as That: Inconsistencies in Symbol Systems of Alleles in Textbooks, and Students’ Justifications for Them. Education Sciences, 8(3), 110. https://doi.org/10.3390/educsci8030110